SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 111
The Sentence Patterns of
Language
Prof. Dr. Nalan KIZILTAN
The language is the set of sentences that are described by
the grammer. The grammer ‘’generates’’ the sentences it
describes and their structural descriptions. Chomsky(1957)
Knowing a language includes the ability to put words
together to form phrases and sentences that express our
thoughts. That part of the grammer that represents a
speaker’s knowledge of the structure of phrases and
sentences is called Syntax.
 Sequences of words that conform to the rules of syntax
are well formed or grammatical.
 Sequences of words that violate the syntactic rules are
ungrammatical.
There are two basic principles of sentence organization:
 Linear Order
 Hierarchical Structure
LINEAR ORDER
The words in a sentence must occur in a particular sequence
if the sentence is to convey the desired meaning.
1) John glanced at Mary.
2) Mary John at glanced.
3) Mary glanced at John. (the meaning is completely
different from that of (1).
HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE
The individual words in a sentence are organized into
natural, semantically coherent groupings which are
themselves organized into larger groupings. These
groupings within a sentence are called constituents of that
sentence. The relationships between constituents in a
sentence form the constituent structure of the sentence.
4) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
Many executives
Eat at really fancy restaurants.
These are constituents
Have meaning of their own.
And each makes a coherent
contribution to the meaning of
(4) as a whole.
Constituent Tests
1) Ability to stand alone
Constituents can be used alone as exclamations or as
answers to questions.
5) What do many executives do?
Eat at really fancy restaurants.
2) Substitution by a pro-form
A constituant can be replaced by a single word having the
same meaning. Pronouns are one type of pro-form; such as
do, be, have; pro-adverbs such as there and then; as well as
a pro-adjectives.
6) They eat at really fancy restaurants
- Who eats at really fancy restaurants?
7) Many executives do.
3) Movement
If some part of a sentence can be moved around – usually
to the beginning or end of the sentence – it is a constituent.
8) At really fancy restaurants, many executives eat.
9) How fancy was it?
- Really fancy
10) Who eats at really fancy restaurants?
Many executives eat at such restaurants.
11) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
In addition to the linear order of words in a sentence, a tree
diagram is another way of representing the hierarchical nature
of constituent structure. Branching structures in which each
constituent forms a “branch”
Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
at really fancy restaurants
really fancy restaurants
fancy restaurants
The child found the puppy
the child found the puppy
the child found the puppy
the puppy
Ambiguity
In every human language we can find individual expressions
which have two or more distinct meanings.
12) Long raises miniature badgers and raccoons.
long raises miniature badgers and raccoons.
raises 1 miniature badgers and raccoons.
miniature badgers and raccoons.
badgers and raccoons.
2 miniature badgers and raccoons.
miniature badgers and raccoons.
miniature badgers
13) We need more intelligent leaders
we 1 need more intelligent leaders
need more intelligent leaders
more intelligent leaders
(A greater quantity of
intelligent leaders)
14) more intelligent leaders
more intelligent leaders
more intelligent
(Leaders who are more intelligent)
Structural ambiguity can be explicitly accounted for by
multiple tree structures.
In (13) Intelligent leaders is a constituent: for this reason (13)
represents the interpretation ‘‘a greater quantity of intelligent
leaders.’’ In (14), however, more intelligent forms a
constituent: (14) therefore represents the meaning ‘‘leaders
who are more intelligent.’’
Although structural ambiguity is a very common kind of
ambiguity, it is not the only kind. Individual words are
sometimes ambiguous: for example, ‘‘crane’’ can refer to
either a kind of bird or to a large construction device. Because
words like crane have no internal constituent structure,
sentences containing such words clearly can`t be structurally
ambiguous. Instead, a sentence containing a word with more
than one meaning is said to be lexically ambiguous. Thus, both
meanings for (13)-(14) are represented with the same
constituent structure.
EXERCISES
0. Apply the constituent tests to determine if the underlined
expressions in the following sentences are constituents.
a. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies.
b. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies.
c. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies.
1) Which of the underlined expressions in the following
sentences are constituents? Which are not? Why?
(Use the constituency tests mentioned before to show this
in each case.)
a. Marianne ate the stale candy.
b. Marianne ate the stale candy
c. My little brother snores.
d. My little brother snores.
2) Apply each of the constituency tests to the underlined
expressions and determine whether they are constituents.
a. The clouds rolled across the sky.
b. My uncle crushed our new car
c. Mattingly hit the ball over the fence.
d. Some students hate computers.
e. The ancient ruins of the temple were covered by the earth.
f. The women wept.
g. Too many noisy birds are nesting on campus.
h. Joggers like to run along the river.
i. The bride and groom ran out of the church.
 1) The rules of syntax combine words into phrases
phrases into sentences
English: SUBJECT - VERB - OBJECT (SVO) language
Turkish: SUBJECT - OBJECT - VERB (SOV) language
 2) Syntactic rules also specify other constraints that
sentences must adhere to.
a) To find + NP = YES
* To find + ADV = NO
b) To sleep + ADV = YES
* To sleep + NP = NO
SYNTACTIC RULES
3) Syntactic rules show how words form groups in a
sentence
 Syntactic rules show how they are hierarchically
arranged with respect to one another.
4) Syntactic rules reveal the grammatical relations
among the words of a sentence as well as their order and
hierarchical organization.
5) Syntactic rules explain how the grouping of words
relates to its meaning, such as when a sentence or phrase
is ambiguous.
6) The rules of syntax permit speakers to produce and
understand a limitless number of sentences never
produced are heard before – the creative aspect of
Linguistic knowledge.
A major goal of linguistics is to show clearly and
explicitly how syntactic rules account for this
knowledge.
A theory of grammar must provide a complete
characterization of what speakers implicitly know about
their knowledge.
WHAT GRAMMATICALITY IS NOT BASED ON
Grammaticality ≠ meaningfulness.
Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.
A verb crumpled the milk
Syntactically well formed
Semantically not acceptable=Nonsense
Grammaticality does not depend on the truth of
sentences. If it did, lying would be impossible.
The syntactic rules that permit us to produce,
understand, make grammaticality judgements are
unconscious rules. The grammar is a mental grammar,
different from the prescriptive one.
(7) S= NP + VP
John snored.
Everyone fled the volcano.
The major smoked a cigar.
A book lay on the table.
(8) NP= Det + N
the mayor
a book
every student
my python
(9) VP= V + NP
fled the volcano
smoked a cigar
imitated a flamingo
squeezed some fresh orange juice
We can represent these tree pieces of information in a
succinct way with the following three phrase structure
rules.
(10) S NP VP
(11) NP Det N
(12) VP V NP
The arrow in these rules can be read as ‘may consist of.’
Thus rule (10) is just a concise way of saying ‘a sentence
may consist of a noun phrase followed by a verb phrase’;
similarly, rule (11) just says ‘a noun phrase may consist of a
determiner followed by a noun.’ Now, what does rule (12)
say?
Recall from File 52 that a syntactic category is a group of
constituents with different structures which share certain
properties. NP’s, for example, could be composed of DET +
N, as in (2) above, bur they may also have the following
structures:
(13) My uncle from France
A rule for this NP would look like:
(14) NP NP PP
Rules (10) (11) (12) and (14) specify five ways in which
constituents can be combined to form larger, more
complex constituents. Some constituents, however, do not
result from the combination of smaller constituents;
instead, they consist of a single word. For example, a noun
phrase may just consist of a proper name (e.g., John,
Paris), a plural noun (e.g., elephants, leaves), or a noun
referring to a substance (e.g., clay, gasoline); similarly, a
verb phrase may just consist of an intransitive verb such as
sneeze, die, vanish, or elapse. These sorts of knowledge
can also be represented with PS rules:
15 NP N
16 VP V
Phrasal Categories
A phrasal category is a set of constituents which behave
the same or share the same functions and distribution.
1) Noun Phrase (NP) A noun or a noun + other words
(1) ____________ ran through the park.
a) susan e) some children
b) students f) a huge lovable bear
c)you g) my friend from turkey
d) most dogs h) the people that we interviewed
2) Verb Phrase (VP) a single verb or verb + other forms
verb + ADV
verb + NP+ PP
(prepositional phrase)
(2) John ________.
a) saw a clown
b) is smart
c) slept
d) found the cake
e) found the cake in the cupboard
3 Adjective Phrase a single adjective or as a constituent of
NPs.
(3) John is ____________.
a) smart
b) very intelligent
c) as tall as his father
d) smarter than the average bear
e) certain to win
4 Adverbial Phrase Adverb or as a constituent of VPs
and ADVPs.
(4) John speaks ____________.
a) fluently
b) as fluent as a native
c) almost certainly
5 Prepositional Phrase preposition followed by a NP.
a) from Turkey
b) at university
c) through the park
d) in the cupboard
6) Sentences are often used by themselves.
(5) it is raining.
I like apple pie.
Phrase Structure Trees
 A diagram with syntactic category information
provided is called phrase structure tree. This tree
shows that a sentence is both a linear string of words
and hierarchical structure with phrases nested in
phrases.
 Three aspects of speakers’ syntactic knowledge of
sentence structure are disclosed in phrase structure
trees:
 1 the linear order of the words in the sentence
 2 the groupings of words into syntactic categories
 3 the hierarchical structure of the syntactic categories
Sentence
NP VP
V NP
(1) The child found the puppy
S
NP VP
Det N Vt NP
Det N
the child found the puppy
(2) my mother likes her cats
S
NP VP
Det N Vt NP
Det N
my mother likes her cats
(3)Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing
hobbies.
S
NP
Det ADJ N
VP
Vt NP PP
Det N P NP
ADJ N
many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies
(4) The boy saw the man with the telescope
S
the boy saw the man with the telescope
‘The boy used a telescope to see the man.’
NP
Det N
VP
Vt NP PP
Det
N P NP
Det
N
(5) The boy saw the man with the telescope
S
the boy saw the man with the telescope
NP
Det N
VP
Vt NP
PP
Det
N
P NP
Det
N
(6) The baby slept
S
the baby slept
NP
Det N
VP
Vit
(7) the girl with the feather on the ribbon on the brim.
NP
the girl with the feather on the ribbon on the brim
NP
Det PP
P
N
PP
P NP
Det N PP
P NP
N
Det
N
Det
 One can repeat the number of NPs under PPs under NPs
without a limit. This property of all human languages is
called recursion. (infinite recursion) (productivity)
The man fled from the posse.
S
the man fled from the posse.
NP
Det N
VP
PP
Vit
NP
P
Det N
A girl laughed at the monkey.
S
a girl laughed at the monkey
NP
Det N
VP
PP
Vit
NP
P
Det N
A large fierce black dog looked out the window.
S
a large fierce black dog looked out the window.
NP
Det Adj
VP
PP
Vt
NP
P
Det
N
Adj Adj N
The man in the kitchen drives a truck.
S
the man in the kitchen drives a truck.
NP
Det N
VP
NP
Vt
Det N
NP PP
NP
P
Det N
Lexical Categories
Each word in a given language is not entirely different in
its behavior from all the other words in that language.
Instead, a large number of words exhibit the same
properties. Words can be grouped into some word classes
based on their morphological and syntactic properties.
These word classes are called lexical categories, because
the lexicon is the list of all the words in a language (plus
various kinds of information, about those words.) For
example, one of the morphological properties of the word
book is that it has a plural form books. All these words can
occur in the following context:
(1) __________ + plural morpheme.
The words that occur in the morphological frame can also
occur in the syntactic frames as in (2) and (3):
(2) DET _________
(3) DET ADJ _________
Verbs
(4)_________ + tense morpheme (sing-song; walk-walked)
(5)________ + third person singular morpheme (I walk, he
walks)
(6)_________ + progressive morpheme (I am walking)
(7)AUX__________ (may go)
(8) (please)___________! ((please) leave!)
Adjectives
(9)_________ + er/est (taller, tallest)
(10) more/most+ _____(more affectionate, most affectionate)
(11) DET____________ N (the unexpected guests)
(12) Linking verbs__________(looks sunny)
(13) ADV___________(amazingly perceptive)
Adverbs
(14) ADV (ADJ+ly) (happily, unexpectedly, skillfully)
(15) ________ ADJ (unusually nice)
__________VERB or VERB PHRASE (usually go)
__________ ADV (quite well)
Transformational Rules
The phrase structure rules characterize an infinite number
of different kinds of sentences; yet there remain many types
of common sentences not accounted for.
The boy is sleeping.
The boy can sleep.
The boy will sleep.
Words like is, can, will are in class of Auxiliary Verbs or
Auxiliary(AUX) which includes be and have as well as may,
might, would, could and several others. They are called
‘helping verbs’ or ‘modals’. They occur in such structures as
this one:
The boy is sleeping.
S
the boy is sleeping
NP
Art N
VP
Vi
AUX
The other sentences which include Auxiliaries:
Is the boy sleeping? The interrogative or question sentences
Can the boy sleep? are related to their ‘declarative’
Will the boy sleep? counterparts in a simple way. In the
questions the AUX occurs at the beginning
of the sentence rather than after the subject NP.
The two sentences may be said to be transformationally related
or related by an operation called a Transformation or
Ttransformational Rule.
S
Is the boy sleeping
AUX
Det N
VP
V
NP
Wh-Question Transformation
(i) Place the first auxiliary of the sentence to the left of the
subject NP
(ii) (the first NP in the sentence).
Move the PROwh (e.g., what, who, where, and how) to the
beginning of the sentence.
In general, transformations change sentences created by
phrase-structure rules into sentences with equivalent
meanings, but different structures. These changes are
accomplished by adding or deleting words, or rearranging
word order.
Part (i) of the wh-question transformation tells us that
(1) Mary can accomplish what?
Can be converted into
(1a) Can Mary accomplish what?
Part (ii) of the transformation further converts this into
(1b) what can Mary accomplish?
Linguists refer to the form of a sentence produced by phrase
structure rules alone as the Deep Structure of a sentence, and
the form that the sentence has after one or more
transformations has applied as the Surface Structure of the
sentence.
What can Mary accomplish?
S
what(x)
what can Mary accomplish?
PRO(wh)
NP
S
VP
AUX
Npr Vt NP
N(wh)
The boy who is sleeping was dreaming.
S
the boy who is sleeping was dreaming
AUX
NP S’
VP
V
NP
NP
Art N AUX VP
PRO V
Was the boy who is sleeping dreaming?
S
Was the boy who is sleeping dreaming?
AUX
NP S’
VP
V
NP
NP
Art N AUX VP
PRO V
The girl who loves John is pretty.
S
The girl who loves John is pretty.
Recursion
Recursion gives language its infinite aspect and accounts for much Linguistic
creativity.
NP S’
VP
V
NP
NP
Art N VP
PRO V NP
ADJP
ADJ
You mean you didnt know that I knew she didn’t know you knew that!
S1
You mean you didnt know that I knew she didn’t know you knew that!
NP
S2
NP Art
VP
V
NP
VP
V S3
Comp NP VP
V S4
AUX VP
V S5
NP VP
V NP
He believes that she loves the cat
He believes that she loves the cat
NP
S
NP
Art
VP
PRO
S’
V
Comp
NP
S
VP
V
PRO
N
SYNTAX:
LINEAR ORDER,
HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE,
AND AMBIGUITY
While a dictionary of all the words in any human language (at a
given time) can be made, it is impossible to compile a dictionary
of all the sentences of a language; unlike words, sentences are not
finite in number, and therefore sentences, unlike words, are not
learned individually. However, native speakers of a language seem
to get along amazingly well without such a dictionary for
sentences – they can use and understand sentences in their
language that they have not previously encountered. Why is it
that we cannot always properly use or understand a word in our
language that we have not heard before, but can spontaneously
produce and understand new sentences? If what we learn is not
the sentences themselves, what do we learn that enables us to
produce and understand an infinite number of sentences? This is
the question we will be concerned with as we consider syntax, the
study of the structure of phrases and sentences.
FILE 50
Though we use sentences all the time, we don’t normally
think about how they are structured. However, a little
consideration reveals that the principles by which words are
organized into sentences are, in fact, quite complex. In this
file, we will consider two basic principles of sentence
organization: linear order and hierarchical structure.
The most obvious principle of sentence organization is linear
order; the words in a sentence must occur in a particular
sequence if the sentence is to convey the desired meaning.
Consider, for example, the following sentence of English.
(1) John glanced at Mary.
If we rearrange the words in the sentence, we either come up
with nonsense, as in (2), (the *denotes an ungrammatical
expression):
(2) *Mary John at glanced.
or with a sentence whose meaning is distinctly different
from that of (1) -
(3) Mary glanced at John.
LINEAR ORDER
Clearly, the ordering of the words in sentences determines,
in part, whether a sentence is grammatically or not, and
what the sentence means.
One of the many rules of English requires that the
grammatical subject of a sentence normally precedes the
main verb, which in turn normally precedes its direct object;
thus, she resembles him is English (where she is subject
and him is the object), but resembles she him and she
him resembles are not. However, an important fact about
rules of word order is that they are language-specific--that
is, languages vary in the ways in which they order words.
HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE
Although linear order is an important principle of sentence
organization, sentences our world than just ordered sequences
of words; they have internal hierarchical structure as well. That
is, the individual words in a sentence are organized into
natural, semantically coherent groupings, which are
themselves organized into larger groupings, the largest
grouping of all being the sentence itself (and the smallest of all
being individual words). These groupings within a sentence are
called constituents of that sentence. The relationships
between constituents in a sentence form the constituent
structure of the sentence.
Ror example, consider the sentence in (4).
(4) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
We can easily distinguish and number of meaningful groups
of words in this sentence: many executives and eat at really
fancy restaurants, for instance, clearly have meaning of their
own, and each makes a coherent contribution to the
meaning of (4) as a whole. For these reasons, they are
constituents of this sentence. On the other hand, some
groups of words in sentence (4) do not naturally form
meaningful units; executives eat at and eat at really, for
example, don't clearly have meanings of their own. Thus,
these groups of words are not constituents of (4).
Constituents Tests
If a constituent is a semantically coherent group, then, of course,
sentences are always constituents, as are the individual words
within a sentence. In sentence (4), for instance, the largest
constituent is the sentence itself; the smallest constituents are the
individual words many, executives, eat, at, really, fancy, and
restaurants. Other constutiuents within a sentence are not always
so easy to identify. However, there are a number of useful tests for
distinguishing constituents, which are synthetic units, from mere
strings of words, which aren’t constituents, and therefore do not
behave as a single unit:
Ability to Stand Alone
Constituents can often be sensibly used alone, for example as
exclamations or as answers to questions:
(5) What do many executives do?
Eat at really fancy restaurants.
This isn't true of nonconstituents: if we were asked ‘‘Do fancy
restaurants do much business?’’ we couldn’t sensibly answer
*’’(Well), executives eat at.’’
Substitution by a Pro-Form
It is often possible to replace a constituent with a single word
having the same meaning as that constituent. For example,
if someone asked ‘‘What do many executives do?’’ we could
answer either with sentence (4) or with sentence (6) in
which the constituent many executives is replaced with the
single word they (which, in this context, would mean the
same thing as many executives). Note that it is a certain
category of word which is used for the substitution test,
namely, a pro-word or (pro-form). Pronouns are one type of
proform (e.g., he, she, it, they, us, her, and that). There are
pro-verbs such as do, be and have, pro-adverbs such as there
and then, as well as a pro-adjective such. You may use this
proforms when attempting to determine constituency.
(6) They eat at really fancy restaurants.
Similarly, if someone asked ‘‘Who eats at really fancy
restaurants?’’ we could answer either with (4) or with (7), in
which the constituent eat at really fancy restaurants is
replaced with the single word do (which would mean the
same thing in this context)
(7) Many executives do.
But there is no word that could possibly replace
nonconstituent eat at really in (4) and mean the same thing,
no matter what the question was asked.
Movement
If some part of a sentence can be moved around - usually to
the beginning or the end of the sentence - it is a constituent.
For example, one could say:
(8) At really fancy restaurants, many executives eat.
It may sound a bit stilted, but it sounds grammatical to many
English speakers in the right context. Thus, at really fancy
restaurants must be a constituent because it can be moved
to the front of the sentence. Compare this with:
(9) *fancy restaurants many executives at really.
This is not grammatical: fancy restaurants is not a
constituent in this sentence according to the movement test
since it cannot be moved.
Other Issues Concerning Constituents
Two points must be kept in mind regarding constituents.
First, given a group of words, we cannot say once and for all
whether or not it is a constituent; rather, we can only say
whether or not it is a constituent relative to a particular
sentence. To illustrate this, consider sentence (9) and (10).
(10) John and Bill raise weasels.
(11) Mary saw John and Bill talked to Jane.
In (10) John and Bill is a constituent: it functions as a
coherent, meaningful unit within the sentence - in
particular, as its subject. In (11), however, the very same
sequence of words is not a constituent: because John is the
direct object of the first clause and Bill is the subject of the
second clause, the sequence John and Bill does not make a
coherent contribution to the meaning of this sentence. We
can apply the constituent tests to verify this claim. For
example, although John and Bill can be replaced with they in
(10), this isn’t impossible in sentence (11). Thus, we can
properly say that a string of words is a constituent only with
respect to a particular sentence.
The second thing that must be kept in mind is that
constituent structure is hierarchical - that is, one constituent
may be part of another. What this means, in turn, is that
sentences are composed of parts which has been grouped
together before they are grouped into the sentence. Consider
sentence (4) again.
(4) many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
Among the constituents in this sentence is the sequence
really fancy. To see this, note that really fancy can be used by
itself:
(12) How fancy was it? - Really fancy.
and that it can be replaced with the single word such:
( 13) Who eats at really fancy restaurants?
Mant executives eat at such restaurants.
But really fancy is also part of a larger constituent, namely
really fancy restaurants; this in turn is part of a larger
constituent, at really fast restaurants, which is itself part of
the still larger constituent eat at really fancy restaurants and
ultimately the largest constituent in the sentence, namely
the sentence itself. If we underline each of the constituents
in (4), the hierarchical nature of its constituent structure is
easier to see:
(14) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
 Underlining is, as in (14), on way of presenting the
hierarchical nature of constituent structure. Another way
is with tree diagrams: branching structures in which each
constituent forms a ‘‘branch.’’ For example, the tree
diagram for sentence (4) is in (15):
S
Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants
(b)
(a) (c)
In this diagram, each of the constituents of sentence (4)
forms of branch: for example, many executives corresponds
to the branch labelled (a); at really fancy restaurants, to the
branch labelled (b) and really fancy, to the branch labelled
(c). Observe, in addition, that groups of words that are not
constituents of sentence (4) do not form branches in this
tree diagram: executives *eat at and *eat at really, for
instance, clearly aren’t constituents according to the diagram
in (15). In principle, underlining is just as good as tree
diagrams for representing constituent structure, but because
tree diagrams are somewhat easier to read, they are usually
preferred.
Ambiguity
In every human language we can find individual expressions
which have two or more distinct meanings. For example, the
italicized portions of the following sentences of English can
be interpreted in more than one way:
(16) a. Larry raises miniature badgers and raccoons.
b. We need more intelligent leaders.
c. The cranes were transported by boats to Minneapolis.
In (16), miniature badgers and raccoons can mean either
‘miniature badgers and miniature raccoons’ or ‘miniature
badgers and raccoons (of any size)’; in (16b), more intelligent
leaders can mean either ‘a greater quantity of intelligent
leaders’ or ‘leaders who are more intelligent’. This property
of having two or more distinct meanings is called
ambiguity; an expression with two or more distinct
meanings is ambiguous.
Often, an expression is ambiguous because it has more than
one possible constituent structure. Consider, for example,
the expression miniature badgers and raccoons in sentence
(16a): it can have either of the following constituent
structures.
(17)
miniature badgers and raccoons
(18)
miniature badgers and raccoons
In (17), badgers and raccoons forms a constituent; (17)
therefore represents the interpretation in which the
adjective miniature applies to both the badgers and
raccoons. In (18), on the other hand, miniature badgers
forms a constituent; (18) therefore represents the
interpretation in which only the badgers are miniature. An
expression which is ambiguous because it has more than
one possible constituent structure is said to be structurally
ambiguous.
The italicized portion of the sentence (16b) is also
structurally ambiguous: it can have either of the following
constituent structures.
more intelligent leaders
SYNTAX:
PHRASAL CATEGORIES
PHRASAL CATEGORIES
So far we have discussed only lexical categories, that is, classes to
which individual word belong. However, there is another kind of
syntactic category, namely phrasal category. Recall that we
determine a word’s category by finding characteristics it shares
with other words. That is, we find words which behave the same,
or have the same distribution, as other words, and these sets of
words we group into a category and give it a name, such as a noun
or verb. Then recall that words can combine with other words to
form semantically coherent groupings ,or constituents. A phrasal
category is a set of constituents which behave the same, or share
the same fractions and distribution.
For instance consider the following sentence:
(1) The joggers ran through the park.
FILE 52
One constituent of this sentence consist of the words the
joggers, as can be demonstrated by applying the constituent
tests discussed in the File 50. Upon examining the lexical
categories involved, we see that this constituent is formed
by the combination of a determiner and a noun. Now
consider each of the words or groups the words below. Note
that each of them could be substituted for the phrase the
joggers in sentence (1), and a grammatical sentence would
result:
(2) a. Susan
b. Students
c. you
d. most dogs
e. some children
f. a huge lovable bear
g. my friend from Brazil
h. the people that the interviewed
Each of the examples in (2) could likewise be shown to be a
constituent in this sentence if it occured in the place of joggers.
Note, however, that some have different structures than DET + N
constituent in the joggers. (a) and (b) are single Ns, (f) is
composed of DET + ADJ + N, (g) is composed of DET + N +
PREP + N, and (h) is different from all of these. Note that other
sets of words that are constituents cannot be substituted for the
joggers, for example, in the tree or made a cake. What we have
discovered is that constituents have different structures can have
the same functions because they can be used in the same
position in the sentence. This means that they belong to the
same category, and since some constituents may involve
combinations of more than one word, these categories are called
phrasal categories. In the category discussed above, a noun alone
or a noun plus other words forms a noun phrase (NP).
Sometimes, a single word can count as an NP all by itself, but not
always. For example, the word dog cannot be a substituted for
the NP slot in (3)
(3)_____________ ran through the park.
Thus, in sentence The dog ran through the park, dog is an
N, but not and NP (whereas dogs, and other nouns in the
plural, would count as both if placed alone in the slot). So
we observe from (2) and (3) that proper nouns, pronouns,
and plural nouns can be used individually as NPs, but some
types of singular Ns cannot be.
An NP can be used as the subject of a sentence, is in (4); as
the direct object, as in (5); as the indirect object, as in (6);
and in many other ways as well. These are descriptions of
the functions which NPs can perform.
(4) Some children like ice cream.
(5) Harold likes some children.
(6) The teacher gave some children a scaffolding this morning.
Now consider the sentence in (7):
(7) The mothers visited their children.
Constituency tests demonstrate that visited their children is
a constituent. It is composed of V + NP. Note that this
particular structure does not share the same properties of
the structures grouped into the category of NP because we
could not insert visited their children in the slot in (3).
Other structures could be substituted for visited their
children. For example:
a. smart
b. love music
c. walk the dog through the park
d. believe that dogs are smart
e. wanted to leave
f. will sleep soundly
g. can lift a hundred pounds
h. are wearing sunglasses
i. go home and have a beauty rest
All of these structures behave the same, and thus can be
grouped into another phrasal category, namely that of verb
phrase (VP). Note that VPs can consist of a single V or a V plus
other words; for example, (f) is V + ADV and (c) is V + NP + PP.
A VP can be used as the predicate of a sentence – i.e., it
combines with a subject NP to form a whole S, as shown in (9):
(9) a. Pat loss music.
b. Henry wanted to leave.
Another phrasal category is that of adjective phrases
(ADJPs), such as those in (10):
(10) a. smart
b. very expensive
c. as tall as his father
d. smarter than the average bear
e. certain to win
Note that each of these could be inserted into the syntactic
frame in (11):
(11) John is ______________.
ADJPS are often used to modify nouns and thus often
appear as elements of noun phrases; for instance, a very
expensive watch; anyone as tall as his father.
Adverbial phrases (ADVPs) such as those in (12) are often
used to modify verbs and adjectives and adverbs, and thus
appear as constituents of VPs and ADJPs, as in (13).
(12) a. soundly
b. fiercely
c. as fluently as native
d. most certainly
(13) a. speak French as fluently a native (VP).
b. fiercely loyal (ADJP)
c. sleep soundly (VP)
Another phrasal syntactic category is that of prepositional
phrases (PPs). PPs always consists of a preposition + NP:
(14) a. from Brazil
b. with Howard and his dog
c. for nothing
d. To the head honcho (the person who is in charge.)
A PP can be an constituent of a wide range of phrases:
(15) a. go to the movies (VP)
b. my friend from Brazil (NP)
c. angry with Howard and his dog (ADJP)
d. separately from the others (ADVP)
Sentences (Ss) also form a phrasal syntactic category.
Sentences are, of course, often used by themselves:
(16) a. it is raining
b. I like apple pie.
But a sentence may also appear as an element of another
expression; for example, each of the following expressions
contains a sentence.
(17) a. the fact that it is raining (NP)
b. student who met Susan last Thursday (NP)
c. discovered that it is raining (VP)
d. glad that it is raining (ADJP)
Note that an expression resulting from the combination of
two or more smaller expressions by a conjunction belongs
to the same category as the smaller ones do. Thus, both
Howard and his dog are each separate NPs, and in (14b) and
(15c) Howard and his dog is also and NP. Likewise, faster
than a speeding bullet and more powerful than a locomotive
is a larger ADJP containing two conjoined ADJP’s and
similarly for the sentence It is raining, but it may sleet which
contains two conjoined Ss.
TREE DIAGRAMS
Tree diagrams are one way of graphically representing the
structure of sentence. In File 50 we saw that tree diagrams
could represent which words grouped together to form
constituents, and which, in turn, formed larger
constituents. Now we see that each constituent in a
sentence belongs either some lexical or phrasal category.
This can also be represented in the tree diagram by labeling
each of the nodes, or points which indicate a constituent,
with the name of the syntactic category to which the lexical
of phrasal constituent belongs. For example, considere the
sentence in (18):
(18) My mother likes her cats.
We can determine (with constituent tests) that the phrasal
constituents of this sentence are the following:
(a) my mother (NP)
(b) likes her cats (VP)
(c) her cats (NP)
(d) my mother likes her cats (S)
We know that each word is a constituent as well, and we can
determine each lexical category. A tree diagram
representing the structure of this sentence looks like:
(19) S
my mother likes her cats.
NP
VP
NP
DET N V DET N
Note that the tree diagram represents many aspects of the
structure of a sentence. First of all, the linear order is
represented because the words appear in some specific
order (in the above case, they are in the proper order).
Secondly, the categories to which words and phrases belong
are indicated; for example, the noun mother is labeled as
being an N, and the phrase my mother is labeled as an NP.
Furthermore, the hierarchical structure is represented by
the lines which indicate which words group to form
constituents, and, in turn, which constituents join to form
larger constituents. For example, the line from the DET
above her and line from the N above cats join at a node to
indicate that the DET + N forms has to constituents. This
constituent joints with the V above likes to form another
constituent as indicated by the lines above NP and V which
join at a node labeled VP.
SYNTAX:
PHRASE STRUCTURE RULES
Part of every language user’s knowledge of his or her
language is the knowledge of how constituents are put
together and categorized in that language. This special
sort of knowledge can be represented as a set of rules
called phrase structure rules (PS rules) .In this file, we
consider the nature of such rules and discuss several
important properties which make them useful for
describing the syntactic competence of language users.
FILE
54
PHRASE STRUCTURE GRAMMAR
Before we discuss phrase structure rules as tools for studying
the syntax of human languages, let us look at the nature of
grammatical descriptions that use phrase structure rules. PS
Rules can be understood as simple instructions for building
larger constituents formed from smaller ones, and they also
give information about the order in which the constituents
appear, and their categories. Thus, the PS rule in (1a) gives
the instructions in (1b) and (1c):
(1)a. S NP VP
b. To build a constituent of the category S, take a
constituent of the category NP and combine it with another
constituent of the category VP.
c. In building an S constituent in this manner, put the NP
constituent first, and the VP constituent after it.
Thus, (1a) merely says a sentence is made up of an NP and a
VP in that order. As part of the descriptive devices available
in the phrase structure syntax, we have tree structures
(already discussed in file 52). There is a direct connection
between phrase structure rules and tree structures. For
instance, the rule in (1a) allows you to construct a tree
structure as in (2a), but the ones in (2b )and (2c) (among
numerous other trees) are not allowed by (1a):
(2) a. S b. S c. S
NP VP N VP VP NP
To gain a precise understanding of instruction-giving aspect
of PS rules, let us for a moment take examples of PS rules
that do not describe English or any other human language.
Let us create what linguists call a ‘‘toy grammar’’ for a
‘‘language’’ whose lexical categories consists of a, b, c, and d.
Its phrasal categories are X and Y, in addition to S (for
instance), which is also a phrasal category. The instructions
(i.e., PS rules) for building sentences in this language are
given in (3).
(3) a. S XY
b. X ab
c. X a
d. Y bed
e. Y edY
f. Y cd
Now what ‘‘sentences’’ are acceptable (i.e., grammatical,
able to be produced with this set of rules) and what
sentences are ungrammatical (i.e., cannot be produced
using this system of rules?) The shortest sentence produced
by this system is acd, the derivation of which is given in (4)
using tree structures:
At this point, no further instructions can be carried out, and
the sequence acd has been successful derived in accordance
with the rules. On the other hand, the sequence *abd or
*abc cannot be derived using the instructions in (3). The
sequences abcd, abbcd, acdcd, abcdcdcd and maybe
abcdcdcdcd are all grammatical in this language, however;
can you work out which rules must apply to derive each of
these?
The simple rule-system in (3), far from being a grammar for
human languages, still is not as simple as it looks. The
‘‘language’’ it produces has some of the salient properties of
the syntax of human languages that we wish to focus on. To
be specific, it has the properties of generativity,
ambiguity, hierarchical structure, and infinite
recursion (i.e., productivity). We will now see how the
grammar in (3) shows these characteristics.
First, it is generative because it does not list a grammatical
sentences in the language; instead, it gives a schematic
strategy for getting the infinite set of grammatical sentences
using a finite set of rules. Recall that languages that have
productivity (i.e., infinite output) cannot be described by
listing. Thus, this toy grammar has one of the desirable
properties that we would want in a grammar for human
languages.
Secondly, human languages contain some structural
ambiguous sentences, and a grammar for human language
should reflect this property. The grammar in (3) has the
power to produce structurally ambiguous sentences. Consider
the sentence abcd in this language. It could be derived in two
different ways, each resulting in a different consistent
structure, i.e., a different way of grouping words together. The
two different derivations are given in (5):
(5)a. S b. S
by(3a) by (3a)
by (3b) &(3f) by (3c)&(3d)
a b c d a b c d
X X
Y Y
The set of rules for NP’s (so far (11), (14), (15), and (16))
express the generalization that all of these structures belong
to one category by the name of NP. However, given the PS
rules we have developed so far, the grammar (i.e., the
collection of all the rules) will generate ungrammatical as
well as grammatical sentences. For example, using the
entire set of rules given above, the following three structure
can be generated:
S
NP VP
DET N V
Now, words from the lexicon must be selected from the
appropriate lexical categories and inserted below the lexical
category nodes in this tree. But any word which belongs to
the category V may be inserted, including intransitive verbs.
Inserting such a word would generate a sentence such as
*The children chased, which of course is not grammatical.
Therefore, information about subcategories must be
included in PS rules. Thus, (16) would be written:
(18) VP Vi
Where the subscript. (i) indicates that this V must be
chosen from the subcategory of intransitive verbs. Transitive
verbs (verbs which must take NP objects) will also be called
for the other PS rules. What would PS rules for VP’s
containing transitive verbs look like?
A similar situation exists for NPs. Leaving (15) as it is would
generate a sentence such as *boy woke up in the middle of
night. How might rule (15) be rewritten to prevent this?
It is very important to understand that phrase structure
rules express generalizations over many individual
sentences. The term ‘‘rule’’ is used because a rule describes a
pattern found in a language. Many sentences can have the
structure in (19):
(19) S
DET
VP
VP
N
NP
PP
P NP
DET N
For example, the dog ran through the woods. The woman
jumped on the bug. Some students read during the night.
Our books fell to the floor, and The rain fell from the sky all
have this structure, and this pattern is reflected in the fact
that the same phrase structure rules generate each of these
sentences.
However, once you understand the structure of a particular
sentence, you can determine what PS rules must be involved
in deriving that structure. For example, using constituency
tests and our knowledge of lexical and phrasal categories,
we can determine that the structure of the sentence The
men in the kitchen drives a truck is as follows:
(20) S
the man in
NP VP
NP PP Vt NP
DET N P NP
DET N
DET N
drives
a truck
the kitchen
Starting at the S node, we can formulate the PS rules with
generate this structure. The S is composed of an NP and a
VP (as usual), so a rule which expresses this fact would be
written as (10) above: S NP VP. The highest NP is
composed of an NP and a PP. A rule which expresses this
fact would be as NP NP PP, and so on.
INFINITY AND RECURSION
One of the things linguists attempt to do is to analyze the
structure of sentences in a given language and construct a
set of rules which can generate the grammatical sentences
of that language but which does not allow generation of
ungrammatical sentences. In addition, the set of rules must
be able to generate an infinite number of sentences. One of
the ways this can be accomplished is by using recursive
rules, as discussed above. One example of recursion is
English involves the pair of rose in (21) and (22) below.
(21) NP NP PP (22) PP P NP
Together these rules generate phrases such as the man with
the dog, the painter from California, some preachers in their
pulpits, a large ant in my drink, and so on. But notice that
when one role is applied in succession, the pair is recursive;
note that since NP occurs on the left of the arrow

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Word vs lexeme by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...
Word vs lexeme  by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...Word vs lexeme  by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...
Word vs lexeme by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...James Jamie
 
Principles of parameters
Principles of parametersPrinciples of parameters
Principles of parametersVelnar
 
Applied linguistics presentation
Applied linguistics  presentationApplied linguistics  presentation
Applied linguistics presentationMuhammad Furqan
 
LEXICOGRAPHY
LEXICOGRAPHY LEXICOGRAPHY
LEXICOGRAPHY mimisy
 
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONAL
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONALMORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONAL
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONALibesti123
 
Transformational Generative Grammar
Transformational Generative GrammarTransformational Generative Grammar
Transformational Generative GrammarHani Khan
 
Derivational affix
Derivational affixDerivational affix
Derivational affixGuy Shine
 
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS Ani Istiana
 
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 Slides
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 SlidesLing1003 08 Lecture 14 Slides
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 SlidesDr. Cupid Lucid
 
Chp 18 language and regional variation
Chp 18 language and regional variationChp 18 language and regional variation
Chp 18 language and regional variationPaulaChapelet
 
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmatics
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmaticsEntailment and presupposition semantics and pragmatics
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmaticsAnna Shelley
 

Mais procurados (20)

Morphology derivation
Morphology derivationMorphology derivation
Morphology derivation
 
Word vs lexeme by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...
Word vs lexeme  by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...Word vs lexeme  by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...
Word vs lexeme by james jamie 2014 presentation assigned by asifa memon lect...
 
MORPHOSYNTAX: GENERATIVE MORPHOLOGY
MORPHOSYNTAX: GENERATIVE MORPHOLOGYMORPHOSYNTAX: GENERATIVE MORPHOLOGY
MORPHOSYNTAX: GENERATIVE MORPHOLOGY
 
Principles of parameters
Principles of parametersPrinciples of parameters
Principles of parameters
 
Applied linguistics presentation
Applied linguistics  presentationApplied linguistics  presentation
Applied linguistics presentation
 
Chapter 11: Discourse analysis
Chapter 11: Discourse analysisChapter 11: Discourse analysis
Chapter 11: Discourse analysis
 
LEXICOGRAPHY
LEXICOGRAPHY LEXICOGRAPHY
LEXICOGRAPHY
 
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONAL
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONALMORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONAL
MORPHOLOGY, A WORD AND ITS FORM :INFLECTIONAL
 
Syllabus format
Syllabus formatSyllabus format
Syllabus format
 
Accent & dialect
Accent & dialectAccent & dialect
Accent & dialect
 
Transformational Generative Grammar
Transformational Generative GrammarTransformational Generative Grammar
Transformational Generative Grammar
 
Some Summary of Morphology
Some Summary of Morphology Some Summary of Morphology
Some Summary of Morphology
 
Derivational affix
Derivational affixDerivational affix
Derivational affix
 
Semantics
SemanticsSemantics
Semantics
 
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS
SEMANTIC = LEXICAL RELATIONS
 
Pragmatics slide 1
Pragmatics slide 1Pragmatics slide 1
Pragmatics slide 1
 
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 Slides
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 SlidesLing1003 08 Lecture 14 Slides
Ling1003 08 Lecture 14 Slides
 
Chp 18 language and regional variation
Chp 18 language and regional variationChp 18 language and regional variation
Chp 18 language and regional variation
 
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmatics
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmaticsEntailment and presupposition semantics and pragmatics
Entailment and presupposition semantics and pragmatics
 
Generative grammar
Generative grammarGenerative grammar
Generative grammar
 

Semelhante a Syntax (2).pptx

Syntax presetation
Syntax presetationSyntax presetation
Syntax presetationqamaraftab6
 
syntaxical process
syntaxical processsyntaxical process
syntaxical processTawarik
 
Introduction to linguistics syntax
Introduction to linguistics syntaxIntroduction to linguistics syntax
Introduction to linguistics syntaxAYOUBDRAOUI
 
Assignment on morphology
Assignment on morphologyAssignment on morphology
Assignment on morphologyLinda Midy
 
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdflesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdfMOHAMEDAADDI
 
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptx
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptxSyntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptx
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptxJamelMirafuentes
 
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguity
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguitygrammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguity
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguityDedew Deviarini
 
Deep structure and surface structure.pptx
Deep structure and surface structure.pptxDeep structure and surface structure.pptx
Deep structure and surface structure.pptxSubramanian Mani
 
Morphology and syntax
Morphology and syntaxMorphology and syntax
Morphology and syntaxSohaibisrar1
 
english-grammar
english-grammarenglish-grammar
english-grammarcjsmann
 
Resume of Semantic
Resume of SemanticResume of Semantic
Resume of SemanticND Arisanti
 

Semelhante a Syntax (2).pptx (20)

Syntax presetation
Syntax presetationSyntax presetation
Syntax presetation
 
syntaxical process
syntaxical processsyntaxical process
syntaxical process
 
Introduction to linguistics syntax
Introduction to linguistics syntaxIntroduction to linguistics syntax
Introduction to linguistics syntax
 
Syntax
SyntaxSyntax
Syntax
 
Grammar Syntax(1).pptx
Grammar Syntax(1).pptxGrammar Syntax(1).pptx
Grammar Syntax(1).pptx
 
Assignment on morphology
Assignment on morphologyAssignment on morphology
Assignment on morphology
 
Syntax
SyntaxSyntax
Syntax
 
Nlp ambiguity presentation
Nlp ambiguity presentationNlp ambiguity presentation
Nlp ambiguity presentation
 
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdflesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
 
What is Syntax in English
What is Syntax in EnglishWhat is Syntax in English
What is Syntax in English
 
Syntax
SyntaxSyntax
Syntax
 
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptx
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptxSyntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptx
Syntactic Features in Mother Tongue.pptx
 
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguity
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguitygrammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguity
grammaticality, deep & surface structure, and ambiguity
 
Deep structure and surface structure.pptx
Deep structure and surface structure.pptxDeep structure and surface structure.pptx
Deep structure and surface structure.pptx
 
Soaps-tone
Soaps-toneSoaps-tone
Soaps-tone
 
Morphology and syntax
Morphology and syntaxMorphology and syntax
Morphology and syntax
 
Presentation (18)-2.pptx
Presentation (18)-2.pptxPresentation (18)-2.pptx
Presentation (18)-2.pptx
 
Syntax.english 12
Syntax.english 12 Syntax.english 12
Syntax.english 12
 
english-grammar
english-grammarenglish-grammar
english-grammar
 
Resume of Semantic
Resume of SemanticResume of Semantic
Resume of Semantic
 

Mais de ssuser61590a

التخطيط للفهم 003.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 003.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 003.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 003.pptxssuser61590a
 
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 002.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptxssuser61590a
 
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 004.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptxssuser61590a
 
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 001.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptxssuser61590a
 
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 005.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptxssuser61590a
 
Business cover.pptx
Business cover.pptxBusiness cover.pptx
Business cover.pptxssuser61590a
 
SLA Week 6 2021.pptx
SLA Week 6 2021.pptxSLA Week 6 2021.pptx
SLA Week 6 2021.pptxssuser61590a
 
Pricing table example.pptx
Pricing table example.pptxPricing table example.pptx
Pricing table example.pptxssuser61590a
 

Mais de ssuser61590a (9)

التخطيط للفهم 003.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 003.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 003.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 003.pptx
 
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 002.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 002.pptx
 
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 004.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 004.pptx
 
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 001.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 001.pptx
 
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptxالتخطيط للفهم 005.pptx
التخطيط للفهم 005.pptx
 
business
businessbusiness
business
 
Business cover.pptx
Business cover.pptxBusiness cover.pptx
Business cover.pptx
 
SLA Week 6 2021.pptx
SLA Week 6 2021.pptxSLA Week 6 2021.pptx
SLA Week 6 2021.pptx
 
Pricing table example.pptx
Pricing table example.pptxPricing table example.pptx
Pricing table example.pptx
 

Último

On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptxInterdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the ClassroomPooky Knightsmith
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxAreebaZafar22
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - Englishneillewis46
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentationcamerronhm
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...pradhanghanshyam7136
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsMebane Rash
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Pooja Bhuva
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structuredhanjurrannsibayan2
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...Amil baba
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSCeline George
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Pooja Bhuva
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 

Último (20)

On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptxInterdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
Interdisciplinary_Insights_Data_Collection_Methods.pptx
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
Sensory_Experience_and_Emotional_Resonance_in_Gabriel_Okaras_The_Piano_and_Th...
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 

Syntax (2).pptx

  • 1. The Sentence Patterns of Language Prof. Dr. Nalan KIZILTAN
  • 2. The language is the set of sentences that are described by the grammer. The grammer ‘’generates’’ the sentences it describes and their structural descriptions. Chomsky(1957) Knowing a language includes the ability to put words together to form phrases and sentences that express our thoughts. That part of the grammer that represents a speaker’s knowledge of the structure of phrases and sentences is called Syntax.
  • 3.  Sequences of words that conform to the rules of syntax are well formed or grammatical.  Sequences of words that violate the syntactic rules are ungrammatical. There are two basic principles of sentence organization:  Linear Order  Hierarchical Structure
  • 4. LINEAR ORDER The words in a sentence must occur in a particular sequence if the sentence is to convey the desired meaning. 1) John glanced at Mary. 2) Mary John at glanced. 3) Mary glanced at John. (the meaning is completely different from that of (1).
  • 5. HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE The individual words in a sentence are organized into natural, semantically coherent groupings which are themselves organized into larger groupings. These groupings within a sentence are called constituents of that sentence. The relationships between constituents in a sentence form the constituent structure of the sentence.
  • 6. 4) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. Many executives Eat at really fancy restaurants. These are constituents Have meaning of their own. And each makes a coherent contribution to the meaning of (4) as a whole.
  • 7. Constituent Tests 1) Ability to stand alone Constituents can be used alone as exclamations or as answers to questions. 5) What do many executives do? Eat at really fancy restaurants.
  • 8. 2) Substitution by a pro-form A constituant can be replaced by a single word having the same meaning. Pronouns are one type of pro-form; such as do, be, have; pro-adverbs such as there and then; as well as a pro-adjectives. 6) They eat at really fancy restaurants - Who eats at really fancy restaurants? 7) Many executives do.
  • 9. 3) Movement If some part of a sentence can be moved around – usually to the beginning or end of the sentence – it is a constituent. 8) At really fancy restaurants, many executives eat. 9) How fancy was it? - Really fancy 10) Who eats at really fancy restaurants? Many executives eat at such restaurants.
  • 10. 11) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
  • 11. In addition to the linear order of words in a sentence, a tree diagram is another way of representing the hierarchical nature of constituent structure. Branching structures in which each constituent forms a “branch” Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. at really fancy restaurants really fancy restaurants fancy restaurants
  • 12. The child found the puppy the child found the puppy the child found the puppy the puppy
  • 13. Ambiguity In every human language we can find individual expressions which have two or more distinct meanings. 12) Long raises miniature badgers and raccoons. long raises miniature badgers and raccoons. raises 1 miniature badgers and raccoons. miniature badgers and raccoons. badgers and raccoons.
  • 14. 2 miniature badgers and raccoons. miniature badgers and raccoons. miniature badgers
  • 15. 13) We need more intelligent leaders we 1 need more intelligent leaders need more intelligent leaders more intelligent leaders (A greater quantity of intelligent leaders)
  • 16. 14) more intelligent leaders more intelligent leaders more intelligent (Leaders who are more intelligent) Structural ambiguity can be explicitly accounted for by multiple tree structures.
  • 17. In (13) Intelligent leaders is a constituent: for this reason (13) represents the interpretation ‘‘a greater quantity of intelligent leaders.’’ In (14), however, more intelligent forms a constituent: (14) therefore represents the meaning ‘‘leaders who are more intelligent.’’ Although structural ambiguity is a very common kind of ambiguity, it is not the only kind. Individual words are sometimes ambiguous: for example, ‘‘crane’’ can refer to either a kind of bird or to a large construction device. Because words like crane have no internal constituent structure, sentences containing such words clearly can`t be structurally ambiguous. Instead, a sentence containing a word with more than one meaning is said to be lexically ambiguous. Thus, both meanings for (13)-(14) are represented with the same constituent structure.
  • 18. EXERCISES 0. Apply the constituent tests to determine if the underlined expressions in the following sentences are constituents. a. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies. b. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies. c. Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies.
  • 19. 1) Which of the underlined expressions in the following sentences are constituents? Which are not? Why? (Use the constituency tests mentioned before to show this in each case.) a. Marianne ate the stale candy. b. Marianne ate the stale candy c. My little brother snores. d. My little brother snores.
  • 20. 2) Apply each of the constituency tests to the underlined expressions and determine whether they are constituents. a. The clouds rolled across the sky. b. My uncle crushed our new car c. Mattingly hit the ball over the fence. d. Some students hate computers. e. The ancient ruins of the temple were covered by the earth. f. The women wept. g. Too many noisy birds are nesting on campus. h. Joggers like to run along the river. i. The bride and groom ran out of the church.
  • 21.  1) The rules of syntax combine words into phrases phrases into sentences English: SUBJECT - VERB - OBJECT (SVO) language Turkish: SUBJECT - OBJECT - VERB (SOV) language  2) Syntactic rules also specify other constraints that sentences must adhere to. a) To find + NP = YES * To find + ADV = NO b) To sleep + ADV = YES * To sleep + NP = NO SYNTACTIC RULES
  • 22. 3) Syntactic rules show how words form groups in a sentence  Syntactic rules show how they are hierarchically arranged with respect to one another. 4) Syntactic rules reveal the grammatical relations among the words of a sentence as well as their order and hierarchical organization. 5) Syntactic rules explain how the grouping of words relates to its meaning, such as when a sentence or phrase is ambiguous.
  • 23. 6) The rules of syntax permit speakers to produce and understand a limitless number of sentences never produced are heard before – the creative aspect of Linguistic knowledge. A major goal of linguistics is to show clearly and explicitly how syntactic rules account for this knowledge. A theory of grammar must provide a complete characterization of what speakers implicitly know about their knowledge.
  • 24. WHAT GRAMMATICALITY IS NOT BASED ON Grammaticality ≠ meaningfulness. Colorless green ideas sleep furiously. A verb crumpled the milk Syntactically well formed Semantically not acceptable=Nonsense Grammaticality does not depend on the truth of sentences. If it did, lying would be impossible. The syntactic rules that permit us to produce, understand, make grammaticality judgements are unconscious rules. The grammar is a mental grammar, different from the prescriptive one.
  • 25. (7) S= NP + VP John snored. Everyone fled the volcano. The major smoked a cigar. A book lay on the table. (8) NP= Det + N the mayor a book every student my python
  • 26. (9) VP= V + NP fled the volcano smoked a cigar imitated a flamingo squeezed some fresh orange juice We can represent these tree pieces of information in a succinct way with the following three phrase structure rules. (10) S NP VP (11) NP Det N (12) VP V NP
  • 27. The arrow in these rules can be read as ‘may consist of.’ Thus rule (10) is just a concise way of saying ‘a sentence may consist of a noun phrase followed by a verb phrase’; similarly, rule (11) just says ‘a noun phrase may consist of a determiner followed by a noun.’ Now, what does rule (12) say? Recall from File 52 that a syntactic category is a group of constituents with different structures which share certain properties. NP’s, for example, could be composed of DET + N, as in (2) above, bur they may also have the following structures: (13) My uncle from France A rule for this NP would look like: (14) NP NP PP
  • 28. Rules (10) (11) (12) and (14) specify five ways in which constituents can be combined to form larger, more complex constituents. Some constituents, however, do not result from the combination of smaller constituents; instead, they consist of a single word. For example, a noun phrase may just consist of a proper name (e.g., John, Paris), a plural noun (e.g., elephants, leaves), or a noun referring to a substance (e.g., clay, gasoline); similarly, a verb phrase may just consist of an intransitive verb such as sneeze, die, vanish, or elapse. These sorts of knowledge can also be represented with PS rules: 15 NP N 16 VP V
  • 29. Phrasal Categories A phrasal category is a set of constituents which behave the same or share the same functions and distribution. 1) Noun Phrase (NP) A noun or a noun + other words (1) ____________ ran through the park. a) susan e) some children b) students f) a huge lovable bear c)you g) my friend from turkey d) most dogs h) the people that we interviewed
  • 30. 2) Verb Phrase (VP) a single verb or verb + other forms verb + ADV verb + NP+ PP (prepositional phrase) (2) John ________. a) saw a clown b) is smart c) slept d) found the cake e) found the cake in the cupboard
  • 31. 3 Adjective Phrase a single adjective or as a constituent of NPs. (3) John is ____________. a) smart b) very intelligent c) as tall as his father d) smarter than the average bear e) certain to win
  • 32. 4 Adverbial Phrase Adverb or as a constituent of VPs and ADVPs. (4) John speaks ____________. a) fluently b) as fluent as a native c) almost certainly 5 Prepositional Phrase preposition followed by a NP. a) from Turkey b) at university c) through the park d) in the cupboard
  • 33. 6) Sentences are often used by themselves. (5) it is raining. I like apple pie.
  • 34. Phrase Structure Trees  A diagram with syntactic category information provided is called phrase structure tree. This tree shows that a sentence is both a linear string of words and hierarchical structure with phrases nested in phrases.  Three aspects of speakers’ syntactic knowledge of sentence structure are disclosed in phrase structure trees:  1 the linear order of the words in the sentence  2 the groupings of words into syntactic categories  3 the hierarchical structure of the syntactic categories
  • 36. (1) The child found the puppy S NP VP Det N Vt NP Det N the child found the puppy
  • 37. (2) my mother likes her cats S NP VP Det N Vt NP Det N my mother likes her cats
  • 38. (3)Many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies. S NP Det ADJ N VP Vt NP PP Det N P NP ADJ N many retired workers spend their time on relaxing hobbies
  • 39. (4) The boy saw the man with the telescope S the boy saw the man with the telescope ‘The boy used a telescope to see the man.’ NP Det N VP Vt NP PP Det N P NP Det N
  • 40. (5) The boy saw the man with the telescope S the boy saw the man with the telescope NP Det N VP Vt NP PP Det N P NP Det N
  • 41. (6) The baby slept S the baby slept NP Det N VP Vit
  • 42. (7) the girl with the feather on the ribbon on the brim. NP the girl with the feather on the ribbon on the brim NP Det PP P N PP P NP Det N PP P NP N Det N Det
  • 43.  One can repeat the number of NPs under PPs under NPs without a limit. This property of all human languages is called recursion. (infinite recursion) (productivity)
  • 44. The man fled from the posse. S the man fled from the posse. NP Det N VP PP Vit NP P Det N
  • 45. A girl laughed at the monkey. S a girl laughed at the monkey NP Det N VP PP Vit NP P Det N
  • 46. A large fierce black dog looked out the window. S a large fierce black dog looked out the window. NP Det Adj VP PP Vt NP P Det N Adj Adj N
  • 47. The man in the kitchen drives a truck. S the man in the kitchen drives a truck. NP Det N VP NP Vt Det N NP PP NP P Det N
  • 48. Lexical Categories Each word in a given language is not entirely different in its behavior from all the other words in that language. Instead, a large number of words exhibit the same properties. Words can be grouped into some word classes based on their morphological and syntactic properties. These word classes are called lexical categories, because the lexicon is the list of all the words in a language (plus various kinds of information, about those words.) For example, one of the morphological properties of the word book is that it has a plural form books. All these words can occur in the following context:
  • 49. (1) __________ + plural morpheme. The words that occur in the morphological frame can also occur in the syntactic frames as in (2) and (3): (2) DET _________ (3) DET ADJ _________ Verbs (4)_________ + tense morpheme (sing-song; walk-walked) (5)________ + third person singular morpheme (I walk, he walks) (6)_________ + progressive morpheme (I am walking) (7)AUX__________ (may go) (8) (please)___________! ((please) leave!)
  • 50. Adjectives (9)_________ + er/est (taller, tallest) (10) more/most+ _____(more affectionate, most affectionate) (11) DET____________ N (the unexpected guests) (12) Linking verbs__________(looks sunny) (13) ADV___________(amazingly perceptive) Adverbs (14) ADV (ADJ+ly) (happily, unexpectedly, skillfully) (15) ________ ADJ (unusually nice) __________VERB or VERB PHRASE (usually go) __________ ADV (quite well)
  • 51. Transformational Rules The phrase structure rules characterize an infinite number of different kinds of sentences; yet there remain many types of common sentences not accounted for. The boy is sleeping. The boy can sleep. The boy will sleep. Words like is, can, will are in class of Auxiliary Verbs or Auxiliary(AUX) which includes be and have as well as may, might, would, could and several others. They are called ‘helping verbs’ or ‘modals’. They occur in such structures as this one:
  • 52. The boy is sleeping. S the boy is sleeping NP Art N VP Vi AUX
  • 53. The other sentences which include Auxiliaries: Is the boy sleeping? The interrogative or question sentences Can the boy sleep? are related to their ‘declarative’ Will the boy sleep? counterparts in a simple way. In the questions the AUX occurs at the beginning of the sentence rather than after the subject NP. The two sentences may be said to be transformationally related or related by an operation called a Transformation or Ttransformational Rule.
  • 54. S Is the boy sleeping AUX Det N VP V NP
  • 55. Wh-Question Transformation (i) Place the first auxiliary of the sentence to the left of the subject NP (ii) (the first NP in the sentence). Move the PROwh (e.g., what, who, where, and how) to the beginning of the sentence. In general, transformations change sentences created by phrase-structure rules into sentences with equivalent meanings, but different structures. These changes are accomplished by adding or deleting words, or rearranging word order.
  • 56. Part (i) of the wh-question transformation tells us that (1) Mary can accomplish what? Can be converted into (1a) Can Mary accomplish what? Part (ii) of the transformation further converts this into (1b) what can Mary accomplish? Linguists refer to the form of a sentence produced by phrase structure rules alone as the Deep Structure of a sentence, and the form that the sentence has after one or more transformations has applied as the Surface Structure of the sentence.
  • 57. What can Mary accomplish? S what(x) what can Mary accomplish? PRO(wh) NP S VP AUX Npr Vt NP N(wh)
  • 58. The boy who is sleeping was dreaming. S the boy who is sleeping was dreaming AUX NP S’ VP V NP NP Art N AUX VP PRO V
  • 59. Was the boy who is sleeping dreaming? S Was the boy who is sleeping dreaming? AUX NP S’ VP V NP NP Art N AUX VP PRO V
  • 60. The girl who loves John is pretty. S The girl who loves John is pretty. Recursion Recursion gives language its infinite aspect and accounts for much Linguistic creativity. NP S’ VP V NP NP Art N VP PRO V NP ADJP ADJ
  • 61. You mean you didnt know that I knew she didn’t know you knew that! S1 You mean you didnt know that I knew she didn’t know you knew that! NP S2 NP Art VP V NP VP V S3 Comp NP VP V S4 AUX VP V S5 NP VP V NP
  • 62. He believes that she loves the cat He believes that she loves the cat NP S NP Art VP PRO S’ V Comp NP S VP V PRO N
  • 63. SYNTAX: LINEAR ORDER, HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE, AND AMBIGUITY While a dictionary of all the words in any human language (at a given time) can be made, it is impossible to compile a dictionary of all the sentences of a language; unlike words, sentences are not finite in number, and therefore sentences, unlike words, are not learned individually. However, native speakers of a language seem to get along amazingly well without such a dictionary for sentences – they can use and understand sentences in their language that they have not previously encountered. Why is it that we cannot always properly use or understand a word in our language that we have not heard before, but can spontaneously produce and understand new sentences? If what we learn is not the sentences themselves, what do we learn that enables us to produce and understand an infinite number of sentences? This is the question we will be concerned with as we consider syntax, the study of the structure of phrases and sentences. FILE 50
  • 64. Though we use sentences all the time, we don’t normally think about how they are structured. However, a little consideration reveals that the principles by which words are organized into sentences are, in fact, quite complex. In this file, we will consider two basic principles of sentence organization: linear order and hierarchical structure.
  • 65. The most obvious principle of sentence organization is linear order; the words in a sentence must occur in a particular sequence if the sentence is to convey the desired meaning. Consider, for example, the following sentence of English. (1) John glanced at Mary. If we rearrange the words in the sentence, we either come up with nonsense, as in (2), (the *denotes an ungrammatical expression): (2) *Mary John at glanced. or with a sentence whose meaning is distinctly different from that of (1) - (3) Mary glanced at John. LINEAR ORDER
  • 66. Clearly, the ordering of the words in sentences determines, in part, whether a sentence is grammatically or not, and what the sentence means. One of the many rules of English requires that the grammatical subject of a sentence normally precedes the main verb, which in turn normally precedes its direct object; thus, she resembles him is English (where she is subject and him is the object), but resembles she him and she him resembles are not. However, an important fact about rules of word order is that they are language-specific--that is, languages vary in the ways in which they order words.
  • 67. HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE Although linear order is an important principle of sentence organization, sentences our world than just ordered sequences of words; they have internal hierarchical structure as well. That is, the individual words in a sentence are organized into natural, semantically coherent groupings, which are themselves organized into larger groupings, the largest grouping of all being the sentence itself (and the smallest of all being individual words). These groupings within a sentence are called constituents of that sentence. The relationships between constituents in a sentence form the constituent structure of the sentence. Ror example, consider the sentence in (4). (4) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
  • 68. We can easily distinguish and number of meaningful groups of words in this sentence: many executives and eat at really fancy restaurants, for instance, clearly have meaning of their own, and each makes a coherent contribution to the meaning of (4) as a whole. For these reasons, they are constituents of this sentence. On the other hand, some groups of words in sentence (4) do not naturally form meaningful units; executives eat at and eat at really, for example, don't clearly have meanings of their own. Thus, these groups of words are not constituents of (4).
  • 69. Constituents Tests If a constituent is a semantically coherent group, then, of course, sentences are always constituents, as are the individual words within a sentence. In sentence (4), for instance, the largest constituent is the sentence itself; the smallest constituents are the individual words many, executives, eat, at, really, fancy, and restaurants. Other constutiuents within a sentence are not always so easy to identify. However, there are a number of useful tests for distinguishing constituents, which are synthetic units, from mere strings of words, which aren’t constituents, and therefore do not behave as a single unit: Ability to Stand Alone Constituents can often be sensibly used alone, for example as exclamations or as answers to questions: (5) What do many executives do? Eat at really fancy restaurants. This isn't true of nonconstituents: if we were asked ‘‘Do fancy restaurants do much business?’’ we couldn’t sensibly answer *’’(Well), executives eat at.’’
  • 70. Substitution by a Pro-Form It is often possible to replace a constituent with a single word having the same meaning as that constituent. For example, if someone asked ‘‘What do many executives do?’’ we could answer either with sentence (4) or with sentence (6) in which the constituent many executives is replaced with the single word they (which, in this context, would mean the same thing as many executives). Note that it is a certain category of word which is used for the substitution test, namely, a pro-word or (pro-form). Pronouns are one type of proform (e.g., he, she, it, they, us, her, and that). There are pro-verbs such as do, be and have, pro-adverbs such as there and then, as well as a pro-adjective such. You may use this proforms when attempting to determine constituency.
  • 71. (6) They eat at really fancy restaurants. Similarly, if someone asked ‘‘Who eats at really fancy restaurants?’’ we could answer either with (4) or with (7), in which the constituent eat at really fancy restaurants is replaced with the single word do (which would mean the same thing in this context) (7) Many executives do. But there is no word that could possibly replace nonconstituent eat at really in (4) and mean the same thing, no matter what the question was asked.
  • 72. Movement If some part of a sentence can be moved around - usually to the beginning or the end of the sentence - it is a constituent. For example, one could say: (8) At really fancy restaurants, many executives eat. It may sound a bit stilted, but it sounds grammatical to many English speakers in the right context. Thus, at really fancy restaurants must be a constituent because it can be moved to the front of the sentence. Compare this with: (9) *fancy restaurants many executives at really. This is not grammatical: fancy restaurants is not a constituent in this sentence according to the movement test since it cannot be moved.
  • 73. Other Issues Concerning Constituents Two points must be kept in mind regarding constituents. First, given a group of words, we cannot say once and for all whether or not it is a constituent; rather, we can only say whether or not it is a constituent relative to a particular sentence. To illustrate this, consider sentence (9) and (10). (10) John and Bill raise weasels. (11) Mary saw John and Bill talked to Jane.
  • 74. In (10) John and Bill is a constituent: it functions as a coherent, meaningful unit within the sentence - in particular, as its subject. In (11), however, the very same sequence of words is not a constituent: because John is the direct object of the first clause and Bill is the subject of the second clause, the sequence John and Bill does not make a coherent contribution to the meaning of this sentence. We can apply the constituent tests to verify this claim. For example, although John and Bill can be replaced with they in (10), this isn’t impossible in sentence (11). Thus, we can properly say that a string of words is a constituent only with respect to a particular sentence.
  • 75. The second thing that must be kept in mind is that constituent structure is hierarchical - that is, one constituent may be part of another. What this means, in turn, is that sentences are composed of parts which has been grouped together before they are grouped into the sentence. Consider sentence (4) again. (4) many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. Among the constituents in this sentence is the sequence really fancy. To see this, note that really fancy can be used by itself: (12) How fancy was it? - Really fancy. and that it can be replaced with the single word such: ( 13) Who eats at really fancy restaurants? Mant executives eat at such restaurants.
  • 76. But really fancy is also part of a larger constituent, namely really fancy restaurants; this in turn is part of a larger constituent, at really fast restaurants, which is itself part of the still larger constituent eat at really fancy restaurants and ultimately the largest constituent in the sentence, namely the sentence itself. If we underline each of the constituents in (4), the hierarchical nature of its constituent structure is easier to see: (14) Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants.
  • 77.  Underlining is, as in (14), on way of presenting the hierarchical nature of constituent structure. Another way is with tree diagrams: branching structures in which each constituent forms a ‘‘branch.’’ For example, the tree diagram for sentence (4) is in (15): S Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants (b) (a) (c)
  • 78. In this diagram, each of the constituents of sentence (4) forms of branch: for example, many executives corresponds to the branch labelled (a); at really fancy restaurants, to the branch labelled (b) and really fancy, to the branch labelled (c). Observe, in addition, that groups of words that are not constituents of sentence (4) do not form branches in this tree diagram: executives *eat at and *eat at really, for instance, clearly aren’t constituents according to the diagram in (15). In principle, underlining is just as good as tree diagrams for representing constituent structure, but because tree diagrams are somewhat easier to read, they are usually preferred.
  • 79. Ambiguity In every human language we can find individual expressions which have two or more distinct meanings. For example, the italicized portions of the following sentences of English can be interpreted in more than one way: (16) a. Larry raises miniature badgers and raccoons. b. We need more intelligent leaders. c. The cranes were transported by boats to Minneapolis. In (16), miniature badgers and raccoons can mean either ‘miniature badgers and miniature raccoons’ or ‘miniature badgers and raccoons (of any size)’; in (16b), more intelligent leaders can mean either ‘a greater quantity of intelligent leaders’ or ‘leaders who are more intelligent’. This property of having two or more distinct meanings is called ambiguity; an expression with two or more distinct meanings is ambiguous.
  • 80. Often, an expression is ambiguous because it has more than one possible constituent structure. Consider, for example, the expression miniature badgers and raccoons in sentence (16a): it can have either of the following constituent structures. (17) miniature badgers and raccoons (18) miniature badgers and raccoons
  • 81. In (17), badgers and raccoons forms a constituent; (17) therefore represents the interpretation in which the adjective miniature applies to both the badgers and raccoons. In (18), on the other hand, miniature badgers forms a constituent; (18) therefore represents the interpretation in which only the badgers are miniature. An expression which is ambiguous because it has more than one possible constituent structure is said to be structurally ambiguous. The italicized portion of the sentence (16b) is also structurally ambiguous: it can have either of the following constituent structures. more intelligent leaders
  • 82. SYNTAX: PHRASAL CATEGORIES PHRASAL CATEGORIES So far we have discussed only lexical categories, that is, classes to which individual word belong. However, there is another kind of syntactic category, namely phrasal category. Recall that we determine a word’s category by finding characteristics it shares with other words. That is, we find words which behave the same, or have the same distribution, as other words, and these sets of words we group into a category and give it a name, such as a noun or verb. Then recall that words can combine with other words to form semantically coherent groupings ,or constituents. A phrasal category is a set of constituents which behave the same, or share the same fractions and distribution. For instance consider the following sentence: (1) The joggers ran through the park. FILE 52
  • 83. One constituent of this sentence consist of the words the joggers, as can be demonstrated by applying the constituent tests discussed in the File 50. Upon examining the lexical categories involved, we see that this constituent is formed by the combination of a determiner and a noun. Now consider each of the words or groups the words below. Note that each of them could be substituted for the phrase the joggers in sentence (1), and a grammatical sentence would result: (2) a. Susan b. Students c. you d. most dogs e. some children f. a huge lovable bear g. my friend from Brazil h. the people that the interviewed
  • 84. Each of the examples in (2) could likewise be shown to be a constituent in this sentence if it occured in the place of joggers. Note, however, that some have different structures than DET + N constituent in the joggers. (a) and (b) are single Ns, (f) is composed of DET + ADJ + N, (g) is composed of DET + N + PREP + N, and (h) is different from all of these. Note that other sets of words that are constituents cannot be substituted for the joggers, for example, in the tree or made a cake. What we have discovered is that constituents have different structures can have the same functions because they can be used in the same position in the sentence. This means that they belong to the same category, and since some constituents may involve combinations of more than one word, these categories are called phrasal categories. In the category discussed above, a noun alone or a noun plus other words forms a noun phrase (NP). Sometimes, a single word can count as an NP all by itself, but not always. For example, the word dog cannot be a substituted for the NP slot in (3) (3)_____________ ran through the park.
  • 85. Thus, in sentence The dog ran through the park, dog is an N, but not and NP (whereas dogs, and other nouns in the plural, would count as both if placed alone in the slot). So we observe from (2) and (3) that proper nouns, pronouns, and plural nouns can be used individually as NPs, but some types of singular Ns cannot be. An NP can be used as the subject of a sentence, is in (4); as the direct object, as in (5); as the indirect object, as in (6); and in many other ways as well. These are descriptions of the functions which NPs can perform. (4) Some children like ice cream. (5) Harold likes some children. (6) The teacher gave some children a scaffolding this morning. Now consider the sentence in (7): (7) The mothers visited their children.
  • 86. Constituency tests demonstrate that visited their children is a constituent. It is composed of V + NP. Note that this particular structure does not share the same properties of the structures grouped into the category of NP because we could not insert visited their children in the slot in (3). Other structures could be substituted for visited their children. For example: a. smart b. love music c. walk the dog through the park d. believe that dogs are smart e. wanted to leave f. will sleep soundly g. can lift a hundred pounds h. are wearing sunglasses i. go home and have a beauty rest
  • 87. All of these structures behave the same, and thus can be grouped into another phrasal category, namely that of verb phrase (VP). Note that VPs can consist of a single V or a V plus other words; for example, (f) is V + ADV and (c) is V + NP + PP. A VP can be used as the predicate of a sentence – i.e., it combines with a subject NP to form a whole S, as shown in (9): (9) a. Pat loss music. b. Henry wanted to leave.
  • 88. Another phrasal category is that of adjective phrases (ADJPs), such as those in (10): (10) a. smart b. very expensive c. as tall as his father d. smarter than the average bear e. certain to win Note that each of these could be inserted into the syntactic frame in (11): (11) John is ______________.
  • 89. ADJPS are often used to modify nouns and thus often appear as elements of noun phrases; for instance, a very expensive watch; anyone as tall as his father. Adverbial phrases (ADVPs) such as those in (12) are often used to modify verbs and adjectives and adverbs, and thus appear as constituents of VPs and ADJPs, as in (13). (12) a. soundly b. fiercely c. as fluently as native d. most certainly (13) a. speak French as fluently a native (VP). b. fiercely loyal (ADJP) c. sleep soundly (VP)
  • 90. Another phrasal syntactic category is that of prepositional phrases (PPs). PPs always consists of a preposition + NP: (14) a. from Brazil b. with Howard and his dog c. for nothing d. To the head honcho (the person who is in charge.) A PP can be an constituent of a wide range of phrases: (15) a. go to the movies (VP) b. my friend from Brazil (NP) c. angry with Howard and his dog (ADJP) d. separately from the others (ADVP)
  • 91. Sentences (Ss) also form a phrasal syntactic category. Sentences are, of course, often used by themselves: (16) a. it is raining b. I like apple pie. But a sentence may also appear as an element of another expression; for example, each of the following expressions contains a sentence. (17) a. the fact that it is raining (NP) b. student who met Susan last Thursday (NP) c. discovered that it is raining (VP) d. glad that it is raining (ADJP)
  • 92. Note that an expression resulting from the combination of two or more smaller expressions by a conjunction belongs to the same category as the smaller ones do. Thus, both Howard and his dog are each separate NPs, and in (14b) and (15c) Howard and his dog is also and NP. Likewise, faster than a speeding bullet and more powerful than a locomotive is a larger ADJP containing two conjoined ADJP’s and similarly for the sentence It is raining, but it may sleet which contains two conjoined Ss.
  • 93. TREE DIAGRAMS Tree diagrams are one way of graphically representing the structure of sentence. In File 50 we saw that tree diagrams could represent which words grouped together to form constituents, and which, in turn, formed larger constituents. Now we see that each constituent in a sentence belongs either some lexical or phrasal category. This can also be represented in the tree diagram by labeling each of the nodes, or points which indicate a constituent, with the name of the syntactic category to which the lexical of phrasal constituent belongs. For example, considere the sentence in (18): (18) My mother likes her cats.
  • 94. We can determine (with constituent tests) that the phrasal constituents of this sentence are the following: (a) my mother (NP) (b) likes her cats (VP) (c) her cats (NP) (d) my mother likes her cats (S)
  • 95. We know that each word is a constituent as well, and we can determine each lexical category. A tree diagram representing the structure of this sentence looks like: (19) S my mother likes her cats. NP VP NP DET N V DET N
  • 96. Note that the tree diagram represents many aspects of the structure of a sentence. First of all, the linear order is represented because the words appear in some specific order (in the above case, they are in the proper order). Secondly, the categories to which words and phrases belong are indicated; for example, the noun mother is labeled as being an N, and the phrase my mother is labeled as an NP. Furthermore, the hierarchical structure is represented by the lines which indicate which words group to form constituents, and, in turn, which constituents join to form larger constituents. For example, the line from the DET above her and line from the N above cats join at a node to indicate that the DET + N forms has to constituents. This constituent joints with the V above likes to form another constituent as indicated by the lines above NP and V which join at a node labeled VP.
  • 97. SYNTAX: PHRASE STRUCTURE RULES Part of every language user’s knowledge of his or her language is the knowledge of how constituents are put together and categorized in that language. This special sort of knowledge can be represented as a set of rules called phrase structure rules (PS rules) .In this file, we consider the nature of such rules and discuss several important properties which make them useful for describing the syntactic competence of language users. FILE 54
  • 98. PHRASE STRUCTURE GRAMMAR Before we discuss phrase structure rules as tools for studying the syntax of human languages, let us look at the nature of grammatical descriptions that use phrase structure rules. PS Rules can be understood as simple instructions for building larger constituents formed from smaller ones, and they also give information about the order in which the constituents appear, and their categories. Thus, the PS rule in (1a) gives the instructions in (1b) and (1c): (1)a. S NP VP b. To build a constituent of the category S, take a constituent of the category NP and combine it with another constituent of the category VP. c. In building an S constituent in this manner, put the NP constituent first, and the VP constituent after it.
  • 99. Thus, (1a) merely says a sentence is made up of an NP and a VP in that order. As part of the descriptive devices available in the phrase structure syntax, we have tree structures (already discussed in file 52). There is a direct connection between phrase structure rules and tree structures. For instance, the rule in (1a) allows you to construct a tree structure as in (2a), but the ones in (2b )and (2c) (among numerous other trees) are not allowed by (1a): (2) a. S b. S c. S NP VP N VP VP NP
  • 100. To gain a precise understanding of instruction-giving aspect of PS rules, let us for a moment take examples of PS rules that do not describe English or any other human language. Let us create what linguists call a ‘‘toy grammar’’ for a ‘‘language’’ whose lexical categories consists of a, b, c, and d. Its phrasal categories are X and Y, in addition to S (for instance), which is also a phrasal category. The instructions (i.e., PS rules) for building sentences in this language are given in (3).
  • 101. (3) a. S XY b. X ab c. X a d. Y bed e. Y edY f. Y cd Now what ‘‘sentences’’ are acceptable (i.e., grammatical, able to be produced with this set of rules) and what sentences are ungrammatical (i.e., cannot be produced using this system of rules?) The shortest sentence produced by this system is acd, the derivation of which is given in (4) using tree structures:
  • 102. At this point, no further instructions can be carried out, and the sequence acd has been successful derived in accordance with the rules. On the other hand, the sequence *abd or *abc cannot be derived using the instructions in (3). The sequences abcd, abbcd, acdcd, abcdcdcd and maybe abcdcdcdcd are all grammatical in this language, however; can you work out which rules must apply to derive each of these? The simple rule-system in (3), far from being a grammar for human languages, still is not as simple as it looks. The ‘‘language’’ it produces has some of the salient properties of the syntax of human languages that we wish to focus on. To be specific, it has the properties of generativity, ambiguity, hierarchical structure, and infinite recursion (i.e., productivity). We will now see how the grammar in (3) shows these characteristics.
  • 103. First, it is generative because it does not list a grammatical sentences in the language; instead, it gives a schematic strategy for getting the infinite set of grammatical sentences using a finite set of rules. Recall that languages that have productivity (i.e., infinite output) cannot be described by listing. Thus, this toy grammar has one of the desirable properties that we would want in a grammar for human languages.
  • 104. Secondly, human languages contain some structural ambiguous sentences, and a grammar for human language should reflect this property. The grammar in (3) has the power to produce structurally ambiguous sentences. Consider the sentence abcd in this language. It could be derived in two different ways, each resulting in a different consistent structure, i.e., a different way of grouping words together. The two different derivations are given in (5): (5)a. S b. S by(3a) by (3a) by (3b) &(3f) by (3c)&(3d) a b c d a b c d X X Y Y
  • 105. The set of rules for NP’s (so far (11), (14), (15), and (16)) express the generalization that all of these structures belong to one category by the name of NP. However, given the PS rules we have developed so far, the grammar (i.e., the collection of all the rules) will generate ungrammatical as well as grammatical sentences. For example, using the entire set of rules given above, the following three structure can be generated: S NP VP DET N V
  • 106. Now, words from the lexicon must be selected from the appropriate lexical categories and inserted below the lexical category nodes in this tree. But any word which belongs to the category V may be inserted, including intransitive verbs. Inserting such a word would generate a sentence such as *The children chased, which of course is not grammatical. Therefore, information about subcategories must be included in PS rules. Thus, (16) would be written: (18) VP Vi Where the subscript. (i) indicates that this V must be chosen from the subcategory of intransitive verbs. Transitive verbs (verbs which must take NP objects) will also be called for the other PS rules. What would PS rules for VP’s containing transitive verbs look like?
  • 107. A similar situation exists for NPs. Leaving (15) as it is would generate a sentence such as *boy woke up in the middle of night. How might rule (15) be rewritten to prevent this? It is very important to understand that phrase structure rules express generalizations over many individual sentences. The term ‘‘rule’’ is used because a rule describes a pattern found in a language. Many sentences can have the structure in (19): (19) S DET VP VP N NP PP P NP DET N
  • 108. For example, the dog ran through the woods. The woman jumped on the bug. Some students read during the night. Our books fell to the floor, and The rain fell from the sky all have this structure, and this pattern is reflected in the fact that the same phrase structure rules generate each of these sentences. However, once you understand the structure of a particular sentence, you can determine what PS rules must be involved in deriving that structure. For example, using constituency tests and our knowledge of lexical and phrasal categories, we can determine that the structure of the sentence The men in the kitchen drives a truck is as follows:
  • 109. (20) S the man in NP VP NP PP Vt NP DET N P NP DET N DET N drives a truck the kitchen
  • 110. Starting at the S node, we can formulate the PS rules with generate this structure. The S is composed of an NP and a VP (as usual), so a rule which expresses this fact would be written as (10) above: S NP VP. The highest NP is composed of an NP and a PP. A rule which expresses this fact would be as NP NP PP, and so on.
  • 111. INFINITY AND RECURSION One of the things linguists attempt to do is to analyze the structure of sentences in a given language and construct a set of rules which can generate the grammatical sentences of that language but which does not allow generation of ungrammatical sentences. In addition, the set of rules must be able to generate an infinite number of sentences. One of the ways this can be accomplished is by using recursive rules, as discussed above. One example of recursion is English involves the pair of rose in (21) and (22) below. (21) NP NP PP (22) PP P NP Together these rules generate phrases such as the man with the dog, the painter from California, some preachers in their pulpits, a large ant in my drink, and so on. But notice that when one role is applied in succession, the pair is recursive; note that since NP occurs on the left of the arrow