SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 74
Maladaptive Perfectionism as a Mediator and Moderator
Between Adult
Attachment and Depressive Mood
Meifen Wei
Iowa State University
Brent Mallinckrodt
University of Missouri–Columbia
Daniel W. Russell and W. Todd Abraham
Iowa State University
This study examined maladaptive perfectionism (concern over
mistakes, doubts about one’s ability to
accomplish tasks, and failure to meet high standards) as both a
mediator and a moderator between adult
attachment (anxiety and avoidance) and depressive mood
(depression and hopelessness). Survey data
were collected from 310 undergraduates and analyzed using
structural equation modeling (SEM)
methods. Results indicated that maladaptive perfectionism
partially mediated the relationship between
attachment anxiety and depressive mood and fully mediated the
relationship between attachment
avoidance and depressive mood. Bootstrap methods were used
to assess the magnitude of the indirect
effects. Significant moderator effects were also found with SEM
methods. The association between
attachment anxiety and depressive mood was stronger as
perfectionism increased. Perfectionism was not
a significant moderator for attachment avoidance and depressive
mood.
Throughout the past decade, there has been a growing interest
among counseling psychologists in applying Bowlby’s (1973,
1980, 1988) attachment theory to understanding adult
development
and the counseling process (Lopez, 1995; Lopez & Brennan,
2000;
Mallinckrodt, 2000). The initial formulations of adult
attachment
posited four qualitative categories of attachment based on
combi-
nations of positive and negative working models of self and
others
(e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). However, research has
failed to confirm the existence of qualitative cutoff points, and
instead supports two continuous dimensions as the best way to
model adult attachment (Fraley & Waller, 1998). In a factor
analysis of data gathered from over 1,000 undergraduates, Bren-
nan, Clark, and Shaver (1998) included all of the extant self-
report
measures of adult attachment (14 measures, 60 subscales, 323
items) and identified two relatively orthogonal dimensions of
Anxiety and Avoidance. Adult attachment anxiety is
characterized
as an excessive need for approval from others and fear of inter -
personal rejection or abandonment. Adult attachment avoidance
involves an excessive need for self-reliance and fear of interper-
sonal closeness or dependence. People with high levels of either
dimension or both dimensions in combination are assumed to
have
an insecure adult attachment orientation. By contrast, people
with
low levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance have the
capacity
for secure adult attachment, a positive sense of personal compe -
tence, and the ability to maintain supportive attachments
(Brennan
et al., 1998; Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mallinckrodt, 2000).
Previous empirical research has provided strong evidence for a
link between insecure attachment and various forms of
psycholog-
ical distress (for reviews, see Lopez & Brennan, 2000;
Mikulincer
& Shaver, 2003). For example, relative to their secure counter -
parts, people with insecure attachment reported greater distress
and
hostility during a laboratory problem-centered discussion
(Simp-
son, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996), greater affective intensity and
emotionality in their daily life (Pietromonaco & Barrett, 1997),
more depressive symptoms (Roberts, Gotlib, & Kassel, 1996),
greater interpersonal problems (Mallinckrodt & Wei, 2003), and
more emotional distress (Collins, 1996). Thus, the link between
various forms of insecure attachment and indices of
psychological
distress (e.g., depressive mood) has been fairly well established.
More recently, research linking attachment insecurity and
distress
(e.g., depressive mood) has been shifting from an examination
of
simple bivariate linear relationships to multivariate
interactional
models that examine the roles of mediators and moderators of
these relationships (Collins, 1996; Lopez, Mitchell, & Gormley,
2002; Roberts et al., 1996; Wei, Heppner, & Mallinckrodt,
2003).
One example of this new emphasis on multivariate models is
recent research that has examined the relationships among
attach-
ment, perfectionism, and adjustment (Rice & Mirzadeh, 2000).
Perfectionism has been conceptualized as a multidimensional
con-
struct, with both adaptive and maladaptive aspects (Flett &
Hewitt,
2002). Adaptive perfectionism involves setting high (but
achiev-
able) personal standards, a preference for order and
organization,
a sense of self-satisfaction, a desire to excel, and a motivation
to
Meifen Wei and W. Todd Abraham, Department of Psychology,
Iowa
State University; Brent Mallinckrodt, Department of
Educational, School,
and Counseling Psychology, University of Missouri–Columbia;
Daniel W.
Russell, Department of Human Development and Family
Studies, Iowa
State University.
We thank Robyn Zakalik, Shanna Behrendsen, Anne Giusto, and
Mike
McGregor for their assistance with data collection.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Meifen
Wei, Department of Psychology, W112 Lagomarcino Hall, Iowa
State
University, Ames, IA 50011-3180. E-mail: [email protected]
Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 2004 by the
American Psychological Association
2004, Vol. 51, No. 2, 201–212 0022-0167/04/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0022-0167.51.2.201
201
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
achieve positive rewards. Maladaptive perfectionism involves
un-
realistically high standards, intense ruminative concern over
mis-
takes, perceived pressure from others to be perfect, a perceived
large discrepancy between one’s performance and personal stan-
dards, compulsive doubting of one’s actions, and motivation to
avoid negative consequences (Enns & Cox, 2002).
Theorists suggest that maladaptive perfectionism results when a
child’s need for acceptance and love from parents is
accompanied
by a parent’s failure to provide the needed acceptance and
positive
regard (Hamachek, 1978). Observational research has shown
that
if caregivers are inconsistent and unreliable in responding to the
emotional or physical needs of young children, anxious
attachment
is frequently the result (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall,
1978).
Serious interpersonal problems may develop in adults whose
par-
ents used a love withdrawal style of discipline involving threats
to
withhold affection as a means of control (Mallinckrodt & Wei,
2003). Children with attachment anxiety may quickly learn that
if
they are “perfect” boys or girls, they may be more likely to gain
their parents’ love and acceptance. This pattern of striving for
perfection as a way to earn acceptance that was only
intermittently
available in childhood may persist as a maladaptive pattern in
adults.
A different dynamic may underlie the connection between per-
fectionism and attachment avoidance. Attachment avoidance is
believed to involve a negative working model of others along
with
a positive working model of self (Bartholomew & Horowitz,
1991). However, striving to be “perfect” in the view of others
may
be an outward defense that masks a deeply wounded inner sense
of
self resulting from the inadequate emotional responsiveness of
caregivers early in development (Lapan & Patton, 1986;
Robbins
& Patton, 1985). Children with avoidant attachment tend to de-
scribe themselves as perfect (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988), but they
may drive themselves to attain perfection to avoid others’
rejection
and to manage their own hidden sense of imperfections. For
example, a child may think, “If I am perfect, no one will hurt
me”
(Flett, Hewitt, Oliver, & Macdonald, 2002). Thus, initially
striving
to be perfect may be a positive coping mechanism for children
whose caregivers are unresponsive or inconsistent in their
respon-
siveness to the child’s needs. However, if striving to be perfect
is
overused as a coping strategy, it may lead to depressive mood in
adulthood. Therefore, the specific form that the maladaptive
striv-
ing for perfection may take might depend on the particular
mixture
of attachment avoidance or attachment anxiety experienced in
adulthood.
Although several theorists have suggested that the origins of
perfectionism are related to problematic attachment in the
parent–
child relationship, until recently there were very few empi rical
studies of perfectionism and attachment. Among the small
number
of available studies, Rice and Mirzadeh (2000) reported that
mal-
adaptive perfectionism was related to insecure attachment,
whereas adaptive perfectionism was related to secure
attachment
in college students. Similarly, Andersson and Perris (2000)
found
that perfectionism was positively associated with insecure
attach-
ment. Additionally, Flett et al. (2001) found that persons with
high
attachment anxiety and avoidance reported higher perceived
pres-
sure from others to be perfect. Thus, previous studies have pro-
vided tentative evidence that attachment avoidance and
attachment
anxiety are positively associated with maladaptive
perfectionism.
Several studies have shown that perfectionis m is positively
associated with depression or hopelessness. For example,
perfec-
tionism in college students was associated with greater
depressive
symptoms (e.g., Chang, 2002; Chang & Sanna, 2001; Cheng,
2001; Hewitt & Flett, 1991) and suicidal preoccupation (Adkins
&
Parker, 1996; Chang, 1998). In longitudinal studies,
perfectionism
has been linked to both depression and hopelessness over time
(Chang & Rand, 2000; Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & Mosher,
1995).
Also, Hewitt and Flett (2002) reported that perceived pressure
from others to be perfect was associated with hopelessness
across
different studies and populations (e.g., Chang & Rand, 2000;
Dean, Range, & Goggin, 1996). On the basis of these previous
studies, in the present study we chose to represent the latent
variable of depressive mood with indicators of depression and
hopelessness.
It is possible that adults with high attachment anxiety or avoid-
ance are likely to develop maladaptive perfectionism and, in
turn,
experience significant depressive mood. Some studies have
exam-
ined how maladaptive perfectionism might serve as a mediator
between parent– child interactions and depressive mood.
Randolph
and Dykman (1998) found that perfectionism fully mediated the
relationship between critical parenting and depression-
proneness
and partially mediated the relationship between perfectionistic
parenting and depression-proneness in undergraduate students.
Enns, Cox, and Clara (2002) reported that maladaptive
perfection-
ism mediated the relationship between harsh parenting (e.g.,
crit-
ical parenting, parental overprotection, and parental lack of
care)
and depression. However, our search of the literature could not
locate any previous study that examined perfectionism as a
medi-
ator between attachment and depressive mood. If maladaptive
perfectionism does serve as a mediator, interventions could be
targeted at adults with attachment anxiety or avoidance to help
decrease their maladaptive perfectionism and in turn decrease
their
depressive mood.
Hewitt and Flett (2002) argued that perfectionism could serve as
a moderator (as well as a mediator) between insecure
attachment
and depressive mood. Several studies have found that specific
dimensions of perfectionism (e.g., pressure from others to be
perfect) interacted with general stress (e.g., major life stress or
self-appraisal stress) to predict increased depression symptoms
or
negative affect (e.g., Chang & Rand, 2000; Cheng, 2001;
Dunkley,
Zuroff, & Blankstein, 2003; Flett et al., 1995). That is, greater
depression or negative affect was reported by participants with
higher combined levels of perfectionism and perceived stress. In
addition, other studies reported that specific dimensions of
perfec-
tionism interacted with specific stressors to predict higher
levels of
depression. Hewitt and Flett (1993) found that perfectionism,
particularly in the form of perceived pressure from others to be
perfect, interacted with interpersonal stressors (e.g.,
relationship
problems or lack of intimacy) to predict depression. It appears
that
maladaptive perfectionism could serve as a potential moderator
of
the relationship between general or specific stressors and
psycho-
logical distress.
Attachment anxiety or attachment avoidance could be viewed as
a source of chronic interpersonal stress. Perfectionism may lead
to
depressive mood because it generates core interpersonal needs
that
are difficult to satisfy (i.e., the need for others’ approval, or the
need to be perfect to avoid others’ rejection). Maladaptive
perfec-
tionism might interact with attachment anxiety or attachment
202 WEI, MALLINCKRODT, RUSSELL, AND ABRAHAM
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
avoidance to worsen depressive mood (Hewitt & Flett, 2002).
From the standpoint of putative causal links, in a mediating sce-
nario attachment insecurity (x1) is believed to cause higher
levels
of maladaptive perfectionism (x2), which in turn causes higher
levels of depressive mood (y). If the mediation is partial rather
than
complete, there would also be a significant direct link between
(x1)
attachment insecurity and (y) depressive mood (Baron & Kenny,
1986; Holmbeck, 1997). By contrast, in a moderating scenario
there is no requirement that x1 causes x2 and, in fact, the two
variables may be uncorrelated. However, the strength of
associa-
tion between x1 (in this case, attachment insecurity) and y
(depres-
sive mood) is believed to vary for differing levels of x2
(maladap-
tive perfectionism). Unfortunately, there has been no empirical
research studying how maladaptive perfectionism might interact
with attachment to predict depressive mood.
Because it is possible for maladaptive perfectionism to serve as
both an intermediate link in the causal chain leading from
attach-
ment insecurity to depressive mood (i.e., as a mediator) and as a
variable that alters the strength of association between
attachment
insecurity and depressive mood (i.e., as a moderator), both
types of
relationships were explored in this study. Specifically, the
purpose
of the present study was to examine whether the maladaptive
aspects of perfectionism (e.g., concern over mistakes, doubts
about
actions, and perceived discrepancy between one’s standards and
performance) serve as a mediator, as a moderator, or as both in
the
context of the relationship between adult attachment insecurity
(anxiety and avoidance) and depressive mood (depression and
hopelessness). Figures 1A and 1B depict both of these hypothe-
sized relationships. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was
used
to test the models depicted in this figure. Slaney, Rice, Mobley,
Trippi, and Ashby (2001) argued that the discrepancy between
high standards and perceptions of performance was a defining
feature of maladaptive perfectionism, whereas high standards
without perceived discrepancy could indicate adaptive
perfection-
ism. Therefore, measures of discrepancy between standards and
performance, concern over mistakes, and doubts about one’s ac-
Figure 1. Hypothesized mediating effects (A) and moderating
effects (B) of maladaptive perfectionism on the
links between attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance
with depressive mood. The moderating effects (B)
of maladaptive perfectionism on the links between attachment
anxiety and attachment avoidance with depressive
mood were examined separately.
203PERFECTIONISM AND ATTACHMENT
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
tions served as the indicators for the construct of maladaptive
perfectionism, in addition to measures of depression and
hopeless-
ness, which served as indicators of the latent variable
depressive
mood.1
Method
Participants
Participants were 310 undergraduate students enrolled in
introductory
psychology classes at a large midwestern university. The
participants were
told that the purpose of the research was “to learn about factors
affecting
college students’ adjustment.” The sample included 225 (73%)
women and
85 (27%) men. Their mean age was 19.27 years (SD � 1.88,
range �
18 –30 yrs.). Approximately 53% of the participants were
freshmen. Ethnic
identification was predominantly White/Caucasian (84%),
followed by
international students of various ethnicities (4.8%), Asian
American
(4.2%), African American (2.3%), Hispanic American (2.3%),
multiracial
American (1.0%), and others (1.3%). Most participants (98.0%)
indicated
they were single or never married. Students received partial
credit toward
their course grade for participating in this study. The amount of
credit
varied depending on their particular section of the course.
Instruments
Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECRS; Brennan et
al., 1998).
The ECRS is a 36-item self-report measure of adult attachment
containing
two 18-item subscales derived from the factor analysis by
Brennan et al.
(1998) described previously. The subscales assess dimensions of
adult
attachment, Anxiety and Avoidance. Participants use a 7-point
Likert-type
scale (1 � disagree strongly, 7 � agree strongly) to rate how
well each
statement describes their typical feelings in romantic
relationships. The
Anxiety subscale taps fears of abandonment and rejection. The
Avoidance
subscale assesses discomfort with dependence and intimate self-
disclosure.
Brennan et al.’s reported coefficient alpha was .91 and .94 for
the Anxiety
and Avoidance subscales, respectively. In the present study,
coefficient
alpha was .90 for the Anxiety subscale and .91 for the
Avoidance subscale.
Brennan et al. also reported that scale scores were correlated in
expected
directions with scores on self-report measures of touch aversion
and
postcoital emotions. Measured indicators for the two latent
variables of
attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were created from
three
6-item parcels for each subscale. Following the recommendation
of Rus-
sell, Kahn, Spoth, and Altmaier (1998), exploratory factor
analyses were
conducted using maximum-likelihood extraction for the two
factors (Anx-
iety and Avoidance) separately. The items were then rank-
ordered on the
basis of the magnitude of the factor loadings and successively
assigned
pairs of the highest and lowest items to each parcel to equalize
the average
loadings of each parcel on its respective factor.
Almost Perfect Scale–Revised (APS-R; Slaney et al., 2001). The
APS-R is a 23-item self-report measure designed to assess
levels of
perfectionism. Respondents use a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 �
strongly
disagree, 7 � strongly agree) in responding to the items. The
APS-R is
made up of three subscales: High Standards, Order, and
Discrepancy. In
this study only the 12-item Discrepancy subscale was used. This
subscale
measures the degree to which respondents perceive themselves
as failing to
meet personal standards for performance. Slaney et al. reported
a coeffi-
cient alpha of .92 for the Discrepancy subscale, whereas
coefficient alpha
was .94 in the present sample. Slaney et al. reported evidence of
construct
validity in the form of significant correlations between the
Discrepancy
subscale and other perfectionism measures such as Concern
Over Mistakes
(r � .55) and Doubts About Actions (r � .62).
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost, Marten,
Lahart, &
Rosenblate, 1990). The FMPS is a 35-item instrument designed
to mea-
sure perfectionism. Each item uses a 5-point Likert-type scale
(1 �
disagree strongly, 5 � agree strongly). Consistent with
Dunkley, Blank-
stein, Halsall, Williams, and Winkworth (2000), only two of the
six FMPS
subscales were used as indicators of perfectionis m in this study:
(a)
Concern Over Mistakes (9 items) taps a tendency to interpret
mistakes as
failures and to believe that one will lose the respect of others
when one
fails; and (b) the Doubts About Actions (4 items) subscale,
which measures
the tendency to doubt one’s ability to accomplish tasks or the
quality of
one’s performance. In the present study, coefficient alphas were
.89 and .74
for Concern Over Mistakes and Doubts About Actions,
respectively. Frost,
Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, and Neubauer (1993) found that
Concern Over
Mistakes and Doubts About Actions not only reflected
maladaptive eval-
uative concerns of perfectionism, but were also the subscales
most strongly
related to depression. Criterion-related validity is evidenced by
correlations
between FMPS subscales and measures of psychological
symptoms (e.g.,
Brief Symptom Inventory) and adjustment such as
compulsiveness, self-
esteem, procrastination, and depression (Frost et al., 1993,
1990).
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson,
Mock, &
Erbaugh, 1961). The BDI is a widely used 21-item self-report
measure of
depressive symptoms. Each item consists of a depression
symptom cluster
scored on a 0 –3 response scale based on the severity of the
symptom.
Scores across the items are summed to obtain a total BDI score,
with higher
scores indicating more severe depression. Internal consistency
for the BDI
for undergraduates ranges from .78 to .92, with a mean
coefficient alpha of
.85. In the present study, coefficient alpha was .86. Test–retest
reliabilities
for nonpsychiatric participants ranged from .60 (7 days) to .83
(1– 6 hr),
with reports of .78 for a 2-week and a 3-week period.
Considerable
evidence of validity has been demonstrated for the BDI as a
measure of
depressive symptoms (Beck, 1967; Bumberry, Oliver, &
McClure, 1978).
Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS; Beck, Weissman, Lester, &
Trexler,
1974). The BHS is a 20-item inventory that assesses the degree
to which
an individual’s cognitive schemata are characterized by
pessimistic expec-
tations. The scale uses a true–false response format. Scores can
range from
0 to 20, with higher scores indicating a greater degree of
hopelessness.
Internal consistency of .93 has been reported, along with
concurrent
validity of .74 with clinical ratings of hopelessness and .60 with
other
scales of hopelessness (Beck et al., 1974). In the present study,
coefficient
alpha for the BHS was .78.
Procedure
The questionnaires were administrated to small groups of 3–25
students
who signed up for one of several data collection times.
Participants were
guaranteed anonymity of their responses and confidentiality of
the data,
given that no personal identifying information was solicited on
the ques-
tionnaires. Completing the entire packet of instruments
typically required
25– 40 min.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for the
13 measured variables are shown in Table 1. Data were checked
for normality, which is a critical assumption underlying the
1 One issue raised by reviewers concerned the fact that we only
used one
measure, the ECRS, to operationalize the attachment variable.
Because the
ECRS was developed on the basis of a factor analysis of
existing measures
of attachment (see Brennan et al., 1998), we felt that this
measure ade-
quately represented the nature of the construct. Indeed, it is
very likely that
items on any other measure of adult attachment would be
redundant with
items on this measure. Therefore, we did not feel it was
necessary to use
other measures of adult attachment in this investigation.
204 WEI, MALLINCKRODT, RUSSELL, AND ABRAHAM
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
maximum-likelihood procedure used in this study. Results indi-
cated univariate normality for all measured variables except the
BHS (Beck et al., 1974; skew Z � 1.87, and kurtosis Z � 4.47).
We therefore conducted a square root transformation for the
BHS
variable. The skew and kurtosis for the transformed BHS were Z
�
.16 and .75, respectively, indicating a normal distribution. The
BHS and the transformed BHS were highly correlated (r � .94).
Therefore, we used the transformed BHS variable in subsequent
analyses.2
Measurement Model for Testing Mediation Effects
The analysis of the proposed mediation model followed the
two-step procedure recommended by Anderson and Gerbing
(1988). First, we used a confirmatory factor analysis to develop
a
measurement model with an acceptable fit to the data. Once an
acceptable measurement model was developed, the structural
model was tested. The measurement model was estimated using
the maximum-likelihood method in the LISREL 8.50 program.
As
suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999) and Quintana and Maxwell
(1999), three indices were used to assess goodness of fit for the
models: the comparative fit index (CFI; values of .95 or greater
are
desirable), the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR;
values of .08 or less are desirable), and the root-mean-square
error
of approximation (RMSEA; values of .06 or less are desirable).
Finally, we used the chi-square difference test to compare
nested
models. An initial test of the measurement model resulted in a
relatively good fit to the data, �2(38, N � 310) � 72.60, p �
.001,
CFI � .98, SRMR � .04, and RMSEA � .05 (95% confidence
interval [CI]: .03, .07).3 All of the loadings of the measured
variables on the latent variables were statistically significant ( p
�
.001; see Table 2). Therefore, all of the latent variables appea r
to
have been adequately measured by their respective indicators.
In
addition, the correlations among the independent (exogenous)
la-
tent variables, the mediator latent variable, and dependent latent
variable were statistically significant ( p � .05; see Table 3)
Structural Model for Testing Mediation Effects
A number of methods have been suggested in the literature for
testing mediation effects. Recently, MacKinnon, Lockwood,
Hoff-
man, West, and Sheets (2002) evaluated 14 methods with regard
to
2 We also tested the multivariate normality of the observed
variables as
a set, including the transformed BHS (Beck et al., 1974)
variable, based on
the test developed by Mardia (see Bollen, 1989). The significant
result,
�2(2, N � 310) � 114.05, p � .001, indicated that the data
were not
multivariate normal. Therefore, we used the procedure
developed by Sa-
torra and Bentler (1988) to adjust the chi-square statistics and
standard
errors of the parameter estimates for the impact of
nonnormality. In the
mediation model, the results after adjusting for the impact of
nonnormality
did not differ from the results when we did not adjust for
nonnormality. In
the moderation model, the results for the path coefficients were
identical
whether or not we adjusted for the impact of nonnormality.
However, the
standard error of the latent interaction term became very large
following
the adjustment for nonnormality. This problem associated with
interaction
terms and the Satorra-Bentler adjustment for nonnormality has
been noted by
others (e.g., Yang-Wallentin & Joreskog, 2001). Therefore, we
report results
for the moderation model without adjusting for the impact of
nonnormality.
3 We examined whether the results would be equivalent for men
and
women in the measurement model, structural model, and the
models with
interaction effect. A series of multiple-group analyses were
conducted
using LISREL 8.50 to examine whether female and male groups
differed
from one another in terms of the measurement model, the
structural model,
and the models with interaction effects (Byrne, 1998). Results
suggested
that the measurement model and structural model were
equivalent for the
male and female groups. However, the models comparing men
and women
that included the interaction effect did not converge. This was
likely
because of the relatively small number of men (n � 85)
included in the
sample. Therefore, we conducted a hierarchical regression
analysis to
examine whether the interaction effect varied for men and
women. Results
of the regression analysis indicated that the three-way
interaction (Attachment
Anxiety � Maladaptive Perfectionism � Gender) predicting
depressive mood
was not significant (� � .002), …
IFSP Sample Template
Child’s name ________________________ Age
_______________________ Date ______________________
Service Coordinator_(this will be
you)_________________________________ Review Date
_(the date you
submit)_____________________
I. Statements of Family Strengths and Resources
II. Statements of Family Concerns and Priorities
*Concerns:
*Priorities:
III. Child’s Present Level of Development and Abilities
Cognitive Skills (Thinking, reasoning, and learning)
Communication Skills (Understanding, communicating with
others, and expressing self with others)
Self- Care/Adaptive Skills (Bathing, feeding, dressing, and
toileting)
Gross and Fine Motor Skills (Moving)
Social-Emotional Development (Feelings, coping, and getting
along with others)
Health/Physical Development (Hearing, vision, and health)
IV. Outcome Statements
• Write one Goal with a strategy or activity that Dashawn must
master before the next meeting in 6month.
• Write one goal with a strategy or activity that his parents must
due before the next meeting in six months.
Dashawn’s Case
Dashawn was referred to the Infant & Toddler Connection of
Wonderland by his Pediatrician, Dr. Purvis. Dr. Purvis sent over
copies of the developmental screening and M-CHAT he
completed with Dashawn. Based on parent report, Dashawn was
born full-term, weighing 8lbs 3 oz., following an uncomplicated
pregnancy. He has a history of ear infections and his sister
received speech therapy when she was a toddler. Dashawn’s
mother is not concerned about his hearing or vision. During the
evaluation, Dashawn played with blocks, a ball, looked at
books, and pointed to some body parts. He made some animal
sounds, used jargon as he played, and said two words. Dashawn
interacted with others around him, responded to his name, and
following simple directions. He took turns in play, put shapes in
a puzzle, and scribbled with crayons. He pretended to feed his
Clifford dog and put him to sleep. Dashawn can take off his
clothes and occasionally seems to be aware that his diaper is
soiled. He eats without any problems except that he chokes
often, according to his mother. Based on the evaluation,
Dashawn was found eligible for weekly speech therapy services
due to developmental delays. He is showing strengths in his
gross and fine motor, social, receptive communication and self-
help skills. Dashawn was born August 12, 2013.
Dashawn was referred to the Infant & Toddler Connection of
Wonderland by his pediatrician, Dr. Purvis, due to concerns for
his expressive language development. Dr. Purvis completed a
developmental screening with Dashawn and his mother at 24
months well-child visit and noted that Dashawn was only using
three words: mama, ball and juice. Dr. Purvis also completed
the M-CHAT but no concerns for autism were noted based on
the screening results. Dr. Purvis recommended that Dashawn be
tested by the developmental pediatrician at the local children’s
hospital, which is scheduled for May. Dashawn also had an
audiological exam on July 3, 2018, which found his hearing to
be within normal limits. Dashawn was born full-term, weighing
8lbs 3oz, following an uncomplicated pregnancy. Based on
review of Dashawn’s medical records, the only complication
noted after delivery was jaundice, three ear infections but
otherwise has been a healthy child. Dashawn passed his
newborn hearing screening at birth and no concerns have been
noted for his vision. All immunizations are up-to-date.
Dashawn’s mother reports a family history of delayed
communication development, as his sister also received early
intervention services due to developmental delay. His
assessment service planning was held in the family’s home with
Dashawn, his mother, a speech therapist, a developmental
service provider, and the service coordinator present. Dashawn
was shy at first, but warmed up quickly to the assessment
activities. He especially enjoyed playing with the ball, putting
pegs in a pegboard, and scribbling with crayons. As Dashawn
warmed up, he began to interact with those around him and
would look around to be sure that everyone was watching and
clapping for him. He took turns in play stacking blocks (up to a
tower of eight) and knocking the tower down and tossing the
ball back and forth. While playing with the ball, at one point
Dashawn hit his head on the underside of the table and went to
his mother for comfort. He quickly recovered and continued
playing. Dashawn was able to follow directions to locate the
ball, to give the ball to his mom, and other 1-2 step commands.
During the assessment, Dashawn responded to his name, pointed
to pictures in a book, and pointed to 5 body parts on a doll and
himself. When looking at the book, Dashawn made the “woof”
and “meow” sounds to pictures of dogs and cats, and used some
jargon in play.
They only true words today were “mama” and “ball”.
Dashawn’s mother said that Dashawn can also say “top’ for
stop, to tell his sister to stop bothering him. Dashawn’s mother
reports that Dashawn has tantrums often during the day when he
gets frustrated or tired. Because Dashawn’s tantrums are so
frequent, his mother said that it is difficult to take him out on
errands and to the mall. Dashawn’s mother and sister have
started trying to teach Dashawn a few signs (more, cracker, car)
but Dashawn have not yet begun to use them.
Dashawn attended well to activities where he played one-on-one
with an adult. He put the shapes in the puzzle and looked to the
educator who was playing with him for help when the triangle
would not fit correctly. He enjoyed scribbling and copying lines
and playing a matching game during which he matched three
objects on request. Dashawn played pretend with his Clifford
stuffed animal, pretending that Clifford was eating and going to
sleep. When Dashawn wanted to play a different game, he
would put his hands on the toy bag that contained the testing
materials, look at the educator, and vocalize “uh-uh” He was
very purposeful in his communication but was not able to
imitate words or sounds in play today.
Dashawn is able to move about independently by walking,
climbing, and running. His mother reports that Dashawn’s
ability to move is his greatest strength. Dashawn can push and
carry large objects, like his child sized chair. He can jump from
the bottom step in his house, climb up onto the couch to sit, and
throw a large ball without falling. Dashawn helps put laundry
away and helps throw trash away when asked. He can take off
his clothes and occasionally seems to be aware of his diaper
being soiled. Dashawn will vocalize to get assistance, using the
same “uh-uh” sounds mentioned earlier, and will point to what
he wants if out of reach. Dashawn eats well but has trouble
chewing his food. His other reports that he will sometimes pack
his cheeks “like a squirrel” then choke trying to swallow. She
also reports that when he was an infant, he took a long time to
drink his bottle. Dashawn can feed himself using fingers and
will sometimes use a spoon. He also drinks from a sippy cup
and will say “hush” to get juice when his cup is empty. Based
on the assessment, Dashawn is showing strength in his gross
and fine motor, social receptive communication, and self-help
skills. He is showing developmental delays in his expressive
communication and cognitive development. His expressive
communication is limited by the fact that Dashawn is only using
4 words consistently at this time and seems to have a limited
variety of sounds. He seems to have some difficulty
coordinating the movements of his mouth to chew and make
sounds. Dashawn’s cognitive delay appears to be related to his
expressive communication, as his problem-solving skills appear
to be appropriate for his age.
Writing IFSP –Dashawn’s casePage 4
The Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP) and the
Individualized Education Program (IEP)
Definition of the IFSP
A plan of services for infants and toddlers and their
families. Such a plan includes statements regarding the
child’s present developmental level, the family’s
strengths and needs, the major outcomes of the plan,
specific interventions and delivery systems to
accomplish outcomes, dates of initiation and duration of
services, and a plan for transition into
public schools.
Definition of the IEP
A program devised to satisfy IDEA’s requirement that
students with disabilities must receive an educational
program based on multidisciplinary assessment and
designed to meet their individual needs. The IEP must
include consideration of the student’s present level of
performance, annual goals, special education and related
services, time in general education,
timeline for special education services,
and an annual evaluation.
FIRST THING!!!!!
(before anything else)PARENTAL CONSENTParents as
partnersCultural, ethnic, and linguistic differencesFamily’s
strengths and needsChild’s strengths and needs
Process of the IFSP
SCREENING. Child Find,
established in the 1960s
(though not added to law
until 1986) as an effort to
find children with disabilities
and other special needs,
does a screening to find
those children. Those that
need further evaluation go
on to diagnostic testing.
Definition of a couple of terms …VALIDITY: a test that
measures what it purports to measure.RELIABILITY: how
accurate, de-pendable, and predictable a test is.SENSITIVITY:
ability of a screening tool to identify correctly children with
disabilities.SPECIFICITY: ability of a screening tool to
correctly identify children who do NOT have a disability
Back to IFSP process
DETERMINING ELIGIBILITY. Eligibility for
special education and related services cannot be
made on a single test or assessment. The data
must be gleaned from several domains and
sources. Assessment tools must be in a child’s
native language. This does not mean a tool that
has been TRANSLATED – this screws with the
test’s validity and reliability.
IFSP process
PLANNING THE PROGRAM – to plan a
program that will benefit the family and the
child; the writing of the IFSP with all domains
considered and a part of the writing. IFSP
outlines what services will be delivered, how
they will be delivered, and what skills and areas
will be addressed.
End of IFSP process . . .
MONITORING PROGRESS and
EVALUATING THE PROGRAM: the IFSP
should be reevaluated at least once a year.
From these evaluations, the program may be
modified to better suit the
child’s strengths and needs.
TERMINOLOGYService coordinator: an interdisciplinary team
member responsible for integrating services and keeping the
family informed and involved.Assistive technology: various
kinds of equipment designed to facilitate learning and
communication for children with disabilities.Itinerant special
education teacher: a consultant that travels between several
classrooms for service delivery
TERMINOLOGYAUDIOLOGIST: a specially certified
professional who focuses on hearing testing and hearing
impairmentsDURATION MEASURES: how long an event or
behavior lasts.FREQUENCY COUNTS: keeping track of how
often a behavior occursINTERDISCIPLINARY TEAM: several
different professionals working together on a common
problemIQ TESTS: intelligence tests, norm-referenced
TERMINOLOGYLEAST RESTRICTIVE ENVIRONMENT:
most normalized environment in which the needs of a child with
disabilities can be met appropriately.MULTIDISCIPLINARY:
involving members of various disciplines who work
independently but exchange findingsNORM-REFERENCED
ASSESSMENT: instrument that compares a child’s
developmental level to a normative sample of same-age peers.
TERMINOLOGYORIENTATION AND MOBILITY
SPECIALIST: therapist who works with children with visual
impairments to teach awareness of their position in the
environment, of significant objects, and how to move safely and
efficiently in the environment.PATHOLOGIST: professional
who focuses on diseases or impairments.PEDIATRIC
OPHTHALMOLOGIST: physician who specializes in diseases
and malfunctioning of the eyes in developmental years.
TERMINOLOGYPORTFOLIO: a carefully selected collection
of a child’s work that is used to document growth and
development.PSYCHOLINGUIST: one who studies and analyzes
the acquisition and production of
languagePSYCHOPATHOLOGIST: one who specializes in
viewing mental disorders from a psychological
perspective.SECONDARY PREVENTION: providing adequate
intervention services before the condition worsens or affects
other areas of development.
TERMINOLOGYSERVICE DELIVERY MODEL: a formal plan
devised by the various agencies involved in providing services
to a child and his/her family.STANDARDIZED TESTS:
assessment instruments that include precise directions for
administering and scoring.TRANSDISCIPLINARY TEAM: a
team that shares the responsibilities for assessment, program
planning, implementation, and evaluation across members.
TYPES OF TEACHER
OBSERVATIONAL
ASSESSMENTS:CHECKLISTSFREQUENCY
COUNTSDURATION MEASURESANECDOTAL
NOTESRUNNING RECORDSLOGS, JOURNALS,
DIARIESTIME SAMPLINGLANGUAGE
SAMPLINGPORTFOLIO
TEACHERS SHOULD NOT:MAKE A DIAGNOSIS!!!Use
LABELS to describe children Raise parents’ ANXIETYTELL
parents what to doJUMP TO CONCLUSIONS without adequate
data
TEACHERS SHOULD:Work well with familiesVoice concerns
to familiesListen carefully and respectfullyBe knowledgeableBe
culturally sensitiveWork collaboratively with parentsRemember
that all children (and all parents) are different and unique.
PROCESS OF THE IEP
Same as for IFSP: screen,
diagnose, plan program,
monitor, and evaluate.Should include child’s present levelsLong
term goalsShort term objectivesSpecific services to be
providedAccountabilityWhere and when
WHO IS ON THE TEAM???PARENTSChild’s regular
classroom teacherChild’s special education teacherPrincipal or
vice-principal or program head (LEA representative)A person
who can interpret the dataService coordinator or
equivalentSchool counselor or equivalentInterpreterPhysical
therapistOccupational therapist
PsychologistMedical DoctorSpeech-Language
PathologistCommunity support personSocial WorkerOrientation
and Mobility SpecialistLawyersBehavior specialistsAssistive
technology specialistVision SpecialistASL interpreterOther
interested or involved parties
Informal Assessment Measures
Checklist: a method
of documenting the
presence or absence of
skill or behavior in a
developmental
sequence.
~class activity
Frequency Count:
keeping track of how
often a behavior
occurs.
~watch how many times players in white shirts pass ball
http://viscog.beckman.uiuc.edu/grafs/demos/15.html
Informal Assessment Measures
Running Records:
detailed account of a
segment of time,
recording all behaviors
and quotes during that
time.
~write down EVERYTHING exactly
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-ulsq4a2SCg&feature=related
*
Informal Assessment Measures
Logs, Journals,
Diaries: a form of
observation technique
that involves making a
page of notes about
children’s behavior in a
cumulative journal.
Time sampling:
method of recording
where children are by
choice at a certain
time, measuring
attention span and
interests.
Informal Assessment Measures
Anecdotal Notes:
Factual narrative of an
incident
Portfolio: a carefully
selected collection of a
child’s work that is
used to document
growth and
development.
Informal Assessment Measures
Duration Measures:
How long an event or
behavior lasts
~ about 4 minutes
~ how many questions Will asks
~ how many letters in his answer?
~ how many times does he use the letter Q?
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GoGZ7rhZNHo
The Career Development of Mexican American Adolescent
Women:
A Test of Social Cognitive Career Theory
Lisa Y. Flores
The Ohio State University
Karen M. O’Brien
University of Maryland, College Park
This study tested R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett’s
(1994) model of career choice with 364
Mexican American adolescent women. Path analyses were run to
determine the influence of contextual
and social cognitive variables on career aspiration, career
choice prestige, and traditionality. Partial
support for the model was evidenced as nontraditional career
self-efficacy, parental support, barriers,
acculturation, and feminist attitudes predicted career choice
prestige. Acculturation, feminist attitudes,
and nontraditional career self-efficacy predicted career choice
traditionality. Feminist attitudes and
parental support predicted career aspiration. The paths between
nontraditional career interests and the 3
outcome variables were not supported. Finally, none of the
background contextual variables in this study
predicted nontraditional career self-efficacy. Implications of the
results and suggestions for future
research are discussed.
Mexican American women constitute a significant portion of the
American population (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1996), are
underrepresented at all levels of education (Carter & Wilson,
1993;
Lango, 1995; McNeill et al., 2001; U.S. Bureau of the Census,
1991), and are overrepresented in low-paying occupations tradi-
tionally occupied by women (Arbona, 1989; Arbona & Novy,
1991; Ortiz, 1995). Relatively little empirical research has been
conducted to identify the variables that contribute to the educa -
tional and occupational underachievement of Mexican American
women. Indeed, researchers have noted that the career develop-
ment of Hispanics has received only slight consideration in the
counseling and vocational literature (Arbona, 1990; Fouad,
1995;
Hoyt, 1989; McNeill et al., 2001), and they have questioned the
generalizability of career development theories to Hispanics
(Ar-
bona, 1990, 1995; Fitzgerald & Betz, 1994; Hackett, Lent, &
Greenhaus, 1991). The purpose of this study was to investigate
the
applicability of a current model of career choice to the
experiences
of Mexican American adolescent women and to extend the
current
model to incorporate variables that are hypothesized to be
salient
to this population.
It is well documented that Hispanics are the least educated
when
compared with other major racial/ethnic groups in the United
States and that, among Hispanics, Mexican Americans have the
lowest high school and college completion rates (47% and 6.5%,
respectively; U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1996). Mexican Ameri -
can women are less likely to graduate from college than their
male
counterparts (Ortiz, 1995; Tinajero, Gonzalez, & Dick, 1991),
and
their representation in higher education decreases significantly
at
each successive level (Carter & Wilson, 1993). Moreover, those
Mexican American women who pursue higher education
confront
many stressors and may experience psychological distress as
they
seek to reconcile their career aspirations with their familial and
cultural values (Niemann, 2001).
Education is related to occupational status, and thus, the re-
stricted employment status among Mexican American women is
not surprising given their low educational attainment. Arbona
(1989) reported that, occupationally, Hispanic women were con-
centrated in low and mid-level technical, service-oriented, and
clerical type jobs. According to Ortiz (1995), Mexican
American
women were less likely to be professionals or private business
owners and earned less money when compared with women
from
other racial/ethnic groups and Mexican American men.
Moreover,
Mexican American women who were in professional
occupations
were more likely to choose traditional and low-status
occupations
(Ortiz, 1995).
A review of the literature on Mexican American women re-
vealed inconsistencies between their educational and vocational
achievements and aspirations. For example, Arbona and Novy
(1991) reported that the majority of Mexican American college
women in their study aspired to investigative and enterprising
type
jobs. It is interesting that the percentage of women who
expected
to enter these fields was smaller than the percentage of women
who aspired to these careers, whereas the opposite was true of
those who aspired and expected to enter fields that have
typically
represented traditional career options for women. Other studies
Lisa Y. Flores, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State
University;
Karen M. O’Brien, Department of Psychology, University of
Maryland,
College Park.
This study was based on the doctoral dissertation of Lisa Y.
Flores,
which was conducted under the direction of Michael J. Patton.
An earlier
version of this article was presented at the 108th Annual
Convention of the
American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, August
2000.
We thank Nancy Betz, Mary Heppner, and Fred Leong for
helpful
feedback on earlier versions of this article; Kristopher Preacher
and Robert
MacCallum for statistical consultation; Jamilla Griffin and
Jason Quaran-
tillo for assistance with coding data; and the students, teachers,
counselors,
and administrators of the participating schools.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Lisa Y.
Flores, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University,
1885 Neil
Avenue Mall, Columbus, Ohio 43210-1222. E-mail:
[email protected]
Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 2002 by the
American Psychological Association, Inc.
2002, Vol. 49, No. 1, 14 –27 0022-0167/02/$5.00 DOI:
10.1037//0022-0167.49.1.14
14
revealed that Mexican American girls aspired to careers that re -
quired a college degree and to obtaining a postsecondary
education
(Hernandez, Vargas-Lew, & Martinez, 1994; Valenzuela, 1993).
Reyes, Kobus, and Gillock’s (1999) study indicated that 87% of
the girls in a sample of predominantly Mexican American 10th-
grade students aspired to nontraditional or male-dominated ca-
reers. Clearly, a difference exists between Mexican American
women’s educational and vocational aspirations and their actual
achievements, suggesting that these women may not be realizing
their educational and career potential.
Prior studies on the career development of Hispanics have
focused primarily on their educational and career aspirations
(Ar-
bona & Novy, 1991; Hernandez et al., 1994; Reyes et al., 1999)
and the factors postulated to be related to their educational
success
(Cardoza, 1991; Fisher & Padmawidjaja, 1999; Gandara, 1982;
Gillock & Reyes, 1999; Hess & D’Amato, 1996; Keith & Licht-
man, 1994; Lango, 1995; Ramos & Sanchez, 1995; Rodriguez,
1996; Valenzuela, 1993; Vasquez, 1982; Wycoff, 1996). Other
studies have examined the barriers that Hispanic students antici -
pate in their educational and career endeavors (Luzzo, 1992;
McWhirter, 1997). The research to date provides insight into the
career development of Hispanic individuals but contains limita -
tions that restrict its use.
First, several studies are descriptive in nature, and while helpful
in understanding patterns of behavior with this group, they do
not
further knowledge regarding the salient predictors of career be -
haviors. Second, several studies included racially/ethnically di -
verse samples (in which the number of Hispanics were
dispropor-
tionately small) or failed to report the ethnic background of
Hispanic participants. Because of the educational and
occupational
differences between racial/ethnic groups and among Hispanics,
investigating ethnically diverse subgroups individually seems
war-
ranted (Arbona, 1995). Another limitation of the existing
studies is
that many included both women and men. Given differences in
Mexican American women’s and men’s educational attainment,
occupational status, and socialization within the culture, women
and men should be investigated separately to understand the ef-
fects of cultural and gender role socialization on career
decisions.
Finally, few studies have assessed the influence of cultural vari -
ables, such as acculturation, on Hispanics’ career-related
behaviors
(Arbona, 1995).
One notable exception to the research described above was a
study investigating the educational plans and career
expectations
of Mexican American high school girls (McWhirter, Hackett, &
Bandalos, 1998). McWhirter and her colleagues studied the
utility
of Farmer’s (1985) model of career commitment and aspirations
in
explaining the educational planning and career expectations of
Mexican American adolescent women. They extended Farmer’s
model by including acculturation and perceived barriers in their
theoretical models. The results of this study indicated that their
models described the educational and career plans of a sample
of
Mexican American girls; however, only a modest amount of
vari-
ance was accounted for by the models. Thus, McWhirter et al.
encouraged researchers to include additional variables when de -
veloping future models of the career development of Mexican
American adolescent women. Moreover, McWhirter et al. sug-
gested that Lent, Brown, and Hackett’s (1994) social cognitive
career theory had promise for advancing knowledge regarding
the
career development of Mexican American women.
Lent and his colleagues (Lent et al., 1994) extended Bandura’s
(1986) social cognitive theory and Hackett and Betz’s (1981)
career self-efficacy theory to develop a social cognitive career
theory (SCCT) that hypothesized the influence of personal, con-
textual, and social cognitive factors on interest formation,
career
goals, and performance. Of interest in this study are the
proposi-
tions of SCCT that background contextual variables exert an
influence on career self-efficacy, which in turn directly
influences
career interests. In addition, Lent et al. posited that career
interests
directly influence career goals and that career self-efficacy both
directly and indirectly (through career interests) influences
career
goals. Finally, proximal contextual variables were hypothesized
to
exert direct effects on career goals (see Figure 1). Lent and his
colleagues suggested that SCCT may be used to guide inquiry
on
the career development of women and racial/ethnic minorities,
and
they recently advocated for more research to test the hypotheses
related to the contextual variables in their model (Lent, Brown,
&
Figure 1. Portions of Lent, Brown, and Hackett’s (1994) model
of career choice tested in the present study.
15MEXICAN AMERICAN WOMEN
Hackett, 2000). Recent studies provided partial support for the
model with racially diverse middle school students (Fouad &
Smith, 1996) as well as Asian American (Tang, Fouad, & Smith,
1999) and Black college students (Gainor & Lent, 1998);
however,
no studies to date have investigated the validity of SCCT with
Mexican American adolescent women.
To test this theory, when operationalizing the constructs ad-
vanced by Lent et al. (1994), we selected variables that were
hypothesized to be salient for racial/ethnic minorities or
women.
Specifically, in our model, we operationalized background
contex-
tual variables to include acculturation level, feminist attitudes,
and
mothers’ modeling through educational attainment and occupa-
tion. Multicultural researchers have identified the importance of
examining within-group differences of racial and ethnic sub-
groups, and Casas and Pytluk (1995) discussed acculturation as
one variable that differentiates Hispanic subgroups or
individuals
within a subgroup. Moreover, McWhirter et al. (1998) noted
that
acculturation was the only variable that they added to Farmer’s
(1985) model that accounted for significant variance i n the edu-
cational aspirations of Mexican American girls. Other
researchers
also documented that acculturation was positively related to
edu-
cational aspirations (Ramos & Sanchez, 1995), in addition to
interest in nontraditional careers (Reyes et al., 1999), college
attendance (Hurtado & Gauvain, 1997), and achievement styles
(Gomez & Fassinger, 1994) among Hispanic students.
Other variables, specifically feminist and gender role attitudes,
have been shown to relate to the career choices of young women
(Betz, 1994; O’Brien & Fassinger, 1993), such that women with
traditional gender role attitudes exhibited lower levels of career
orientation and aspiration than women holding liberal gender
role
attitudes. Among Mexican American girls, nontraditional gender
role attitudes were positively related to higher levels of
educational
and career expectations (McWhirter et al., 1998) and academic
achievement (Valenzuela, 1993; Vasquez-Nuttal, Romero-
Garcia,
& De Leon, 1987). For Mexican American women, cultural ex-
pectations about gender roles may result in traditional gender
role
attitudes or nonfeminist attitudes (Ginorio, Gutierrez, Cauce, &
Acosta, 1995; Reid, Haritos, Kelly, & Holland, 1995), which in
turn may contribute to lower levels of career achievement.
In addition, parental factors, such as occupation and educational
level, were found to relate to academic achievement and
parental
involvement in Mexican American students’ educational and ca-
reer planning (Keith & Lichtman, 1994). With regard to the
influence of mothers, having a mother who attended college was
predictive of college attendance and persistence among Latinas
(Cardoza, 1991). However, other studies that assessed the role
of
parents’ educational or occupational attainment in children’s
edu-
cational and career aspirations reported no relation (Fisher &
Padmawidjaja, 1999; Hernandez et al., 1994; Hess & D’Amato,
1996; Lango, 1995; Reyes et al., 1999), possibly because of the
highly skewed number of parents with lower educational and
occupational levels in these samples. The influence of mothers’
educational level and occupational traditionality were included
in
the present study to determine their influence on daughters’
career
development.
According to SCCT, these background variables were hypoth-
esized to influence nontraditional career self-efficacy or confi-
dence in pursuing nontraditional career-related tasks for women
(Lent et al., 1994). In turn, nontraditional career self-efficacy
should exert a direct effect on both nontraditional career
interests
and career goals (i.e., career choice prestige, career choice
tradi-
tionality, and career aspirations). Indeed, these relations have
been
supported in prior studies, which reported that career self-
efficacy
was related to career interests and careers considered among
Hispanic students (Bores-Rangel, Church, Szendre, & Reeves,
1990; Church, Teresa, Rosebrook, & Szendre, 1992; Lauver &
Jones, 1991). In addition, research has shown that career
interests
were related to careers considered among Hispanic students
(Bores-Rangel et al., 1990; Church et al., 1992). These findings
were consistent with SCCT, which posited a direct link between
career interests and career goals.
We also hypothesized, in accordance with SCCT (Lent et al.,
1994), that the proximal contextual variables of perceived
support
from parents and perceptions of barriers will influence career
choice prestige, traditionality, and career aspirations. Among
Lati-
nas, encouragement and emotional support from families have
been found to be predictive of educational achievement
(Hernan-
dez et al., 1994; Keith & Lichtman, 1994; Ramos & Sanchez,
1995) and college attendance (Vasquez, 1982; Wycoff, 1996).
With regard to perceived barriers, Hispanic students reported
ex-
periencing more barriers to education than students from other
racial/ethnic groups (Luzzo, 1992; McWhirter, 1997), and Mexi -
can American women who experienced negative family attitudes
related to their college attendance were more likely to attend
college close to home (Wycoff, 1996). McWhirter et al. (1998)
found no relation among perceived barriers and Mexican Ameri -
can girls’ educational or career plans. However, they suggested
that the influence of perceived barriers on academic and
vocational
goals be further tested with additional samples. It is possible
that
Mexican American adolescent women’s increased levels of per -
ceived barriers to their educational or career goals may alter
their
decision making, such that they plan to pursue careers that
present
the least resistance.
In summary, this study was designed to test several tenets of
SCCT (Lent et al., 1994) with a sample of Mexican American
adolescent women. Specifically, we explored the influence of
background contextual variables, namely, acculturation level,
fem-
inist attitudes, mother’s educational level, and mother’s occupa-
tional traditionality on nontraditional career self-efficacy. Addi-
tionally, we investigated the contributions of nontraditional
career
self-efficacy, nontraditional career interests, parental support,
and
perceived barriers to career choice prestige, career choice tradi -
tionality, and career aspirations. These dependent variables
were
selected because of their importance to women’s career
develop-
ment (Fitzgerald, Fassinger, & Betz, 1995; O’Brien &
Fassinger,
1993). A secondary purpose of this study was to obtain
descriptive
information regarding participants’ demographic characteristics,
career choices, plans following high school graduation, choice
of
colleges/universities, and reasons for choosing these schools,
given the lack of data regarding this population and their career
plans.
Method
Participants
Participants were Mexican American adolescent women enrolled
in their
senior year of high school. At the same time, Mexican American
adoles-
16 FLORES AND O’BRIEN
cent men were surveyed for a later study. Participants were
drawn from two
large public high schools in a mid-sized town (a population of
approxi-
mately 30,000) in south Texas. The community is close to the
United
States–Mexican border and is heavily influenced by the
Mexican culture.
A high percentage of U.S. citizens who are of Mexican descent
live in this
area, and this is reflected in the student population at the high
schools, in
which almost 95% of the students are Mexican American.
A total of 931 surveys were distributed to students; 831 were
returned
(450 female, 381 male), resulting in an 89% overall return rate.
Women
who were in their senior year of high school and who identified
as Mexican
American were included in this study (n � 377). Of these
women, 13 were
dropped from the study because of incomplete data, resulting in
a total
sample of 364. Participants ranged in age from 16 to 21 years
with a mean
age of 17.47 (SD � 0.70). The average number of people living
at home
was 4.83 (SD � 1.71; range � 2 to 13).
Eighteen percent of the students (n � 65) reported that they
were
first-generation Mexican American, with 37.9% (n � 138)
second gener-
ation, 11.3% (n � 41) third generation, 19.2% (n � 70) fourth
generation,
and 10.7% (n � 39) fifth generation. With regard to
acculturation level,
17% (n � 61) were categorized as “very Mexican oriented,”
38% (n �
138) “Mexican oriented to approximately balanced bicultural,”
34% (n �
123) “slightly Anglo oriented bicultural,” 10% (n � 37)
“strongly Anglo
oriented,” and 1% (n � 5) “very assimilated, Anglicized.”
The educational level of the female and male head of household,
respectively, was as follows: completed elementary school, 24%
and 21%;
attended high school, 25% and 23%; high school graduate, 19%
and 21%;
attended college/university, 14% and 12%; college/university
graduate,
10% and 12%; and graduate or professional degree, 2% and 1%.
Eighty-seven percent (n � 317) of the students planned to
attend a 2- or
4-year college/university following their high school
graduation, with the
remaining students indicating plans to attend technical school
(5.5%), work
(3.2%), enlist in the military (2.1%), and marry or stay at home
(0.5%).
Among students with intentions to continue their education at a
2- or 4-year
college/university, almost half (43.2%, n � 137) reported that
they would
work either full time (1.9%, n � 6) or part time (41.3%, n �
131). Over
a third (39.1%, n � 124) planned to attend the local 4-year
university,
and 19.2% (n � 61) planned to attend the local 2-year
community college.
The most often cited reasons for choosing to attend the college
or univer-
sity of their choice were because it was close to home and
family (36.5%,
n � 116), had a good program of study (10.7%, n � 34), was a
good
college/university (6.9%, n � 22), and was affordable or
inexpensive to
attend (4.1%, n � 13). Sixty-eight percent (n � 214) indicated
that they
would rely on financial aid (e.g., loans, grants, and work study)
to finance
their education, whereas 31.5% (n � 100) hoped to earn
scholar-
ships, 26.5% (n � 84) planned to receive financial support from
their
parents or other family members, and 25% (n � 78) planned to
work.
Procedure
Data collection occurred during the fall semester of the school
year.
Student participation was solicited through English IV classes
because
every senior was required to enroll in this class. Data collection
occurred
across 4 days, and Lisa Y. Flores met with every English IV
section (n �
46) at both schools. English teachers escorted their students to a
central
room at the beginning of the class period and stayed to monitor
students’
behaviors.
Packets containing an informed-assent form, an entry form for
cash
prizes, and the research instruments were distributed to students
as they
entered the room. The questionnaires were counterbalanced to
avoid order
effects from fatigue. Participants were told that the investigator
was inter-
ested in studying the career development of Mexican American
adoles-
cents. Students were told that it would take them most, if not
all, of the
class period to complete the questionnaires and were
encouraged to work
quickly. The investigator told the students that two of the
surveys looked
very similar (each listed the same occupations and educational
programs),
but these surveys asked students to rate either interests or skills.
Students
were informed of a possible follow-up study and were invited to
participate
in future studies. As an incentive to participate in the study,
students who
completed and returned the surveys were eligible for a random
drawing for
cash prizes (10 prizes for $20 and 1 prize for $50).
Instruments
Acculturation level. The Acculturation Rating Scale for
Mexican
Americans (ARSMA–II; Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995)
was a
30-item scale that assessed association with and identity with
the Mexican
and Anglo cultures on two independent subscales. Participants
responded
to the items using a 5-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to
extremely
often or almost always (5). An acculturation score was
calculated by
subtracting the mean score for items on the Anglo Orientation
Subscale
(AOS) from the mean score for items on the Mexican
Orientation Subscale
(MOS). On the basis of their acculturation score, participants
were cate-
gorized into one of the five acculturation levels described by
Cuellar et al.
(1995). Levels range from very Mexican oriented (1) to very
assimilated
(5). Middle categories represented bicultural individuals. Thus,
high scores
were indicative of a strong orientation toward the Anglo
culture.
The ARSMA–II, as well as prior to its revision, the ARSMA, is
one of
the most widely used measures to assess acculturation among
Mexican
Americans, and evidence suggests that it is a reliable and valid
instrument.
Adequate internal consistency coefficients have been reported
for the two
subscales with multiple samples (range from .79 to .83 for the
AOS and .87
to .91 for the MOS; Cuellar et al., 1995; Cuellar & Roberts,
1997;
Lessenger, 1997). Reliability coefficients of .77 for the AOS
and .91 for the
MOS were obtained in the present study.
Cuellar and his colleagues also reported a test–retest reliability
estimate
for the AOS and MOS over a 2-week interval of .94 and .96,
respectively.
Concurrent validity was assessed by comparing scores on the
ARSMA–II
with scores on the ARSMA and yielded a correlation coefficient
of .89.
Concurrent validity for the ARSMA–II was further supported
when its two
subscales correlated in the expected direction with the dominant
group and
ethnic group subscales of the Stephenson Multigroup
Acculturation Scale
(Stephenson, 2000). Lessenger (1997) provided additional
support for
concurrent validity when she reported that acculturation scores
on the
ARSMA–II correlated positively with other acculturation
measures. Con-
struct validity was supported when acculturation scores on the
ARSMA–II
were compared across generations, and differences were found
between
generation levels in the expected directions (Cuellar et al.,
1995; Lessen-
ger, 1997).
Feminist attitudes. The Attitudes Toward Feminism and the
Women’s
Movement Scale (FWM; Fassinger, 1994) was used to measure
feminist
attitudes. The FWM is a 10-item scale that assessed attitudes
about the
feminist movement. Participants rated their agreement with the
items along
a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly
agree (5).
Scale scores were obtained by averaging the items; high scores
reflect
profeminist attitudes.
Fassinger (1994) reported that the FWM had high internal
consistency
(� � .89), and O’Brien and Fassinger (1993) reported an
internal reliability
coefficient of .82 for the FWM with a sample of adolescent
women.
Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .68. Enns and Hackett
(1990)
reported a 2-week test–retest reliability coefficient of .81 with
female
college students. Convergent validity for the FWM was
supported when the
FWM was positively correlated with measures assessing
attitudes toward
women, gender roles, and feminism (Enns & Hackett, 1990;
Fassinger,
1994). In addition, the FWM correlated positively with items
assessing
feminist identification and favorability toward the women’s
movement
(Fassinger, 1994). Finally, Enns and Hackett (1990) reported
that the FWM
correlated in the expected directions with both interest and
involvement in
feminist activities. Divergent validity estimates revealed that
the FWM was
17MEXICAN AMERICAN WOMEN
not measuring gender role characteristics, dogmatism, and
social desirabil-
ity (Fassinger, 1994).
Mother’s level of education. A single item asked participants to
indi-
cate the highest level of education completed by their mother.
Options
ranged from elementary school to graduate/professional school.
High
scores represented high levels of education.
Mother’s occupational traditionality. An item asked participants
to
indicate their mother’s occupation, which was later categorized
according
to traditionality. Traditionality of mother’s career was
computed on the
basis of the percentage of women employed in a given career
and was
obtained through the Statistical Abstract of the United States
(1998), a
publication of the U.S. Bureau of the Census. The U.S. Census
Bureau
relies on information from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics
and Em-
ployment and Earnings to report these data. Scores ranged from
6 to 99,
with high scores representing careers with high concentrations
of women.
This indicator of career orientation has been used in previous
studies of
women’s career development (O’Brien, 1996; O’Brien &
Fassinger, 1993;
O’Brien, Friedman, Tipton, & Linn, 2000).
Nontraditional career self-efficacy. Self-efficacy expectations
with re-
gard to nontraditional occupations were assessed using a short
form of the
occupational self-efficacy questionnaire used by Church et al.
(1992). The
self-efficacy questionnaire used in this study was comparable
with career
self-efficacy measures used by Betz and Hackett (1981) and
Lauver and
Jones (1991). The original occupational questionnaire contained
a total
of 31 occupations for which participants rated their confidence
in their
ability to successfully learn to perform the job. The
nontraditional career
self-efficacy scale used for this study was modified to include
seven
male-dominated occupations (e.g., electronic equipment
repairer, police
officer, mechanical engineer). Occupations were categorized
according to
the percentage of women in the occupation according to U.S.
census data
(U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1998). A brief description of the
occupation
was provided for each job title.
Participants were asked to rate their confidence in their ability
and skills
to successfully learn to do the jobs. Participants responded to
the items
using a scale ranging from very unsure (1) to very sure (4).
Although
studies typically use 5-point scales to measure strength of self-
efficacy, we
followed the reasoning of Bores-Rangel et al. (1990), whose
sample
predominantly consisted of Hispanic students, that students may
depend-
ably and meaningfully discriminate these four bipolar levels.
Occupational
self-efficacy scores for male-dominated occupations were
obtained by
averaging the responses to the items. High scores reflected
strong levels of
nontraditional career self-efficacy.
Church et al. (1992) reported an …

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...inventionjournals
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...worldwideww
 
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...worldwideww
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer JAu Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer Jjbroyer
 
Literature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftLiterature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftSherri Wielgos
 
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different CognitiveCritically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitivesazoliver
 
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...Florida International University
 
D Lee Presentation
D Lee PresentationD Lee Presentation
D Lee Presentationddlee82
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...Jonathan Dunnemann
 
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014Sarah Howard
 

Mais procurados (20)

CHAPTER TWO.docx
CHAPTER TWO.docxCHAPTER TWO.docx
CHAPTER TWO.docx
 
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
Spiritual Well-Being and Parenting Stress in Caring for Children with Neuro-D...
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
 
RJunaid Final Paper
RJunaid Final PaperRJunaid Final Paper
RJunaid Final Paper
 
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
 
Self determination
Self determinationSelf determination
Self determination
 
Alberti Center Colloquium Series - Dr. Jamie Ostrov
Alberti Center Colloquium Series - Dr. Jamie OstrovAlberti Center Colloquium Series - Dr. Jamie Ostrov
Alberti Center Colloquium Series - Dr. Jamie Ostrov
 
Family Therapy Basic Concepts Nov 2016
Family Therapy Basic Concepts Nov 2016Family Therapy Basic Concepts Nov 2016
Family Therapy Basic Concepts Nov 2016
 
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
An Elaboration on the Distinction Between Controversial Parenting and Therape...
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer JAu Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
 
Literature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draftLiterature Review_final draft
Literature Review_final draft
 
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different CognitiveCritically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
 
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...
Divorce, Separation, Intact Families, Parenting Styles, Parental Conflict And...
 
D Lee Presentation
D Lee PresentationD Lee Presentation
D Lee Presentation
 
Bowenian
BowenianBowenian
Bowenian
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
 
Reflection
ReflectionReflection
Reflection
 
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014
AERA Mot sig keynote address 2014
 
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
 
Final theory paper
Final theory paperFinal theory paper
Final theory paper
 

Semelhante a Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between

Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMaternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMegan Dean-Mahoney
 
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docx
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docxWorking Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docx
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docxericbrooks84875
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peersgaz12000
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxdaniahendric
 
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year ReviewAdult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year ReviewJoe Andelija
 
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Joshua Gorinson
 
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxRunning head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxcowinhelen
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewroxcine
 
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsAttachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsDemona Demona
 
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1Amber Toler
 
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxAdolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxnettletondevon
 
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory Essay
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory EssayAn Evalution Of The Attachment Theory Essay
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory EssayPaper Writing Help Wise
 
Adult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisAdult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisRachel Davis
 
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing Lab
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing LabAPA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing Lab
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing LabJonathan Underwood
 
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...Scott Faria
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)MarioBuzz1
 

Semelhante a Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between (20)

Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child DevelopmentMaternal Anxiety and Child Development
Maternal Anxiety and Child Development
 
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docx
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docxWorking Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docx
Working Models of Attachment and Reactions to Different Forms .docx
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
 
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year ReviewAdult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
 
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
 
Writing Sample
Writing Sample Writing Sample
Writing Sample
 
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docxRunning head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
Running head IMPACTS OF CHILD ABUSE ON GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT.docx
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
 
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline PatientsAttachment Studies With Borderline Patients
Attachment Studies With Borderline Patients
 
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1
ATOLER Group_5_LiteratureReviewFinal-1
 
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxAdolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
 
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory Essay
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory EssayAn Evalution Of The Attachment Theory Essay
An Evalution Of The Attachment Theory Essay
 
Anxiety Research
Anxiety ResearchAnxiety Research
Anxiety Research
 
Adult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisAdult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment Analysis
 
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing Lab
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing LabAPA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing Lab
APA Literature Review Example by Purdue Online Writing Lab
 
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
 
Literature review
Literature reviewLiterature review
Literature review
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
 
Pinto gouveia
Pinto gouveia Pinto gouveia
Pinto gouveia
 

Mais de sherni1

Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2 exampleInte.docx
Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2  exampleInte.docxPart 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2  exampleInte.docx
Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2 exampleInte.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docx
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docxPart 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docx
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docx
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docxPart 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docx
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docxsherni1
 
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docx
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docxPART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docx
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docxPart 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docx
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docxPart 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docx
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docx
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docxPart 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docx
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docx
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docxPart 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docx
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docx
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docxPart 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docx
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docx
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docxPart 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docx
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docx
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docxPart 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docx
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docx
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docxPart 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docx
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docx
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docxPart 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docx
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docxPart 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docx
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docxPart 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docx
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docxPart 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docxPart 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docx
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docxPart 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docx
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docx
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docxPart 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docx
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docxsherni1
 
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docx
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docxPart 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docx
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docxsherni1
 

Mais de sherni1 (20)

Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2 exampleInte.docx
Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2  exampleInte.docxPart 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2  exampleInte.docx
Part 1 Think an example speak up anythingPart 2 exampleInte.docx
 
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docx
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docxPart 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docx
Part 1 Progress NoteUsing the client from your Week 3 Assignmen.docx
 
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docx
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docxPart 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docx
Part 1 Older Adult InterviewInterview an older adult of you.docx
 
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docx
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docxPART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docx
PART 1 OVERVIEWIn this project you are asked to conduct your own.docx
 
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docxPart 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial mea.docx
 
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docx
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docxPart 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docx
Part 1 Legislation GridBased on the health-related bill (pr.docx
 
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docx
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docxPart 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docx
Part 1 Financial Acumen1. Review at least three (3) articles on.docx
 
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docx
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docxPart 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docx
Part 1 Parent NewsletterAn article explaining the school’s po.docx
 
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docx
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docxPart 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docx
Part 1 ResearchConduct  some independent research. Using Rasmus.docx
 
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docx
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docxPart 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docx
Part 1 What are some challenges with syndromic surveillance P.docx
 
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docx
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docxPart 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docx
Part 1 Procedure and purpose 10.0 Procedures are well-develop.docx
 
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docx
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docxPart 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docx
Part 1 Post your own definition of school readiness (and offer .docx
 
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docx
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docxPart 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docx
Part 1 Art selectionInstitute Part 1 Art sel.docx
 
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docxPart 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseVarious reform groups with various causes.docx
 
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docx
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docxPart 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docx
Part 1 Assessment SummaryIn 500-750-words, summarize the fo.docx
 
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docxPart 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docx
Part 1 Post a ResponseDuring the Reconstruction Era, the So.docx
 
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docxPart 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docx
Part 1 Financial AcumenKeeping abreast of the financial measure.docx
 
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docx
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docxPart 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docx
Part 1 Do an independently guided tour of news and media coverage.docx
 
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docx
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docxPart 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docx
Part 1 Describe the scopescale of the problem. Problemado.docx
 
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docx
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docxPart 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docx
Part 1 Art CreationSelect one of the visual art pieces from Cha.docx
 

Último

TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfNirmal Dwivedi
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17Celine George
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and ModificationsMJDuyan
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxAmanpreet Kaur
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Association for Project Management
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...pradhanghanshyam7136
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfPoh-Sun Goh
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxnegromaestrong
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfSherif Taha
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptRamjanShidvankar
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 

Último (20)

TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student briefSpatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 

Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between

  • 1. Maladaptive Perfectionism as a Mediator and Moderator Between Adult Attachment and Depressive Mood Meifen Wei Iowa State University Brent Mallinckrodt University of Missouri–Columbia Daniel W. Russell and W. Todd Abraham Iowa State University This study examined maladaptive perfectionism (concern over mistakes, doubts about one’s ability to accomplish tasks, and failure to meet high standards) as both a mediator and a moderator between adult attachment (anxiety and avoidance) and depressive mood (depression and hopelessness). Survey data were collected from 310 undergraduates and analyzed using structural equation modeling (SEM) methods. Results indicated that maladaptive perfectionism partially mediated the relationship between attachment anxiety and depressive mood and fully mediated the relationship between attachment avoidance and depressive mood. Bootstrap methods were used to assess the magnitude of the indirect effects. Significant moderator effects were also found with SEM methods. The association between attachment anxiety and depressive mood was stronger as perfectionism increased. Perfectionism was not a significant moderator for attachment avoidance and depressive
  • 2. mood. Throughout the past decade, there has been a growing interest among counseling psychologists in applying Bowlby’s (1973, 1980, 1988) attachment theory to understanding adult development and the counseling process (Lopez, 1995; Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mallinckrodt, 2000). The initial formulations of adult attachment posited four qualitative categories of attachment based on combi- nations of positive and negative working models of self and others (e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). However, research has failed to confirm the existence of qualitative cutoff points, and instead supports two continuous dimensions as the best way to model adult attachment (Fraley & Waller, 1998). In a factor analysis of data gathered from over 1,000 undergraduates, Bren- nan, Clark, and Shaver (1998) included all of the extant self- report measures of adult attachment (14 measures, 60 subscales, 323 items) and identified two relatively orthogonal dimensions of Anxiety and Avoidance. Adult attachment anxiety is characterized as an excessive need for approval from others and fear of inter - personal rejection or abandonment. Adult attachment avoidance involves an excessive need for self-reliance and fear of interper- sonal closeness or dependence. People with high levels of either dimension or both dimensions in combination are assumed to have an insecure adult attachment orientation. By contrast, people with low levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance have the capacity
  • 3. for secure adult attachment, a positive sense of personal compe - tence, and the ability to maintain supportive attachments (Brennan et al., 1998; Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mallinckrodt, 2000). Previous empirical research has provided strong evidence for a link between insecure attachment and various forms of psycholog- ical distress (for reviews, see Lopez & Brennan, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). For example, relative to their secure counter - parts, people with insecure attachment reported greater distress and hostility during a laboratory problem-centered discussion (Simp- son, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996), greater affective intensity and emotionality in their daily life (Pietromonaco & Barrett, 1997), more depressive symptoms (Roberts, Gotlib, & Kassel, 1996), greater interpersonal problems (Mallinckrodt & Wei, 2003), and more emotional distress (Collins, 1996). Thus, the link between various forms of insecure attachment and indices of psychological distress (e.g., depressive mood) has been fairly well established. More recently, research linking attachment insecurity and distress (e.g., depressive mood) has been shifting from an examination of simple bivariate linear relationships to multivariate interactional models that examine the roles of mediators and moderators of these relationships (Collins, 1996; Lopez, Mitchell, & Gormley, 2002; Roberts et al., 1996; Wei, Heppner, & Mallinckrodt, 2003). One example of this new emphasis on multivariate models is recent research that has examined the relationships among
  • 4. attach- ment, perfectionism, and adjustment (Rice & Mirzadeh, 2000). Perfectionism has been conceptualized as a multidimensional con- struct, with both adaptive and maladaptive aspects (Flett & Hewitt, 2002). Adaptive perfectionism involves setting high (but achiev- able) personal standards, a preference for order and organization, a sense of self-satisfaction, a desire to excel, and a motivation to Meifen Wei and W. Todd Abraham, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University; Brent Mallinckrodt, Department of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, University of Missouri–Columbia; Daniel W. Russell, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Iowa State University. We thank Robyn Zakalik, Shanna Behrendsen, Anne Giusto, and Mike McGregor for their assistance with data collection. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Meifen Wei, Department of Psychology, W112 Lagomarcino Hall, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011-3180. E-mail: [email protected] Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 51, No. 2, 201–212 0022-0167/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0167.51.2.201
  • 9. ss em in at ed b ro ad ly . achieve positive rewards. Maladaptive perfectionism involves un- realistically high standards, intense ruminative concern over mis- takes, perceived pressure from others to be perfect, a perceived large discrepancy between one’s performance and personal stan- dards, compulsive doubting of one’s actions, and motivation to avoid negative consequences (Enns & Cox, 2002). Theorists suggest that maladaptive perfectionism results when a child’s need for acceptance and love from parents is accompanied by a parent’s failure to provide the needed acceptance and positive regard (Hamachek, 1978). Observational research has shown that if caregivers are inconsistent and unreliable in responding to the emotional or physical needs of young children, anxious attachment
  • 10. is frequently the result (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). Serious interpersonal problems may develop in adults whose par- ents used a love withdrawal style of discipline involving threats to withhold affection as a means of control (Mallinckrodt & Wei, 2003). Children with attachment anxiety may quickly learn that if they are “perfect” boys or girls, they may be more likely to gain their parents’ love and acceptance. This pattern of striving for perfection as a way to earn acceptance that was only intermittently available in childhood may persist as a maladaptive pattern in adults. A different dynamic may underlie the connection between per- fectionism and attachment avoidance. Attachment avoidance is believed to involve a negative working model of others along with a positive working model of self (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). However, striving to be “perfect” in the view of others may be an outward defense that masks a deeply wounded inner sense of self resulting from the inadequate emotional responsiveness of caregivers early in development (Lapan & Patton, 1986; Robbins & Patton, 1985). Children with avoidant attachment tend to de- scribe themselves as perfect (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988), but they may drive themselves to attain perfection to avoid others’ rejection and to manage their own hidden sense of imperfections. For example, a child may think, “If I am perfect, no one will hurt me” (Flett, Hewitt, Oliver, & Macdonald, 2002). Thus, initially
  • 11. striving to be perfect may be a positive coping mechanism for children whose caregivers are unresponsive or inconsistent in their respon- siveness to the child’s needs. However, if striving to be perfect is overused as a coping strategy, it may lead to depressive mood in adulthood. Therefore, the specific form that the maladaptive striv- ing for perfection may take might depend on the particular mixture of attachment avoidance or attachment anxiety experienced in adulthood. Although several theorists have suggested that the origins of perfectionism are related to problematic attachment in the parent– child relationship, until recently there were very few empi rical studies of perfectionism and attachment. Among the small number of available studies, Rice and Mirzadeh (2000) reported that mal- adaptive perfectionism was related to insecure attachment, whereas adaptive perfectionism was related to secure attachment in college students. Similarly, Andersson and Perris (2000) found that perfectionism was positively associated with insecure attach- ment. Additionally, Flett et al. (2001) found that persons with high attachment anxiety and avoidance reported higher perceived pres- sure from others to be perfect. Thus, previous studies have pro- vided tentative evidence that attachment avoidance and attachment
  • 12. anxiety are positively associated with maladaptive perfectionism. Several studies have shown that perfectionis m is positively associated with depression or hopelessness. For example, perfec- tionism in college students was associated with greater depressive symptoms (e.g., Chang, 2002; Chang & Sanna, 2001; Cheng, 2001; Hewitt & Flett, 1991) and suicidal preoccupation (Adkins & Parker, 1996; Chang, 1998). In longitudinal studies, perfectionism has been linked to both depression and hopelessness over time (Chang & Rand, 2000; Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & Mosher, 1995). Also, Hewitt and Flett (2002) reported that perceived pressure from others to be perfect was associated with hopelessness across different studies and populations (e.g., Chang & Rand, 2000; Dean, Range, & Goggin, 1996). On the basis of these previous studies, in the present study we chose to represent the latent variable of depressive mood with indicators of depression and hopelessness. It is possible that adults with high attachment anxiety or avoid- ance are likely to develop maladaptive perfectionism and, in turn, experience significant depressive mood. Some studies have exam- ined how maladaptive perfectionism might serve as a mediator between parent– child interactions and depressive mood. Randolph and Dykman (1998) found that perfectionism fully mediated the relationship between critical parenting and depression- proneness
  • 13. and partially mediated the relationship between perfectionistic parenting and depression-proneness in undergraduate students. Enns, Cox, and Clara (2002) reported that maladaptive perfection- ism mediated the relationship between harsh parenting (e.g., crit- ical parenting, parental overprotection, and parental lack of care) and depression. However, our search of the literature could not locate any previous study that examined perfectionism as a medi- ator between attachment and depressive mood. If maladaptive perfectionism does serve as a mediator, interventions could be targeted at adults with attachment anxiety or avoidance to help decrease their maladaptive perfectionism and in turn decrease their depressive mood. Hewitt and Flett (2002) argued that perfectionism could serve as a moderator (as well as a mediator) between insecure attachment and depressive mood. Several studies have found that specific dimensions of perfectionism (e.g., pressure from others to be perfect) interacted with general stress (e.g., major life stress or self-appraisal stress) to predict increased depression symptoms or negative affect (e.g., Chang & Rand, 2000; Cheng, 2001; Dunkley, Zuroff, & Blankstein, 2003; Flett et al., 1995). That is, greater depression or negative affect was reported by participants with higher combined levels of perfectionism and perceived stress. In addition, other studies reported that specific dimensions of perfec- tionism interacted with specific stressors to predict higher levels of depression. Hewitt and Flett (1993) found that perfectionism,
  • 14. particularly in the form of perceived pressure from others to be perfect, interacted with interpersonal stressors (e.g., relationship problems or lack of intimacy) to predict depression. It appears that maladaptive perfectionism could serve as a potential moderator of the relationship between general or specific stressors and psycho- logical distress. Attachment anxiety or attachment avoidance could be viewed as a source of chronic interpersonal stress. Perfectionism may lead to depressive mood because it generates core interpersonal needs that are difficult to satisfy (i.e., the need for others’ approval, or the need to be perfect to avoid others’ rejection). Maladaptive perfec- tionism might interact with attachment anxiety or attachment 202 WEI, MALLINCKRODT, RUSSELL, AND ABRAHAM T hi s do cu m en t i s
  • 19. avoidance to worsen depressive mood (Hewitt & Flett, 2002). From the standpoint of putative causal links, in a mediating sce- nario attachment insecurity (x1) is believed to cause higher levels of maladaptive perfectionism (x2), which in turn causes higher levels of depressive mood (y). If the mediation is partial rather than complete, there would also be a significant direct link between (x1) attachment insecurity and (y) depressive mood (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Holmbeck, 1997). By contrast, in a moderating scenario there is no requirement that x1 causes x2 and, in fact, the two variables may be uncorrelated. However, the strength of associa- tion between x1 (in this case, attachment insecurity) and y (depres- sive mood) is believed to vary for differing levels of x2 (maladap- tive perfectionism). Unfortunately, there has been no empirical research studying how maladaptive perfectionism might interact with attachment to predict depressive mood. Because it is possible for maladaptive perfectionism to serve as both an intermediate link in the causal chain leading from attach- ment insecurity to depressive mood (i.e., as a mediator) and as a variable that alters the strength of association between attachment insecurity and depressive mood (i.e., as a moderator), both types of relationships were explored in this study. Specifically, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether the maladaptive aspects of perfectionism (e.g., concern over mistakes, doubts
  • 20. about actions, and perceived discrepancy between one’s standards and performance) serve as a mediator, as a moderator, or as both in the context of the relationship between adult attachment insecurity (anxiety and avoidance) and depressive mood (depression and hopelessness). Figures 1A and 1B depict both of these hypothe- sized relationships. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the models depicted in this figure. Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, and Ashby (2001) argued that the discrepancy between high standards and perceptions of performance was a defining feature of maladaptive perfectionism, whereas high standards without perceived discrepancy could indicate adaptive perfection- ism. Therefore, measures of discrepancy between standards and performance, concern over mistakes, and doubts about one’s ac- Figure 1. Hypothesized mediating effects (A) and moderating effects (B) of maladaptive perfectionism on the links between attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance with depressive mood. The moderating effects (B) of maladaptive perfectionism on the links between attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance with depressive mood were examined separately. 203PERFECTIONISM AND ATTACHMENT T hi s do cu m
  • 25. ly . tions served as the indicators for the construct of maladaptive perfectionism, in addition to measures of depression and hopeless- ness, which served as indicators of the latent variable depressive mood.1 Method Participants Participants were 310 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology classes at a large midwestern university. The participants were told that the purpose of the research was “to learn about factors affecting college students’ adjustment.” The sample included 225 (73%) women and 85 (27%) men. Their mean age was 19.27 years (SD � 1.88, range � 18 –30 yrs.). Approximately 53% of the participants were freshmen. Ethnic identification was predominantly White/Caucasian (84%), followed by international students of various ethnicities (4.8%), Asian American (4.2%), African American (2.3%), Hispanic American (2.3%), multiracial American (1.0%), and others (1.3%). Most participants (98.0%)
  • 26. indicated they were single or never married. Students received partial credit toward their course grade for participating in this study. The amount of credit varied depending on their particular section of the course. Instruments Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECRS; Brennan et al., 1998). The ECRS is a 36-item self-report measure of adult attachment containing two 18-item subscales derived from the factor analysis by Brennan et al. (1998) described previously. The subscales assess dimensions of adult attachment, Anxiety and Avoidance. Participants use a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 � disagree strongly, 7 � agree strongly) to rate how well each statement describes their typical feelings in romantic relationships. The Anxiety subscale taps fears of abandonment and rejection. The Avoidance subscale assesses discomfort with dependence and intimate self- disclosure. Brennan et al.’s reported coefficient alpha was .91 and .94 for the Anxiety and Avoidance subscales, respectively. In the present study, coefficient alpha was .90 for the Anxiety subscale and .91 for the Avoidance subscale. Brennan et al. also reported that scale scores were correlated in expected directions with scores on self-report measures of touch aversion
  • 27. and postcoital emotions. Measured indicators for the two latent variables of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were created from three 6-item parcels for each subscale. Following the recommendation of Rus- sell, Kahn, Spoth, and Altmaier (1998), exploratory factor analyses were conducted using maximum-likelihood extraction for the two factors (Anx- iety and Avoidance) separately. The items were then rank- ordered on the basis of the magnitude of the factor loadings and successively assigned pairs of the highest and lowest items to each parcel to equalize the average loadings of each parcel on its respective factor. Almost Perfect Scale–Revised (APS-R; Slaney et al., 2001). The APS-R is a 23-item self-report measure designed to assess levels of perfectionism. Respondents use a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 � strongly disagree, 7 � strongly agree) in responding to the items. The APS-R is made up of three subscales: High Standards, Order, and Discrepancy. In this study only the 12-item Discrepancy subscale was used. This subscale measures the degree to which respondents perceive themselves as failing to meet personal standards for performance. Slaney et al. reported a coeffi- cient alpha of .92 for the Discrepancy subscale, whereas coefficient alpha
  • 28. was .94 in the present sample. Slaney et al. reported evidence of construct validity in the form of significant correlations between the Discrepancy subscale and other perfectionism measures such as Concern Over Mistakes (r � .55) and Doubts About Actions (r � .62). Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990). The FMPS is a 35-item instrument designed to mea- sure perfectionism. Each item uses a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 � disagree strongly, 5 � agree strongly). Consistent with Dunkley, Blank- stein, Halsall, Williams, and Winkworth (2000), only two of the six FMPS subscales were used as indicators of perfectionis m in this study: (a) Concern Over Mistakes (9 items) taps a tendency to interpret mistakes as failures and to believe that one will lose the respect of others when one fails; and (b) the Doubts About Actions (4 items) subscale, which measures the tendency to doubt one’s ability to accomplish tasks or the quality of one’s performance. In the present study, coefficient alphas were .89 and .74 for Concern Over Mistakes and Doubts About Actions, respectively. Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, and Neubauer (1993) found that Concern Over Mistakes and Doubts About Actions not only reflected
  • 29. maladaptive eval- uative concerns of perfectionism, but were also the subscales most strongly related to depression. Criterion-related validity is evidenced by correlations between FMPS subscales and measures of psychological symptoms (e.g., Brief Symptom Inventory) and adjustment such as compulsiveness, self- esteem, procrastination, and depression (Frost et al., 1993, 1990). Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961). The BDI is a widely used 21-item self-report measure of depressive symptoms. Each item consists of a depression symptom cluster scored on a 0 –3 response scale based on the severity of the symptom. Scores across the items are summed to obtain a total BDI score, with higher scores indicating more severe depression. Internal consistency for the BDI for undergraduates ranges from .78 to .92, with a mean coefficient alpha of .85. In the present study, coefficient alpha was .86. Test–retest reliabilities for nonpsychiatric participants ranged from .60 (7 days) to .83 (1– 6 hr), with reports of .78 for a 2-week and a 3-week period. Considerable evidence of validity has been demonstrated for the BDI as a measure of depressive symptoms (Beck, 1967; Bumberry, Oliver, & McClure, 1978).
  • 30. Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS; Beck, Weissman, Lester, & Trexler, 1974). The BHS is a 20-item inventory that assesses the degree to which an individual’s cognitive schemata are characterized by pessimistic expec- tations. The scale uses a true–false response format. Scores can range from 0 to 20, with higher scores indicating a greater degree of hopelessness. Internal consistency of .93 has been reported, along with concurrent validity of .74 with clinical ratings of hopelessness and .60 with other scales of hopelessness (Beck et al., 1974). In the present study, coefficient alpha for the BHS was .78. Procedure The questionnaires were administrated to small groups of 3–25 students who signed up for one of several data collection times. Participants were guaranteed anonymity of their responses and confidentiality of the data, given that no personal identifying information was solicited on the ques- tionnaires. Completing the entire packet of instruments typically required 25– 40 min. Results Descriptive Statistics
  • 31. Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for the 13 measured variables are shown in Table 1. Data were checked for normality, which is a critical assumption underlying the 1 One issue raised by reviewers concerned the fact that we only used one measure, the ECRS, to operationalize the attachment variable. Because the ECRS was developed on the basis of a factor analysis of existing measures of attachment (see Brennan et al., 1998), we felt that this measure ade- quately represented the nature of the construct. Indeed, it is very likely that items on any other measure of adult attachment would be redundant with items on this measure. Therefore, we did not feel it was necessary to use other measures of adult attachment in this investigation. 204 WEI, MALLINCKRODT, RUSSELL, AND ABRAHAM T hi s do cu m en t i s
  • 36. maximum-likelihood procedure used in this study. Results indi- cated univariate normality for all measured variables except the BHS (Beck et al., 1974; skew Z � 1.87, and kurtosis Z � 4.47). We therefore conducted a square root transformation for the BHS variable. The skew and kurtosis for the transformed BHS were Z � .16 and .75, respectively, indicating a normal distribution. The BHS and the transformed BHS were highly correlated (r � .94). Therefore, we used the transformed BHS variable in subsequent analyses.2 Measurement Model for Testing Mediation Effects The analysis of the proposed mediation model followed the two-step procedure recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988). First, we used a confirmatory factor analysis to develop a measurement model with an acceptable fit to the data. Once an acceptable measurement model was developed, the structural model was tested. The measurement model was estimated using the maximum-likelihood method in the LISREL 8.50 program. As suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999) and Quintana and Maxwell (1999), three indices were used to assess goodness of fit for the models: the comparative fit index (CFI; values of .95 or greater are desirable), the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR; values of .08 or less are desirable), and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA; values of .06 or less are desirable). Finally, we used the chi-square difference test to compare nested models. An initial test of the measurement model resulted in a relatively good fit to the data, �2(38, N � 310) � 72.60, p �
  • 37. .001, CFI � .98, SRMR � .04, and RMSEA � .05 (95% confidence interval [CI]: .03, .07).3 All of the loadings of the measured variables on the latent variables were statistically significant ( p � .001; see Table 2). Therefore, all of the latent variables appea r to have been adequately measured by their respective indicators. In addition, the correlations among the independent (exogenous) la- tent variables, the mediator latent variable, and dependent latent variable were statistically significant ( p � .05; see Table 3) Structural Model for Testing Mediation Effects A number of methods have been suggested in the literature for testing mediation effects. Recently, MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoff- man, West, and Sheets (2002) evaluated 14 methods with regard to 2 We also tested the multivariate normality of the observed variables as a set, including the transformed BHS (Beck et al., 1974) variable, based on the test developed by Mardia (see Bollen, 1989). The significant result, �2(2, N � 310) � 114.05, p � .001, indicated that the data were not multivariate normal. Therefore, we used the procedure developed by Sa- torra and Bentler (1988) to adjust the chi-square statistics and standard errors of the parameter estimates for the impact of nonnormality. In the
  • 38. mediation model, the results after adjusting for the impact of nonnormality did not differ from the results when we did not adjust for nonnormality. In the moderation model, the results for the path coefficients were identical whether or not we adjusted for the impact of nonnormality. However, the standard error of the latent interaction term became very large following the adjustment for nonnormality. This problem associated with interaction terms and the Satorra-Bentler adjustment for nonnormality has been noted by others (e.g., Yang-Wallentin & Joreskog, 2001). Therefore, we report results for the moderation model without adjusting for the impact of nonnormality. 3 We examined whether the results would be equivalent for men and women in the measurement model, structural model, and the models with interaction effect. A series of multiple-group analyses were conducted using LISREL 8.50 to examine whether female and male groups differed from one another in terms of the measurement model, the structural model, and the models with interaction effects (Byrne, 1998). Results suggested that the measurement model and structural model were equivalent for the male and female groups. However, the models comparing men and women that included the interaction effect did not converge. This was
  • 39. likely because of the relatively small number of men (n � 85) included in the sample. Therefore, we conducted a hierarchical regression analysis to examine whether the interaction effect varied for men and women. Results of the regression analysis indicated that the three-way interaction (Attachment Anxiety � Maladaptive Perfectionism � Gender) predicting depressive mood was not significant (� � .002), … IFSP Sample Template Child’s name ________________________ Age _______________________ Date ______________________ Service Coordinator_(this will be you)_________________________________ Review Date _(the date you submit)_____________________ I. Statements of Family Strengths and Resources II. Statements of Family Concerns and Priorities *Concerns:
  • 40. *Priorities: III. Child’s Present Level of Development and Abilities Cognitive Skills (Thinking, reasoning, and learning) Communication Skills (Understanding, communicating with others, and expressing self with others) Self- Care/Adaptive Skills (Bathing, feeding, dressing, and toileting) Gross and Fine Motor Skills (Moving) Social-Emotional Development (Feelings, coping, and getting along with others)
  • 41. Health/Physical Development (Hearing, vision, and health) IV. Outcome Statements • Write one Goal with a strategy or activity that Dashawn must master before the next meeting in 6month. • Write one goal with a strategy or activity that his parents must due before the next meeting in six months. Dashawn’s Case Dashawn was referred to the Infant & Toddler Connection of Wonderland by his Pediatrician, Dr. Purvis. Dr. Purvis sent over copies of the developmental screening and M-CHAT he completed with Dashawn. Based on parent report, Dashawn was born full-term, weighing 8lbs 3 oz., following an uncomplicated pregnancy. He has a history of ear infections and his sister received speech therapy when she was a toddler. Dashawn’s mother is not concerned about his hearing or vision. During the evaluation, Dashawn played with blocks, a ball, looked at books, and pointed to some body parts. He made some animal sounds, used jargon as he played, and said two words. Dashawn interacted with others around him, responded to his name, and following simple directions. He took turns in play, put shapes in a puzzle, and scribbled with crayons. He pretended to feed his
  • 42. Clifford dog and put him to sleep. Dashawn can take off his clothes and occasionally seems to be aware that his diaper is soiled. He eats without any problems except that he chokes often, according to his mother. Based on the evaluation, Dashawn was found eligible for weekly speech therapy services due to developmental delays. He is showing strengths in his gross and fine motor, social, receptive communication and self- help skills. Dashawn was born August 12, 2013. Dashawn was referred to the Infant & Toddler Connection of Wonderland by his pediatrician, Dr. Purvis, due to concerns for his expressive language development. Dr. Purvis completed a developmental screening with Dashawn and his mother at 24 months well-child visit and noted that Dashawn was only using three words: mama, ball and juice. Dr. Purvis also completed the M-CHAT but no concerns for autism were noted based on the screening results. Dr. Purvis recommended that Dashawn be tested by the developmental pediatrician at the local children’s hospital, which is scheduled for May. Dashawn also had an audiological exam on July 3, 2018, which found his hearing to be within normal limits. Dashawn was born full-term, weighing 8lbs 3oz, following an uncomplicated pregnancy. Based on review of Dashawn’s medical records, the only complication noted after delivery was jaundice, three ear infections but otherwise has been a healthy child. Dashawn passed his newborn hearing screening at birth and no concerns have been noted for his vision. All immunizations are up-to-date. Dashawn’s mother reports a family history of delayed communication development, as his sister also received early intervention services due to developmental delay. His assessment service planning was held in the family’s home with Dashawn, his mother, a speech therapist, a developmental service provider, and the service coordinator present. Dashawn was shy at first, but warmed up quickly to the assessment activities. He especially enjoyed playing with the ball, putting pegs in a pegboard, and scribbling with crayons. As Dashawn warmed up, he began to interact with those around him and
  • 43. would look around to be sure that everyone was watching and clapping for him. He took turns in play stacking blocks (up to a tower of eight) and knocking the tower down and tossing the ball back and forth. While playing with the ball, at one point Dashawn hit his head on the underside of the table and went to his mother for comfort. He quickly recovered and continued playing. Dashawn was able to follow directions to locate the ball, to give the ball to his mom, and other 1-2 step commands. During the assessment, Dashawn responded to his name, pointed to pictures in a book, and pointed to 5 body parts on a doll and himself. When looking at the book, Dashawn made the “woof” and “meow” sounds to pictures of dogs and cats, and used some jargon in play. They only true words today were “mama” and “ball”. Dashawn’s mother said that Dashawn can also say “top’ for stop, to tell his sister to stop bothering him. Dashawn’s mother reports that Dashawn has tantrums often during the day when he gets frustrated or tired. Because Dashawn’s tantrums are so frequent, his mother said that it is difficult to take him out on errands and to the mall. Dashawn’s mother and sister have started trying to teach Dashawn a few signs (more, cracker, car) but Dashawn have not yet begun to use them. Dashawn attended well to activities where he played one-on-one with an adult. He put the shapes in the puzzle and looked to the educator who was playing with him for help when the triangle would not fit correctly. He enjoyed scribbling and copying lines and playing a matching game during which he matched three objects on request. Dashawn played pretend with his Clifford stuffed animal, pretending that Clifford was eating and going to sleep. When Dashawn wanted to play a different game, he would put his hands on the toy bag that contained the testing materials, look at the educator, and vocalize “uh-uh” He was very purposeful in his communication but was not able to imitate words or sounds in play today. Dashawn is able to move about independently by walking, climbing, and running. His mother reports that Dashawn’s
  • 44. ability to move is his greatest strength. Dashawn can push and carry large objects, like his child sized chair. He can jump from the bottom step in his house, climb up onto the couch to sit, and throw a large ball without falling. Dashawn helps put laundry away and helps throw trash away when asked. He can take off his clothes and occasionally seems to be aware of his diaper being soiled. Dashawn will vocalize to get assistance, using the same “uh-uh” sounds mentioned earlier, and will point to what he wants if out of reach. Dashawn eats well but has trouble chewing his food. His other reports that he will sometimes pack his cheeks “like a squirrel” then choke trying to swallow. She also reports that when he was an infant, he took a long time to drink his bottle. Dashawn can feed himself using fingers and will sometimes use a spoon. He also drinks from a sippy cup and will say “hush” to get juice when his cup is empty. Based on the assessment, Dashawn is showing strength in his gross and fine motor, social receptive communication, and self-help skills. He is showing developmental delays in his expressive communication and cognitive development. His expressive communication is limited by the fact that Dashawn is only using 4 words consistently at this time and seems to have a limited variety of sounds. He seems to have some difficulty coordinating the movements of his mouth to chew and make sounds. Dashawn’s cognitive delay appears to be related to his expressive communication, as his problem-solving skills appear to be appropriate for his age. Writing IFSP –Dashawn’s casePage 4 The Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP) and the Individualized Education Program (IEP)
  • 45. Definition of the IFSP A plan of services for infants and toddlers and their families. Such a plan includes statements regarding the child’s present developmental level, the family’s strengths and needs, the major outcomes of the plan, specific interventions and delivery systems to accomplish outcomes, dates of initiation and duration of services, and a plan for transition into public schools. Definition of the IEP A program devised to satisfy IDEA’s requirement that students with disabilities must receive an educational program based on multidisciplinary assessment and designed to meet their individual needs. The IEP must include consideration of the student’s present level of performance, annual goals, special education and related services, time in general education, timeline for special education services, and an annual evaluation. FIRST THING!!!!! (before anything else)PARENTAL CONSENTParents as partnersCultural, ethnic, and linguistic differencesFamily’s strengths and needsChild’s strengths and needs Process of the IFSP
  • 46. SCREENING. Child Find, established in the 1960s (though not added to law until 1986) as an effort to find children with disabilities and other special needs, does a screening to find those children. Those that need further evaluation go on to diagnostic testing. Definition of a couple of terms …VALIDITY: a test that measures what it purports to measure.RELIABILITY: how accurate, de-pendable, and predictable a test is.SENSITIVITY: ability of a screening tool to identify correctly children with disabilities.SPECIFICITY: ability of a screening tool to correctly identify children who do NOT have a disability Back to IFSP process DETERMINING ELIGIBILITY. Eligibility for special education and related services cannot be made on a single test or assessment. The data must be gleaned from several domains and sources. Assessment tools must be in a child’s native language. This does not mean a tool that has been TRANSLATED – this screws with the test’s validity and reliability. IFSP process PLANNING THE PROGRAM – to plan a
  • 47. program that will benefit the family and the child; the writing of the IFSP with all domains considered and a part of the writing. IFSP outlines what services will be delivered, how they will be delivered, and what skills and areas will be addressed. End of IFSP process . . . MONITORING PROGRESS and EVALUATING THE PROGRAM: the IFSP should be reevaluated at least once a year. From these evaluations, the program may be modified to better suit the child’s strengths and needs. TERMINOLOGYService coordinator: an interdisciplinary team member responsible for integrating services and keeping the family informed and involved.Assistive technology: various kinds of equipment designed to facilitate learning and communication for children with disabilities.Itinerant special education teacher: a consultant that travels between several classrooms for service delivery TERMINOLOGYAUDIOLOGIST: a specially certified professional who focuses on hearing testing and hearing impairmentsDURATION MEASURES: how long an event or behavior lasts.FREQUENCY COUNTS: keeping track of how often a behavior occursINTERDISCIPLINARY TEAM: several different professionals working together on a common problemIQ TESTS: intelligence tests, norm-referenced
  • 48. TERMINOLOGYLEAST RESTRICTIVE ENVIRONMENT: most normalized environment in which the needs of a child with disabilities can be met appropriately.MULTIDISCIPLINARY: involving members of various disciplines who work independently but exchange findingsNORM-REFERENCED ASSESSMENT: instrument that compares a child’s developmental level to a normative sample of same-age peers. TERMINOLOGYORIENTATION AND MOBILITY SPECIALIST: therapist who works with children with visual impairments to teach awareness of their position in the environment, of significant objects, and how to move safely and efficiently in the environment.PATHOLOGIST: professional who focuses on diseases or impairments.PEDIATRIC OPHTHALMOLOGIST: physician who specializes in diseases and malfunctioning of the eyes in developmental years. TERMINOLOGYPORTFOLIO: a carefully selected collection of a child’s work that is used to document growth and development.PSYCHOLINGUIST: one who studies and analyzes the acquisition and production of languagePSYCHOPATHOLOGIST: one who specializes in viewing mental disorders from a psychological perspective.SECONDARY PREVENTION: providing adequate intervention services before the condition worsens or affects other areas of development.
  • 49. TERMINOLOGYSERVICE DELIVERY MODEL: a formal plan devised by the various agencies involved in providing services to a child and his/her family.STANDARDIZED TESTS: assessment instruments that include precise directions for administering and scoring.TRANSDISCIPLINARY TEAM: a team that shares the responsibilities for assessment, program planning, implementation, and evaluation across members. TYPES OF TEACHER OBSERVATIONAL ASSESSMENTS:CHECKLISTSFREQUENCY COUNTSDURATION MEASURESANECDOTAL NOTESRUNNING RECORDSLOGS, JOURNALS, DIARIESTIME SAMPLINGLANGUAGE SAMPLINGPORTFOLIO TEACHERS SHOULD NOT:MAKE A DIAGNOSIS!!!Use LABELS to describe children Raise parents’ ANXIETYTELL parents what to doJUMP TO CONCLUSIONS without adequate data TEACHERS SHOULD:Work well with familiesVoice concerns to familiesListen carefully and respectfullyBe knowledgeableBe culturally sensitiveWork collaboratively with parentsRemember that all children (and all parents) are different and unique.
  • 50. PROCESS OF THE IEP Same as for IFSP: screen, diagnose, plan program, monitor, and evaluate.Should include child’s present levelsLong term goalsShort term objectivesSpecific services to be providedAccountabilityWhere and when WHO IS ON THE TEAM???PARENTSChild’s regular classroom teacherChild’s special education teacherPrincipal or vice-principal or program head (LEA representative)A person who can interpret the dataService coordinator or equivalentSchool counselor or equivalentInterpreterPhysical therapistOccupational therapist PsychologistMedical DoctorSpeech-Language PathologistCommunity support personSocial WorkerOrientation and Mobility SpecialistLawyersBehavior specialistsAssistive technology specialistVision SpecialistASL interpreterOther interested or involved parties Informal Assessment Measures Checklist: a method of documenting the presence or absence of skill or behavior in a developmental sequence. ~class activity Frequency Count: keeping track of how often a behavior occurs.
  • 51. ~watch how many times players in white shirts pass ball http://viscog.beckman.uiuc.edu/grafs/demos/15.html Informal Assessment Measures Running Records: detailed account of a segment of time, recording all behaviors and quotes during that time. ~write down EVERYTHING exactly http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-ulsq4a2SCg&feature=related * Informal Assessment Measures Logs, Journals, Diaries: a form of observation technique that involves making a page of notes about children’s behavior in a cumulative journal. Time sampling: method of recording where children are by choice at a certain time, measuring attention span and interests.
  • 52. Informal Assessment Measures Anecdotal Notes: Factual narrative of an incident Portfolio: a carefully selected collection of a child’s work that is used to document growth and development. Informal Assessment Measures Duration Measures: How long an event or behavior lasts ~ about 4 minutes ~ how many questions Will asks ~ how many letters in his answer? ~ how many times does he use the letter Q? http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GoGZ7rhZNHo The Career Development of Mexican American Adolescent Women: A Test of Social Cognitive Career Theory
  • 53. Lisa Y. Flores The Ohio State University Karen M. O’Brien University of Maryland, College Park This study tested R. W. Lent, S. D. Brown, and G. Hackett’s (1994) model of career choice with 364 Mexican American adolescent women. Path analyses were run to determine the influence of contextual and social cognitive variables on career aspiration, career choice prestige, and traditionality. Partial support for the model was evidenced as nontraditional career self-efficacy, parental support, barriers, acculturation, and feminist attitudes predicted career choice prestige. Acculturation, feminist attitudes, and nontraditional career self-efficacy predicted career choice traditionality. Feminist attitudes and parental support predicted career aspiration. The paths between nontraditional career interests and the 3 outcome variables were not supported. Finally, none of the background contextual variables in this study predicted nontraditional career self-efficacy. Implications of the results and suggestions for future research are discussed. Mexican American women constitute a significant portion of the American population (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1996), are underrepresented at all levels of education (Carter & Wilson, 1993; Lango, 1995; McNeill et al., 2001; U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1991), and are overrepresented in low-paying occupations tradi- tionally occupied by women (Arbona, 1989; Arbona & Novy, 1991; Ortiz, 1995). Relatively little empirical research has been conducted to identify the variables that contribute to the educa -
  • 54. tional and occupational underachievement of Mexican American women. Indeed, researchers have noted that the career develop- ment of Hispanics has received only slight consideration in the counseling and vocational literature (Arbona, 1990; Fouad, 1995; Hoyt, 1989; McNeill et al., 2001), and they have questioned the generalizability of career development theories to Hispanics (Ar- bona, 1990, 1995; Fitzgerald & Betz, 1994; Hackett, Lent, & Greenhaus, 1991). The purpose of this study was to investigate the applicability of a current model of career choice to the experiences of Mexican American adolescent women and to extend the current model to incorporate variables that are hypothesized to be salient to this population. It is well documented that Hispanics are the least educated when compared with other major racial/ethnic groups in the United States and that, among Hispanics, Mexican Americans have the lowest high school and college completion rates (47% and 6.5%, respectively; U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1996). Mexican Ameri - can women are less likely to graduate from college than their male counterparts (Ortiz, 1995; Tinajero, Gonzalez, & Dick, 1991), and their representation in higher education decreases significantly at each successive level (Carter & Wilson, 1993). Moreover, those Mexican American women who pursue higher education confront many stressors and may experience psychological distress as they
  • 55. seek to reconcile their career aspirations with their familial and cultural values (Niemann, 2001). Education is related to occupational status, and thus, the re- stricted employment status among Mexican American women is not surprising given their low educational attainment. Arbona (1989) reported that, occupationally, Hispanic women were con- centrated in low and mid-level technical, service-oriented, and clerical type jobs. According to Ortiz (1995), Mexican American women were less likely to be professionals or private business owners and earned less money when compared with women from other racial/ethnic groups and Mexican American men. Moreover, Mexican American women who were in professional occupations were more likely to choose traditional and low-status occupations (Ortiz, 1995). A review of the literature on Mexican American women re- vealed inconsistencies between their educational and vocational achievements and aspirations. For example, Arbona and Novy (1991) reported that the majority of Mexican American college women in their study aspired to investigative and enterprising type jobs. It is interesting that the percentage of women who expected to enter these fields was smaller than the percentage of women who aspired to these careers, whereas the opposite was true of those who aspired and expected to enter fields that have typically represented traditional career options for women. Other studies Lisa Y. Flores, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State
  • 56. University; Karen M. O’Brien, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park. This study was based on the doctoral dissertation of Lisa Y. Flores, which was conducted under the direction of Michael J. Patton. An earlier version of this article was presented at the 108th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, August 2000. We thank Nancy Betz, Mary Heppner, and Fred Leong for helpful feedback on earlier versions of this article; Kristopher Preacher and Robert MacCallum for statistical consultation; Jamilla Griffin and Jason Quaran- tillo for assistance with coding data; and the students, teachers, counselors, and administrators of the participating schools. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lisa Y. Flores, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 1885 Neil Avenue Mall, Columbus, Ohio 43210-1222. E-mail: [email protected] Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 49, No. 1, 14 –27 0022-0167/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-0167.49.1.14 14
  • 57. revealed that Mexican American girls aspired to careers that re - quired a college degree and to obtaining a postsecondary education (Hernandez, Vargas-Lew, & Martinez, 1994; Valenzuela, 1993). Reyes, Kobus, and Gillock’s (1999) study indicated that 87% of the girls in a sample of predominantly Mexican American 10th- grade students aspired to nontraditional or male-dominated ca- reers. Clearly, a difference exists between Mexican American women’s educational and vocational aspirations and their actual achievements, suggesting that these women may not be realizing their educational and career potential. Prior studies on the career development of Hispanics have focused primarily on their educational and career aspirations (Ar- bona & Novy, 1991; Hernandez et al., 1994; Reyes et al., 1999) and the factors postulated to be related to their educational success (Cardoza, 1991; Fisher & Padmawidjaja, 1999; Gandara, 1982; Gillock & Reyes, 1999; Hess & D’Amato, 1996; Keith & Licht- man, 1994; Lango, 1995; Ramos & Sanchez, 1995; Rodriguez, 1996; Valenzuela, 1993; Vasquez, 1982; Wycoff, 1996). Other studies have examined the barriers that Hispanic students antici - pate in their educational and career endeavors (Luzzo, 1992; McWhirter, 1997). The research to date provides insight into the career development of Hispanic individuals but contains limita - tions that restrict its use. First, several studies are descriptive in nature, and while helpful in understanding patterns of behavior with this group, they do not further knowledge regarding the salient predictors of career be - haviors. Second, several studies included racially/ethnically di -
  • 58. verse samples (in which the number of Hispanics were dispropor- tionately small) or failed to report the ethnic background of Hispanic participants. Because of the educational and occupational differences between racial/ethnic groups and among Hispanics, investigating ethnically diverse subgroups individually seems war- ranted (Arbona, 1995). Another limitation of the existing studies is that many included both women and men. Given differences in Mexican American women’s and men’s educational attainment, occupational status, and socialization within the culture, women and men should be investigated separately to understand the ef- fects of cultural and gender role socialization on career decisions. Finally, few studies have assessed the influence of cultural vari - ables, such as acculturation, on Hispanics’ career-related behaviors (Arbona, 1995). One notable exception to the research described above was a study investigating the educational plans and career expectations of Mexican American high school girls (McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998). McWhirter and her colleagues studied the utility of Farmer’s (1985) model of career commitment and aspirations in explaining the educational planning and career expectations of Mexican American adolescent women. They extended Farmer’s model by including acculturation and perceived barriers in their theoretical models. The results of this study indicated that their models described the educational and career plans of a sample of
  • 59. Mexican American girls; however, only a modest amount of vari- ance was accounted for by the models. Thus, McWhirter et al. encouraged researchers to include additional variables when de - veloping future models of the career development of Mexican American adolescent women. Moreover, McWhirter et al. sug- gested that Lent, Brown, and Hackett’s (1994) social cognitive career theory had promise for advancing knowledge regarding the career development of Mexican American women. Lent and his colleagues (Lent et al., 1994) extended Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory and Hackett and Betz’s (1981) career self-efficacy theory to develop a social cognitive career theory (SCCT) that hypothesized the influence of personal, con- textual, and social cognitive factors on interest formation, career goals, and performance. Of interest in this study are the proposi- tions of SCCT that background contextual variables exert an influence on career self-efficacy, which in turn directly influences career interests. In addition, Lent et al. posited that career interests directly influence career goals and that career self-efficacy both directly and indirectly (through career interests) influences career goals. Finally, proximal contextual variables were hypothesized to exert direct effects on career goals (see Figure 1). Lent and his colleagues suggested that SCCT may be used to guide inquiry on the career development of women and racial/ethnic minorities, and they recently advocated for more research to test the hypotheses related to the contextual variables in their model (Lent, Brown,
  • 60. & Figure 1. Portions of Lent, Brown, and Hackett’s (1994) model of career choice tested in the present study. 15MEXICAN AMERICAN WOMEN Hackett, 2000). Recent studies provided partial support for the model with racially diverse middle school students (Fouad & Smith, 1996) as well as Asian American (Tang, Fouad, & Smith, 1999) and Black college students (Gainor & Lent, 1998); however, no studies to date have investigated the validity of SCCT with Mexican American adolescent women. To test this theory, when operationalizing the constructs ad- vanced by Lent et al. (1994), we selected variables that were hypothesized to be salient for racial/ethnic minorities or women. Specifically, in our model, we operationalized background contex- tual variables to include acculturation level, feminist attitudes, and mothers’ modeling through educational attainment and occupa- tion. Multicultural researchers have identified the importance of examining within-group differences of racial and ethnic sub- groups, and Casas and Pytluk (1995) discussed acculturation as one variable that differentiates Hispanic subgroups or individuals within a subgroup. Moreover, McWhirter et al. (1998) noted that acculturation was the only variable that they added to Farmer’s (1985) model that accounted for significant variance i n the edu- cational aspirations of Mexican American girls. Other
  • 61. researchers also documented that acculturation was positively related to edu- cational aspirations (Ramos & Sanchez, 1995), in addition to interest in nontraditional careers (Reyes et al., 1999), college attendance (Hurtado & Gauvain, 1997), and achievement styles (Gomez & Fassinger, 1994) among Hispanic students. Other variables, specifically feminist and gender role attitudes, have been shown to relate to the career choices of young women (Betz, 1994; O’Brien & Fassinger, 1993), such that women with traditional gender role attitudes exhibited lower levels of career orientation and aspiration than women holding liberal gender role attitudes. Among Mexican American girls, nontraditional gender role attitudes were positively related to higher levels of educational and career expectations (McWhirter et al., 1998) and academic achievement (Valenzuela, 1993; Vasquez-Nuttal, Romero- Garcia, & De Leon, 1987). For Mexican American women, cultural ex- pectations about gender roles may result in traditional gender role attitudes or nonfeminist attitudes (Ginorio, Gutierrez, Cauce, & Acosta, 1995; Reid, Haritos, Kelly, & Holland, 1995), which in turn may contribute to lower levels of career achievement. In addition, parental factors, such as occupation and educational level, were found to relate to academic achievement and parental involvement in Mexican American students’ educational and ca- reer planning (Keith & Lichtman, 1994). With regard to the influence of mothers, having a mother who attended college was predictive of college attendance and persistence among Latinas (Cardoza, 1991). However, other studies that assessed the role of
  • 62. parents’ educational or occupational attainment in children’s edu- cational and career aspirations reported no relation (Fisher & Padmawidjaja, 1999; Hernandez et al., 1994; Hess & D’Amato, 1996; Lango, 1995; Reyes et al., 1999), possibly because of the highly skewed number of parents with lower educational and occupational levels in these samples. The influence of mothers’ educational level and occupational traditionality were included in the present study to determine their influence on daughters’ career development. According to SCCT, these background variables were hypoth- esized to influence nontraditional career self-efficacy or confi- dence in pursuing nontraditional career-related tasks for women (Lent et al., 1994). In turn, nontraditional career self-efficacy should exert a direct effect on both nontraditional career interests and career goals (i.e., career choice prestige, career choice tradi- tionality, and career aspirations). Indeed, these relations have been supported in prior studies, which reported that career self- efficacy was related to career interests and careers considered among Hispanic students (Bores-Rangel, Church, Szendre, & Reeves, 1990; Church, Teresa, Rosebrook, & Szendre, 1992; Lauver & Jones, 1991). In addition, research has shown that career interests were related to careers considered among Hispanic students (Bores-Rangel et al., 1990; Church et al., 1992). These findings were consistent with SCCT, which posited a direct link between career interests and career goals.
  • 63. We also hypothesized, in accordance with SCCT (Lent et al., 1994), that the proximal contextual variables of perceived support from parents and perceptions of barriers will influence career choice prestige, traditionality, and career aspirations. Among Lati- nas, encouragement and emotional support from families have been found to be predictive of educational achievement (Hernan- dez et al., 1994; Keith & Lichtman, 1994; Ramos & Sanchez, 1995) and college attendance (Vasquez, 1982; Wycoff, 1996). With regard to perceived barriers, Hispanic students reported ex- periencing more barriers to education than students from other racial/ethnic groups (Luzzo, 1992; McWhirter, 1997), and Mexi - can American women who experienced negative family attitudes related to their college attendance were more likely to attend college close to home (Wycoff, 1996). McWhirter et al. (1998) found no relation among perceived barriers and Mexican Ameri - can girls’ educational or career plans. However, they suggested that the influence of perceived barriers on academic and vocational goals be further tested with additional samples. It is possible that Mexican American adolescent women’s increased levels of per - ceived barriers to their educational or career goals may alter their decision making, such that they plan to pursue careers that present the least resistance. In summary, this study was designed to test several tenets of SCCT (Lent et al., 1994) with a sample of Mexican American adolescent women. Specifically, we explored the influence of background contextual variables, namely, acculturation level, fem-
  • 64. inist attitudes, mother’s educational level, and mother’s occupa- tional traditionality on nontraditional career self-efficacy. Addi- tionally, we investigated the contributions of nontraditional career self-efficacy, nontraditional career interests, parental support, and perceived barriers to career choice prestige, career choice tradi - tionality, and career aspirations. These dependent variables were selected because of their importance to women’s career develop- ment (Fitzgerald, Fassinger, & Betz, 1995; O’Brien & Fassinger, 1993). A secondary purpose of this study was to obtain descriptive information regarding participants’ demographic characteristics, career choices, plans following high school graduation, choice of colleges/universities, and reasons for choosing these schools, given the lack of data regarding this population and their career plans. Method Participants Participants were Mexican American adolescent women enrolled in their senior year of high school. At the same time, Mexican American adoles- 16 FLORES AND O’BRIEN cent men were surveyed for a later study. Participants were
  • 65. drawn from two large public high schools in a mid-sized town (a population of approxi- mately 30,000) in south Texas. The community is close to the United States–Mexican border and is heavily influenced by the Mexican culture. A high percentage of U.S. citizens who are of Mexican descent live in this area, and this is reflected in the student population at the high schools, in which almost 95% of the students are Mexican American. A total of 931 surveys were distributed to students; 831 were returned (450 female, 381 male), resulting in an 89% overall return rate. Women who were in their senior year of high school and who identified as Mexican American were included in this study (n � 377). Of these women, 13 were dropped from the study because of incomplete data, resulting in a total sample of 364. Participants ranged in age from 16 to 21 years with a mean age of 17.47 (SD � 0.70). The average number of people living at home was 4.83 (SD � 1.71; range � 2 to 13). Eighteen percent of the students (n � 65) reported that they were first-generation Mexican American, with 37.9% (n � 138) second gener- ation, 11.3% (n � 41) third generation, 19.2% (n � 70) fourth generation, and 10.7% (n � 39) fifth generation. With regard to
  • 66. acculturation level, 17% (n � 61) were categorized as “very Mexican oriented,” 38% (n � 138) “Mexican oriented to approximately balanced bicultural,” 34% (n � 123) “slightly Anglo oriented bicultural,” 10% (n � 37) “strongly Anglo oriented,” and 1% (n � 5) “very assimilated, Anglicized.” The educational level of the female and male head of household, respectively, was as follows: completed elementary school, 24% and 21%; attended high school, 25% and 23%; high school graduate, 19% and 21%; attended college/university, 14% and 12%; college/university graduate, 10% and 12%; and graduate or professional degree, 2% and 1%. Eighty-seven percent (n � 317) of the students planned to attend a 2- or 4-year college/university following their high school graduation, with the remaining students indicating plans to attend technical school (5.5%), work (3.2%), enlist in the military (2.1%), and marry or stay at home (0.5%). Among students with intentions to continue their education at a 2- or 4-year college/university, almost half (43.2%, n � 137) reported that they would work either full time (1.9%, n � 6) or part time (41.3%, n � 131). Over a third (39.1%, n � 124) planned to attend the local 4-year university, and 19.2% (n � 61) planned to attend the local 2-year community college.
  • 67. The most often cited reasons for choosing to attend the college or univer- sity of their choice were because it was close to home and family (36.5%, n � 116), had a good program of study (10.7%, n � 34), was a good college/university (6.9%, n � 22), and was affordable or inexpensive to attend (4.1%, n � 13). Sixty-eight percent (n � 214) indicated that they would rely on financial aid (e.g., loans, grants, and work study) to finance their education, whereas 31.5% (n � 100) hoped to earn scholar- ships, 26.5% (n � 84) planned to receive financial support from their parents or other family members, and 25% (n � 78) planned to work. Procedure Data collection occurred during the fall semester of the school year. Student participation was solicited through English IV classes because every senior was required to enroll in this class. Data collection occurred across 4 days, and Lisa Y. Flores met with every English IV section (n � 46) at both schools. English teachers escorted their students to a central room at the beginning of the class period and stayed to monitor students’ behaviors. Packets containing an informed-assent form, an entry form for
  • 68. cash prizes, and the research instruments were distributed to students as they entered the room. The questionnaires were counterbalanced to avoid order effects from fatigue. Participants were told that the investigator was inter- ested in studying the career development of Mexican American adoles- cents. Students were told that it would take them most, if not all, of the class period to complete the questionnaires and were encouraged to work quickly. The investigator told the students that two of the surveys looked very similar (each listed the same occupations and educational programs), but these surveys asked students to rate either interests or skills. Students were informed of a possible follow-up study and were invited to participate in future studies. As an incentive to participate in the study, students who completed and returned the surveys were eligible for a random drawing for cash prizes (10 prizes for $20 and 1 prize for $50). Instruments Acculturation level. The Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans (ARSMA–II; Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995) was a 30-item scale that assessed association with and identity with the Mexican
  • 69. and Anglo cultures on two independent subscales. Participants responded to the items using a 5-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to extremely often or almost always (5). An acculturation score was calculated by subtracting the mean score for items on the Anglo Orientation Subscale (AOS) from the mean score for items on the Mexican Orientation Subscale (MOS). On the basis of their acculturation score, participants were cate- gorized into one of the five acculturation levels described by Cuellar et al. (1995). Levels range from very Mexican oriented (1) to very assimilated (5). Middle categories represented bicultural individuals. Thus, high scores were indicative of a strong orientation toward the Anglo culture. The ARSMA–II, as well as prior to its revision, the ARSMA, is one of the most widely used measures to assess acculturation among Mexican Americans, and evidence suggests that it is a reliable and valid instrument. Adequate internal consistency coefficients have been reported for the two subscales with multiple samples (range from .79 to .83 for the AOS and .87 to .91 for the MOS; Cuellar et al., 1995; Cuellar & Roberts, 1997; Lessenger, 1997). Reliability coefficients of .77 for the AOS and .91 for the MOS were obtained in the present study.
  • 70. Cuellar and his colleagues also reported a test–retest reliability estimate for the AOS and MOS over a 2-week interval of .94 and .96, respectively. Concurrent validity was assessed by comparing scores on the ARSMA–II with scores on the ARSMA and yielded a correlation coefficient of .89. Concurrent validity for the ARSMA–II was further supported when its two subscales correlated in the expected direction with the dominant group and ethnic group subscales of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (Stephenson, 2000). Lessenger (1997) provided additional support for concurrent validity when she reported that acculturation scores on the ARSMA–II correlated positively with other acculturation measures. Con- struct validity was supported when acculturation scores on the ARSMA–II were compared across generations, and differences were found between generation levels in the expected directions (Cuellar et al., 1995; Lessen- ger, 1997). Feminist attitudes. The Attitudes Toward Feminism and the Women’s Movement Scale (FWM; Fassinger, 1994) was used to measure feminist attitudes. The FWM is a 10-item scale that assessed attitudes about the feminist movement. Participants rated their agreement with the
  • 71. items along a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Scale scores were obtained by averaging the items; high scores reflect profeminist attitudes. Fassinger (1994) reported that the FWM had high internal consistency (� � .89), and O’Brien and Fassinger (1993) reported an internal reliability coefficient of .82 for the FWM with a sample of adolescent women. Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .68. Enns and Hackett (1990) reported a 2-week test–retest reliability coefficient of .81 with female college students. Convergent validity for the FWM was supported when the FWM was positively correlated with measures assessing attitudes toward women, gender roles, and feminism (Enns & Hackett, 1990; Fassinger, 1994). In addition, the FWM correlated positively with items assessing feminist identification and favorability toward the women’s movement (Fassinger, 1994). Finally, Enns and Hackett (1990) reported that the FWM correlated in the expected directions with both interest and involvement in feminist activities. Divergent validity estimates revealed that the FWM was 17MEXICAN AMERICAN WOMEN
  • 72. not measuring gender role characteristics, dogmatism, and social desirabil- ity (Fassinger, 1994). Mother’s level of education. A single item asked participants to indi- cate the highest level of education completed by their mother. Options ranged from elementary school to graduate/professional school. High scores represented high levels of education. Mother’s occupational traditionality. An item asked participants to indicate their mother’s occupation, which was later categorized according to traditionality. Traditionality of mother’s career was computed on the basis of the percentage of women employed in a given career and was obtained through the Statistical Abstract of the United States (1998), a publication of the U.S. Bureau of the Census. The U.S. Census Bureau relies on information from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics and Em- ployment and Earnings to report these data. Scores ranged from 6 to 99, with high scores representing careers with high concentrations of women. This indicator of career orientation has been used in previous studies of women’s career development (O’Brien, 1996; O’Brien & Fassinger, 1993;
  • 73. O’Brien, Friedman, Tipton, & Linn, 2000). Nontraditional career self-efficacy. Self-efficacy expectations with re- gard to nontraditional occupations were assessed using a short form of the occupational self-efficacy questionnaire used by Church et al. (1992). The self-efficacy questionnaire used in this study was comparable with career self-efficacy measures used by Betz and Hackett (1981) and Lauver and Jones (1991). The original occupational questionnaire contained a total of 31 occupations for which participants rated their confidence in their ability to successfully learn to perform the job. The nontraditional career self-efficacy scale used for this study was modified to include seven male-dominated occupations (e.g., electronic equipment repairer, police officer, mechanical engineer). Occupations were categorized according to the percentage of women in the occupation according to U.S. census data (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1998). A brief description of the occupation was provided for each job title. Participants were asked to rate their confidence in their ability and skills to successfully learn to do the jobs. Participants responded to the items using a scale ranging from very unsure (1) to very sure (4). Although
  • 74. studies typically use 5-point scales to measure strength of self- efficacy, we followed the reasoning of Bores-Rangel et al. (1990), whose sample predominantly consisted of Hispanic students, that students may depend- ably and meaningfully discriminate these four bipolar levels. Occupational self-efficacy scores for male-dominated occupations were obtained by averaging the responses to the items. High scores reflected strong levels of nontraditional career self-efficacy. Church et al. (1992) reported an …