SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 12
Baixar para ler offline
Vol. 9, No. 1.
                                      ISSN: 1473-8376
                                 www.heacademy.ac.uk/johlste

                                     PRACTICE PAPER

      Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a
             higher education sports class
  Kevin Morgan (kmorgan@uwic.ac.uk) and Kieran Kingston (kkingston@uwic.ac.uk)
     School of Sport, University of Wales Institute Cardiff, Cyncoed Road, Cardiff, CF23 6XD, UK

                                   DOI:10.3794/johlste.91.236
                 ©Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education

Abstract
The lecturer can aim to develop a motivational climate that could strongly influence the
degree to which students perceive mastery of the tasks or outperforming others as important.
This study aimed to evaluate the effects of an intervention programme to promote a mastery
motivational climate on lecturing behaviours and student learning experiences in an
undergraduate practical soccer module. As a consequence of the intervention, observational
analysis of lecturer behaviours showed increases in student-set mastery goals, greater
differentiation of tasks, increased lecturer feedback on effort and progress to individual
students, and more flexible time to learn. Group interviews with students revealed that the
mastery programme had a positive impact on their motivation and learning experiences.
Keywords: task; authority; recognition; grouping; evaluation; target; TARGET

Introduction
According to achievement goal theory (AGT) (Nicholls, 1984, 1989), in achievement
situations the goal of participants is to demonstrate competence or avoid demonstrating
incompetence. Competence, however, can be construed in a number of ways, for example,
outperforming others (ego-involved goal), or improving one's own learning and mastering the
demands of the task (mastery-involved goal) (Ames, 1992a). According to Roberts (2001)
these conceptions of competence are determined by both dispositional and situational
factors.

While much of the research into AGT has focused on individual differences in dispositional
goal orientations (tendencies to be task or ego involved in achievement settings) and
associated patterns of cognition and affect (Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Nicholls,
1984, 1989), some has considered how the structure and demands of the learning
environment (referred to as the motivational climate) can evoke different achievement goals
and motivational patterns (Ames, 1984, 1992a, 1992b; Ames & Archer, 1988). The premise
of this research adopting a situational perspective is that individuals’ perceptions of the
motivational climate determine their goals and subsequent motivational responses (Treasure,
2001). Motivational climate is, therefore, defined as a situationally induced psychological
environment directing goals of action (Ames, 1992a).



Dr Kevin Morgan (Lead Author) is a Senior Lecturer in the UWIC School of Sport and specialises in
the area of Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy.

Dr Kieran Kingston is a Senior Lecturer in the UWIC School of Sport and specialises in the area of
Sport Psychology.
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
In school physical education (PE) and youth sport settings, adaptive learning and
motivational patterns; for example, a positive attitude towards the activity, feelings of
satisfaction, high perceptions of ability, the choice of challenging tasks, high intrinsic
motivation, and placing a high value on effort and the process of learning; have been
consistently associated with perceptions of a mastery climate (e.g., Carpenter & Morgan,
1999; Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Treasure, 1997). In contrast, a perceived ego climate has
been linked to less adaptive cognitive and affective responses, such as boredom, beliefs that
ability rather than effort leads to success, a lack of enjoyment, and a negative attitude toward
the subject matter (e.g., Carpenter & Morgan, 1999; Ommundsen & Roberts, 1999;
Treasure, 1997).

Epstein (1989) identified the task, authority, recognition, grouping, evaluation, and time
structures (TARGET) as influential in determining motivation in school and home
environments. Later, Ames (1992a) adopted the TARGET acronym to encapsulate the
structures that foster a mastery motivational climate in achievement situations.

According to Ames, to foster a mastery teaching environment the task structure should
involve: (a) students in setting their own personal goals focused on self-referenced
improvement, (b) multiple activities in order to reduce the opportunity for normative
comparisons of ability, and (c) tasks which are differentiated to optimally challenge all
students. The authority structure should encourage students to be involved in decision
making and leadership roles. Recognition and evaluation from the teacher should be given
privately and be individually based on effort and progress; further students should be
involved in self-evaluation against personal goals. Students should be grouped into small
mixed ability and co-operative groups and be given the opportunity to change groups both
within and between sessions. Finally, the time structure should allow flexible time for
improvement and maximise time to practice and learn.

In contrast, an ego climate would emphasise uni-dimensional competitive tasks, teacher
authority, normatively based public recognition and evaluation, homogenous ability groups,
and time to practice would be inflexible (see Table 1). The TARGET guidelines were written
for school classroom lessons and have been applied to the youth sport and PE settings
(Ames, 1992c). Some of these structures may not always be applicable to settings beyond
these settings (e.g., Higher Education) because, for example, of the constraints of the
learning environment with regards to the grouping and learning outcomes.

      TARGET behaviour        Mastery climate                        Ego climate
      Task                    Self-referenced goals,                 Comparative goals,
                              multi-dimensional, varied &            uni-dimensional &
                              differentiated                         undifferentiated
      Authority               Participants given leadership roles    Teacher makes all the
                              & involved in decision making          decisions
      Recognition             Private recognition of improvement,    Public recognition of
                              effort and accomplishments             normative ability and
                                                                     comparative performances
      Grouping                Small mixed ability & co-operative     Ability groups
                              groups                                 Whole class activities

      Evaluation              Self-referenced. Private               Normative & public
                              consultations with teacher based on
                              improvement & effort
      Time                    Flexible time for task completion      Inflexible time for task
                              and maximum time to learn              completion

         Table 1: TARGET behaviours that influence motivational climate (Epstein, 1989; Ames,
         1992b)



Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        74
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
Intervention studies manipulating the TARGET structures to create a mastery focused
teaching environment (Digelidis, Papaioannou, Laparidis, & Christodoulidis., 2004; Morgan &
Carpenter, 2002; Solmon, 1996; Treasure, 1993) have described the enhancement of
students’ motivation and learning in PE settings. Specifically, students’ tended to be more
task oriented and less ego oriented; had higher levels of perceived competence, satisfaction
and enjoyment; were less bored; preferred to engage in more challenging tasks; and
believed success was the result of effort. In contrast, when the TARGET structures were
more ego-involving, students’ tended to be more ego oriented; had lower levels of perceived
ability, satisfaction and enjoyment; were less interested in achievement tasks; and believed
success was the result of ability and deception.

Taking this research further, Morgan, Sproule, Weigand and Carpenter (2005) used the
Behavioural Evaluation Strategies and Taxonomies (BEST) software (Sharpe & Koperwas,
1999) to create a computer-based observational measure of the TARGET structures in PE
for students aged 11-14. This measure allows researchers to film practical sessions and
code the teacher’s behaviours as mastery, ego or neutral, based on the TARGET concept.
Furthermore, the computer software allows practitioners to self-evaluate teaching behaviours
against Ames’ (1992a) guidelines for fostering a mastery climate.

Based on self-observation and analysis of filmed lessons, using the behavioural TARGET
measure (Morgan et al., 2005), Morgan and Kingston (2008) developed a mastery
intervention programme for PE teachers of 13-14 year old students. There were five stages
to the intervention programme with each of the PE teachers. In Stage 1 the teachers were
filmed adopting their typical teaching behaviours and surveys were administered to measure
students’ perceptions of the motivational climate and their cognitive and affective responses.
Stage 2 involved an individual introduction to the TARGET behaviours (Ames, 1992a), which
included an explanation of the different TARGET structures followed by practical examples
from PE lessons and practice at coding the behaviours as mastery, performance or neither.
In Stage 3 the teachers coded their own filmed lessons and the lead researcher conducted a
separate analysis using Morgan et al.’s (2005) measure. In Stage 4 the lead researcher
discussed the teaching behaviours displayed in the filmed lessons with each individual
teacher and the teachers then re-planned their original filmed lesson to be more mastery-
involving. Finally, in Stage 5, they were re-filmed whilst following their new mastery focused
lesson plan and pupils completed a post-intervention survey.

Results revealed that the mastery intervention programme was successful in fostering more
mastery involving teaching behaviours and higher perceptions of mastery involving TARGET
behaviours. Statistical analysis revealed that the more disaffected pupils significantly
improved their motivational responses whereas the more highly motivated pupils did not.
However, a limitation was that qualitative methods were not used in order to explore the
students learning experiences in more depth.

The purpose of the present study was to further extend this line of research into higher
education (HE) and to evaluate the effects of a mastery intervention programme on the
lecturing behaviours that influence the motivational climate in a practical sports class.
Additionally, qualitative analysis was used to gain a greater depth of understanding about the
students’ learning experiences in a mastery condition than previous research of this nature.

Method
Participants
Two HE lecturers were randomly assigned to an intervention group (1 male, aged 35; group
n = 16: 15 male, 1 female) and a control group (1 female, aged 25: group n = 18: 12 male, 6
female). Participants provided voluntary informed consent to take part in the study and were
participating in a 12 week, Level 2 undergraduate practical soccer module. Ethical approval
for all procedures was gained from the researchers’ university ethics committee.



Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        75
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class

Observational measure
Analysis of the filmed sessions was conducted using Morgan et al.’s (2005) observational
measure of TARGET (see Table 2). The measure permits immediate analysis of data
gathered through observations of teaching from video and audio recording of lessons.
Validity was established by four researchers, experienced in teacher education and
motivational climate research, who met and agreed upon the teaching behaviours that
matched with the different TARGET structures. Acceptable intra- and inter-reliability to ≥ 0.80
(Bakeman & Gottman, 1986) was established during the development of the measure
(Morgan et al., 2005).

                  Mastery                         Neutral                     Ego
  Task            0 = Teacher set self-           3 = No clear goals          2 = Competitive goal
                  referenced goal or
                  cooperative group-              4 = Warm up/cool down
                  referenced goal

                  1 = Students set own self-
                  referenced goal or
                  cooperative group-
                  referenced goal

                  5 = Multi-dimensional/                                      6 = Uni-dimensional/
                  different tasks                                             same task

                  7 = Differentiated/suitably                                 8 = Undifferentiated/
                  challenging for all                                         not suitably challenging
                                                                              for all
  Authority       9 = Pupils involved in                                      10 = Teacher makes all
  (Duration-      leadership roles and / or                                   the decisions
  toggle)         decision making
  Recognition     P = Recognition/evaluation      E = General                 W = Recognition/
  & Evaluation    focused on individual effort,   assessment/feedback         evaluation focused on
                  improvement/progress and        (to no one in particular)   individual effort,
                  accomplishment in private                                   improvement/progress
                                                  R = Focus on luck           and accomplishment in
                  Q = Evaluation that allows                                  Public
                  equal opportunity for
                  recognition and rewards                                     T = Recognition/
                                                                              evaluation focused on
                  Y = Self- Evaluate against                                  normative comparisons
                  a set goal
  Grouping        S= Small heterogeneous/                                     A = Homogeneous/
  (Duration-      mixed ability groups                                        ability groups
  toggle)
                  G = Change of groups                                        D = Large group/whole
                                                                              class
  Timing          Z = Flexible time to            C = Inactive time           X = Inflexible time to
  (Duration-      practice, plan or evaluate                                  practice, plan or
  toggle)                                                                     evaluate

         Table 2: TARGET coding for the analysis of teaching behaviours that influence
         motivational climate (Morgan et al., 2005)

Procedures
There were five stages to the intervention programme, as identified in Figure 1. In Stage 1
both HE lecturers were filmed teaching in their typical way. Both lecturers were familiar with
being filmed during practical sessions and were therefore not distracted by the camera.


Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                              76
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
Stage 2 of the intervention involved the lead researcher educating the intervention group
lecturer with regards to the TARGET teaching behaviours (Ames, 1992a) associated with
mastery and ego motivational climates (Table 1). This involved a verbal explanation of the
different TARGET structures and practical examples of the TARGET behaviours from video
footage of previously filmed sports teaching sessions. Further 10 minute video clips of
different practical sports teaching episodes were then viewed and the intervention group
lecturer was trained to recognise and code the teaching structures as mastery, performance
or neutral, using the computer based behavioural measure of TARGET (Morgan et al., 2005)
(see Table 2 for the coding categories).

                                 •   voluntary informed consent
     Stage 1                     •   teachers filmed while adopting usual
                                     teaching style



                                 •   education of intervention group teacher with
     Stage 2                         regards to recognition and coding of
                                     TARGET behaviours


                                 •   lead researcher and intervention group
                                     teacher jointly coding session from Stage 1
     Stage 3                         to assess TARGET behaviours
                                 •   education of intervention group teacher on
                                     modifying teaching behaviours to support a
                                     mastery climate


                                 •   both intervention and control teachers (and
                                     groups) filmed
     Stage 4                     •   lead researcher and intervention group
                                     teacher analysed lessons with respect to
                                     TARGET structures
                                 •   further education on modifying teaching
                                     behaviours to intervention group teacher



                                •    teachers filmed while adopting usual
                                     teaching style
     Stage 5                    •    lead researcher and intervention group
                                     teacher analysed lessons with respect to
                                     TARGET structures


         Figure 1: Diagrammatical representation of the intervention

Stage 3 involved the lead researcher and the intervention group lecturer jointly coding the
teaching session described in Stage 1. Based on this analysis certain teaching behaviours
were identified to enhance the mastery focus of the soccer sessions and suggestions were
made by the lecturer for modifying the teaching behaviours during the next filmed session.
The implementation of Stages 1 to 3 of the intervention with the control group lecturer took
place “weeks after the completion of the module, consistent with the university’s ethical
procedures, and the control group students benefited accordingly in their soccer lectures in
the following academic year.

In Stage 4 both lecturers were filmed for the second time. Following this, the intervention
group lecturer and the lead researcher jointly analysed the TARGET behaviours to identify
any changes in the mastery involving focus of the session, and to set further mastery

Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        77
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
behavioural objectives for the final filmed session. During this second video analysis phase
the intervention group lecturer took the lead in coding the behaviours and the lead
researcher assisted. The purpose of this was to develop the HE lecturer’s ability to code
independently in the next stage in order to use the software as a means of reflection on
practice.

Stage 5 involved both lecturers being filmed for the third and final time. The intervention
group lecturer and the lead researcher then analysed the teaching behaviours individually to
identify any changes in the mastery involving focus of the third filmed session and met to
discuss their findings.

Group interviews
One week after the completion of the 12 week teaching programme, the lead researcher
conducted four group interviews (two with the intervention group students and two with the
control group students), with four students in each group. In this study, the researcher was
not the lecturer and students were made aware that their responses were totally confidential
and that the lecturer would not get to hear them. The researcher facilitated the discussion
and ensured equal input amongst participants. The interviews were recorded on a digital
voice recorder.

Following an introduction to the purpose of the interview, the participants were asked if they
had noticed any significant changes in the teaching behaviours since the first filmed session.
The researcher then questioned the participants about each of the TARGET structures and
asked them to share their thoughts and feelings about any changes they had perceived in
the lecturers’ behaviours and what impact this had on their learning experience.

Results
Observational and group interview data
This section reports the observational findings combined with the group interview responses
in relation to each of the TARGET structures. Using Morgan et al.’s (2005) TARGET
measure, two researchers simultaneously undertook video analysis of the TARGET (Ames,
1992a) behaviours of both lecturers in their three filmed sessions. One hundred percent
agreement was achieved in assessing the behaviours. This was possible because of the
flexibility of the BEST software (Sharpe & Koperwas, 1999), which permitted the two
researchers to pause the system for discussion until complete and unambiguous agreement
was reached. Discussions were short and decisions were reached quickly and easily due to
the fact that the researchers were experienced at this type of analysis.

For each of the three filmed sessions in both groups, the mean percentage frequency of
mastery, ego and neutral behaviours (as a proportion of all coded behaviours in that
particular TARGET structure) was calculated for the task, recognition and evaluation. The
mean percentage duration of mastery, ego and neutral behaviours (as a proportion of the
total session time) was calculated for authority, grouping and time. Group interviews were
deductively analysed based on Ames’s (1992a) description of the TARGET structures.

The interview responses clearly indicated that there were perceived changes in TARGET
related teacher behaviours by participants in the intervention group, whereas the TARGET
behaviours of the control group lecturer were perceived to be consistent throughout. There
were a number of mastery structures observed and reported by the students in the control
group including the setting of clear learning aims, the questioning and giving of authority to
students, positive and corrective feedback on effort and improvement, and mixed ability
cooperative groups. However, both the behavioural analysis of the TARGET structures (see
Tables 3a, 3b and 3c) and the student interview responses illustrated a higher level of
change in the intervention group lecturer’s behaviours compared to the control group. These
changes in observed behaviours, supported by specific detail from the group interview data,
are reported under the TARGET headings in the following sections.


Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        78
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class

                                    Task goals (M% frequency)                      Task design                Task
                                                                                 (M% frequency)          differentiation
                                                                                                        (M% frequency)
 Group        Session          St       Lec        Ego        No      Warm       Multi-     Uni-         Diff         Undiff
                              Mas       Mas                   set      up         dim       dim
 I                1            0        89         11             0     0          0         100          0            100
                  2           20        80          0             0     0          10        90           60           40
                  3            0        86          0         14        0          33        67           33           67
 C                1            0        100         0             0     0          0         100          31           69
                  2            0        86          0             0     14         0         100          0            100
                  3            0        100         0             0     0          0         100          0            100

             Table 3a: Observed TARGET Behaviours


                                 Authority                               Recognition & Evaluation
                               (M% duration)                                 (M% frequency)
     Group       Session       Mast          Ego         Private:     Public:     Self-     Ego        General        Luck
                                                         Effort &     Effort &    eval
                                                           Imp          Imp
     I                1            30         70              2         46          0        8           43             1
                      2            62         38              8         58          2        0           26             6
                      3            82         18              2         60          3        0           32             3
     C                1            25         75              4         58          0        0           38             0
                      2            52         48              2         60          0        0           38             0
                      3            62         38              4         53          0        0           43             0

             Table 3b: Observed TARGET Behaviours


                                        Grouping (M% duration)                          Time (M% duration)
         Group        Session       Mix ability         Ability       Whole      Flexible   Inflexible        Inactive
                                     groups             groups        class
         I                1             31                0            69           0             40             60
                          2             37                0            63           0             64             36
                          3             63                0            37          58             29             13
         C                1             35                0            65           0             44             56
                          2             26                0            74           0             57             43
                          3             24                0            76           0             70             30

             Table 3c: Observed TARGET Behaviours
             Group I = intervention, C = control

Task
There was a 0-20% increase in observed student set mastery goals from filmed session one
to two in the intervention group, but no evidence of students setting their own goals in the
control group. Focus group data supported this finding and highlighted the positive impact of
individual goal setting on student motivation. For example:

             He developed goal setting a few weeks into the sessions and then you were
             continually reminded throughout the sessions to check your goals. You were
             constantly thinking have you achieved your goals and evaluating yourself.


Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                                                  79
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
There was a strong feeling amongst the groups that this improved intrinsic motivation
because:

         It helps you get something out of the session that you want to achieve instead of it
         being something that the lecturer wants you to have. If you can relate it to yourself it
         keeps you interested.

There was some caution expressed by students regarding requests to set their own goals.
They felt that its value depended on the experience of the participants, as suggested by one
student:

         If you’ve been playing for a while you know the sort of goals to set, but if you are a
         beginner you need to be told that these are the things you need to know for a
         particular skill. You have to be realistic as well, as otherwise your motivation will
         decrease if you haven’t achieved them.

Lecturer-set mastery goals, as opposed to competitive goals, were clearly evident in both
groups from the first observed session and this did not change as a result of the intervention.
However, there was also a feeling amongst some group members that competitive goals
were included and an important aspect within sport, especially in team games as articulated
by the following:

         He does bring some competition into it because it’s a competitive sport isn’t it. If you
         are defending against two attackers then you have to be competitive to try and stop
         them getting to the goal. He does include competitive goals but not too much that it
         doesn’t fit with the overall goals. I think because we are sports students that
         competitiveness is innate.

The setting of multi-dimensional tasks by the lecturer of the intervention group increased
from 0 to 33% of all tasks, from filmed session one to three; these changes were not
observed for the control group. Furthermore, the frequency of task differentiation was more
evident in sessions two and three compared to session one in the intervention group,
whereas in the control group no such changes were evident. Consistent with the
observational findings, interview data revealed perceptions of greater differentiation within
the intervention group sessions as the weeks progressed:

         Last week he came around and told us to do something more difficult because we
         had the technical ability, while others continued doing something else. So we went
         straight into something at say the third level of difficulty, whereas others were still on
         the first level.

This increased differentiation combined with personal goal setting of the intervention group
appears to have had a positive effect on engagement and confidence, as evidenced by the
following:

         Whereas in the beginning possibly it was only the better players who were having
         more of an influence on the game, today a lot more people were getting involved
         and were a lot more confident with the ball, which I think comes from focusing on
         developing your own skills during the sessions.

Authority
The decision making opportunities and leadership roles increased in both groups as the
module progressed, though the change was more marked in the intervention group. This was
primarily due to the learning outcomes and session content which was planned for the latter
part of the module involving the students in coaching tasks. For the intervention group, this
change in the authority structure was also reflected in the group interviews:




Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        80
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
         He asked us how we would progress a basic four on two onto another level to
         challenge the defenders. So we had to think as a group how we could make this
         benefit the defenders. In the past weeks he has done that himself but today he
         asked us to do it.

The positive effects of student authority was acknowledged and valued in the intervention
group interviews:

         There was no power divide the power was amongst the group if you like. He gave a
         lot of autonomy to the group and that’s important to enhance your motivation, and
         you don’t realise it until you reflect on it.

However, although students generally reflected in a positive manner to being given authority
within sessions, a minority would have preferred it if the lecturer had adopted a more
autocratic approach: “I’d personally rather it if he just did it himself…I think it’s just time”.

Recognition and evaluation
Incidences of private individual feedback were found to increase four-fold from observed
session one to two in the intervention group, but this reverted to baseline levels at session
three. This may be explained by the added focus on this aspect by the researcher during
Stage 3 of the intervention, but less of a focus in Stage 4, resulting in a return to more typical
teaching behaviours in filmed session three. This may be a limitation of the intervention that
needs to be considered in future research of this nature.

Interview data supported the change to more private individual feedback by the intervention
group lecturer in session two. For example, one student commented that:

         He comes around well and talks to you individually. He doesn’t stop it and say now
         we’ll do this, he just has a quiet word while other groups are still working, which I
         think is good.

The wider benefits of such private individual feedback were evident from quotes such as:

         I think it’s a bit more personal if he comes up to you privately. He can say well done
         in front of everyone without really thinking but if he’s actually come on to you to say
         it privately, perhaps it’s a bit more important.

However, the logistical difficulties of providing private individual feedback to a large group
with only one member of staff were also acknowledged: “I get personal individual feedback
sometimes but to give individual feedback to everyone in the group is not practical in this
situation”.

There was a small increase in the frequency of mastery feedback given publicly to individuals
(in situations where others could hear) in the intervention group. Although public feedback is
considered to be more ego-involving (Ames, 1992a), some students felt that “it gives you
confidence if you get a well done in front of other people” and that “it’s more important how
he criticises you, publicly or privately….if I get criticised publicly it can give me a kick up the
ass like, but other people may take it to heart”. Furthermore it was perceived by some that
public feedback is important for the learning of the whole group: “if he does give individual
feedback he makes sure others are around so that they can benefit from the feedback as
well”.

Although there were few instances, self-evaluation increased in the intervention group but
was not evident in the control group. Normatively, comparative feedback was observed in
session one for the intervention group but was eliminated in sessions two and three. It was
not observed in any of the three sessions for the control group. General feedback to the
whole group (neutral) decreased from session one in the intervention group, yet increased
albeit marginally in the control group.

Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        81
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class

Grouping
The duration of the session spent in mixed ability cooperative groups, as opposed to whole
class, increased markedly from session one to three (31% to 69% of the whole time in mixed
ability groups) for the intervention group, whereas in the control group it decreased to a small
degree (35% to 24%). The positive impact of heterogeneous grouping on the lower ability
students’ learning was acknowledged in the interviews: “last week we were all in mixed ability
groups, so people of a lower ability were working with people of a higher level and they seem
to be getting better as a result”. However, there was a feeling from some students that if they
were “put into ability groups then the standard would go up, because there is no doubt that in
mixed ability groups some people do bring the standard down”. Consistent with Ames’
(1992a) recommendations for a mastery climate, there was also variety of groupings within
sessions, illustrated by the following quote: “you tend to change groups throughout the
session so you end up in a different group to the one you started in”.

There was an interesting conflict identified between task differentiation, which is a mastery
structure (Ames, 1992a), and mixed ability grouping which, according to some students,
made it more difficult to differentiate tasks between groups because not all students were of
the same ability level. Therefore, the mixed ability groups tended to work at similar levels,
whereas some students felt that if the groups had been based on ability it would have been
easier for the lecturer to add further challenges to some groups to develop them at their own
level of ability. This was illustrated by one student who felt that mixed ability groups “can
bring the better players down and put added pressure on the weaker players, which they can
take as a challenge or adopt an attitude that they don’t want to play”. This student went on to
suggest that groups of the students’ own choice were the most effective.

Time
Flexible time increased from 0% to 58% of the whole session in the intervention group from
observed session one to three, whereas there was no flexible time evident in the control
group sessions. There was a large decrease in inactive time (when students were not
actively engaged in a learning task but were listening to the teacher) from 60% to 13% of the
total class time in the intervention group over the three observed sessions. The inactive time
also decreased from 56% to 30% in the control group. The increase in intensity of the
sessions and activity time was evident in the intervention group interviews:

      I think we have started to work a lot harder over the last few weeks. I remember one of
      the first sessions we had we were in four grids and it was really static. It was a cold
      morning and everyone was just standing around and didn’t look interested. I think
      we’ve learned and these last few weeks everyone has started pretty sharp. They move
      now and they want to get involved.

The mastery climate fostered by the intervention group lecturer was encapsulated by one
student in the group interviews when he said, “when I started enjoying the sessions I began
setting my own motivational goals and became more determined at improving”. A further
response clearly identified the increasing levels of intrinsic motivation of the students as a
result of the intervention programme:

      I was in the car on the way up and …….. and me were talking about it. Its 9am on a
      Friday morning and its tipping down with rain and we thought to ourselves, why are we
      doing this? …..and then we said it’s because we actually enjoy it”.

Conclusion
The behavioural analysis of the TARGET structures, supported by the group interviews,
identified a number of positive effects associated with the mastery intervention programme.
Specifically, findings revealed increases in student set mastery goals, greater differentiation
of tasks, more individual feedback on effort and progress, increased mixed ability groupings,
and flexible and active time in sessions. There were also a number of issues identified in
relation to the implementation of Ames (1992a) TARGET structures in a HE environment.

Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                        82
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
Some of these issues included the level of students’ experience required to set effective
learning goals, the need and desire for competitive goals, the level of authority to give
students within sessions, the administering of public versus private feedback, and the conflict
between mixed ability groups and differentiation of tasks for optimal challenge. Future
research will need to consider these structures in more detail in order to identify a model of
best practice in HE. The broader implications of this study could also be evaluated by
focusing on other subject areas and the interrelationship of the TARGET structures. Such
pedagogical research could potentially shape future teaching and learning strategies, and
assist HE lecturers in creating more effective learning environments.

References
Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individualistic goal structures: A motivational analysis.
   In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: Student motivation (pp.177-
   199). New York: Academic Press.
Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals and the classroom motivational climate. In J. L. Meece & D. H.
   Schunck (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 327-348). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational
   Psychology, 84, 261-271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261
Ames, C. (1992c). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and motivational processes. In G. C.
   Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp.161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and
   motivational processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267. doi:10.1037/0022-
   0663.80.3.260
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Observing instruction: An introduction to sequential analysis.
   New York: Cambridge University Press.
Carpenter, P. J., & Morgan, K. (1999). Motivational climate, personal goal perspectives, and cognitive
   and affective responses in physical education classes. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 4,
   31-44.
Digelidis, N., Papaioannou, A., Laparidis, K., & Christodoulidis, T. (2004). A one year intervention in 7th
   grade physical education classes aiming to change motivational climate and attitudes towards
   exercise. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 195-210. doi:10.1016/S1469-0292(02)00002-X
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040-
   1048. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1040
Dweck ,C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and personality.
   Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256
Epstein, J. (1989). Family structures and student motivation: A developmental perspective. In C. Ames
   & R. Ames (Eds.) Research on motivation in education: Goals and cognitions (pp. 259-295). New
   York: Academic Press.
Goudas, M., & Biddle, S. (1994). Perceived motivational climate and intrinsic motivation in school
   physical education classes. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2, 241-250.
   doi:10.1007/BF03172783
Morgan, K., & Carpenter, P. (2002). Effects of manipulating the motivational climate in physical
   education lessons. European Physical Education Review, 8, 207-229.
   doi:10.1177/1356336X020083003
Morgan, K., & Kingston, K. (2008). Development of a self-observation mastery intervention programme
   for teacher education. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 13, 109-129.
   doi:10.1080/17408980701345634
Morgan, K., Sproule, J., Weigand, D., & Carpenter, P. (2005). A computer-based observational
   assessment of the teaching behaviours the influence motivational climate in physical education.
   Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 10, 83-105. doi:10.1080/1740898042000334926
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task
   choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.328
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
   University Press.
Ommundsen, Y., & Roberts, G. C. (1999). Effect of motivational climate profiles on motivational
   indices in team sport. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 9, 389-397.
   doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.1999.tb00261.x
Roberts, G. C. (2001). Advances in motivation in sport and exercise: conceptual constraints and
   convergence. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 1-50).
   Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sharpe, T., & Koperwas, J. (1999). BEST: Behavioral evaluation strategy and taxonomy software.
   Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.


Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                             83
Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports
class
Solmon, M. A. (1996). Impact of motivational climate on students’ behaviors and perceptions in a
   physical education setting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 731-738. doi:10.1037/0022-
   0663.88.4.731
Treasure, D. (1993). A social-cognitive approach to understanding children's achievement behavior,
   cognitions, and affect in competitive sport. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois,
   Urbana-Champaign.
Treasure, D. C. (1997). Perceptions of the motivational climate and elementary school children’s
   cognitive and affective response. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 19, 278-290.
Treasure, D. (2001). Enhancing young people’s motivation in youth sport: An achievement goal
   approach. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.) Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 79-100).
   Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Submitted February 2009. Revised July 2009. Final Version August 2009. Accepted September 2009.




Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84                              84

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Teacher+qualities+and+collaboration
Teacher+qualities+and+collaborationTeacher+qualities+and+collaboration
Teacher+qualities+and+collaboration
Joanpan
 
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
inventionjournals
 
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinstOr ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
Ola7
 

Mais procurados (20)

Devi subramaniam
Devi subramaniamDevi subramaniam
Devi subramaniam
 
Research Students Teaching
Research Students TeachingResearch Students Teaching
Research Students Teaching
 
STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL REVIEWING RELATIONSHIPS AMONG GOAL ORIENTATION, ACA...
STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL REVIEWING RELATIONSHIPS AMONG GOAL ORIENTATION, ACA...STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL REVIEWING RELATIONSHIPS AMONG GOAL ORIENTATION, ACA...
STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODEL REVIEWING RELATIONSHIPS AMONG GOAL ORIENTATION, ACA...
 
Teacher+qualities+and+collaboration
Teacher+qualities+and+collaborationTeacher+qualities+and+collaboration
Teacher+qualities+and+collaboration
 
In The Name Of Allah Almightyiii,
In The Name Of Allah Almightyiii,In The Name Of Allah Almightyiii,
In The Name Of Allah Almightyiii,
 
Mountain Climbing Analogy
Mountain Climbing AnalogyMountain Climbing Analogy
Mountain Climbing Analogy
 
Need and function of teaching by anand
Need and function of teaching by anandNeed and function of teaching by anand
Need and function of teaching by anand
 
Finalsubmission educ696 classroommanagement
Finalsubmission educ696 classroommanagementFinalsubmission educ696 classroommanagement
Finalsubmission educ696 classroommanagement
 
Model&theory comparison
Model&theory comparisonModel&theory comparison
Model&theory comparison
 
00016338
0001633800016338
00016338
 
Teaching game sense powerpoint
Teaching game sense powerpointTeaching game sense powerpoint
Teaching game sense powerpoint
 
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
A Study On TheRelationshipBetween Teachers' EducationalBeliefsAndTheirLevel O...
 
Instructional Coaching
Instructional CoachingInstructional Coaching
Instructional Coaching
 
Intro to Game Sense
Intro to Game SenseIntro to Game Sense
Intro to Game Sense
 
Teaching learning process
Teaching learning  processTeaching learning  process
Teaching learning process
 
اتساق المنهج مع مخرجات التعلم
اتساق المنهج مع مخرجات التعلماتساق المنهج مع مخرجات التعلم
اتساق المنهج مع مخرجات التعلم
 
Concept of teaching
Concept of teachingConcept of teaching
Concept of teaching
 
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinstOr ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
Or ws tea_elem_02_diffinst
 
Class weebly movement education slides
Class weebly movement education slidesClass weebly movement education slides
Class weebly movement education slides
 
ASSESSING STUDENTS PERFOMANCE PURPOSE AND TECHNIQUE.
ASSESSING STUDENTS PERFOMANCE  PURPOSE AND  TECHNIQUE.ASSESSING STUDENTS PERFOMANCE  PURPOSE AND  TECHNIQUE.
ASSESSING STUDENTS PERFOMANCE PURPOSE AND TECHNIQUE.
 

Destaque

楊璧慈2[1]
楊璧慈2[1]楊璧慈2[1]
楊璧慈2[1]
輝 哲
 
Steven spielberg asier lazkano
Steven spielberg asier lazkanoSteven spielberg asier lazkano
Steven spielberg asier lazkano
artxandape
 
Tyler's Webquest
Tyler's WebquestTyler's Webquest
Tyler's Webquest
tjschultz85
 
من نفس عن مؤمن كربة
من نفس عن مؤمن كربةمن نفس عن مؤمن كربة
من نفس عن مؤمن كربة
amani166
 
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todasCasting 2014 planilla general cat todas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todas
Andrés Canclini
 
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
Hakan Turkkusu
 
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damasCasting 2014 planilla general cat damas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damas
Andrés Canclini
 
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
iclaret
 
Design for complexity
Design for complexityDesign for complexity
Design for complexity
Lextant
 

Destaque (20)

How To Build Scrum Task Boards that Radiate Information
How To Build Scrum Task Boards that Radiate Information How To Build Scrum Task Boards that Radiate Information
How To Build Scrum Task Boards that Radiate Information
 
Комсомольская правда
Комсомольская правдаКомсомольская правда
Комсомольская правда
 
UCAMI 2013
UCAMI 2013UCAMI 2013
UCAMI 2013
 
楊璧慈2[1]
楊璧慈2[1]楊璧慈2[1]
楊璧慈2[1]
 
Steven spielberg asier lazkano
Steven spielberg asier lazkanoSteven spielberg asier lazkano
Steven spielberg asier lazkano
 
Tyler's Webquest
Tyler's WebquestTyler's Webquest
Tyler's Webquest
 
Crisis Communications
Crisis CommunicationsCrisis Communications
Crisis Communications
 
من نفس عن مؤمن كربة
من نفس عن مؤمن كربةمن نفس عن مؤمن كربة
من نفس عن مؤمن كربة
 
Nietas
NietasNietas
Nietas
 
Profesor Steven E. Jones and Ancient American Equus
Profesor Steven E. Jones and Ancient American EquusProfesor Steven E. Jones and Ancient American Equus
Profesor Steven E. Jones and Ancient American Equus
 
Distributed DBMS - Unit 6 - Query Processing
Distributed DBMS - Unit 6 - Query ProcessingDistributed DBMS - Unit 6 - Query Processing
Distributed DBMS - Unit 6 - Query Processing
 
Infinity investimentos
Infinity investimentosInfinity investimentos
Infinity investimentos
 
2015 2016 elections in africa
2015 2016 elections in africa2015 2016 elections in africa
2015 2016 elections in africa
 
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todasCasting 2014 planilla general cat todas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat todas
 
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
Mediacat dergisi, Event Marketing eki, 2009/10
 
Health 24 & Virgin Active - Arm Exercises
Health 24 & Virgin Active - Arm ExercisesHealth 24 & Virgin Active - Arm Exercises
Health 24 & Virgin Active - Arm Exercises
 
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damasCasting 2014 planilla general cat damas
Casting 2014 planilla general cat damas
 
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
Presentació Pwerp. famílies Cicle Mitjà 2014
 
Presentacion krashen
Presentacion krashenPresentacion krashen
Presentacion krashen
 
Design for complexity
Design for complexityDesign for complexity
Design for complexity
 

Semelhante a 07 pp236morgankingston final73to84

Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
Language Acquisition Resource Center
 
ChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSEChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSE
ianmcnee
 
Assessment and Evaulation Task
Assessment and Evaulation TaskAssessment and Evaulation Task
Assessment and Evaulation Task
Aandre Wessels
 
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
Ching Nemis
 
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional ObjectivesPerformance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
Dr. N. Asokan
 
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
Ching Nemis
 
Designing and conducting formative evaluations
Designing and conducting formative evaluationsDesigning and conducting formative evaluations
Designing and conducting formative evaluations
JCrawford62
 

Semelhante a 07 pp236morgankingston final73to84 (20)

Assessment
AssessmentAssessment
Assessment
 
Assessment (1)
Assessment (1)Assessment (1)
Assessment (1)
 
Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
Foreign Language Classroom Assessment in Support of Teaching and Learning wit...
 
ChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSEChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSE
 
ChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSEChartTSTSE
ChartTSTSE
 
Ej1090151
Ej1090151Ej1090151
Ej1090151
 
Assessment and Evaulation Task
Assessment and Evaulation TaskAssessment and Evaulation Task
Assessment and Evaulation Task
 
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01 (1)
 
Creating the Environment for Learning
Creating the Environment for LearningCreating the Environment for Learning
Creating the Environment for Learning
 
ProfEd7reportf.pptx
ProfEd7reportf.pptxProfEd7reportf.pptx
ProfEd7reportf.pptx
 
Features of Classroom Formative Assessment
Features of Classroom Formative AssessmentFeatures of Classroom Formative Assessment
Features of Classroom Formative Assessment
 
Curriculum Development-Aroona Hashmi
Curriculum Development-Aroona HashmiCurriculum Development-Aroona Hashmi
Curriculum Development-Aroona Hashmi
 
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional ObjectivesPerformance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
Performance Assessment fo students using specific Instructional Objectives
 
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
Alyssamoduleiv copy-120823032932-phpapp01
 
UNIT. 3.pdf
UNIT. 3.pdfUNIT. 3.pdf
UNIT. 3.pdf
 
Types of evaluation by dr.thanuja.k
Types of evaluation by dr.thanuja.k Types of evaluation by dr.thanuja.k
Types of evaluation by dr.thanuja.k
 
Assessment-of-learning
 Assessment-of-learning Assessment-of-learning
Assessment-of-learning
 
Hrd 13
Hrd 13Hrd 13
Hrd 13
 
Designing and conducting formative evaluations
Designing and conducting formative evaluationsDesigning and conducting formative evaluations
Designing and conducting formative evaluations
 
Guidelines in Assessing Knowledge, Process, Understanding, and Performance/Pr...
Guidelines in Assessing Knowledge, Process, Understanding, and Performance/Pr...Guidelines in Assessing Knowledge, Process, Understanding, and Performance/Pr...
Guidelines in Assessing Knowledge, Process, Understanding, and Performance/Pr...
 

Último

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
PECB
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
MateoGardella
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Chris Hunter
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
QucHHunhnh
 

Último (20)

Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 

07 pp236morgankingston final73to84

  • 1. Vol. 9, No. 1. ISSN: 1473-8376 www.heacademy.ac.uk/johlste PRACTICE PAPER Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Kevin Morgan (kmorgan@uwic.ac.uk) and Kieran Kingston (kkingston@uwic.ac.uk) School of Sport, University of Wales Institute Cardiff, Cyncoed Road, Cardiff, CF23 6XD, UK DOI:10.3794/johlste.91.236 ©Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education Abstract The lecturer can aim to develop a motivational climate that could strongly influence the degree to which students perceive mastery of the tasks or outperforming others as important. This study aimed to evaluate the effects of an intervention programme to promote a mastery motivational climate on lecturing behaviours and student learning experiences in an undergraduate practical soccer module. As a consequence of the intervention, observational analysis of lecturer behaviours showed increases in student-set mastery goals, greater differentiation of tasks, increased lecturer feedback on effort and progress to individual students, and more flexible time to learn. Group interviews with students revealed that the mastery programme had a positive impact on their motivation and learning experiences. Keywords: task; authority; recognition; grouping; evaluation; target; TARGET Introduction According to achievement goal theory (AGT) (Nicholls, 1984, 1989), in achievement situations the goal of participants is to demonstrate competence or avoid demonstrating incompetence. Competence, however, can be construed in a number of ways, for example, outperforming others (ego-involved goal), or improving one's own learning and mastering the demands of the task (mastery-involved goal) (Ames, 1992a). According to Roberts (2001) these conceptions of competence are determined by both dispositional and situational factors. While much of the research into AGT has focused on individual differences in dispositional goal orientations (tendencies to be task or ego involved in achievement settings) and associated patterns of cognition and affect (Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1984, 1989), some has considered how the structure and demands of the learning environment (referred to as the motivational climate) can evoke different achievement goals and motivational patterns (Ames, 1984, 1992a, 1992b; Ames & Archer, 1988). The premise of this research adopting a situational perspective is that individuals’ perceptions of the motivational climate determine their goals and subsequent motivational responses (Treasure, 2001). Motivational climate is, therefore, defined as a situationally induced psychological environment directing goals of action (Ames, 1992a). Dr Kevin Morgan (Lead Author) is a Senior Lecturer in the UWIC School of Sport and specialises in the area of Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy. Dr Kieran Kingston is a Senior Lecturer in the UWIC School of Sport and specialises in the area of Sport Psychology.
  • 2. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class In school physical education (PE) and youth sport settings, adaptive learning and motivational patterns; for example, a positive attitude towards the activity, feelings of satisfaction, high perceptions of ability, the choice of challenging tasks, high intrinsic motivation, and placing a high value on effort and the process of learning; have been consistently associated with perceptions of a mastery climate (e.g., Carpenter & Morgan, 1999; Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Treasure, 1997). In contrast, a perceived ego climate has been linked to less adaptive cognitive and affective responses, such as boredom, beliefs that ability rather than effort leads to success, a lack of enjoyment, and a negative attitude toward the subject matter (e.g., Carpenter & Morgan, 1999; Ommundsen & Roberts, 1999; Treasure, 1997). Epstein (1989) identified the task, authority, recognition, grouping, evaluation, and time structures (TARGET) as influential in determining motivation in school and home environments. Later, Ames (1992a) adopted the TARGET acronym to encapsulate the structures that foster a mastery motivational climate in achievement situations. According to Ames, to foster a mastery teaching environment the task structure should involve: (a) students in setting their own personal goals focused on self-referenced improvement, (b) multiple activities in order to reduce the opportunity for normative comparisons of ability, and (c) tasks which are differentiated to optimally challenge all students. The authority structure should encourage students to be involved in decision making and leadership roles. Recognition and evaluation from the teacher should be given privately and be individually based on effort and progress; further students should be involved in self-evaluation against personal goals. Students should be grouped into small mixed ability and co-operative groups and be given the opportunity to change groups both within and between sessions. Finally, the time structure should allow flexible time for improvement and maximise time to practice and learn. In contrast, an ego climate would emphasise uni-dimensional competitive tasks, teacher authority, normatively based public recognition and evaluation, homogenous ability groups, and time to practice would be inflexible (see Table 1). The TARGET guidelines were written for school classroom lessons and have been applied to the youth sport and PE settings (Ames, 1992c). Some of these structures may not always be applicable to settings beyond these settings (e.g., Higher Education) because, for example, of the constraints of the learning environment with regards to the grouping and learning outcomes. TARGET behaviour Mastery climate Ego climate Task Self-referenced goals, Comparative goals, multi-dimensional, varied & uni-dimensional & differentiated undifferentiated Authority Participants given leadership roles Teacher makes all the & involved in decision making decisions Recognition Private recognition of improvement, Public recognition of effort and accomplishments normative ability and comparative performances Grouping Small mixed ability & co-operative Ability groups groups Whole class activities Evaluation Self-referenced. Private Normative & public consultations with teacher based on improvement & effort Time Flexible time for task completion Inflexible time for task and maximum time to learn completion Table 1: TARGET behaviours that influence motivational climate (Epstein, 1989; Ames, 1992b) Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 74
  • 3. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Intervention studies manipulating the TARGET structures to create a mastery focused teaching environment (Digelidis, Papaioannou, Laparidis, & Christodoulidis., 2004; Morgan & Carpenter, 2002; Solmon, 1996; Treasure, 1993) have described the enhancement of students’ motivation and learning in PE settings. Specifically, students’ tended to be more task oriented and less ego oriented; had higher levels of perceived competence, satisfaction and enjoyment; were less bored; preferred to engage in more challenging tasks; and believed success was the result of effort. In contrast, when the TARGET structures were more ego-involving, students’ tended to be more ego oriented; had lower levels of perceived ability, satisfaction and enjoyment; were less interested in achievement tasks; and believed success was the result of ability and deception. Taking this research further, Morgan, Sproule, Weigand and Carpenter (2005) used the Behavioural Evaluation Strategies and Taxonomies (BEST) software (Sharpe & Koperwas, 1999) to create a computer-based observational measure of the TARGET structures in PE for students aged 11-14. This measure allows researchers to film practical sessions and code the teacher’s behaviours as mastery, ego or neutral, based on the TARGET concept. Furthermore, the computer software allows practitioners to self-evaluate teaching behaviours against Ames’ (1992a) guidelines for fostering a mastery climate. Based on self-observation and analysis of filmed lessons, using the behavioural TARGET measure (Morgan et al., 2005), Morgan and Kingston (2008) developed a mastery intervention programme for PE teachers of 13-14 year old students. There were five stages to the intervention programme with each of the PE teachers. In Stage 1 the teachers were filmed adopting their typical teaching behaviours and surveys were administered to measure students’ perceptions of the motivational climate and their cognitive and affective responses. Stage 2 involved an individual introduction to the TARGET behaviours (Ames, 1992a), which included an explanation of the different TARGET structures followed by practical examples from PE lessons and practice at coding the behaviours as mastery, performance or neither. In Stage 3 the teachers coded their own filmed lessons and the lead researcher conducted a separate analysis using Morgan et al.’s (2005) measure. In Stage 4 the lead researcher discussed the teaching behaviours displayed in the filmed lessons with each individual teacher and the teachers then re-planned their original filmed lesson to be more mastery- involving. Finally, in Stage 5, they were re-filmed whilst following their new mastery focused lesson plan and pupils completed a post-intervention survey. Results revealed that the mastery intervention programme was successful in fostering more mastery involving teaching behaviours and higher perceptions of mastery involving TARGET behaviours. Statistical analysis revealed that the more disaffected pupils significantly improved their motivational responses whereas the more highly motivated pupils did not. However, a limitation was that qualitative methods were not used in order to explore the students learning experiences in more depth. The purpose of the present study was to further extend this line of research into higher education (HE) and to evaluate the effects of a mastery intervention programme on the lecturing behaviours that influence the motivational climate in a practical sports class. Additionally, qualitative analysis was used to gain a greater depth of understanding about the students’ learning experiences in a mastery condition than previous research of this nature. Method Participants Two HE lecturers were randomly assigned to an intervention group (1 male, aged 35; group n = 16: 15 male, 1 female) and a control group (1 female, aged 25: group n = 18: 12 male, 6 female). Participants provided voluntary informed consent to take part in the study and were participating in a 12 week, Level 2 undergraduate practical soccer module. Ethical approval for all procedures was gained from the researchers’ university ethics committee. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 75
  • 4. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Observational measure Analysis of the filmed sessions was conducted using Morgan et al.’s (2005) observational measure of TARGET (see Table 2). The measure permits immediate analysis of data gathered through observations of teaching from video and audio recording of lessons. Validity was established by four researchers, experienced in teacher education and motivational climate research, who met and agreed upon the teaching behaviours that matched with the different TARGET structures. Acceptable intra- and inter-reliability to ≥ 0.80 (Bakeman & Gottman, 1986) was established during the development of the measure (Morgan et al., 2005). Mastery Neutral Ego Task 0 = Teacher set self- 3 = No clear goals 2 = Competitive goal referenced goal or cooperative group- 4 = Warm up/cool down referenced goal 1 = Students set own self- referenced goal or cooperative group- referenced goal 5 = Multi-dimensional/ 6 = Uni-dimensional/ different tasks same task 7 = Differentiated/suitably 8 = Undifferentiated/ challenging for all not suitably challenging for all Authority 9 = Pupils involved in 10 = Teacher makes all (Duration- leadership roles and / or the decisions toggle) decision making Recognition P = Recognition/evaluation E = General W = Recognition/ & Evaluation focused on individual effort, assessment/feedback evaluation focused on improvement/progress and (to no one in particular) individual effort, accomplishment in private improvement/progress R = Focus on luck and accomplishment in Q = Evaluation that allows Public equal opportunity for recognition and rewards T = Recognition/ evaluation focused on Y = Self- Evaluate against normative comparisons a set goal Grouping S= Small heterogeneous/ A = Homogeneous/ (Duration- mixed ability groups ability groups toggle) G = Change of groups D = Large group/whole class Timing Z = Flexible time to C = Inactive time X = Inflexible time to (Duration- practice, plan or evaluate practice, plan or toggle) evaluate Table 2: TARGET coding for the analysis of teaching behaviours that influence motivational climate (Morgan et al., 2005) Procedures There were five stages to the intervention programme, as identified in Figure 1. In Stage 1 both HE lecturers were filmed teaching in their typical way. Both lecturers were familiar with being filmed during practical sessions and were therefore not distracted by the camera. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 76
  • 5. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Stage 2 of the intervention involved the lead researcher educating the intervention group lecturer with regards to the TARGET teaching behaviours (Ames, 1992a) associated with mastery and ego motivational climates (Table 1). This involved a verbal explanation of the different TARGET structures and practical examples of the TARGET behaviours from video footage of previously filmed sports teaching sessions. Further 10 minute video clips of different practical sports teaching episodes were then viewed and the intervention group lecturer was trained to recognise and code the teaching structures as mastery, performance or neutral, using the computer based behavioural measure of TARGET (Morgan et al., 2005) (see Table 2 for the coding categories). • voluntary informed consent Stage 1 • teachers filmed while adopting usual teaching style • education of intervention group teacher with Stage 2 regards to recognition and coding of TARGET behaviours • lead researcher and intervention group teacher jointly coding session from Stage 1 Stage 3 to assess TARGET behaviours • education of intervention group teacher on modifying teaching behaviours to support a mastery climate • both intervention and control teachers (and groups) filmed Stage 4 • lead researcher and intervention group teacher analysed lessons with respect to TARGET structures • further education on modifying teaching behaviours to intervention group teacher • teachers filmed while adopting usual teaching style Stage 5 • lead researcher and intervention group teacher analysed lessons with respect to TARGET structures Figure 1: Diagrammatical representation of the intervention Stage 3 involved the lead researcher and the intervention group lecturer jointly coding the teaching session described in Stage 1. Based on this analysis certain teaching behaviours were identified to enhance the mastery focus of the soccer sessions and suggestions were made by the lecturer for modifying the teaching behaviours during the next filmed session. The implementation of Stages 1 to 3 of the intervention with the control group lecturer took place “weeks after the completion of the module, consistent with the university’s ethical procedures, and the control group students benefited accordingly in their soccer lectures in the following academic year. In Stage 4 both lecturers were filmed for the second time. Following this, the intervention group lecturer and the lead researcher jointly analysed the TARGET behaviours to identify any changes in the mastery involving focus of the session, and to set further mastery Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 77
  • 6. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class behavioural objectives for the final filmed session. During this second video analysis phase the intervention group lecturer took the lead in coding the behaviours and the lead researcher assisted. The purpose of this was to develop the HE lecturer’s ability to code independently in the next stage in order to use the software as a means of reflection on practice. Stage 5 involved both lecturers being filmed for the third and final time. The intervention group lecturer and the lead researcher then analysed the teaching behaviours individually to identify any changes in the mastery involving focus of the third filmed session and met to discuss their findings. Group interviews One week after the completion of the 12 week teaching programme, the lead researcher conducted four group interviews (two with the intervention group students and two with the control group students), with four students in each group. In this study, the researcher was not the lecturer and students were made aware that their responses were totally confidential and that the lecturer would not get to hear them. The researcher facilitated the discussion and ensured equal input amongst participants. The interviews were recorded on a digital voice recorder. Following an introduction to the purpose of the interview, the participants were asked if they had noticed any significant changes in the teaching behaviours since the first filmed session. The researcher then questioned the participants about each of the TARGET structures and asked them to share their thoughts and feelings about any changes they had perceived in the lecturers’ behaviours and what impact this had on their learning experience. Results Observational and group interview data This section reports the observational findings combined with the group interview responses in relation to each of the TARGET structures. Using Morgan et al.’s (2005) TARGET measure, two researchers simultaneously undertook video analysis of the TARGET (Ames, 1992a) behaviours of both lecturers in their three filmed sessions. One hundred percent agreement was achieved in assessing the behaviours. This was possible because of the flexibility of the BEST software (Sharpe & Koperwas, 1999), which permitted the two researchers to pause the system for discussion until complete and unambiguous agreement was reached. Discussions were short and decisions were reached quickly and easily due to the fact that the researchers were experienced at this type of analysis. For each of the three filmed sessions in both groups, the mean percentage frequency of mastery, ego and neutral behaviours (as a proportion of all coded behaviours in that particular TARGET structure) was calculated for the task, recognition and evaluation. The mean percentage duration of mastery, ego and neutral behaviours (as a proportion of the total session time) was calculated for authority, grouping and time. Group interviews were deductively analysed based on Ames’s (1992a) description of the TARGET structures. The interview responses clearly indicated that there were perceived changes in TARGET related teacher behaviours by participants in the intervention group, whereas the TARGET behaviours of the control group lecturer were perceived to be consistent throughout. There were a number of mastery structures observed and reported by the students in the control group including the setting of clear learning aims, the questioning and giving of authority to students, positive and corrective feedback on effort and improvement, and mixed ability cooperative groups. However, both the behavioural analysis of the TARGET structures (see Tables 3a, 3b and 3c) and the student interview responses illustrated a higher level of change in the intervention group lecturer’s behaviours compared to the control group. These changes in observed behaviours, supported by specific detail from the group interview data, are reported under the TARGET headings in the following sections. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 78
  • 7. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Task goals (M% frequency) Task design Task (M% frequency) differentiation (M% frequency) Group Session St Lec Ego No Warm Multi- Uni- Diff Undiff Mas Mas set up dim dim I 1 0 89 11 0 0 0 100 0 100 2 20 80 0 0 0 10 90 60 40 3 0 86 0 14 0 33 67 33 67 C 1 0 100 0 0 0 0 100 31 69 2 0 86 0 0 14 0 100 0 100 3 0 100 0 0 0 0 100 0 100 Table 3a: Observed TARGET Behaviours Authority Recognition & Evaluation (M% duration) (M% frequency) Group Session Mast Ego Private: Public: Self- Ego General Luck Effort & Effort & eval Imp Imp I 1 30 70 2 46 0 8 43 1 2 62 38 8 58 2 0 26 6 3 82 18 2 60 3 0 32 3 C 1 25 75 4 58 0 0 38 0 2 52 48 2 60 0 0 38 0 3 62 38 4 53 0 0 43 0 Table 3b: Observed TARGET Behaviours Grouping (M% duration) Time (M% duration) Group Session Mix ability Ability Whole Flexible Inflexible Inactive groups groups class I 1 31 0 69 0 40 60 2 37 0 63 0 64 36 3 63 0 37 58 29 13 C 1 35 0 65 0 44 56 2 26 0 74 0 57 43 3 24 0 76 0 70 30 Table 3c: Observed TARGET Behaviours Group I = intervention, C = control Task There was a 0-20% increase in observed student set mastery goals from filmed session one to two in the intervention group, but no evidence of students setting their own goals in the control group. Focus group data supported this finding and highlighted the positive impact of individual goal setting on student motivation. For example: He developed goal setting a few weeks into the sessions and then you were continually reminded throughout the sessions to check your goals. You were constantly thinking have you achieved your goals and evaluating yourself. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 79
  • 8. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class There was a strong feeling amongst the groups that this improved intrinsic motivation because: It helps you get something out of the session that you want to achieve instead of it being something that the lecturer wants you to have. If you can relate it to yourself it keeps you interested. There was some caution expressed by students regarding requests to set their own goals. They felt that its value depended on the experience of the participants, as suggested by one student: If you’ve been playing for a while you know the sort of goals to set, but if you are a beginner you need to be told that these are the things you need to know for a particular skill. You have to be realistic as well, as otherwise your motivation will decrease if you haven’t achieved them. Lecturer-set mastery goals, as opposed to competitive goals, were clearly evident in both groups from the first observed session and this did not change as a result of the intervention. However, there was also a feeling amongst some group members that competitive goals were included and an important aspect within sport, especially in team games as articulated by the following: He does bring some competition into it because it’s a competitive sport isn’t it. If you are defending against two attackers then you have to be competitive to try and stop them getting to the goal. He does include competitive goals but not too much that it doesn’t fit with the overall goals. I think because we are sports students that competitiveness is innate. The setting of multi-dimensional tasks by the lecturer of the intervention group increased from 0 to 33% of all tasks, from filmed session one to three; these changes were not observed for the control group. Furthermore, the frequency of task differentiation was more evident in sessions two and three compared to session one in the intervention group, whereas in the control group no such changes were evident. Consistent with the observational findings, interview data revealed perceptions of greater differentiation within the intervention group sessions as the weeks progressed: Last week he came around and told us to do something more difficult because we had the technical ability, while others continued doing something else. So we went straight into something at say the third level of difficulty, whereas others were still on the first level. This increased differentiation combined with personal goal setting of the intervention group appears to have had a positive effect on engagement and confidence, as evidenced by the following: Whereas in the beginning possibly it was only the better players who were having more of an influence on the game, today a lot more people were getting involved and were a lot more confident with the ball, which I think comes from focusing on developing your own skills during the sessions. Authority The decision making opportunities and leadership roles increased in both groups as the module progressed, though the change was more marked in the intervention group. This was primarily due to the learning outcomes and session content which was planned for the latter part of the module involving the students in coaching tasks. For the intervention group, this change in the authority structure was also reflected in the group interviews: Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 80
  • 9. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class He asked us how we would progress a basic four on two onto another level to challenge the defenders. So we had to think as a group how we could make this benefit the defenders. In the past weeks he has done that himself but today he asked us to do it. The positive effects of student authority was acknowledged and valued in the intervention group interviews: There was no power divide the power was amongst the group if you like. He gave a lot of autonomy to the group and that’s important to enhance your motivation, and you don’t realise it until you reflect on it. However, although students generally reflected in a positive manner to being given authority within sessions, a minority would have preferred it if the lecturer had adopted a more autocratic approach: “I’d personally rather it if he just did it himself…I think it’s just time”. Recognition and evaluation Incidences of private individual feedback were found to increase four-fold from observed session one to two in the intervention group, but this reverted to baseline levels at session three. This may be explained by the added focus on this aspect by the researcher during Stage 3 of the intervention, but less of a focus in Stage 4, resulting in a return to more typical teaching behaviours in filmed session three. This may be a limitation of the intervention that needs to be considered in future research of this nature. Interview data supported the change to more private individual feedback by the intervention group lecturer in session two. For example, one student commented that: He comes around well and talks to you individually. He doesn’t stop it and say now we’ll do this, he just has a quiet word while other groups are still working, which I think is good. The wider benefits of such private individual feedback were evident from quotes such as: I think it’s a bit more personal if he comes up to you privately. He can say well done in front of everyone without really thinking but if he’s actually come on to you to say it privately, perhaps it’s a bit more important. However, the logistical difficulties of providing private individual feedback to a large group with only one member of staff were also acknowledged: “I get personal individual feedback sometimes but to give individual feedback to everyone in the group is not practical in this situation”. There was a small increase in the frequency of mastery feedback given publicly to individuals (in situations where others could hear) in the intervention group. Although public feedback is considered to be more ego-involving (Ames, 1992a), some students felt that “it gives you confidence if you get a well done in front of other people” and that “it’s more important how he criticises you, publicly or privately….if I get criticised publicly it can give me a kick up the ass like, but other people may take it to heart”. Furthermore it was perceived by some that public feedback is important for the learning of the whole group: “if he does give individual feedback he makes sure others are around so that they can benefit from the feedback as well”. Although there were few instances, self-evaluation increased in the intervention group but was not evident in the control group. Normatively, comparative feedback was observed in session one for the intervention group but was eliminated in sessions two and three. It was not observed in any of the three sessions for the control group. General feedback to the whole group (neutral) decreased from session one in the intervention group, yet increased albeit marginally in the control group. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 81
  • 10. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Grouping The duration of the session spent in mixed ability cooperative groups, as opposed to whole class, increased markedly from session one to three (31% to 69% of the whole time in mixed ability groups) for the intervention group, whereas in the control group it decreased to a small degree (35% to 24%). The positive impact of heterogeneous grouping on the lower ability students’ learning was acknowledged in the interviews: “last week we were all in mixed ability groups, so people of a lower ability were working with people of a higher level and they seem to be getting better as a result”. However, there was a feeling from some students that if they were “put into ability groups then the standard would go up, because there is no doubt that in mixed ability groups some people do bring the standard down”. Consistent with Ames’ (1992a) recommendations for a mastery climate, there was also variety of groupings within sessions, illustrated by the following quote: “you tend to change groups throughout the session so you end up in a different group to the one you started in”. There was an interesting conflict identified between task differentiation, which is a mastery structure (Ames, 1992a), and mixed ability grouping which, according to some students, made it more difficult to differentiate tasks between groups because not all students were of the same ability level. Therefore, the mixed ability groups tended to work at similar levels, whereas some students felt that if the groups had been based on ability it would have been easier for the lecturer to add further challenges to some groups to develop them at their own level of ability. This was illustrated by one student who felt that mixed ability groups “can bring the better players down and put added pressure on the weaker players, which they can take as a challenge or adopt an attitude that they don’t want to play”. This student went on to suggest that groups of the students’ own choice were the most effective. Time Flexible time increased from 0% to 58% of the whole session in the intervention group from observed session one to three, whereas there was no flexible time evident in the control group sessions. There was a large decrease in inactive time (when students were not actively engaged in a learning task but were listening to the teacher) from 60% to 13% of the total class time in the intervention group over the three observed sessions. The inactive time also decreased from 56% to 30% in the control group. The increase in intensity of the sessions and activity time was evident in the intervention group interviews: I think we have started to work a lot harder over the last few weeks. I remember one of the first sessions we had we were in four grids and it was really static. It was a cold morning and everyone was just standing around and didn’t look interested. I think we’ve learned and these last few weeks everyone has started pretty sharp. They move now and they want to get involved. The mastery climate fostered by the intervention group lecturer was encapsulated by one student in the group interviews when he said, “when I started enjoying the sessions I began setting my own motivational goals and became more determined at improving”. A further response clearly identified the increasing levels of intrinsic motivation of the students as a result of the intervention programme: I was in the car on the way up and …….. and me were talking about it. Its 9am on a Friday morning and its tipping down with rain and we thought to ourselves, why are we doing this? …..and then we said it’s because we actually enjoy it”. Conclusion The behavioural analysis of the TARGET structures, supported by the group interviews, identified a number of positive effects associated with the mastery intervention programme. Specifically, findings revealed increases in student set mastery goals, greater differentiation of tasks, more individual feedback on effort and progress, increased mixed ability groupings, and flexible and active time in sessions. There were also a number of issues identified in relation to the implementation of Ames (1992a) TARGET structures in a HE environment. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 82
  • 11. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Some of these issues included the level of students’ experience required to set effective learning goals, the need and desire for competitive goals, the level of authority to give students within sessions, the administering of public versus private feedback, and the conflict between mixed ability groups and differentiation of tasks for optimal challenge. Future research will need to consider these structures in more detail in order to identify a model of best practice in HE. The broader implications of this study could also be evaluated by focusing on other subject areas and the interrelationship of the TARGET structures. Such pedagogical research could potentially shape future teaching and learning strategies, and assist HE lecturers in creating more effective learning environments. References Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individualistic goal structures: A motivational analysis. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: Student motivation (pp.177- 199). New York: Academic Press. Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals and the classroom motivational climate. In J. L. Meece & D. H. Schunck (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 327-348). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261 Ames, C. (1992c). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp.161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivational processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267. doi:10.1037/0022- 0663.80.3.260 Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Observing instruction: An introduction to sequential analysis. New York: Cambridge University Press. Carpenter, P. J., & Morgan, K. (1999). Motivational climate, personal goal perspectives, and cognitive and affective responses in physical education classes. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 4, 31-44. Digelidis, N., Papaioannou, A., Laparidis, K., & Christodoulidis, T. (2004). A one year intervention in 7th grade physical education classes aiming to change motivational climate and attitudes towards exercise. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 195-210. doi:10.1016/S1469-0292(02)00002-X Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040- 1048. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1040 Dweck ,C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256 Epstein, J. (1989). Family structures and student motivation: A developmental perspective. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.) Research on motivation in education: Goals and cognitions (pp. 259-295). New York: Academic Press. Goudas, M., & Biddle, S. (1994). Perceived motivational climate and intrinsic motivation in school physical education classes. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2, 241-250. doi:10.1007/BF03172783 Morgan, K., & Carpenter, P. (2002). Effects of manipulating the motivational climate in physical education lessons. European Physical Education Review, 8, 207-229. doi:10.1177/1356336X020083003 Morgan, K., & Kingston, K. (2008). Development of a self-observation mastery intervention programme for teacher education. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 13, 109-129. doi:10.1080/17408980701345634 Morgan, K., Sproule, J., Weigand, D., & Carpenter, P. (2005). A computer-based observational assessment of the teaching behaviours the influence motivational climate in physical education. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 10, 83-105. doi:10.1080/1740898042000334926 Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.328 Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Ommundsen, Y., & Roberts, G. C. (1999). Effect of motivational climate profiles on motivational indices in team sport. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 9, 389-397. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.1999.tb00261.x Roberts, G. C. (2001). Advances in motivation in sport and exercise: conceptual constraints and convergence. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 1-50). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Sharpe, T., & Koperwas, J. (1999). BEST: Behavioral evaluation strategy and taxonomy software. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 83
  • 12. Morgan and Kingston (2010) Promoting a mastery motivational climate in a higher education sports class Solmon, M. A. (1996). Impact of motivational climate on students’ behaviors and perceptions in a physical education setting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 731-738. doi:10.1037/0022- 0663.88.4.731 Treasure, D. (1993). A social-cognitive approach to understanding children's achievement behavior, cognitions, and affect in competitive sport. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Treasure, D. C. (1997). Perceptions of the motivational climate and elementary school children’s cognitive and affective response. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 19, 278-290. Treasure, D. (2001). Enhancing young people’s motivation in youth sport: An achievement goal approach. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.) Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 79-100). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Submitted February 2009. Revised July 2009. Final Version August 2009. Accepted September 2009. Journal of Hospitality, Leisure, Sport and Tourism Education, 9(1), 73 – 84 84