SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 71
SECTION 2
Directions
Imagine you are a self-help speaker. Write an 800 WORD
PERSUASIVE SPEECH on “How to eliminate negativity in your
life.”
1. Think about a speech that you would like to present on a
topic of your choice.
2. Complete the following template, including all parts. Fill out
all cells using complete sentences, aiming for three complete
sentences for each cell of the template (if acceptable)
3. In section 4 Reflection write 5 sentences.
4. Consider your audience, purpose, and thesis and complete
Part 1 of the template.
5. Utilize your resources, the internet or a local library to find
three credible sources that are relevant to your speech and
complete Part 2 of the template.
6. Read through your sources to identify five pieces of evidence
that support your thesis and complete Part 3 of the template. Be
sure to use at least three different types of sources (example,
statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony).
7. And then answer all the questions in the attached form.
PART 1: TOPIC
Purpose
What are you hoping to achieve with this speech?
Audience
Who will be in your audience? What do you know about them?
Thesis
What is the key message of your speech?
PART 2: SOURCES
SOURCE 1
Source Title
Author(s) Name
Retrieved from
Where did you get this source? Include location, book,
periodical, url, etc.
Age
How recent is the data or information presented in your source?
When was the source published? If the source is a website,
when was the website last updated?
Depth
What does the source cover?
How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much
depth is the topic covered in?
How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose,
audience, and message?
Author
How is the author qualified to speak on the topic?
What is the author's purpose?
What biases might they have?
Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported
by cited facts?
Money
Who funds the website or publication?
Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items?
What is the reputation of the website or publication?
SOURCE 2
Source Title
Author(s) Name
Retrieved from
Where did you get this source? Include location, book,
periodical, url, etc.
Age
How recent is the data or information presented in your source?
When was the source published? If the source is a website,
when was the website last updated?
Depth
What does the source cover?
How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much
depth is the topic covered in?
How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose,
audience, and message?
Author
How is the author qualified to speak on the topic?
What is the author's purpose?
What biases might they have?
Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported
by cited facts?
Money
Who funds the website or publication?
Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items?
What is the reputation of the website or publication?
SOURCE 3
Source Title
Author(s) Name
Retrieved from
Where did you get this source? Include location, book,
periodical, url, etc.
Age
How recent is the data or information presented in your source?
When was the source published? If the source is a website,
when was the website last updated?
Depth
What does the source cover?
How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much
depth is the topic covered in?
How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose,
audience, and message?
Author
How is the author qualified to speak on the topic?
What is the author's purpose?
What biases might they have?
Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported
by cited facts?
Money
Who funds the website or publication?
Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items?
What is the reputation of the website or publication?
PART 3: SUPPORT
SUPPORT #1
Support
What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in
your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…”
Type of Support
What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an
analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
Relevance
How does this piece of support reinforce your message?
SUPPORT #2
Support
What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in
your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…”
Type of Support
What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an
analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
Relevance
How does this piece of support reinforce your message?
SUPPORT #3
Support
What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in
your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…”
Type of Support
What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an
analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
Relevance
How does this piece of support reinforce your message?
SUPPORT #4
Support
What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in
your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…”
Type of Support
What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an
analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
Relevance
How does this piece of support reinforce your message?
SUPPORT #5
Support
What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in
your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…”
Type of Support
What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an
analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
Relevance
How does this piece of support reinforce your message?
PART 4: REFLECTION
What have you learned from completing this activity?
Section 3
Directions
1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like to
present on a topic of your choice. The speech can be for any
context and any length, but it must be persuasive. If your speech
for Unit 1 or Unit 2 was persuasive, you can use the same topic,
or you can choose a new one if you prefer.
2. See the list of example speech occasions and purposes for
inspiration, if needed.
3. Plan your speech, considering what your introduction, main
points, and conclusion will include.
4. Organize your speech, following the structure of Monroe’s
Motivated Sequence. Your speech should include an
introduction, body, and conclusion. The introduction should
contain your key message. The body should cover your main
topics and support to back up your main points. Make sure that
all support is relevant and from credible sources. Your
conclusion should summarize your main points and provide a
call to action.
5. Create notes or bullet points that you can refer to while
presenting your speech.
6. Practice presenting your speech. Aim for a speech that is 3 to
5 minutes in length.
7. Before filming, review the rubric to ensure that you
understand how you will be evaluated.
8. Film yourself presenting the speech. Be sure that you can be
easily seen and heard, and direct your speech to the camera.
9. Review your video to ensure that you can be seen and heard.
Refilm as needed.
10. Review the checklist and requirements to ensure that your
Touchstone is complete.
11. Upload your video using the blue button at the top of this
page.
Section 4
Directions
1. Review the video of the persuasive speech that you delivered
in Unit 3 as well as the feedback that you have received.
2. Take notes as you review the video and the feedback,
considering edits that you should make to your speech.
3. Based on your notes, revise your speech, adjusting the
content, language, and approach to delivery as needed.
4. Think of an effective way to incorporate at least one visual
aid that will be relevant to your purpose and message and will
enhance your speech.
5. Create a new set of notes or bullet points that you can refer
to while filming your speech.
6. Practice presenting your speech, focusing on your voice, your
nonverbal communication, and your use of one or more visual
aids. Aim for a speech 3-5 minutes in length.
7. Before filming, review the rubric to ensure that you
understand how you will be evaluated.
8. Film yourself presenting the speech. Be sure that you can be
easily seen and heard, and direct your speech to the camera.
9. Review your video, be sure that you can be easily seen and
heard, and that you have directed your speech to the camera.
Refilm as needed.
10. Review the checklist and requirements to ensure that your
Touchstone is complete.
11. Upload your video using the blue button at the top of this
page.
• Academy of Management fleview
1999, Vol. 24, No. 3. 522-537,
AN ORGANIZATIONAL LEARNING
FRAMEWORK: FROM INTUITION TO
INSTITUTION
MARY M. CROSSAN
HENRY W. LANE
RODERICK E. WHITE
Richard Ivey School of Business
Although interest In organizational learning has grown
dramatically in recent years,
a general theory of organizational leaming has remained
elusive. We identily re-
newal of the overall enterprise as the underlying phenomenon of
interest and organ-
izational learning as a principal means to this end. With this
perspective we develop
a framework for the process of organizational learning,
presenting organizational
leaming as four processes—intuiting, interpreting, integrating,
and institutionaliz-
ing—linking the individual, group, and organizational levels.
Organizational learning has existed in our
lexicon at least since Cangelosi and Dill (1965)
discussed the topic over 30 years ago. Lately, its
popularity has grown dramatically (Crossan &
Guatto, 1996), yet little convergence or consen-
sus on what is meant by the term, or its basic
nature, has emerged {Huber, 1991; Kim, 1993).
In large part, convergence has not occurred
because different researchers have applied the
concept of organizational learning, or at least
the terminology, to different domains. For exam-
ple, Huber (1991) takes an information-process-
ing perspective of organizational learning,
whereas Nonaka and Takeuchi (1995) are con-
cerned with product innovation, and March and
Olsen (1975) are interested in exploring how the
cognitive limitations of managers affect learn-
ing. These works share some common threads,
but the domains differ significantly. They con-
cern different phenomena: information process-
ing, product innovation, or bounded rationality.
Although the phenomenological domains of var-
ious researchers do sometimes overlap, the dif-
ferences in domains do much to explain the lack
of convergence among organizational learning
frameworks.
In this article we identify strategic renewal a s
the underlying phenomenon of interest. Re-
We gratefully acknowledge the Canadian Centre for
Management Development, the Social Sciences and Human-
ities Research Council, and the National Centre for Manage-
ment Research and Development for their financial support
of this project.
newal harmonizes continuity and change at the
level of the enterprise (Hurst, 1995; Hurst, Rush,
& White, 1989). Organizational learning can be
conceived of a s a principal means of achieving
the strategic renewal of an enterprise. As we
argue in this article, strategic renewal places
additional demands on a theory of organization-
al learning. Renewal requires that organiza-
tions explore and learn new ways while concur-
rently exploiting what they h a v e a l r e a d y
learned (March, 1991). In contrast, learning ap-
plied to the domain of new product develop-
ment, for example, tends to focus on the explo-
ration side of the exploration-exploitation
tension identified by March. Recognizing and
managing the tension between exploration and
exploitation are two of the critical challenges of
renewal and, hence, become a central require-
ment in a theory of organizational learning.
For renewal to be strategic it should encom-
pass the entire enterprise—not simply the indi-
vidual or group—and it should recognize that
the organization operates in an open system,
rather than having a solely internal focus {Dun-
can & Weiss, 1979). Although theorists have rec-
ognized the strategic importance of organization-
al l e a r n i n g a s a m e a n s of providing a
sustainable competitive a d v a n t a g e (DeGeus,
1988; Stata, 1989), few organizational learning
frameworks have illustrated the tension be-
tween exploration and exploitation that is at the
heart of strategic renewal (see Table 1).
Here we develop an organizational learning
framework to address the phenomenon of re-
522
1999 Crossan. Lane, and White 523
TABLE 1
Propositions Applied to Established Organizational Learning
Frameworks
Source
March & Olsen
(1975)
Daft 8E Weick {1984)
S e n g e {1990)
Huber (1991)
March {1991)
Wat kins & Marsick
(1993)
Nonaka & Takeuchi
(1995)
Strategic
Renewal
Tension
Nat considered
Not considered
Not considered
Not considered
Yes
Not considered
Not considered
Multilevel
Framework
No group level
Not considered
No organizational
level
Yes
No group level
Yes
Recognized, but
not a
substantial part
of the model
One Level
Afiects the
Others
Not considered
Not considered
Not considered
Not considered
Not considered
Not considered
Some
discussion of
the link
between
individual
and group
Process Linking Levels
Not considered
Processes described but
not a levels perspective
Processes focus on
individual and group—
not a levels-related
model
Processes within level but
no model or processes
to link levels
Not considered
Six action imperatives of
the learning
organization
Focuses on processes that
link individual and
group—weak on link
between group and
organization
Cognition/
Action Link
Yes
Learning is a
change in
behavior
Yes
Cognition
affects
behaviors
Yes
Consistent
with
Senge's
perspective
Knowledge
focus
newal. A framework defines the territory and
takes us a step closer to a theory. A good frame-
work has several requirements. First, it should
identify the phenomenon of interest: in this case
strategic renewal. Second, the key premises or
assumptions underlying the framework need to
be stated (Bacharach, 1989). Third, the relation-
ship among the elements of the framework
needs to be described (Sutton & Staw, 1995;
Weick, 1995a; Whetton, 1989). As Sutton and
Staw state, "Theory is about connections among
phenomena, a story about why acts, events,
structure and thoughts occur" (1995: 378). Our
framework makes high-level connections. Fur-
ther theory development will expand and
deepen these connections and will enable de-
velopment of testable hypotheses.
Four key premises or assumptions form the
underpinnings of this framework and support
one central proposition:
Premise 1: Organizational leaming involves a
tension between assimilating new
learning (exploration) and using
what has been learned (exploita-
tion).
Premise 2: Organizational learning is multi-
level: individual, group, and organ-
ization.
Premise 3: The three levels of organizational
learning are linked by social and
psychological processes: intuiting,
interpreting, integrating, and insti-
tutionalizing (4rs).
Premise 4: Cognition affects action (and vice
versa).
Proposifion: The 4l's are related in
feed-forward and feedback processes
across the levels.
As stated in Premise 1, organizational learn-
ing reveals a tension between exploration and
exploitation (March, 1991). March focuses more
on the balance rather than the tension, but he
recognizes its fundamental role in strategic re-
newal: "Maintaining an appropriate balance be-
tween exploration and exploitation is a primary
524 Academy of Management Review July
factor in system survival and prosperity Both
exploration and exploitation are essential for
organizations, but they compete for scarce re-
sources" (1991: 71).
This competition for resources creates a ten-
sion. As we discuss in subsequent sections, this
tension is seen in the feed-forward and feed-
back processes of learning across the individ-
ual, group, and organization levels. Feed for-
ward relates to exploration. It is the transference
of learning from individuals and groups through
to the learning that becomes embedded—or in-
stitutionalized—in the form of systems, struc-
tures, strategies, and procedures (Hedberg, 1981;
Shrivastava, 1983). Feedback relates to exploita-
tion and to the way in which institutionalized
leaming affects individuals and groups.
As noted in Premise 2, organizational learning
is multilevel. A basic assumption is that insight
and innovative ideas occur to individuals—not
organizations (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995; Simon,
1991}. However, knowledge generated by the in-
dividual does not come to bear on the organiza-
tion independently. Ideas are shared, actions
taken, and common meaning developed (Argyris
& Schon, 1978, 1996; Daft & Weick, 1984; Huber,
1991; Stata, 1989). Complex organizations are
more than ad hoc communities or collections of
individuals. Relationships become structured,
and some of the individual learning and shared
understandings developed by groups become
institutionalized a s organization artifacts (Hed-
berg, 1981: Shrivastava, 1983). There is a reason-
able degree of consensus that a theory of organ-
i z a t i o n a l l e a r n i n g n e e d s to c o n s i d e r the
individual, group, a n d organizational levels
(Crossan, Lane, White, & Djurfeldt, 1995).
The 41 processes introduced in Premise 3 are
described in detail in the next section. Through-
out the feed-forward and feedback processes,
the interactive relationship between cognition
and action (Premise 4) is critical—one cannot be
divorced from the other (Neisser, 1976). Under-
standing guides action, but action also informs
u n d e r s t a n d i n g {Seely-Brown & Duguid, 1991;
Weick, 1979). Organizational learning links cog-
nition and action. This differentiates it from the
related fields of knowledge management and
intellectual capital. In spite of arguments to the
contrary (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995), the fields of
knowledge management (Conner & Prahalad,
1996; Davenport & Prusak, 1997; Foss, 1996;
Grant, 1996; Kogut & Zander, 1992) a n d intellec-
tual capital (Edvinsson & Malone, 1997; Stewart,
1997) remain largely focused on cognition. How-
ever, these fields share common ground with
organizational learning in recognizing the im-
portance of knowledge to the success of the en-
terprise. Quinn suggests that "looking beyond
mere product lines to a strategy built around
core intellectual or service competencies pro-
vides both a rigorously maintainable strategic
focus and long-term flexibility" (1992: 216). Re-
search in knowledge management and intellec-
tual capital informs organizational learning, but
it does not capture the ongoing cycle of action
taking a n d knowledge acquisition found in
learning theories.
There have been several reviews of the organ-
izational learning literature (Fiol & Lyles, 1985;
Huber, 1991; Levitf & March, 1988), but scholars
have not recognized the importance of under-
standing organizational learning from the per-
spective of strategic renewal. As noted in Table
1, few of the well-known organizational learning
frameworks (Daft & Weick, 1984; Huber, 1991;
March, 1991; March & Olsen, 1975; Nonaka &
Takeuchi, 1995; Senge, 1990; Watkins & Marsick,
1993) recognize the tension of strategic renewal.
Further, the frameworks vary in the degree they
address the other key premises.
In the following section we expand these key
premises by describing the 41 processes of or-
ganizational learning that link the levels, using
the well-known story of Apple Computer to illus-
trate these processes. We then discuss the dy-
namic nature of the 41 processes a s they relate to
fhe feed-forward a n d feedback processes of
learning. Finally, we present implications for
research a n d management.
THE 41 FRAMEWORK OF ORGANIZATIONAL
LEARNING
The 41 framework of organizational learning
contains four related (sub)processes—intuiting,
interpreting, integrating, a n d institutionaliz-
ing—that occur over three levels: individual,
group, and organization. The three learning lev-
els define the structure through which organiza-
tional learning takes place. The processes form
the glue that binds the structure together; they
are, therefore, a key facet of the framework. In-
tuiting a n d interpreting occur at the individual
level, interpreting and integrating occur at the
group level, and integrating and institutionaliz-
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 52S
TABLE 2
Learning/Renewal in Organizations: Four
Processes Through Three Levels
Level
Individual
Group
Process
Intuiting
Interpreting
Integrating
Inputs/Outcomes
Experiences
Images
Metaphors
Language
Cognitive map
Conversation/dialogue
Shared understandings
Mutual adjustment
Interactive systems
Routines
Organization Institutionalizing Diagnostic systems
Rules and procedures
ing occur at the organizational level (see Table
2). There are a sequence and progression to
these processes through the different levels, and
while there is some "spillover" from level to
level, not every process occurs at every level.
For example, intuition is a uniquely individ-
ual process. It may happen within a group or
organizational context, but the recognition of a
pattern or possibility comes from within an in-
dividual. Organizations do not intuit. This is a
uniquely human attribute that organizations do
not possess. Similarly, organizations do not in-
terpret. Interpreting has to do with refining and
developing intuitive insights. The development
of language, principally through an interactive
conversational process, is a basic interpretive
process. The proverbial person on a deserted
island could have an intuitive insight and begin
to make sense of it through an internal conver-
sational process (i.e., talking to one's self), but
the interpretive process is likely to be much
richer and more robust if the conversations and
interactions are with others. This process spans
the individual and group levels, but it does not
extend to the organizational level.
When actions take place in concert with other
members of a workgroup, the interpreting pro-
cess quite naturally blends into the integrating
process. Integrating entails the development of
shared understanding and the taking of coordi-
nated action by members of a workgroup. Ac-
tions that are deemed to be effective will be
repeated. Initially, the workgroup informally
makes this judgment about what actions should
be replicated. Eventually, fhe workgroup may
establish formal rules and procedures, and rou-
tines become embedded. The process of institu-
tionalizing occurs.
The process of institutionalizing is an organi-
zation-level phenomenon. Organizations, like
other social institutions, are socially constructed
(Berger & Luckmann, 1968). The routines and
rules that make up an enduring organization
exist independently of any one individual (al-
though individuals and their actions are af-
fected by these rules and routines).
The 41*8 Defined and Developed
We define the learning processes a s follows:
/nfui(ing is the preconscious recognition of the
pattern and/or possibilities inherent in a per-
sonal stream of experience (Weick, 1995b: 25).
This process can affect the intuitive individual's
actions, but it only affects others when they at-
tempt to (inter)act with that individual. Interpret-
ing is the explaining, through words and/or ac-
tions, of an insight or idea to one's self and to
others. This process goes from the preverbal to
the verbal, resulting in the development of lan-
guage. Integrating is the process of developing
shared understanding among individuals and
of taking coordinated action through mutual ad-
justment. Dialogue and joint action are crucial
to the development of shared understanding.
This process will initially be ad hoc and infor-
mal, but if the coordinated action taking is re-
curring and significant, it will be institutional-
ized. Institutionalizing is the process of ensuring
that routinized actions occur. Tasks are defined,
actions specified, and organizational mecha-
nisms put in place to ensure that certain actions
occur. Institutionalizing is the process of embed-
ding leaming that h a s occurred by individuals
and groups into the organization, and it includes
systems, structures, procedures, and strategy.
The four leaming processes operate over the
three levels. Because the processes naturally flow
from one into another, it is difficult to define pre-
cisely where one ends and the next begins. Quite
clearly, intuiting occurs at the individual level
and institutionalizing at the organizational level;
however, interpreting bridges the individual and
group levels, while integrating links the group
and organizational levels. Insights, the seeds of
526 Academy ol Management Review July
adaptiveness and exploration, begin wifh the in-
dividual but, if "successful," eventually become
embedded in the formal organization.
We describe the framework in a sequential
way, although there are necessarily many feed-
back loops among the levels, given the recursive
nature of the phenomenon (as we discuss in
subsequent sections). In the following discus-
sion we develop each of the 41 learning pro-
cesses in greater conceptual detail.
Intuiting
Scholars often assume that learning, whether
it be at the individual, group, or organization
level, is a conscious, analytical process. How-
ever, Underwood (1982) suggests that the links
between experience, knowledge, and conscious-
ness are more complex than generally assumed.
The subconscious is critical to understanding
how people come to discern and comprehend
something new, for which there was no prior
explanation. A theory of learning needs to be
able to address how this occurs. Accordingly,
the process of intuiting—a largely subconscious
process—is an important part of the framework
presented here.
At its most basic level, individual learning
involves perceiving similarities and differences—
patterns and possibilities. Although there are
many definitions of intuition, most involve some
sort of pattern recognition (Behling & Eckel,
1991). The expert and entrepreneurial views of
intuition are most closely aligned with the
framework presented here.
The expert view of intuiting is a process of (past)
pattem recognition. A highly sophisticated and
complex map enables the expert to perceive pat-
terns that novices cannot (Neisser, 1976). Prietula
and Simon (1989) suggest that becoming an expert
takes 10 years and requires the acquisition of
50,000 chunks of knowledge. Neisser (1976) has
used the example of chess masters to explain ex-
pert intuition. One must play a lot of chess, reflect
on past experiences, and leam about great plays;
all this and much more are required fo become a
grandmaster. But an interesting thing seems to
happen on the way to expertise. What once re-
quired conscious, deliberate, and explicit thought
no longer does. What once would have taken
much deliberation and planning becomes the ob-
vious thing to do. What has been learnt becomes
tacit knowledge (Polanyi, 1967).
The expert no longer has to think consciously
about action. Having been in the same, or simi-
lar, situations and recognizing the pattern, the
expert knows, almost spontaneously, what to do.
Indeed, if asked to explain their actions, experts
may be unable fo do so. While the pattern (and
associated actions) is familiar, the underlying
justification has receded from conscious mem-
ory. In a simple way expertise can be thought of
a s unconscious recollection. This helps explain
why expertise is so hard fo transfer from one
person to anofher. It is highly subjective; deeply
rooted in individual experiences; and very diffi-
cult to surface, examine, and explain.
Whereas expert intuition provides insight into
the important process of pattern recognition, en-
trepreneurial intuition has more to do with inno-
vation and change. No two situations are the
same, and patterns, while similar, are never
identical. The ability to make novel connections
and to discern possibilities is also key to intuit-
ing. "Entrepreneurs" are able to make these
novel connections, perceive new or emergent
relationships, and discern possibilities that
have not been identified previously. Whereas
expert intuition may be past pattern oriented,
entrepreneurial intuition is future possibility
oriented.
Expert intuition supports exploitation; entre-
preneurial intuition supports exploration. Entre-
preneurial intuiting generates new insights.
Koestler (1976) suggests that in the natural sci-
ences such insights, when they occur, happen
after the individual has had a long period of
immersion in the problem, followed by a brief
period of disassociation from the specifics of the
problem. Although this may be true for break-
through insights, more mundane acts of innova-
tion may have more humble beginnings (Ander-
son, 1992). Imagery and metaphor also seem to
be important in this process.
For entrepreneurs in a business situation,
there is always fhe question of whether these
individuals are intuitive or just lucky. However,
this question is difficult to answer because
novel, intuitive insights cannot be judged right
or wrong ex ante. They are simply possibilities.
It is rare to see a business entrepreneur able to
convert intuitive insight into business reality on
a consistent basis. Fred Smith perceived the po-
tential of reliable, overnight, small package de-
livery, and Federal Express emerged a s a very
successful business (Maisfer & Wyckoff, 1974).
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 527
He was unable to replicate this success with
Zapmail—an electronic mail service. There are
exceptions, however. Howard Head, the entre-
preneurial genius behind the Head metal ski,
was also the inventor of the Prince oversized
tennis racket.
The connection between quality of intuition
and commercial success is difficult fo make. In-
tuition is the beginning of new learning. Even-
tual commercial success is dependent upon ef-
fective learning at all levels—not simply the
original intuitive insights of the entrepreneur.
Intuiting, especially of the entrepreneurial
type, appears to be a largely subconscious pro-
cess. In fact, trying to force it to a conscious level
too soon may prevent it from h a p p e n i n g
(Watson, 1969). The outcome of individual intu-
iting is an inexplicable sense of the possible, of
what might be done. Entrepreneurial intuitions
are preverbal, and expert intuitions may be non-
verbal a s well. No language exists to describe
the insight or to explain the intended action.
Consequently, intuition may guide the actions
of the individual, but this intuition is difficult to
share with others (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995).
Imagery, sometimes called "visions," and meta-
phors aid the individual in his or her interpreta-
tion of the insight and in communicating it to
others.
Scholars have recognized metaphors a s a crit-
ical link in the evolution from individual intui-
tive insighf to shared interpretation. Individuals
use metaphors to help explain their intuition to
themselves and fo share it with others. As Tsou-
kas explains, "Metaphors involve the transfer of
information from a relatively familiar domain
. . . to a new and relatively unknown domain"
(1991: 568)—that is, from fhe known fo the un-
known, from that for which we share literal lan-
guage to that emerging insight for which lan-
guage does not yet exist. As such, metaphors
mark the beginning of the interpreting process.
Srivastava and Barrett provide the example of a
child trying to describe for the first time to his
mother that his foot is asleep. The child has no
literal language to relay this strange sensation:
In frustration, he says to his mother: "It feels like
there are stars hitting my foot." Having no avail-
able literal terms, the child associates a new
unfamiliar experience with one he understands.
He has a sparkling, glittering, tingling sensation
that seems to impact his foot from somewhere
outside his body. At the age of four he is unable to
say, "Mother, there is a certain numbness in my
foot which is a result of an inadequate supply of
blood which I have inadvertently seemed to cir-
cumvent" (1991: 568).
In this example the child perceives something
he has no words to describe, although the words
do exist, and no doubt his mother would explain
that the word "numbness" can be used to de-
scribe the sensation.
True innovators have a problem akin to the
child. They have a sensation—an insight into a
possibility—but they have no literal language
to describe it. Unfortunately, they do not have a
"parent" to provide that language; indeed, none
exists if the insight is truly novel. Individuals
employ metaphors to bound and describe the
insight. As Tsoukas elaborates.
In lay discourse, metaphors constitute an eco-
nomical way of relaying primarily experiential
information in a vivid manner, and they can be
used as variety reduction mechanism in situa-
tions where experience cannot be segmented and
imparted through literal language (1991: 567).
Indeed, for entrepreneurial insights, metaphors
may be the only language available for one to
communicate with another.
Early in the evolution of the personal com-
puter, Steve Jobs of Apple employed the "appli-
ance" metaphor. This metaphor evokes a whole
set of subsidiary images: easy to use, small,
affordable. Subsequently, more literal language
was used to name more precisely many of the
attributes associated with the original metaphor
(e.g., graphical user interface). This example
also points to the reciprocity between thinking
and acting that is inherent in fhe development
of language.
Naming also directs actions towards fhe object
(or image) you have named because it promotes
activity consistent with the related attribution
{i.e., the name or the metaphor) it carries. To
change the name of an object connotes changing
your relationship to it because when we name
something, we direct anticipations, expectations,
and evaluations toward it (Srivastava & Barrett,
1988: 34-35).
Using this reasoning, if lobs had used a different
metaphor to describe his initial insight, perhaps
the personal computer as "business assistant,"
it would have led to very different actions, and
Apple would have become a very different com-
pany. Early in a company's development, when
it is far from equilibrium, small differences in
528 Academy of Management Review July
the metaphors employed and the ways in which
conversations unfold and language develops
may ultimately result in great differences in
where the company ends up.
Interpreting
Whereas intuiting focuses on the subcon-
scious process of developing insights, interpret-
ing begins picking up on the conscious elements
of the individual learning process. Through the
process of interpreting, individuals develop cog-
nitive maps about the various domains in which
they operate (Huff, 1990). Language plays a piv-
otal role in the development of these maps,
since it enables individuals to name and begin
to explain what were once simply feelings,
hunches, or sensations. Further, once things are
named, individuals can make more explicit con-
nections among them.
Interpreting takes place in relation to a do-
main or an environment. The nature or texture of
the domain within which individuals and organ-
izations operate, and from which they extract
data, is crucial to understanding the interpretive
process. The precision of the language that
evolves will reflect the texture of the domain,
given the tasks being attempted. The well-
known example of fhe Inuit having over a dozen
different words for (various types of) snow illus-
trates the rich interaction between the fask do-
main and the sophistication of language. More-
over, a person with very rich a n d complex
cognitive maps of a domain, like the chess mas-
fer, will be able to see things and act in ways
that others cannot.
The cognitive map is affected by the domain
or environment, but it also guides what is inter-
preted from that domain. As Weick (1979) sug-
gests, people are more likely to "see something
when they believe it" rather than "believe it
when they see it." As a result, individuals will
interpret the same stimulus differently, based
on their established cognitive maps. The same
stimulus can evoke a different or equivocal
meaning for differenf people (Hambrick & Ma-
son, 1984; Walsh, 1988). This difference is not a
result of uncertainty about the stimulus. Uncer-
tainty is related to the quality of information. But
for any group of people, even high-qualify infor-
mation may be equivocal: it may hold multiple,
and often conflicting, meanings (Daft & Huber,
1987). Although equivocality is an issue in the
development of both individual understanding
a n d s h a r e d u n d e r s t a n d i n g within a group,
equivocal situations are often resolved through
a group interpretive process (Weick & Van Or-
den, 1990).
Just a s language plays a pivotal role in en-
abling individuals to develop fheir cognitive
maps, it is also pivotal in enabling individuals
to develop a sense of shared understanding.
Interpreting is a social activity that creates and
refines common language, clarifies images, and
creates shared meaning and understanding.
Equivocality is reduced through interpreting by
"shared observations and discussion until a
common grammar and course of action can be
agreed upon" (Daft & Weick, 1984: 291). Groups
will have an interpretive capacity related to the
makeup of the group and to the group dynamics
(Hurst et a l , 1989). As the interpretive process
moves beyond the individual and becomes em-
bedded within the workgroup, it becomes inte-
grafive. Individual interpretive processes come
together around a shared u n d e r s t a n d i n g of
what is possible, and individuals interact and
attempt to enact that possibility.
Integrating
Whereas the focus of interpreting is change in
the individual's understanding and actions, the
focus of integrating is coherent, collective ac-
tion. For coherence to evolve, shared under-
standing by members of the group is required. It
is through the continuing conversation among
members of the community and through shared
practice (Seely-Brown & Duguid, 1991) that
shared understanding or collective mind (Weick
& Roberts, 1993) develops and mutual adjust-
ment and negotiated action (Simons, 1991) take
place.
The evolution of language extends the pro-
cess of interpreting to interactions among indi-
viduals: the realm of workgroups, organizations,
communities, and even societies. Language de-
veloped through conversation and dialogue al-
lows the evolution of shared meaning for the
group. As Daft and Weick explain:
The distinctive feature... is sharing. A piece of
data, a perception, a cognitive map is shared
among managers.... Passing a startling obser-
vation among members, or discussing a puzzling
development enables managers to converge on
an approximate interpretation (1984: 285).
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 529
Language not only helps us learn—it pre-
serves, for better and for worse, what has been
learned. For an organization to learn and renew,
its language must evolve. Conversation can be
used not only to convey established meaning
but also to evolve new meaning.
Not all conversational styles are equally ef-
fective, however, for developing shared mean-
ing. Isaacs suggests that "dialogue is a disci-
pline of collective thinking and inquiry, a
process for transforming the quality of conver-
sation and, in particular the thinking that lies
beneath it" (1993: 25). Through dialogue the
group can evolve new and deeper shared under-
standings. This shared meaning can cause
those who have participated to more or less
spontaneously make mutual adjustments to
their actions. As Isaacs goes on to explain.
Dialogue proposes t h a t . . . some of the most pow-
erful forms of coordination may come through
participation in unfolding meaning, which might
even be perceived differently by different people.
A flock of birds suddenly taking flight from a tree
reveals fhe potential coordination of dialogue:
this movement all at once, a wholeness and lis-
tening together that permits individual differen-
tiation but is still highly interconnected {1993: 25).
The dialogue process attempts to convey both
the message and a deep interconnected mean-
ing. A consensual approach that attempts to get
agreement on the message without delving into
the underlying meaning(s) risks a groupthink
outcome (Janis, 1982).
As with the process of interpreting, the context
surrounding the integrating process is critical.
Seely-Brown and Duguid's (1991) notion of "com-
munities of practice" captures the importance of
the infegrative context. These authors and their
colleagues have been involved in ethnographic
research on workplace practices and suggest
that understanding and impacting leaming and
innovation require one to study and understand
the situation in which practice occurs. Neither
occurs ex situ:
Practice is essential to understanding work. Ab-
stractions detached from practice distort or ob-
scure intricacies of that practice. Without a clear
understanding of those intricacies and fhe role
they play, the practice itself cannot be well un-
derstood, engendered (through training) or en-
hanced (through innovation) (1391: 40).
Observations from these ethnographic studies
reveal that actual practice is not what is speci-
fied in manuals or necessarily what is taught in
classrooms. Rather, it is captured and promul-
gated by stories told by community members.
Storytelling is a significant part of the learning
process. Stories reflect the complexity of actual
practice rather than the abstractions taught in
classrooms. As stories evolve, richer under-
standing of the phenomenon is developed, and
new integrated approaches to solving problems
are created. Stories themselves become fhe re-
pository of wisdom—part of the collective mind/
memory (Weick & Roberts, 1993).
Institutionalizing
The process of institutionalizing sets organi-
zational learning apart from individual or ad
hoc group learning. The underlying assumption
is that organizations are more than simply a
collection of individuals; organizational learn-
ing is different from the simple sum of the learn-
ing of its members. Although individuals may
come and go, what they have learned as indi-
viduals or in groups does not necessarily leave
with them. Some leaming is embedded in the
systems, structures, strategy, routines, pre-
scribed practices of the organization, and in-
vestments in information systems and infra-
structure.
For new organizations there are few estab-
lished routines or structures: there is no organ-
izational memory. Often by the nature of their
small size, their open communication, and their
formation based on common interest and
dreams, individual and group learning domi-
nate in young organizations. As organizations
mature, however, individuals begin to fall into
patterns of interaction and communication, and
the organizations attempt to capture the pat-
terns of interaction by formalizing them.
This institutionalization is a means for organ-
izations to leverage fhe learning of the individ-
ual members. Structures, systems, and proce-
dures provide a context for interactions. Over
time, spontaneous individual and group learn-
ing become less prevalent, as the prior learning
becomes embedded in the organization and be-
gins to guide the actions and learning of organ-
izational members.
Organizations naturally outgrow fheir ability
to exclusively use spontaneous interactions to
interpret, integrate, and take coherent action.
Relationships become formalized. Coherent ac-
530 Academy of Management fleview July
tion is achieved with the help of plans and other
formal systems. If the plan produces favorable
outcomes, then the actions deemed to be consis-
tent with the plan become routines. There is a
need to ensure that the routines continue to be
carried out and that the organization produces
and performs. This is the role for what Simons
calls "diagnostic systems" (1991,1994). An organ-
ization uses these systems to regulate the day-
to-day routines of the business—to exploit the
current understanding of the business. Simons
also identifies another type of formal system he
calls "interactive." Organizations use interac-
tive systems to consider how the future can or
may be different from the past.
As one moves from the individual level of in-
tuiting/interpreting through group integrating to
organizational institutionalizing, the process of
learning is less fluid and incremental a n d be-
comes more staccato and disjointed. Generally,
that which becomes institutionalized in organi-
zations h a s received, at some point, a certain
degree of consensus or shared understanding
among the influential members of the organiza-
tion. Before a formal organizational system or
structure is established or changed, the modifi-
cation generally undergoes some process of
consideration. Once something is institutional-
ized, if usually endures for a period of time.
Changes in systems, structures, and routines
occur relatively infrequently in organizations;
a s a result, although the underlying processes of
intuiting, interpreting, and integrating are more
fluid and continual, significant changes in the
i n s t i t u t i o n a l i z e d organization typically a r e
punctuated. For this reason much organization-
al change is interpreted a s being radical or
transformational, rather than incremental, in
nature. However, even though fhe institutional
changes may a p p e a r disjointed, the underlying
learning processes of intuiting and interpreting
at the individual and group levels that result in
these changes may be more continuous.
Institutionalized learning cannot capture all
the ongoing learning at the individual a n d
group levels. It takes time to transfer learning
from individuals to groups and from groups to
the organization. As the environment changes,
the learning that has been institutionalized may
no longer fit the context; there may be a g a p
between what the organization needs to do a n d
what it h a s learned to do. As the g a p widens, the
organization places more reliance on individual
learning a n d initiative. For example, Seely-
Brown (1993) reports on studies that examined
the informal routines of e x p e r i e n c e d order
clerks in comparison with the formal institution-
alized system. Although the product of their
day's work gave the appearance that the clerks
had followed the formal routine, they had, in
fact, improvised in ways that proved more effi-
cient and effective.
Given that the environment is constantly
changing, the challenge for organizations is to
m a n a g e the tension between the embedded in-
stitutionalized learning from the past, which en-
ables it to exploit learning, and fhe new learn-
ing t h a t must be a l l o w e d to feed forward
through the processes of intuiting, interpreting,
and integrating. Although the 4rs have been
presented in a linear fashion for e a s e of expla-
nation, appreciating the iterative nature of the
processes is critical, a s we will discuss in a
subsequent section.
In the following section we present the Apple
story in a linear fashion to illustrate the 41 pro-
cesses. The focus of the story is on the explora-
tion side of strategic renewal. We then broaden
the discussion to examine the dynamic nature of
organizational learning.
Understanding the Learning Processes: The
Apple Story
The relationship between each of the levels
and processes perhaps can best be illustrated
by way of a story—that of Apple Computer.^ By
all accounts, Steve lobs, through an intuitive
process, had the insights upon which Apple was
founded, lobs perceived patterns and evolved
certain images'^ about possibilities, which de-
veloped into a metaphor (e.g.. the personal com-
puter a s an appliance—one in every home) that
guided Apple during its early years. These im-
ages were based, at least in part, upon his
unique experiences and cognitive orientation.
He provided much of the insight and energy that
were the genesis of Apple.
' This description is not represented as an accurate case
history. Rather, it is a story we use to help illustrate the
conceptual model; we do not employ it as empirical support
for the model.
^ The italicized words represent the key words used to
describe the inputs and outcomes of the processes as shown
in Table 2.
1999 Crossan, Lane, and Whife 531
But these initial images were necessarily
vague when it came to specific actions. At the
intuitive stage actions are improvised, rooted
more in feeling than thinking (Hurst et aL, 1989).
The language used to explain improvised action
is necessarily underdeveloped, vague, and im-
precise. On the basis of lobs' own experiences
and his perceptions of the events, along with
Wozniak's technical expertise, these men impro-
vised actions as they went along.
Language plays a pivotal role as insights be-
come more sophisticated and concrete through
the interpretive process. In part, it was through
the group process of dialogue and conversation
that Jobs' own understanding and individual
cognitive complexity were enhanced. Talking
and acting with others, developing words to de-
scribe what had been vague insights, and en-
acting these insights enabled a deeper meaning
to evolve (Bruner, 1990).
Many researchers talk about this evolution of
meaning in terms of cognitive complexity and
cognitive maps (Huff, 1990). Although one must
be careful with this metaphor, it is helpful to
think of Jobs initially navigating his chosen ter-
ritory more or less mapless, guided only by
some vague vision of what lay over the next hill.
As he, with others, experimented and explored
the territory, a mental picture or map slowly
emerged, with finer and finer levels of detail.
(This metaphor is only helpful to a point. Jobs
was not just exploring the territory—through his
actions he was helping to create or enact the
territory.)
As insightful as Jobs was, he could not accom-
plish his vision alone. He needed to involve oth-
ers. The conversation and dialogue, which
served to develop his understanding, also
helped to integrate the cognitive maps of the
group—to develop a shared understanding. Lan-
guage, which plays a critical role in the devel-
opment of individual maps, is essential as a
means of integrating ideas and negotiating ac-
tions with others. Through conversation, work-
groups identify areas of difference and agree-
ment, gain language precision, and develop a
shared understanding of their task domain.
They quite naturally, as a part of this process,
use their common language and fhe conversa-
tional process to negotiate mutual adjustments
to their actions. These adjustments are an inte-
gral part of the learning process. The assump-
tion is that a certain coherence of actions should
emerge from a shared understanding of the
business situation—that is, the emergent strat-
egy (Mintzberg, 1994).
But what is the context within which shared
understandings and mutual adjustments occur?
Early in an organization's life, as was the case
with Apple, these processes are largely informal
and spontaneous. As organizations grow larger
and more people are involved, informal interac-
tions do not suffice. What had happened more or
less spontaneously must now be arranged; what
had been an informal conversation over coffee
about the future of the company becomes a for-
mal planning process with in(erac(ive systems
(Simons, 1991, 1994).
The organization naturally outgrows its abil-
ity to exclusively use spontaneous interactions
to interpret, integrate, and take concerted ac-
tion. Relationships become formalized and rou-
fines develop. There is a need to ensure that the
routines continue fo be carried out and that the
organization produces and performs. This is the
role of diagnostic systems (Simons, 1991, 1994).
In the Apple situation John Sculley was
brought in, at least in part, to provide needed
systems, structures, and other formal mecha-
nisms. Individual and communal learning be-
came institutionalized in the hope that the
learning could be more systematically ex-
ploited. Institutionalization contributes to more
efficient operations, enabling the organization
to better deliver on the founder's original vision.
With Apple, however, it also may have hindered
the organization's ability to renew itself by intu-
iting, interpreting, and integrating emerging
patterns and new possibilities. Unable to realize
his new vision within Apple, Steve Jobs left to
start a new enterprise, appropriately called
NeXT.
Essentially, the process of institutionalizing
embeds learned behaviors that have worked in
the past into the routines of the organization.
Diagnostic systems develop ruies and proce-
dures to facilitate the repetition of routines. But
the process of institutionalizing also feeds back
by creating a context through which subsequent
events and experiences are interpreted. This
context may facilitate and/or impede the organ-
ization's ability to (re)inferpret and respond to
its environment. The Apple example, while use-
ful, is a simplification. In entrepreneurial
startup situations like Apple, there is originally
little or no past learning embedded in fhe formal
532 Academy of Management Review July
organization. Indeed, there is no formal organi-
zation. Established organizations do have past
learning embedded within them. As such, learn-
ing and renewal in these situations must deal
with this difference in context and its associated
challenges.
Organizational Learning As a Dynamic
Process
Organizational learning is a dynamic process.
Not only does learning occur over time and
across levels, but it also creates a tension be-
tween assimilating new learning (feed forward)
and exploiting or using what has already been
learned (feedback). Through feed-forward pro-
cesses, new ideas and actions flow from the
individual to fhe group to the organization lev-
els. Af the same time, what has already been
learned feeds back from the organization to
group and individual levels, affecting how peo-
ple act and think. The concurrent nature of the
feed-forward and feedback processes creates a
tension, which can be understood by arraying
the levels against one another, as shown in Fig-
ure 1. Doing so illustrates that, in addition to the
processes that feed forward learning from the
individual and groups to the organization,
learning that has been institutionalized feeds
back and impacts individual and group learn-
ing. The importance of these interactions can be
highlighted by two relationships that are espe-
cially problematic: i n t e r p r e t i n g - i n t e g r a t i n g
(feed forward) and institutionalizing-intuiting
(feedback).
Moving from interpreting to integrating (feed
forward) requires a shift from individual learn-
ing to learning among individuals or groups. It
entails faking personally constructed cognitive
maps and integrating them in a way that devel-
ops a shared understanding among the group
members. There are many challenges in chang-
ing an existing shared reality. The first is that
individuals need to be able to communicate,
through words and actions, their own cognitive
map. Since many aspects of cognitive maps are
tacit, communicating them requires a process of
surfacing and articulating ideas and concepts.
This process makes tacit knowledge explicit
(Polanyi, 19B7).
Assuming individuals can surface and articu-
late their maps, a second challenge arises from
HGURE I
Organizational Learning As a Dynamic Process
Individual
Group
Organizational
Individual Group Organizational
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 533
the collective interpretation of the maps. Making
something explicit does not necessarily mean
the understanding is shared. Imprecision of lan-
guage is complicated by cognitive maps that act
as unique filters on the communication: we tend
to "see/hear what we believe" rather than "be-
lieve what we see." The real test of shared un-
derstanding is coherent action. Yet, for novel
ideas, shared understanding may not evolve un-
less shared action or experimentation is at-
tempted. The learning perspective suggests that
leading with action, rather than bluntly focusing
on cognition, may provide a different migration
path fo shared understanding. As in experien-
tial learning (Crossan et al., 1995), action pro-
vides the opportunity to share a common expe-
rience, which may aid in the development of
shared understanding.
The second problematic interaction is be-
tween institutionalizing and intuiting (feed-
back). Institutionalization can easily drive out
intuition. Intuiting within established organiza-
tions with a high degree of institutionalized
learning requires what Schumpeter (1959) refers
to as "creative destruction"^—destroying, or at
least setting aside, the institutional order to en-
act variations thaf allow intuitive insights and
actions to surface and be pursued. This is ex-
tremely difficult because the language and
logic that form the collective mindset of the or-
ganization and the resulting investment in as-
sets present a formidable fortress of physical
and cognitive barriers to change. Further, mem-
bers of the organization must step back from
proven, objective successes and allow un-
proven, subjectively based experimentation.
One example of the tension and the potential
for resolution is in the resource allocation pro-
cess (institutionalized learning). Many resource
allocation processes inhibit fhe development of
new insights, given their emphasis on track
record and proven success (Bower, 1970;
Burgelman, 1983). However, some firms, such as
3M, have recognized this problem and have in-
stitutionalized a different resource allocation
process that provides funding for new projects,
and also holds the business accountable for
having a significant portion of the revenue de-
rived from new products (Hurst, 1995). The sys-
tem tries to ensure that exploitation (feedback)
does not drive out exploration (feed forward).
The tension between assimilating new leam-
ing (feed forward) and using what has already
been learned (feedback) arises because the in-
stitutionalized learning (what has already been
learned) impedes the assimilation of new learn-
ing. Fully assimilating new learning requires
the feed forward of leaming from the individual
and group to become institutionalized within
the organization. Utilizing what has been
learned is a feedback loop of institutionalized
learning from the organization to groups and
individuals. For example, rules and routines
that once captured the logic and learning of how
to facilitate learning at the individual level may
no longer apply in a changed circumstance, yet
the systems still focus an individual's energy
and attention in ways that impede the assimila-
tion and feed forward of new learning (Mintz-
berg, 1994). Or an organization structure that has
a strong impact on who talks to whom in the
organization may impede conversation that
could develop valuable new shared understand-
ings. Therefore, any theory of organizational
learning needs to recognize the levels, pro-
cesses, and dynamic nature of the learning pro-
cess itself that create a tension between the feed
forward and feedback of learning.
Conceiving of learning as a dynamic flow
raises the possibility that these flows can be
constrained. Consider for a moment the paral-
lels between production flow and learning flow.
Production flow must ensure that the level of
work-in-process inventories does not exceed the
capacity of any parf of the system to absorb and
process them. Concepts like throughput, capac-
ity utilization, cycle time, and bottlenecks have
aided our understanding of what it takes to bal-
ance a production line to ensure smooth flow. A
dynamic theory of organizational learning rec-
ognizes that there may be bottlenecks in fhe
ability of the organization to absorb (Cohen &
Levinthal, 1990) the feed forward of learning
from the individual to the group and organiza-
tion. Investment in individual learning and
pressures for new product innovation may be-
come stockpiled if the organization has limited
capacity to absorb the learning. However, in the
production process, work-in-process inventory
does not "care" whether it is stockpiled, whereas
in the learning process individuals (and their
ideas) do. As a result, individuals may become
frustrated and disenchanted, and may even
leave the organization.
A dynamic theory of organizational leaming
provides a means of understanding the funda-
534 Academy of Managemenf fievfew July
mental tensions of strategic renewal: the ten-
sion between exploration (feed forward) and ex-
ploitation (feedback). Although one may b e
t e m p t e d to e q u a t e o r g a n i z a t i o n a l l e a r n i n g
solely with the innovative feed-forward process,
in doing so one fails to recognize that the feed-
back process provides the means to exploit what
h a s been learned (Crossan & Sorrenti, 1987).
However, because learning that h a s become in-
stitutionalized at the organization level is often
difficult to change, it runs the risk of becoming
irrelevant and may even obstruct feed-forward
learning flows. This h a s led fo the call to liber-
ate organizations and destroy bureaucracy (Pin-
chot & Pinchot, 1993), yet bureaucracy (or insti-
tutionalization) is not necessarily n e g a t i v e .
Institutionalizing learning is necessary to reap
the ongoing benefits of what h a s already been
learned.
With the 41 framework we identify the flow of
learning between levels a n d the tension be-
tween feed-forward (exploration) and feed-back
(exploitation) processes a s fundamental chal-
lenges of strategic renewal. There are many fac-
tors that could facilitate and inhibit this process,
some of which are part of the institutionalized
learning itself (e.g., reward systems, information
systems, resource allocation systems, strategic
planning systems, and structure). However, in
the 41 model we recognize that ideas occur to
individuals a n d that individuals ultimately
share those ideas through a n integrating pro-
cess. It is the individuals, and the social pro-
cesses and group dynamics through which they
interact, that may facilitate or inhibit organiza-
tional leaming. One promising area for further
research is to examine the role of leadership
and management of the 41 learning process.
IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND
MANAGEMENT
The central contribution of this work is the 4rs
and the related feed-forward and feed-back pro-
cesses. Further, the interplay between the levels
and the processes reveals the tensions associ-
ated with strategic renewal. It is our hope that
this framework will stir a reaction in the organ-
izational leaming community and help scholars
research the links among the levels and the
tensions inherent in organizational learning.
The same questions that we as researchers
seek to answer form the basis of inquiry for
managers. Is there a satisfactory level of intui-
tive, innovative insights in the organization? Do
individuals have the motivation, understanding,
capability, and opportunity to interpret their en-
vironment? How do individual and group expe-
riences help to develop shared understanding?
How well do individual insights become shared,
integrated, and institutionalized in the organi-
zation? What impediments are there to integrat-
ing individual perspectives? How much of the
organization's intellectual capital resides in in-
dividual heads? Is there enough institutional-
ized learning? How does institutionalized learn-
ing facilitate or impede intuiting, interpreting,
and integrating? What is the nature of the inter-
play between the feed-forward and feedback
processes?
The responses to these questions need to take
into account the dynamic nature of organization-
al learning as it relates to strategic renewal.
Compartmentalization of the issues will lead to
a simplification that disguises the many essen-
tial challenges of the phenomenon. For exam-
ple, in the case of the first question, a simple
focus on intuiting may yield a better under-
standing of the individual processes of innova-
tion. However, such a focus will miss the tension
and, hence, challenge of feeding forward infui-
tive insights with the hope of interpreting, inte-
grating, and institutionalizing them, while con-
currently working within a setting where
institutionalized learning continues to posi-
tively impact upon the performance of the enter-
prise.
The question of whether individuals have fhe
motivation, understanding, capability, and op-
portunity to interpret their environment sug-
gests the need to examine more than just indi-
viduals. It requires an examination of fhe link
between interpreting and institutionalizing. In-
dividuals may be motivated and capable, but if
they turn their attention toward interpreting
things that have little impact, the organization
will reap few benefits from that learning. Fur-
thermore, even if individuals are interpreting
things of relevance, their learning needs to be
integrated and institutionalized to realize its fu-
ture value. This theory suggests it is not simply
a matter of transferring data, information, or
knowledge—it is a matter of organizational
learning.
The role of experience in the development of
shared understanding reinforces the learning
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 535
premise that cognition (knowledge, understand-
ing, and beliefs) and action (behaviors) are
tightly intertwined, and changes in knowledge
do not necessarily lead to changes in action. In
contrast to knowledge management and intel-
lectual capital, which focus management and
research attention on cognition, this view of or-
ganizational learning acknowledges the rich in-
terrelationship between cognition and action.
The foregoing examples emphasize the need
to pursue questions of organizational learning
with a dynamic perspective. We encourage re-
searchers and managers to extend their think-
ing to consider how differenf parts of the organ-
izational learning system impact one another.
This framework should serve as a map to help
researchers and managers expand their hori-
zons.
While this framework should encourage and
assist the pursuit of a more holistic understand-
ing of organizational learning, there are two
particular areas of research that will help ad-
vance theory. The first is understanding the
mechanisms that enhance or restrict the stocks
and flows of learning. Here we have suggested
that learning processes can be compared to pro-
duction processes. This point should generate
substantial dialogue, because it begins to ques-
tion some of the traditional leverage points for
organizational learning. For example, continued
investment in individual and even group learn-
ing may be counterproductive if fhe organiza-
tion does not have the capacity to absorb or
utilize it. If this is the case, future research in
organizational learning needs to move from fhe
reasonably well-developed undersfanding of in-
dividual- and group-level learning to under-
standing the flows of learning (feed forward and
feedback) between fhe levels.
Yet, all intuitive insights should not, and can-
not, be immediately interpreted, integrated, and
institutionalized. What enables the organization
to "separate the wheat from the chaff"—the
good from the bad—as ideas and practices de-
velop and are refined over time? We have sug-
gested that the strategic context helps to frame
things that are more or less relevant, but the
decision rules, criteria, and processes are not so
clear. For example, if 3M had framed the discov-
ery of a glue that does not stick in a narrow
strategic context, we would not have reaped the
benefits of Post-It® Notes.
A second area that will advance theory is an
understanding of how to reconcile the tension
between exploitation and exploration—be-
tween continuity and change. The 41 model di-
rects our attention to the interplay of these pro-
cesses, but it does not specifically address how
organizations deal with this tension. Although a
few management scholars have considered this
problem (Hurst, 1995; Miller, 1990; Pascale, 1990),
answers have proven elusive. This important
question merits further consideration and inves-
tigation.
This dynamic framework of organizational
learning will place significant demands on both
researchers and managers. It requires capabil-
ity for cross-level examination with a critical
eye for the tensions inherent in the feed-forward
and feedback processes. It requires the capabil-
ity to link human resource management, strate-
gic management, and the management of infor-
mation technology and systems as a means to
facilitate the flow of learning. Although such
research poses challenges, the potential bene-
fits are significant. Strategic renewal is one of
the central challenges of every organization.
This dynamic process of organizational leaming
could yield important insights into strategic re-
newal.
In summary, in this article we have pushed in
the direction of advancing a theory of organiza-
tional learning by describing an organizational
learning framework that incorporates the dy-
namic multilevel nature of the phenomenon and
captures the rich interplay between process and
level. This framework should provide clarity,
promote dialogue, foster convergence (Pfeffer,
1993), and encourage new directions in research
that begin to examine organizational learning
flows that enable strategic renewal.
REFERENCES
Anderson, I. V. 1992. Weirder than fiction: The reality and
myths oi creativity. Academy of Management Executive,
6(4): 40-47.
Argyris, C , & Schon, D. A. 1978. Organizationa/ learning: A
theory of action perspective. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
Argyris, C, & Schon, D. A. 1996. Organizatfona/ learning It:
Theory, method, and practice. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
Bacharach, S. 1989. Organizational theories: Some criteria
for evaluation. Academy of Management Review, 14:
496-515.
536 .Academy of Management Review luly
Behling, O., & Eckel, H. 1991. Making sense out oi intuition.
Academy of Management Executive, 5(1): 46-54.
Berger, P., 8E Luckmann, T. 1966. The social consfruc(ion of
reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Garden
City, NY: Anchor Books.
Bower, J. L. 1970. Managing the resource allocation process.
Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Bruner, J. 1990, Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Burgelman, R. A. 1983. A model of the interaction of strategic
behavior, corporate context and the concept oi strategy.
Academy of Management Review, 8: 61-70.
Cangelosi, V. E., & Dill, W. R. 1965. Organizational learning
observations: Toward a theory. Administrative Science
Quarteriy, 10: 175-203.
Cohen. W. M., & Levinthal, D. A. 1990. Absorptive capacity: A
new perspective on learning and innovation. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 35: 128-152.
Conner. K., & Prahalad. C. 1998. A resource-based theory of
the firm: Knowledge versus opportunism, Organizafion
Science, 7: 469.
Crossan. M., & Guatto, T. 1996. Organizational leaming re-
search profile. Journal of Organizational Change Man-
agement, ^{l): 107-112,
Crossan. M.. Lane, H., White, R. E., Djurieldt, L. 1995. Organ-
izational learning: Dimensions for a theory. Interna-
tional Journal of Organizationai Analysis, 3: 337-360.
Crossan. M., 8f Sorrenti. M. 1997. Making sense of improvisa-
tion. In A. Huff & J. Walsh (Eds.), Advances in strategic
management, vol. 14: 155-180. Stamford, CT: JAI Press.
Dait. R. L.. 8f Huber, G. 1987. How organizations leam: A
communication framework. Research in the Sociology of
Organizations. 5(2): 1-36.
Daft, R, L., & Weick, K, E. 1984. Toward a model of organiza-
tions a s interpretation systems. Academy oi Manage-
ment Review, 9: 284-295.
Davenport, T., & Prusak, L. 1997, Informafion ecoJogy: Mas-
tering the information and knowledge environment.
New York: Oxford University Press.
DeGeus, A. 1988. Planning as leaming. Harvard Susiness
fleview, 66{March-April): 70-74.
Duncan, R, B,, & Weiss, A. 1979. Organizational leaming:
Implications for organizational design. Research in Or-
ganizationai BehaWor, 1(4): 75-124.
Edvinsson, L,, 8E Malone, M, 1997. /nteiJecfuai capital. New
York: Harper Business.
Fiol, C. M., & Lyles, M. A. 1985, Organizational learning.
Academy of Management Review, 10: 803-813.
Foss, N. 1996. Knowledge-based approaches lo the theory of
the firm: Some critical comments. Organization Science.
7: 470-476.
Grant, R. M. 1996. Toward a knowledge-based theory ol the
firm. Strategic Management/ournal, 17: 109-122.
Hambrick. D., & Mason, P. A. 1984. Upper echelons: The
organization as a reflection oi its top managers. Acad-
emy of Management Review, 9: 193-206,
Hedberg. B. 1981. How organizations leam and unlearn. In
P. C. Nystrom & W. H, Starbuck (Eds.). Handbook ol
organizational design: 3-27. New York: Oxiord Univer-
sity Press.
Huber, G, P. 1991. Organizational learning: The contributing
processes and the literatures. Organization Science. 2:
88-115.
Huff, A. S. 1990, Mapping strategic thought. New York: Wiley.
Hurst. D. K. 1995. Crisis and renewal. Boston: Harvard Busi-
ness School Press.
Hurst, D. K., Rush, J. C , & White, R. E. 1989. Top
management
teams and organizational renewal. Strategic Manage-
ment Journal, 10: 87-105.
Isaacs, W. H. 1993. Dialogue, collective thinking, and
organizational learning. Organizational Dynamics.
22(2): 24-39,
Janis, I. 1982. Groupfhinfc Psychologica/ studies of policy
decisions and fiascoes. Boston: Houghton Mifilin,
Kim, D. H. 1993. The link between individual and organiza-
tional learning, Sloan Management Review, 33(1): 37-50.
Koestler, A, 1976. The act of creation. London: Hutchinson.
Kogut. B.. & Zander, U. 1992, Knowledge oi the firm, combi-
native capabilities, and the replication oi technology.
Organization Science. 3: 383-397.
Levitt. B., & March. J. G. 1988. Organizational learning.
Annuai Review ol Sociology, 14: 319-340.
Maister. D,, & Wyckofi, D. 1974, Federal Express (A) & (B).
Case No. 9-674-093-094. Boston: Harvard Business School
Case Services.
March, J. G. 1991. Exploration and exploitation in organiza-
tion learning. Organization Science, 2: 71-87,
March, J, G., & Olsen, ]. P. !975. Organizational learning
under ambiguity. European Journal of Policy Review,
3(2): 147-171.
Miller. D, 1990. The Icarus paradox: How exceptional compa-
nies bring about their own downfall. New York: Harper
Business,
Mintzberg, H, 1994. The rise and fall of strategic planning.
New York: Free Press,
Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Free-
man.
Nonaka. L, & Takeuchi, H, 1995. The knowledge creating
company. Oxiord, England: Oxford University Press.
Pascale, R, T. 1990, Managing on the edge: How the smartest
companies use conflict fo stay ahead. New York: Simon
and Schuster.
Pfeffer, I. 1993. Barrier to the advance of organizational sci-
ence: Paradigm development as a dependent variable.
Academy of Management Review. 18(4): 599-620,
Pinchot, G.. & Pinchot. E. 1993. The end of bureaucracy and
the rise of the intelligent organization, San Francisco:
Berrett-Koehler,
1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 537
Polanyi, M. 1967. The tacit dimension. London: Routledge.
Prietula. M. J., & Simon, H. A. 1989. The experts in your midst.
Harvard Business Review, 61Ganuary-February): 120-124.
Quinn. J, B, 1992, /nteiiigent enterprise: A tnowiedge and ser-
vice based paradigm lor industry. New York: Free Press,
Schumpeter, I. A. 1959. The theory ol economic development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press,
Seely-Brown, L 1993. Thinking, working, and leaming. In
M, M. Crossan, H. W. Lane, J, Rush, & R. E. White (Eds.),
Learning in organizations monograph: 81-99. London,
Ontario: Western Business School.
Seely-Brown, I. & Duguid, P, 1991. Organizational learning
and communities of practice: Toward a uniiied view of
working, learning and innovation. Organization Sci-
ence, 2: 40-57.
Senge, P. 1990. The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the
learning organization. New York: Doubleday/Currency,
Shrivastava, P. 1983. A typology ot organizational leaming
systems. Journal ol Management Studies. 20(1): 7-28,
Simon, H. A. 1991. Bounded rationality and organizational
leaming. Organization Science. 2: 125-134.
Simons, R. 1991, Strategic orientation and top management
attention to control systems. Strategic Management
Journal 12: 49-62.
Simons, R. 1994, How new top managers use control systems
as levers of strategic renewal. Strategic Management
Journal 15: 169-189,
Srivastava, S., & Barrett, F. J. 1988. The transforming nature oi
metaphors in group development: A study in group the-
ory. Human Relations. 41: 31-64.
Stata, R. 1989. Organizational learning: The key to man-
agement innovation. Sloan Management Review,
30{3): 63-74.
Stewart, T. A. 1997. JnfeJJectua/ capital- The wealth of na-
tions. New York: Doubleday/Currency.
Sutton. R., & Staw, B, M. 1995. What theory is not. Adminis-
trative Science QuarteWy. 40: 371-384,
Tsoukas, H, 1991. The missing link: A transformational view
oi metaphors in organizational science. Academy of
Management Review, 16: 566-585.
Underwood, B. J. 1982. Studies in learning and memory:
Selected papers. New York: Praeger.
Watkins, K. E., & Marsick, W. I. 1993, Scuipting the leaming
organization. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Walsh. J. P. 1988. Selectivity and selective perception: An
investigation of managers' beliel structures and infor-
mation processing. Academy of Management Journal
31:873-896.
Watson. I. D. 1969, The double helix. New York: Mentar
Books.
Weick, K. 1979. The sociai psychoiogy of organizing. Reading,
MA.: Addison-Wesley.
Weick, K, 1995a. What theory is not, theorizing is. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 40: 385-390.
Weick, K. E. 1995b. Sensemaitingin organizations. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage,
Weick, K.. & Roberts, K. 1993. Collective mind and organiza-
tional reliability: The case of flight operations in an
aircraft carrier deck. Adminisfrafiv© Science Quarterly,
38: 357-381,
Weick, K.. & Van Orden, P. W. 1990. Organizing on a global
scale: A research and teaching agenda. Human Re-
source Management, 29: 49-61.
Whetton, D. A. 1989. What constitutes a theoretical contribu-
tion? Academy of Management Review, 14: 490-495.
Mary M, Crossan is an associate professor in general
management at the Richard Ivey
School ol Business, She received her MBA and Ph.D. in
business policy from the
University of Western Ontario. Her research focuses on
organizational learning and
improvisation for strategic renewal.
Henry W. Lane holds the Fred and Darla Brodsky Chair in
International Business at
Northeastern University in Boston. Until recently, he was at the
Richard Ivey School of
Business. He received his DBA in organizational behavior from
the Harvard Business
School. His research interests are intemationa] business and
organizational leaming
and strategic renewal,
Roderick E. White is an associate professor in general
management at the Richard
Ivey School of Business, He received his DBA and MBA from
Harvard University and
his honors bachelor of arts (business) from the University of
Western Ontario. His
research interests focus on the renewal of mature organizations
and the evolution of
sociai structure.
Running head: POLITICAL ROLES IN NATURAL DISASTER
MANAGEMENT
1Political Roles in Natural Disaster Management
Political Roles in Natural Disaster Management
Student name:
Course:
Instructor name:
Month, day, year:
Abstract
Natural disaster management is a fundamental act in the US
both in economic, social-cultural, and political dimensions.
Researchers and policymakers record that due to Hurricane
Agnes and nuclear accidents in the United States of America, a
rise to government participation at local and federal was
evidence through the formation of the Federal Emergency
Management Agency (FEMA) (Mener A, 2007). The purpose of
this paper is to review and political analysis roles in planning,
preparation, and response to a natural disaster.
Introduction
The impact and occurrence of natural disasters are on an
administrative capacity. Despite the autonomy of genetic
distance relationships with a political standpoint, human
political actions display myriad ways in which political
intervention leads to planning, preparation, and response to
such circumstances beyond one's control (Cohen C and Weker
E, 2008). Political designs and models are vital in disaster
control, mitigation, and control over a given geographical
jurisdiction and administration capability of political power in
natural disasters such as earthquake and hurricane mitigation
and monitoring. Despite an extensive literature review on the
political contribution in disaster management, there is a need
for further research to comprehend planning, preparation, and
response to natural disasters from a political perspective.
Planning
Disaster planning is a systematic approach to natural disaster
management and control. Politics play myriad roles in planning
for natural disaster mitigation and control (Choudhury Z, 2013).
Politics shape the systematic planning of natural disaster
management prevention, alerts, response, and recovery of the
losses. The federal and local government authority plays roles
to control natural disaster in various approaches.
Planning of inspection methods and alert preposition are vital in
the preparation of the natural disaster mitigation.
Administration formulated purposes and means to regulate and
control potential natural hazard occurrence. Similarly, in
planning to natural disaster counteraction, politics administer
routinely monitor measures and precaution in the future
presence of non-made made disaster. Furthermore, federal and
local authorities participate in disaster planning through
installation of automatic and computerized alarms in water
disaster or hurricane to ensure timely evacuation of the victims.
Moreover, the political setting of a region, accommodate
mandate and administration privilege to create rules and
regulation in planning to control natural disaster management.
For instance, the creation of FEMA to respond to emergencies
in managing natural disaster impacts at local and national levels
of political environment establish guideline in panning to
natural disaster control and mitigation. Planning creates a
groundwork for natural disaster mitigation approaches and
analysis.
Preparation
In emergency control and management, legislators are at the
forefront to formulate policies and regulations for natural
disaster mitigations (DeLeo R, 2018). Government institutions
and provide a framework for natural disaster preparedness,
influencing the participation of non-governmental organizations
on the mitigation process. There are various strategies in which
politics play roles in Preparation to natural disaster control.
Firstly, the provision of policies and regulation by local and
federal government establishes a ground plan for natural
disaster mitigation. Secondly, astigmatic selection and alertness
is a strategy useful for politics intervention and participation in
natural disaster mitigation and control. Furthermore, political
leadership skills and flexibility establish a significant role in
predation to natural disaster control and management. Political
perspective is, therefore, fundamental in preparation for natural
disaster management in multiple methods; thus, a subject of
concern in politics of natural disaster management across the
globe.
Response
Response to a natural disaster is an integral segment in disaster
management, showing positive response to hurricane cases in
the United States of America. For instance, in Texas and
Florida's example of a hurricane, the reports, analysis, and
documentation indicates that the response was faster and
reliable (Wilson C, Singer P, Creary M and Greer S, 2019). In
the provision of safe, credible, and swift response, politics
contributes to various systematic approaches to impact natural
disaster management. Arousal of the disaster management team
is a prominent response strategy to natural disaster control.
Similarly, politics play a role in the identification and
examination of the crisis in disaster management. Furthermore,
identification of tasks such as alert, financial provision, and
practical mechanism are variables of explicit influence from
federal and regional governments. The politics role in response
to natural disasters is vital since it provides a fundamental
segment of recovery and compensation of the victims and
resentment t in an en event of deadliest natural disasters.
Conclusion
Despite the absence of a direct correlation between natural
disasters and politics, human factors, and involvement in
controlling hazards and crises in a natural disaster are a direct
influence of the political answerable and implementation.
Therefore politics play fundamental roles in planning,
preparation, and response to natural disaster occurrence,
management, and control. Disaster management is a systematic
approach that should simulate and implement methodical
political, economic, and social perspectives on risk and natural
disaster mitigation and prevention.
Work Cited
Choudhury, Z. (2013). Politics of Natural Disaster: How
Governments Maintain Legitimacy in the Wake of Major
Disasters, 1990 – 2010. Retrieved on February 15, 2020, from
https://ir.uiowa.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=5618&context=
etdDeLeo, R. (2018). Political Science Perspectives. Retrieved
on February 15, 2020, from
https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-319-92722-0_6
Mener, A. (2007). Disaster Response in the United States of
America: An Analysis of the Bureaucratic and Political History
of a Failing System. Retrieved from:
http://www.unm.edu/~marivera/524%20readings%201/Mener_D
isaster%20Response%20in%20the%20US.pdf
Wilson, C., Singer, P., Creary, M, and Greer S. (2019).
Quantifying inequities in US federal response to hurricane
disaster in Texas and Florida compared with Puerto Rico.
Retrieved from:
https://gh.bmj.com/content/bmjgh/4/1/e001191.full.pdf

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Semelhante a SECTION 2DirectionsImagine you are a self-help speaker. Wr.docx

Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docxBelow are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
simonlbentley59018
 
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docxThe proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
write5
 
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
intel-writers.com
 
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docxQuality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
amrit47
 
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
The Mackay School
 
Links to an external What type of speech.docx
Links to an external What type of speech.docxLinks to an external What type of speech.docx
Links to an external What type of speech.docx
write31
 
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docxThis assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
shandicollingwood
 
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about foThis week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
rochellwa9f
 
HTY 110HA Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
HTY 110HA  Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions HTY 110HA  Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
HTY 110HA Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
NarcisaBrandenburg70
 
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docxDirections1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
robert345678
 
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docxMock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
roushhsiu
 
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docxCanons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
hacksoni
 
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-GuideWritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
Jas Mahay
 

Semelhante a SECTION 2DirectionsImagine you are a self-help speaker. Wr.docx (20)

Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docxBelow are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
Below are the guidelines for the Informative Speech. Refresh your me.docx
 
Principles-in-Speech-Writing.pdf
Principles-in-Speech-Writing.pdfPrinciples-in-Speech-Writing.pdf
Principles-in-Speech-Writing.pdf
 
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docxThe proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
The proposition of this speech may be one of or.docx
 
Preparing your podcast
Preparing your podcastPreparing your podcast
Preparing your podcast
 
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
12 Reasons for Rhetorical Analysis updated 2023 intel tuesday docu 5.docx
 
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docxQuality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
Quality Resources CIS 110 – Introduction to Computers Pa.docx
 
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
Public speaking 2011 british week 2011
 
LIB300 Week 5 persuasive messages
LIB300 Week 5 persuasive messagesLIB300 Week 5 persuasive messages
LIB300 Week 5 persuasive messages
 
Links to an external What type of speech.docx
Links to an external What type of speech.docxLinks to an external What type of speech.docx
Links to an external What type of speech.docx
 
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docxThis assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
This assignment should include the outline of PERSUASIVE SPEECH.docx
 
CHAPTER 5 [Autosaved].pptx
CHAPTER 5 [Autosaved].pptxCHAPTER 5 [Autosaved].pptx
CHAPTER 5 [Autosaved].pptx
 
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about foThis week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
This week, we have learned in Chapter 4 of the textbook about fo
 
Chapter9
Chapter9Chapter9
Chapter9
 
HTY 110HA Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
HTY 110HA  Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions HTY 110HA  Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
HTY 110HA Module 8 Presentation Project Instructions
 
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docxDirections1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
Directions1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like.docx
 
Persuasive Speech.pdf
Persuasive Speech.pdfPersuasive Speech.pdf
Persuasive Speech.pdf
 
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docxMock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
Mock Public Address Speech Write-up Mock Speech Write-up.docx
 
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docxCanons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
Canons of Rhetoric Speech AnalysisSo what are the characteristi.docx
 
How To Build A Successful Career In Public Speaking
How To Build A Successful Career In Public SpeakingHow To Build A Successful Career In Public Speaking
How To Build A Successful Career In Public Speaking
 
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-GuideWritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
WritingProcessForBusinessAnalysis-Guide
 

Mais de rtodd280

Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docxSebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
rtodd280
 
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docxSeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
rtodd280
 
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docxSeattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
rtodd280
 
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docxSeba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
rtodd280
 
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docxSearching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
rtodd280
 
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docxSearching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
rtodd280
 
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docxSearch WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
rtodd280
 
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docxSearch the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
rtodd280
 

Mais de rtodd280 (20)

Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docxSebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
Sebika Darnal Devarani ArumugamENGL-1302-51008 Mar -03- 2020.docx
 
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docxSeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
SeaWorldBusch GardensPrimates4-8 Classroom Activities.docx
 
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docxSeattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
Seattle Take Home Final Exam h There are Four Different Versio.docx
 
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docxSeba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
Seba Alwayel517 Catawba circle Columbia, SC 29201 · 8032372950.docx
 
Searching for the Wrong-Eyed Jesus, clip 1, Due Saturday, February .docx
Searching for the Wrong-Eyed Jesus, clip 1, Due Saturday, February .docxSearching for the Wrong-Eyed Jesus, clip 1, Due Saturday, February .docx
Searching for the Wrong-Eyed Jesus, clip 1, Due Saturday, February .docx
 
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docxSearching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
Searching Databases (APA 7 format and at least 3-4 references)Wh.docx
 
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docxSearching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
Searching for help with this For this two-part assessment,.docx
 
Search the Internet for an article where physical security failed.docx
Search the Internet for an article where physical security failed.docxSearch the Internet for an article where physical security failed.docx
Search the Internet for an article where physical security failed.docx
 
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find the.docx
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find the.docxSearch Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find the.docx
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find the.docx
 
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find.docx
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find.docxSearch Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find.docx
Search Yahoo Finance orand any other credible source(s) to find.docx
 
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docxSearch WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
Search WarrantAffidavit Project Paper 3-6 pages, double spa.docx
 
Search the internet for best practices for developing technolo.docx
Search the internet for best practices for developing technolo.docxSearch the internet for best practices for developing technolo.docx
Search the internet for best practices for developing technolo.docx
 
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing a Biometrics Usage .docx
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing a Biometrics Usage .docxSearch the Internet for articles on Implementing a Biometrics Usage .docx
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing a Biometrics Usage .docx
 
Search the Human Rights Watch website for examples of human ri.docx
Search the Human Rights Watch website for examples of human ri.docxSearch the Human Rights Watch website for examples of human ri.docx
Search the Human Rights Watch website for examples of human ri.docx
 
Search the Internet and locate a victim impact statement (vide.docx
Search the Internet and locate a victim impact statement (vide.docxSearch the Internet and locate a victim impact statement (vide.docx
Search the Internet and locate a victim impact statement (vide.docx
 
Search the Internet and watch the first 6 minutes of PBS Idea Ch.docx
Search the Internet and watch the first 6 minutes of PBS Idea Ch.docxSearch the Internet and watch the first 6 minutes of PBS Idea Ch.docx
Search the Internet and watch the first 6 minutes of PBS Idea Ch.docx
 
Search the Internet for any short article related to our topics this.docx
Search the Internet for any short article related to our topics this.docxSearch the Internet for any short article related to our topics this.docx
Search the Internet for any short article related to our topics this.docx
 
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docxSearch the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
Search the internet and learn about the cases of nurses Julie Th.docx
 
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing Biometrics To.docx
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing Biometrics To.docxSearch the Internet for articles on Implementing Biometrics To.docx
Search the Internet for articles on Implementing Biometrics To.docx
 
Search the Web for reports of cloud system failures. Write a 3 to 4 .docx
Search the Web for reports of cloud system failures. Write a 3 to 4 .docxSearch the Web for reports of cloud system failures. Write a 3 to 4 .docx
Search the Web for reports of cloud system failures. Write a 3 to 4 .docx
 

Último

Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Chris Hunter
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
QucHHunhnh
 

Último (20)

Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 

SECTION 2DirectionsImagine you are a self-help speaker. Wr.docx

  • 1. SECTION 2 Directions Imagine you are a self-help speaker. Write an 800 WORD PERSUASIVE SPEECH on “How to eliminate negativity in your life.” 1. Think about a speech that you would like to present on a topic of your choice. 2. Complete the following template, including all parts. Fill out all cells using complete sentences, aiming for three complete sentences for each cell of the template (if acceptable) 3. In section 4 Reflection write 5 sentences. 4. Consider your audience, purpose, and thesis and complete Part 1 of the template. 5. Utilize your resources, the internet or a local library to find three credible sources that are relevant to your speech and complete Part 2 of the template. 6. Read through your sources to identify five pieces of evidence that support your thesis and complete Part 3 of the template. Be sure to use at least three different types of sources (example, statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony). 7. And then answer all the questions in the attached form. PART 1: TOPIC Purpose What are you hoping to achieve with this speech?
  • 2. Audience Who will be in your audience? What do you know about them? Thesis What is the key message of your speech? PART 2: SOURCES SOURCE 1 Source Title Author(s) Name Retrieved from Where did you get this source? Include location, book, periodical, url, etc. Age How recent is the data or information presented in your source? When was the source published? If the source is a website, when was the website last updated? Depth
  • 3. What does the source cover? How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much depth is the topic covered in? How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose, audience, and message? Author How is the author qualified to speak on the topic? What is the author's purpose? What biases might they have? Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported by cited facts? Money Who funds the website or publication? Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items? What is the reputation of the website or publication? SOURCE 2 Source Title Author(s) Name
  • 4. Retrieved from Where did you get this source? Include location, book, periodical, url, etc. Age How recent is the data or information presented in your source? When was the source published? If the source is a website, when was the website last updated? Depth What does the source cover? How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much depth is the topic covered in? How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose, audience, and message? Author How is the author qualified to speak on the topic? What is the author's purpose? What biases might they have? Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported by cited facts?
  • 5. Money Who funds the website or publication? Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items? What is the reputation of the website or publication? SOURCE 3 Source Title Author(s) Name Retrieved from Where did you get this source? Include location, book, periodical, url, etc. Age How recent is the data or information presented in your source? When was the source published? If the source is a website, when was the website last updated?
  • 6. Depth What does the source cover? How well-researched and detailed is the source? How much depth is the topic covered in? How is this content and level of detail relevant to your purpose, audience, and message? Author How is the author qualified to speak on the topic? What is the author's purpose? What biases might they have? Is the information the author's opinion alone or is it supported by cited facts? Money Who funds the website or publication? Are there items endorsed or for sale? If so, what items? What is the reputation of the website or publication?
  • 7. PART 3: SUPPORT SUPPORT #1 Support What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…” Type of Support What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony? Relevance How does this piece of support reinforce your message? SUPPORT #2 Support What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…” Type of Support What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony?
  • 8. Relevance How does this piece of support reinforce your message? SUPPORT #3 Support What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…” Type of Support What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony? Relevance How does this piece of support reinforce your message? SUPPORT #4 Support What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy,
  • 9. definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…” Type of Support What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony? Relevance How does this piece of support reinforce your message? SUPPORT #5 Support What is the piece of support (example, statistic, analogy, definition, visual, story, testimony) that you are going to use in your speech? E.g. “50% of all American voters…” Type of Support What type of support is it? Is it an example, a statistic, an analogy, a definition, a visual, a story, or a testimony? Relevance
  • 10. How does this piece of support reinforce your message? PART 4: REFLECTION What have you learned from completing this activity? Section 3 Directions 1. Think about a persuasive speech that you would like to present on a topic of your choice. The speech can be for any context and any length, but it must be persuasive. If your speech for Unit 1 or Unit 2 was persuasive, you can use the same topic, or you can choose a new one if you prefer.
  • 11. 2. See the list of example speech occasions and purposes for inspiration, if needed. 3. Plan your speech, considering what your introduction, main points, and conclusion will include. 4. Organize your speech, following the structure of Monroe’s Motivated Sequence. Your speech should include an introduction, body, and conclusion. The introduction should contain your key message. The body should cover your main topics and support to back up your main points. Make sure that all support is relevant and from credible sources. Your conclusion should summarize your main points and provide a call to action. 5. Create notes or bullet points that you can refer to while presenting your speech. 6. Practice presenting your speech. Aim for a speech that is 3 to 5 minutes in length. 7. Before filming, review the rubric to ensure that you understand how you will be evaluated. 8. Film yourself presenting the speech. Be sure that you can be easily seen and heard, and direct your speech to the camera. 9. Review your video to ensure that you can be seen and heard. Refilm as needed. 10. Review the checklist and requirements to ensure that your Touchstone is complete. 11. Upload your video using the blue button at the top of this page. Section 4 Directions 1. Review the video of the persuasive speech that you delivered in Unit 3 as well as the feedback that you have received. 2. Take notes as you review the video and the feedback, considering edits that you should make to your speech. 3. Based on your notes, revise your speech, adjusting the content, language, and approach to delivery as needed.
  • 12. 4. Think of an effective way to incorporate at least one visual aid that will be relevant to your purpose and message and will enhance your speech. 5. Create a new set of notes or bullet points that you can refer to while filming your speech. 6. Practice presenting your speech, focusing on your voice, your nonverbal communication, and your use of one or more visual aids. Aim for a speech 3-5 minutes in length. 7. Before filming, review the rubric to ensure that you understand how you will be evaluated. 8. Film yourself presenting the speech. Be sure that you can be easily seen and heard, and direct your speech to the camera. 9. Review your video, be sure that you can be easily seen and heard, and that you have directed your speech to the camera. Refilm as needed. 10. Review the checklist and requirements to ensure that your Touchstone is complete. 11. Upload your video using the blue button at the top of this page. • Academy of Management fleview 1999, Vol. 24, No. 3. 522-537, AN ORGANIZATIONAL LEARNING FRAMEWORK: FROM INTUITION TO INSTITUTION MARY M. CROSSAN HENRY W. LANE RODERICK E. WHITE Richard Ivey School of Business
  • 13. Although interest In organizational learning has grown dramatically in recent years, a general theory of organizational leaming has remained elusive. We identily re- newal of the overall enterprise as the underlying phenomenon of interest and organ- izational learning as a principal means to this end. With this perspective we develop a framework for the process of organizational learning, presenting organizational leaming as four processes—intuiting, interpreting, integrating, and institutionaliz- ing—linking the individual, group, and organizational levels. Organizational learning has existed in our lexicon at least since Cangelosi and Dill (1965) discussed the topic over 30 years ago. Lately, its popularity has grown dramatically (Crossan & Guatto, 1996), yet little convergence or consen- sus on what is meant by the term, or its basic nature, has emerged {Huber, 1991; Kim, 1993). In large part, convergence has not occurred because different researchers have applied the concept of organizational learning, or at least the terminology, to different domains. For exam- ple, Huber (1991) takes an information-process- ing perspective of organizational learning, whereas Nonaka and Takeuchi (1995) are con- cerned with product innovation, and March and Olsen (1975) are interested in exploring how the cognitive limitations of managers affect learn- ing. These works share some common threads, but the domains differ significantly. They con- cern different phenomena: information process-
  • 14. ing, product innovation, or bounded rationality. Although the phenomenological domains of var- ious researchers do sometimes overlap, the dif- ferences in domains do much to explain the lack of convergence among organizational learning frameworks. In this article we identify strategic renewal a s the underlying phenomenon of interest. Re- We gratefully acknowledge the Canadian Centre for Management Development, the Social Sciences and Human- ities Research Council, and the National Centre for Manage- ment Research and Development for their financial support of this project. newal harmonizes continuity and change at the level of the enterprise (Hurst, 1995; Hurst, Rush, & White, 1989). Organizational learning can be conceived of a s a principal means of achieving the strategic renewal of an enterprise. As we argue in this article, strategic renewal places additional demands on a theory of organization- al learning. Renewal requires that organiza- tions explore and learn new ways while concur- rently exploiting what they h a v e a l r e a d y learned (March, 1991). In contrast, learning ap- plied to the domain of new product develop- ment, for example, tends to focus on the explo- ration side of the exploration-exploitation tension identified by March. Recognizing and managing the tension between exploration and exploitation are two of the critical challenges of renewal and, hence, become a central require- ment in a theory of organizational learning.
  • 15. For renewal to be strategic it should encom- pass the entire enterprise—not simply the indi- vidual or group—and it should recognize that the organization operates in an open system, rather than having a solely internal focus {Dun- can & Weiss, 1979). Although theorists have rec- ognized the strategic importance of organization- al l e a r n i n g a s a m e a n s of providing a sustainable competitive a d v a n t a g e (DeGeus, 1988; Stata, 1989), few organizational learning frameworks have illustrated the tension be- tween exploration and exploitation that is at the heart of strategic renewal (see Table 1). Here we develop an organizational learning framework to address the phenomenon of re- 522 1999 Crossan. Lane, and White 523 TABLE 1 Propositions Applied to Established Organizational Learning Frameworks Source March & Olsen (1975) Daft 8E Weick {1984) S e n g e {1990)
  • 16. Huber (1991) March {1991) Wat kins & Marsick (1993) Nonaka & Takeuchi (1995) Strategic Renewal Tension Nat considered Not considered Not considered Not considered Yes Not considered Not considered Multilevel Framework No group level Not considered No organizational
  • 17. level Yes No group level Yes Recognized, but not a substantial part of the model One Level Afiects the Others Not considered Not considered Not considered Not considered Not considered Not considered Some discussion of the link between individual and group
  • 18. Process Linking Levels Not considered Processes described but not a levels perspective Processes focus on individual and group— not a levels-related model Processes within level but no model or processes to link levels Not considered Six action imperatives of the learning organization Focuses on processes that link individual and group—weak on link between group and organization Cognition/ Action Link Yes Learning is a change in behavior
  • 19. Yes Cognition affects behaviors Yes Consistent with Senge's perspective Knowledge focus newal. A framework defines the territory and takes us a step closer to a theory. A good frame- work has several requirements. First, it should identify the phenomenon of interest: in this case strategic renewal. Second, the key premises or assumptions underlying the framework need to be stated (Bacharach, 1989). Third, the relation- ship among the elements of the framework needs to be described (Sutton & Staw, 1995; Weick, 1995a; Whetton, 1989). As Sutton and Staw state, "Theory is about connections among phenomena, a story about why acts, events, structure and thoughts occur" (1995: 378). Our framework makes high-level connections. Fur- ther theory development will expand and deepen these connections and will enable de- velopment of testable hypotheses. Four key premises or assumptions form the
  • 20. underpinnings of this framework and support one central proposition: Premise 1: Organizational leaming involves a tension between assimilating new learning (exploration) and using what has been learned (exploita- tion). Premise 2: Organizational learning is multi- level: individual, group, and organ- ization. Premise 3: The three levels of organizational learning are linked by social and psychological processes: intuiting, interpreting, integrating, and insti- tutionalizing (4rs). Premise 4: Cognition affects action (and vice versa). Proposifion: The 4l's are related in feed-forward and feedback processes across the levels. As stated in Premise 1, organizational learn- ing reveals a tension between exploration and exploitation (March, 1991). March focuses more on the balance rather than the tension, but he recognizes its fundamental role in strategic re- newal: "Maintaining an appropriate balance be- tween exploration and exploitation is a primary
  • 21. 524 Academy of Management Review July factor in system survival and prosperity Both exploration and exploitation are essential for organizations, but they compete for scarce re- sources" (1991: 71). This competition for resources creates a ten- sion. As we discuss in subsequent sections, this tension is seen in the feed-forward and feed- back processes of learning across the individ- ual, group, and organization levels. Feed for- ward relates to exploration. It is the transference of learning from individuals and groups through to the learning that becomes embedded—or in- stitutionalized—in the form of systems, struc- tures, strategies, and procedures (Hedberg, 1981; Shrivastava, 1983). Feedback relates to exploita- tion and to the way in which institutionalized leaming affects individuals and groups. As noted in Premise 2, organizational learning is multilevel. A basic assumption is that insight and innovative ideas occur to individuals—not organizations (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995; Simon, 1991}. However, knowledge generated by the in- dividual does not come to bear on the organiza- tion independently. Ideas are shared, actions taken, and common meaning developed (Argyris & Schon, 1978, 1996; Daft & Weick, 1984; Huber, 1991; Stata, 1989). Complex organizations are more than ad hoc communities or collections of individuals. Relationships become structured, and some of the individual learning and shared understandings developed by groups become
  • 22. institutionalized a s organization artifacts (Hed- berg, 1981: Shrivastava, 1983). There is a reason- able degree of consensus that a theory of organ- i z a t i o n a l l e a r n i n g n e e d s to c o n s i d e r the individual, group, a n d organizational levels (Crossan, Lane, White, & Djurfeldt, 1995). The 41 processes introduced in Premise 3 are described in detail in the next section. Through- out the feed-forward and feedback processes, the interactive relationship between cognition and action (Premise 4) is critical—one cannot be divorced from the other (Neisser, 1976). Under- standing guides action, but action also informs u n d e r s t a n d i n g {Seely-Brown & Duguid, 1991; Weick, 1979). Organizational learning links cog- nition and action. This differentiates it from the related fields of knowledge management and intellectual capital. In spite of arguments to the contrary (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995), the fields of knowledge management (Conner & Prahalad, 1996; Davenport & Prusak, 1997; Foss, 1996; Grant, 1996; Kogut & Zander, 1992) a n d intellec- tual capital (Edvinsson & Malone, 1997; Stewart, 1997) remain largely focused on cognition. How- ever, these fields share common ground with organizational learning in recognizing the im- portance of knowledge to the success of the en- terprise. Quinn suggests that "looking beyond mere product lines to a strategy built around core intellectual or service competencies pro- vides both a rigorously maintainable strategic focus and long-term flexibility" (1992: 216). Re- search in knowledge management and intellec- tual capital informs organizational learning, but
  • 23. it does not capture the ongoing cycle of action taking a n d knowledge acquisition found in learning theories. There have been several reviews of the organ- izational learning literature (Fiol & Lyles, 1985; Huber, 1991; Levitf & March, 1988), but scholars have not recognized the importance of under- standing organizational learning from the per- spective of strategic renewal. As noted in Table 1, few of the well-known organizational learning frameworks (Daft & Weick, 1984; Huber, 1991; March, 1991; March & Olsen, 1975; Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995; Senge, 1990; Watkins & Marsick, 1993) recognize the tension of strategic renewal. Further, the frameworks vary in the degree they address the other key premises. In the following section we expand these key premises by describing the 41 processes of or- ganizational learning that link the levels, using the well-known story of Apple Computer to illus- trate these processes. We then discuss the dy- namic nature of the 41 processes a s they relate to fhe feed-forward a n d feedback processes of learning. Finally, we present implications for research a n d management. THE 41 FRAMEWORK OF ORGANIZATIONAL LEARNING The 41 framework of organizational learning contains four related (sub)processes—intuiting, interpreting, integrating, a n d institutionaliz- ing—that occur over three levels: individual, group, and organization. The three learning lev-
  • 24. els define the structure through which organiza- tional learning takes place. The processes form the glue that binds the structure together; they are, therefore, a key facet of the framework. In- tuiting a n d interpreting occur at the individual level, interpreting and integrating occur at the group level, and integrating and institutionaliz- 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 52S TABLE 2 Learning/Renewal in Organizations: Four Processes Through Three Levels Level Individual Group Process Intuiting Interpreting Integrating Inputs/Outcomes Experiences Images
  • 25. Metaphors Language Cognitive map Conversation/dialogue Shared understandings Mutual adjustment Interactive systems Routines Organization Institutionalizing Diagnostic systems Rules and procedures ing occur at the organizational level (see Table 2). There are a sequence and progression to these processes through the different levels, and while there is some "spillover" from level to level, not every process occurs at every level. For example, intuition is a uniquely individ- ual process. It may happen within a group or organizational context, but the recognition of a pattern or possibility comes from within an in- dividual. Organizations do not intuit. This is a uniquely human attribute that organizations do not possess. Similarly, organizations do not in- terpret. Interpreting has to do with refining and developing intuitive insights. The development of language, principally through an interactive conversational process, is a basic interpretive process. The proverbial person on a deserted island could have an intuitive insight and begin to make sense of it through an internal conver-
  • 26. sational process (i.e., talking to one's self), but the interpretive process is likely to be much richer and more robust if the conversations and interactions are with others. This process spans the individual and group levels, but it does not extend to the organizational level. When actions take place in concert with other members of a workgroup, the interpreting pro- cess quite naturally blends into the integrating process. Integrating entails the development of shared understanding and the taking of coordi- nated action by members of a workgroup. Ac- tions that are deemed to be effective will be repeated. Initially, the workgroup informally makes this judgment about what actions should be replicated. Eventually, fhe workgroup may establish formal rules and procedures, and rou- tines become embedded. The process of institu- tionalizing occurs. The process of institutionalizing is an organi- zation-level phenomenon. Organizations, like other social institutions, are socially constructed (Berger & Luckmann, 1968). The routines and rules that make up an enduring organization exist independently of any one individual (al- though individuals and their actions are af- fected by these rules and routines). The 41*8 Defined and Developed We define the learning processes a s follows: /nfui(ing is the preconscious recognition of the pattern and/or possibilities inherent in a per-
  • 27. sonal stream of experience (Weick, 1995b: 25). This process can affect the intuitive individual's actions, but it only affects others when they at- tempt to (inter)act with that individual. Interpret- ing is the explaining, through words and/or ac- tions, of an insight or idea to one's self and to others. This process goes from the preverbal to the verbal, resulting in the development of lan- guage. Integrating is the process of developing shared understanding among individuals and of taking coordinated action through mutual ad- justment. Dialogue and joint action are crucial to the development of shared understanding. This process will initially be ad hoc and infor- mal, but if the coordinated action taking is re- curring and significant, it will be institutional- ized. Institutionalizing is the process of ensuring that routinized actions occur. Tasks are defined, actions specified, and organizational mecha- nisms put in place to ensure that certain actions occur. Institutionalizing is the process of embed- ding leaming that h a s occurred by individuals and groups into the organization, and it includes systems, structures, procedures, and strategy. The four leaming processes operate over the three levels. Because the processes naturally flow from one into another, it is difficult to define pre- cisely where one ends and the next begins. Quite clearly, intuiting occurs at the individual level and institutionalizing at the organizational level; however, interpreting bridges the individual and group levels, while integrating links the group and organizational levels. Insights, the seeds of
  • 28. 526 Academy ol Management Review July adaptiveness and exploration, begin wifh the in- dividual but, if "successful," eventually become embedded in the formal organization. We describe the framework in a sequential way, although there are necessarily many feed- back loops among the levels, given the recursive nature of the phenomenon (as we discuss in subsequent sections). In the following discus- sion we develop each of the 41 learning pro- cesses in greater conceptual detail. Intuiting Scholars often assume that learning, whether it be at the individual, group, or organization level, is a conscious, analytical process. How- ever, Underwood (1982) suggests that the links between experience, knowledge, and conscious- ness are more complex than generally assumed. The subconscious is critical to understanding how people come to discern and comprehend something new, for which there was no prior explanation. A theory of learning needs to be able to address how this occurs. Accordingly, the process of intuiting—a largely subconscious process—is an important part of the framework presented here. At its most basic level, individual learning involves perceiving similarities and differences— patterns and possibilities. Although there are many definitions of intuition, most involve some
  • 29. sort of pattern recognition (Behling & Eckel, 1991). The expert and entrepreneurial views of intuition are most closely aligned with the framework presented here. The expert view of intuiting is a process of (past) pattem recognition. A highly sophisticated and complex map enables the expert to perceive pat- terns that novices cannot (Neisser, 1976). Prietula and Simon (1989) suggest that becoming an expert takes 10 years and requires the acquisition of 50,000 chunks of knowledge. Neisser (1976) has used the example of chess masters to explain ex- pert intuition. One must play a lot of chess, reflect on past experiences, and leam about great plays; all this and much more are required fo become a grandmaster. But an interesting thing seems to happen on the way to expertise. What once re- quired conscious, deliberate, and explicit thought no longer does. What once would have taken much deliberation and planning becomes the ob- vious thing to do. What has been learnt becomes tacit knowledge (Polanyi, 1967). The expert no longer has to think consciously about action. Having been in the same, or simi- lar, situations and recognizing the pattern, the expert knows, almost spontaneously, what to do. Indeed, if asked to explain their actions, experts may be unable fo do so. While the pattern (and associated actions) is familiar, the underlying justification has receded from conscious mem- ory. In a simple way expertise can be thought of a s unconscious recollection. This helps explain why expertise is so hard fo transfer from one person to anofher. It is highly subjective; deeply
  • 30. rooted in individual experiences; and very diffi- cult to surface, examine, and explain. Whereas expert intuition provides insight into the important process of pattern recognition, en- trepreneurial intuition has more to do with inno- vation and change. No two situations are the same, and patterns, while similar, are never identical. The ability to make novel connections and to discern possibilities is also key to intuit- ing. "Entrepreneurs" are able to make these novel connections, perceive new or emergent relationships, and discern possibilities that have not been identified previously. Whereas expert intuition may be past pattern oriented, entrepreneurial intuition is future possibility oriented. Expert intuition supports exploitation; entre- preneurial intuition supports exploration. Entre- preneurial intuiting generates new insights. Koestler (1976) suggests that in the natural sci- ences such insights, when they occur, happen after the individual has had a long period of immersion in the problem, followed by a brief period of disassociation from the specifics of the problem. Although this may be true for break- through insights, more mundane acts of innova- tion may have more humble beginnings (Ander- son, 1992). Imagery and metaphor also seem to be important in this process. For entrepreneurs in a business situation, there is always fhe question of whether these individuals are intuitive or just lucky. However, this question is difficult to answer because
  • 31. novel, intuitive insights cannot be judged right or wrong ex ante. They are simply possibilities. It is rare to see a business entrepreneur able to convert intuitive insight into business reality on a consistent basis. Fred Smith perceived the po- tential of reliable, overnight, small package de- livery, and Federal Express emerged a s a very successful business (Maisfer & Wyckoff, 1974). 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 527 He was unable to replicate this success with Zapmail—an electronic mail service. There are exceptions, however. Howard Head, the entre- preneurial genius behind the Head metal ski, was also the inventor of the Prince oversized tennis racket. The connection between quality of intuition and commercial success is difficult fo make. In- tuition is the beginning of new learning. Even- tual commercial success is dependent upon ef- fective learning at all levels—not simply the original intuitive insights of the entrepreneur. Intuiting, especially of the entrepreneurial type, appears to be a largely subconscious pro- cess. In fact, trying to force it to a conscious level too soon may prevent it from h a p p e n i n g (Watson, 1969). The outcome of individual intu- iting is an inexplicable sense of the possible, of what might be done. Entrepreneurial intuitions are preverbal, and expert intuitions may be non- verbal a s well. No language exists to describe
  • 32. the insight or to explain the intended action. Consequently, intuition may guide the actions of the individual, but this intuition is difficult to share with others (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995). Imagery, sometimes called "visions," and meta- phors aid the individual in his or her interpreta- tion of the insight and in communicating it to others. Scholars have recognized metaphors a s a crit- ical link in the evolution from individual intui- tive insighf to shared interpretation. Individuals use metaphors to help explain their intuition to themselves and fo share it with others. As Tsou- kas explains, "Metaphors involve the transfer of information from a relatively familiar domain . . . to a new and relatively unknown domain" (1991: 568)—that is, from fhe known fo the un- known, from that for which we share literal lan- guage to that emerging insight for which lan- guage does not yet exist. As such, metaphors mark the beginning of the interpreting process. Srivastava and Barrett provide the example of a child trying to describe for the first time to his mother that his foot is asleep. The child has no literal language to relay this strange sensation: In frustration, he says to his mother: "It feels like there are stars hitting my foot." Having no avail- able literal terms, the child associates a new unfamiliar experience with one he understands. He has a sparkling, glittering, tingling sensation that seems to impact his foot from somewhere outside his body. At the age of four he is unable to say, "Mother, there is a certain numbness in my
  • 33. foot which is a result of an inadequate supply of blood which I have inadvertently seemed to cir- cumvent" (1991: 568). In this example the child perceives something he has no words to describe, although the words do exist, and no doubt his mother would explain that the word "numbness" can be used to de- scribe the sensation. True innovators have a problem akin to the child. They have a sensation—an insight into a possibility—but they have no literal language to describe it. Unfortunately, they do not have a "parent" to provide that language; indeed, none exists if the insight is truly novel. Individuals employ metaphors to bound and describe the insight. As Tsoukas elaborates. In lay discourse, metaphors constitute an eco- nomical way of relaying primarily experiential information in a vivid manner, and they can be used as variety reduction mechanism in situa- tions where experience cannot be segmented and imparted through literal language (1991: 567). Indeed, for entrepreneurial insights, metaphors may be the only language available for one to communicate with another. Early in the evolution of the personal com- puter, Steve Jobs of Apple employed the "appli- ance" metaphor. This metaphor evokes a whole set of subsidiary images: easy to use, small, affordable. Subsequently, more literal language was used to name more precisely many of the
  • 34. attributes associated with the original metaphor (e.g., graphical user interface). This example also points to the reciprocity between thinking and acting that is inherent in fhe development of language. Naming also directs actions towards fhe object (or image) you have named because it promotes activity consistent with the related attribution {i.e., the name or the metaphor) it carries. To change the name of an object connotes changing your relationship to it because when we name something, we direct anticipations, expectations, and evaluations toward it (Srivastava & Barrett, 1988: 34-35). Using this reasoning, if lobs had used a different metaphor to describe his initial insight, perhaps the personal computer as "business assistant," it would have led to very different actions, and Apple would have become a very different com- pany. Early in a company's development, when it is far from equilibrium, small differences in 528 Academy of Management Review July the metaphors employed and the ways in which conversations unfold and language develops may ultimately result in great differences in where the company ends up. Interpreting Whereas intuiting focuses on the subcon-
  • 35. scious process of developing insights, interpret- ing begins picking up on the conscious elements of the individual learning process. Through the process of interpreting, individuals develop cog- nitive maps about the various domains in which they operate (Huff, 1990). Language plays a piv- otal role in the development of these maps, since it enables individuals to name and begin to explain what were once simply feelings, hunches, or sensations. Further, once things are named, individuals can make more explicit con- nections among them. Interpreting takes place in relation to a do- main or an environment. The nature or texture of the domain within which individuals and organ- izations operate, and from which they extract data, is crucial to understanding the interpretive process. The precision of the language that evolves will reflect the texture of the domain, given the tasks being attempted. The well- known example of fhe Inuit having over a dozen different words for (various types of) snow illus- trates the rich interaction between the fask do- main and the sophistication of language. More- over, a person with very rich a n d complex cognitive maps of a domain, like the chess mas- fer, will be able to see things and act in ways that others cannot. The cognitive map is affected by the domain or environment, but it also guides what is inter- preted from that domain. As Weick (1979) sug- gests, people are more likely to "see something when they believe it" rather than "believe it when they see it." As a result, individuals will
  • 36. interpret the same stimulus differently, based on their established cognitive maps. The same stimulus can evoke a different or equivocal meaning for differenf people (Hambrick & Ma- son, 1984; Walsh, 1988). This difference is not a result of uncertainty about the stimulus. Uncer- tainty is related to the quality of information. But for any group of people, even high-qualify infor- mation may be equivocal: it may hold multiple, and often conflicting, meanings (Daft & Huber, 1987). Although equivocality is an issue in the development of both individual understanding a n d s h a r e d u n d e r s t a n d i n g within a group, equivocal situations are often resolved through a group interpretive process (Weick & Van Or- den, 1990). Just a s language plays a pivotal role in en- abling individuals to develop fheir cognitive maps, it is also pivotal in enabling individuals to develop a sense of shared understanding. Interpreting is a social activity that creates and refines common language, clarifies images, and creates shared meaning and understanding. Equivocality is reduced through interpreting by "shared observations and discussion until a common grammar and course of action can be agreed upon" (Daft & Weick, 1984: 291). Groups will have an interpretive capacity related to the makeup of the group and to the group dynamics (Hurst et a l , 1989). As the interpretive process moves beyond the individual and becomes em- bedded within the workgroup, it becomes inte- grafive. Individual interpretive processes come together around a shared u n d e r s t a n d i n g of
  • 37. what is possible, and individuals interact and attempt to enact that possibility. Integrating Whereas the focus of interpreting is change in the individual's understanding and actions, the focus of integrating is coherent, collective ac- tion. For coherence to evolve, shared under- standing by members of the group is required. It is through the continuing conversation among members of the community and through shared practice (Seely-Brown & Duguid, 1991) that shared understanding or collective mind (Weick & Roberts, 1993) develops and mutual adjust- ment and negotiated action (Simons, 1991) take place. The evolution of language extends the pro- cess of interpreting to interactions among indi- viduals: the realm of workgroups, organizations, communities, and even societies. Language de- veloped through conversation and dialogue al- lows the evolution of shared meaning for the group. As Daft and Weick explain: The distinctive feature... is sharing. A piece of data, a perception, a cognitive map is shared among managers.... Passing a startling obser- vation among members, or discussing a puzzling development enables managers to converge on an approximate interpretation (1984: 285). 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 529
  • 38. Language not only helps us learn—it pre- serves, for better and for worse, what has been learned. For an organization to learn and renew, its language must evolve. Conversation can be used not only to convey established meaning but also to evolve new meaning. Not all conversational styles are equally ef- fective, however, for developing shared mean- ing. Isaacs suggests that "dialogue is a disci- pline of collective thinking and inquiry, a process for transforming the quality of conver- sation and, in particular the thinking that lies beneath it" (1993: 25). Through dialogue the group can evolve new and deeper shared under- standings. This shared meaning can cause those who have participated to more or less spontaneously make mutual adjustments to their actions. As Isaacs goes on to explain. Dialogue proposes t h a t . . . some of the most pow- erful forms of coordination may come through participation in unfolding meaning, which might even be perceived differently by different people. A flock of birds suddenly taking flight from a tree reveals fhe potential coordination of dialogue: this movement all at once, a wholeness and lis- tening together that permits individual differen- tiation but is still highly interconnected {1993: 25). The dialogue process attempts to convey both the message and a deep interconnected mean- ing. A consensual approach that attempts to get agreement on the message without delving into the underlying meaning(s) risks a groupthink
  • 39. outcome (Janis, 1982). As with the process of interpreting, the context surrounding the integrating process is critical. Seely-Brown and Duguid's (1991) notion of "com- munities of practice" captures the importance of the infegrative context. These authors and their colleagues have been involved in ethnographic research on workplace practices and suggest that understanding and impacting leaming and innovation require one to study and understand the situation in which practice occurs. Neither occurs ex situ: Practice is essential to understanding work. Ab- stractions detached from practice distort or ob- scure intricacies of that practice. Without a clear understanding of those intricacies and fhe role they play, the practice itself cannot be well un- derstood, engendered (through training) or en- hanced (through innovation) (1391: 40). Observations from these ethnographic studies reveal that actual practice is not what is speci- fied in manuals or necessarily what is taught in classrooms. Rather, it is captured and promul- gated by stories told by community members. Storytelling is a significant part of the learning process. Stories reflect the complexity of actual practice rather than the abstractions taught in classrooms. As stories evolve, richer under- standing of the phenomenon is developed, and new integrated approaches to solving problems are created. Stories themselves become fhe re- pository of wisdom—part of the collective mind/
  • 40. memory (Weick & Roberts, 1993). Institutionalizing The process of institutionalizing sets organi- zational learning apart from individual or ad hoc group learning. The underlying assumption is that organizations are more than simply a collection of individuals; organizational learn- ing is different from the simple sum of the learn- ing of its members. Although individuals may come and go, what they have learned as indi- viduals or in groups does not necessarily leave with them. Some leaming is embedded in the systems, structures, strategy, routines, pre- scribed practices of the organization, and in- vestments in information systems and infra- structure. For new organizations there are few estab- lished routines or structures: there is no organ- izational memory. Often by the nature of their small size, their open communication, and their formation based on common interest and dreams, individual and group learning domi- nate in young organizations. As organizations mature, however, individuals begin to fall into patterns of interaction and communication, and the organizations attempt to capture the pat- terns of interaction by formalizing them. This institutionalization is a means for organ- izations to leverage fhe learning of the individ- ual members. Structures, systems, and proce- dures provide a context for interactions. Over time, spontaneous individual and group learn-
  • 41. ing become less prevalent, as the prior learning becomes embedded in the organization and be- gins to guide the actions and learning of organ- izational members. Organizations naturally outgrow fheir ability to exclusively use spontaneous interactions to interpret, integrate, and take coherent action. Relationships become formalized. Coherent ac- 530 Academy of Management fleview July tion is achieved with the help of plans and other formal systems. If the plan produces favorable outcomes, then the actions deemed to be consis- tent with the plan become routines. There is a need to ensure that the routines continue to be carried out and that the organization produces and performs. This is the role for what Simons calls "diagnostic systems" (1991,1994). An organ- ization uses these systems to regulate the day- to-day routines of the business—to exploit the current understanding of the business. Simons also identifies another type of formal system he calls "interactive." Organizations use interac- tive systems to consider how the future can or may be different from the past. As one moves from the individual level of in- tuiting/interpreting through group integrating to organizational institutionalizing, the process of learning is less fluid and incremental a n d be- comes more staccato and disjointed. Generally, that which becomes institutionalized in organi-
  • 42. zations h a s received, at some point, a certain degree of consensus or shared understanding among the influential members of the organiza- tion. Before a formal organizational system or structure is established or changed, the modifi- cation generally undergoes some process of consideration. Once something is institutional- ized, if usually endures for a period of time. Changes in systems, structures, and routines occur relatively infrequently in organizations; a s a result, although the underlying processes of intuiting, interpreting, and integrating are more fluid and continual, significant changes in the i n s t i t u t i o n a l i z e d organization typically a r e punctuated. For this reason much organization- al change is interpreted a s being radical or transformational, rather than incremental, in nature. However, even though fhe institutional changes may a p p e a r disjointed, the underlying learning processes of intuiting and interpreting at the individual and group levels that result in these changes may be more continuous. Institutionalized learning cannot capture all the ongoing learning at the individual a n d group levels. It takes time to transfer learning from individuals to groups and from groups to the organization. As the environment changes, the learning that has been institutionalized may no longer fit the context; there may be a g a p between what the organization needs to do a n d what it h a s learned to do. As the g a p widens, the organization places more reliance on individual learning a n d initiative. For example, Seely-
  • 43. Brown (1993) reports on studies that examined the informal routines of e x p e r i e n c e d order clerks in comparison with the formal institution- alized system. Although the product of their day's work gave the appearance that the clerks had followed the formal routine, they had, in fact, improvised in ways that proved more effi- cient and effective. Given that the environment is constantly changing, the challenge for organizations is to m a n a g e the tension between the embedded in- stitutionalized learning from the past, which en- ables it to exploit learning, and fhe new learn- ing t h a t must be a l l o w e d to feed forward through the processes of intuiting, interpreting, and integrating. Although the 4rs have been presented in a linear fashion for e a s e of expla- nation, appreciating the iterative nature of the processes is critical, a s we will discuss in a subsequent section. In the following section we present the Apple story in a linear fashion to illustrate the 41 pro- cesses. The focus of the story is on the explora- tion side of strategic renewal. We then broaden the discussion to examine the dynamic nature of organizational learning. Understanding the Learning Processes: The Apple Story The relationship between each of the levels and processes perhaps can best be illustrated by way of a story—that of Apple Computer.^ By all accounts, Steve lobs, through an intuitive
  • 44. process, had the insights upon which Apple was founded, lobs perceived patterns and evolved certain images'^ about possibilities, which de- veloped into a metaphor (e.g.. the personal com- puter a s an appliance—one in every home) that guided Apple during its early years. These im- ages were based, at least in part, upon his unique experiences and cognitive orientation. He provided much of the insight and energy that were the genesis of Apple. ' This description is not represented as an accurate case history. Rather, it is a story we use to help illustrate the conceptual model; we do not employ it as empirical support for the model. ^ The italicized words represent the key words used to describe the inputs and outcomes of the processes as shown in Table 2. 1999 Crossan, Lane, and Whife 531 But these initial images were necessarily vague when it came to specific actions. At the intuitive stage actions are improvised, rooted more in feeling than thinking (Hurst et aL, 1989). The language used to explain improvised action is necessarily underdeveloped, vague, and im- precise. On the basis of lobs' own experiences and his perceptions of the events, along with Wozniak's technical expertise, these men impro- vised actions as they went along. Language plays a pivotal role as insights be-
  • 45. come more sophisticated and concrete through the interpretive process. In part, it was through the group process of dialogue and conversation that Jobs' own understanding and individual cognitive complexity were enhanced. Talking and acting with others, developing words to de- scribe what had been vague insights, and en- acting these insights enabled a deeper meaning to evolve (Bruner, 1990). Many researchers talk about this evolution of meaning in terms of cognitive complexity and cognitive maps (Huff, 1990). Although one must be careful with this metaphor, it is helpful to think of Jobs initially navigating his chosen ter- ritory more or less mapless, guided only by some vague vision of what lay over the next hill. As he, with others, experimented and explored the territory, a mental picture or map slowly emerged, with finer and finer levels of detail. (This metaphor is only helpful to a point. Jobs was not just exploring the territory—through his actions he was helping to create or enact the territory.) As insightful as Jobs was, he could not accom- plish his vision alone. He needed to involve oth- ers. The conversation and dialogue, which served to develop his understanding, also helped to integrate the cognitive maps of the group—to develop a shared understanding. Lan- guage, which plays a critical role in the devel- opment of individual maps, is essential as a means of integrating ideas and negotiating ac- tions with others. Through conversation, work- groups identify areas of difference and agree-
  • 46. ment, gain language precision, and develop a shared understanding of their task domain. They quite naturally, as a part of this process, use their common language and fhe conversa- tional process to negotiate mutual adjustments to their actions. These adjustments are an inte- gral part of the learning process. The assump- tion is that a certain coherence of actions should emerge from a shared understanding of the business situation—that is, the emergent strat- egy (Mintzberg, 1994). But what is the context within which shared understandings and mutual adjustments occur? Early in an organization's life, as was the case with Apple, these processes are largely informal and spontaneous. As organizations grow larger and more people are involved, informal interac- tions do not suffice. What had happened more or less spontaneously must now be arranged; what had been an informal conversation over coffee about the future of the company becomes a for- mal planning process with in(erac(ive systems (Simons, 1991, 1994). The organization naturally outgrows its abil- ity to exclusively use spontaneous interactions to interpret, integrate, and take concerted ac- tion. Relationships become formalized and rou- fines develop. There is a need to ensure that the routines continue fo be carried out and that the organization produces and performs. This is the role of diagnostic systems (Simons, 1991, 1994). In the Apple situation John Sculley was
  • 47. brought in, at least in part, to provide needed systems, structures, and other formal mecha- nisms. Individual and communal learning be- came institutionalized in the hope that the learning could be more systematically ex- ploited. Institutionalization contributes to more efficient operations, enabling the organization to better deliver on the founder's original vision. With Apple, however, it also may have hindered the organization's ability to renew itself by intu- iting, interpreting, and integrating emerging patterns and new possibilities. Unable to realize his new vision within Apple, Steve Jobs left to start a new enterprise, appropriately called NeXT. Essentially, the process of institutionalizing embeds learned behaviors that have worked in the past into the routines of the organization. Diagnostic systems develop ruies and proce- dures to facilitate the repetition of routines. But the process of institutionalizing also feeds back by creating a context through which subsequent events and experiences are interpreted. This context may facilitate and/or impede the organ- ization's ability to (re)inferpret and respond to its environment. The Apple example, while use- ful, is a simplification. In entrepreneurial startup situations like Apple, there is originally little or no past learning embedded in fhe formal 532 Academy of Management Review July organization. Indeed, there is no formal organi-
  • 48. zation. Established organizations do have past learning embedded within them. As such, learn- ing and renewal in these situations must deal with this difference in context and its associated challenges. Organizational Learning As a Dynamic Process Organizational learning is a dynamic process. Not only does learning occur over time and across levels, but it also creates a tension be- tween assimilating new learning (feed forward) and exploiting or using what has already been learned (feedback). Through feed-forward pro- cesses, new ideas and actions flow from the individual to fhe group to the organization lev- els. Af the same time, what has already been learned feeds back from the organization to group and individual levels, affecting how peo- ple act and think. The concurrent nature of the feed-forward and feedback processes creates a tension, which can be understood by arraying the levels against one another, as shown in Fig- ure 1. Doing so illustrates that, in addition to the processes that feed forward learning from the individual and groups to the organization, learning that has been institutionalized feeds back and impacts individual and group learn- ing. The importance of these interactions can be highlighted by two relationships that are espe- cially problematic: i n t e r p r e t i n g - i n t e g r a t i n g (feed forward) and institutionalizing-intuiting (feedback).
  • 49. Moving from interpreting to integrating (feed forward) requires a shift from individual learn- ing to learning among individuals or groups. It entails faking personally constructed cognitive maps and integrating them in a way that devel- ops a shared understanding among the group members. There are many challenges in chang- ing an existing shared reality. The first is that individuals need to be able to communicate, through words and actions, their own cognitive map. Since many aspects of cognitive maps are tacit, communicating them requires a process of surfacing and articulating ideas and concepts. This process makes tacit knowledge explicit (Polanyi, 19B7). Assuming individuals can surface and articu- late their maps, a second challenge arises from HGURE I Organizational Learning As a Dynamic Process Individual Group Organizational Individual Group Organizational 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 533 the collective interpretation of the maps. Making something explicit does not necessarily mean
  • 50. the understanding is shared. Imprecision of lan- guage is complicated by cognitive maps that act as unique filters on the communication: we tend to "see/hear what we believe" rather than "be- lieve what we see." The real test of shared un- derstanding is coherent action. Yet, for novel ideas, shared understanding may not evolve un- less shared action or experimentation is at- tempted. The learning perspective suggests that leading with action, rather than bluntly focusing on cognition, may provide a different migration path fo shared understanding. As in experien- tial learning (Crossan et al., 1995), action pro- vides the opportunity to share a common expe- rience, which may aid in the development of shared understanding. The second problematic interaction is be- tween institutionalizing and intuiting (feed- back). Institutionalization can easily drive out intuition. Intuiting within established organiza- tions with a high degree of institutionalized learning requires what Schumpeter (1959) refers to as "creative destruction"^—destroying, or at least setting aside, the institutional order to en- act variations thaf allow intuitive insights and actions to surface and be pursued. This is ex- tremely difficult because the language and logic that form the collective mindset of the or- ganization and the resulting investment in as- sets present a formidable fortress of physical and cognitive barriers to change. Further, mem- bers of the organization must step back from proven, objective successes and allow un- proven, subjectively based experimentation.
  • 51. One example of the tension and the potential for resolution is in the resource allocation pro- cess (institutionalized learning). Many resource allocation processes inhibit fhe development of new insights, given their emphasis on track record and proven success (Bower, 1970; Burgelman, 1983). However, some firms, such as 3M, have recognized this problem and have in- stitutionalized a different resource allocation process that provides funding for new projects, and also holds the business accountable for having a significant portion of the revenue de- rived from new products (Hurst, 1995). The sys- tem tries to ensure that exploitation (feedback) does not drive out exploration (feed forward). The tension between assimilating new leam- ing (feed forward) and using what has already been learned (feedback) arises because the in- stitutionalized learning (what has already been learned) impedes the assimilation of new learn- ing. Fully assimilating new learning requires the feed forward of leaming from the individual and group to become institutionalized within the organization. Utilizing what has been learned is a feedback loop of institutionalized learning from the organization to groups and individuals. For example, rules and routines that once captured the logic and learning of how to facilitate learning at the individual level may no longer apply in a changed circumstance, yet the systems still focus an individual's energy and attention in ways that impede the assimila- tion and feed forward of new learning (Mintz- berg, 1994). Or an organization structure that has
  • 52. a strong impact on who talks to whom in the organization may impede conversation that could develop valuable new shared understand- ings. Therefore, any theory of organizational learning needs to recognize the levels, pro- cesses, and dynamic nature of the learning pro- cess itself that create a tension between the feed forward and feedback of learning. Conceiving of learning as a dynamic flow raises the possibility that these flows can be constrained. Consider for a moment the paral- lels between production flow and learning flow. Production flow must ensure that the level of work-in-process inventories does not exceed the capacity of any parf of the system to absorb and process them. Concepts like throughput, capac- ity utilization, cycle time, and bottlenecks have aided our understanding of what it takes to bal- ance a production line to ensure smooth flow. A dynamic theory of organizational learning rec- ognizes that there may be bottlenecks in fhe ability of the organization to absorb (Cohen & Levinthal, 1990) the feed forward of learning from the individual to the group and organiza- tion. Investment in individual learning and pressures for new product innovation may be- come stockpiled if the organization has limited capacity to absorb the learning. However, in the production process, work-in-process inventory does not "care" whether it is stockpiled, whereas in the learning process individuals (and their ideas) do. As a result, individuals may become frustrated and disenchanted, and may even leave the organization.
  • 53. A dynamic theory of organizational leaming provides a means of understanding the funda- 534 Academy of Managemenf fievfew July mental tensions of strategic renewal: the ten- sion between exploration (feed forward) and ex- ploitation (feedback). Although one may b e t e m p t e d to e q u a t e o r g a n i z a t i o n a l l e a r n i n g solely with the innovative feed-forward process, in doing so one fails to recognize that the feed- back process provides the means to exploit what h a s been learned (Crossan & Sorrenti, 1987). However, because learning that h a s become in- stitutionalized at the organization level is often difficult to change, it runs the risk of becoming irrelevant and may even obstruct feed-forward learning flows. This h a s led fo the call to liber- ate organizations and destroy bureaucracy (Pin- chot & Pinchot, 1993), yet bureaucracy (or insti- tutionalization) is not necessarily n e g a t i v e . Institutionalizing learning is necessary to reap the ongoing benefits of what h a s already been learned. With the 41 framework we identify the flow of learning between levels a n d the tension be- tween feed-forward (exploration) and feed-back (exploitation) processes a s fundamental chal- lenges of strategic renewal. There are many fac- tors that could facilitate and inhibit this process, some of which are part of the institutionalized learning itself (e.g., reward systems, information systems, resource allocation systems, strategic
  • 54. planning systems, and structure). However, in the 41 model we recognize that ideas occur to individuals a n d that individuals ultimately share those ideas through a n integrating pro- cess. It is the individuals, and the social pro- cesses and group dynamics through which they interact, that may facilitate or inhibit organiza- tional leaming. One promising area for further research is to examine the role of leadership and management of the 41 learning process. IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND MANAGEMENT The central contribution of this work is the 4rs and the related feed-forward and feed-back pro- cesses. Further, the interplay between the levels and the processes reveals the tensions associ- ated with strategic renewal. It is our hope that this framework will stir a reaction in the organ- izational leaming community and help scholars research the links among the levels and the tensions inherent in organizational learning. The same questions that we as researchers seek to answer form the basis of inquiry for managers. Is there a satisfactory level of intui- tive, innovative insights in the organization? Do individuals have the motivation, understanding, capability, and opportunity to interpret their en- vironment? How do individual and group expe- riences help to develop shared understanding? How well do individual insights become shared, integrated, and institutionalized in the organi- zation? What impediments are there to integrat-
  • 55. ing individual perspectives? How much of the organization's intellectual capital resides in in- dividual heads? Is there enough institutional- ized learning? How does institutionalized learn- ing facilitate or impede intuiting, interpreting, and integrating? What is the nature of the inter- play between the feed-forward and feedback processes? The responses to these questions need to take into account the dynamic nature of organization- al learning as it relates to strategic renewal. Compartmentalization of the issues will lead to a simplification that disguises the many essen- tial challenges of the phenomenon. For exam- ple, in the case of the first question, a simple focus on intuiting may yield a better under- standing of the individual processes of innova- tion. However, such a focus will miss the tension and, hence, challenge of feeding forward infui- tive insights with the hope of interpreting, inte- grating, and institutionalizing them, while con- currently working within a setting where institutionalized learning continues to posi- tively impact upon the performance of the enter- prise. The question of whether individuals have fhe motivation, understanding, capability, and op- portunity to interpret their environment sug- gests the need to examine more than just indi- viduals. It requires an examination of fhe link between interpreting and institutionalizing. In- dividuals may be motivated and capable, but if they turn their attention toward interpreting things that have little impact, the organization
  • 56. will reap few benefits from that learning. Fur- thermore, even if individuals are interpreting things of relevance, their learning needs to be integrated and institutionalized to realize its fu- ture value. This theory suggests it is not simply a matter of transferring data, information, or knowledge—it is a matter of organizational learning. The role of experience in the development of shared understanding reinforces the learning 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 535 premise that cognition (knowledge, understand- ing, and beliefs) and action (behaviors) are tightly intertwined, and changes in knowledge do not necessarily lead to changes in action. In contrast to knowledge management and intel- lectual capital, which focus management and research attention on cognition, this view of or- ganizational learning acknowledges the rich in- terrelationship between cognition and action. The foregoing examples emphasize the need to pursue questions of organizational learning with a dynamic perspective. We encourage re- searchers and managers to extend their think- ing to consider how differenf parts of the organ- izational learning system impact one another. This framework should serve as a map to help researchers and managers expand their hori- zons.
  • 57. While this framework should encourage and assist the pursuit of a more holistic understand- ing of organizational learning, there are two particular areas of research that will help ad- vance theory. The first is understanding the mechanisms that enhance or restrict the stocks and flows of learning. Here we have suggested that learning processes can be compared to pro- duction processes. This point should generate substantial dialogue, because it begins to ques- tion some of the traditional leverage points for organizational learning. For example, continued investment in individual and even group learn- ing may be counterproductive if fhe organiza- tion does not have the capacity to absorb or utilize it. If this is the case, future research in organizational learning needs to move from fhe reasonably well-developed undersfanding of in- dividual- and group-level learning to under- standing the flows of learning (feed forward and feedback) between fhe levels. Yet, all intuitive insights should not, and can- not, be immediately interpreted, integrated, and institutionalized. What enables the organization to "separate the wheat from the chaff"—the good from the bad—as ideas and practices de- velop and are refined over time? We have sug- gested that the strategic context helps to frame things that are more or less relevant, but the decision rules, criteria, and processes are not so clear. For example, if 3M had framed the discov- ery of a glue that does not stick in a narrow strategic context, we would not have reaped the benefits of Post-It® Notes.
  • 58. A second area that will advance theory is an understanding of how to reconcile the tension between exploitation and exploration—be- tween continuity and change. The 41 model di- rects our attention to the interplay of these pro- cesses, but it does not specifically address how organizations deal with this tension. Although a few management scholars have considered this problem (Hurst, 1995; Miller, 1990; Pascale, 1990), answers have proven elusive. This important question merits further consideration and inves- tigation. This dynamic framework of organizational learning will place significant demands on both researchers and managers. It requires capabil- ity for cross-level examination with a critical eye for the tensions inherent in the feed-forward and feedback processes. It requires the capabil- ity to link human resource management, strate- gic management, and the management of infor- mation technology and systems as a means to facilitate the flow of learning. Although such research poses challenges, the potential bene- fits are significant. Strategic renewal is one of the central challenges of every organization. This dynamic process of organizational leaming could yield important insights into strategic re- newal. In summary, in this article we have pushed in the direction of advancing a theory of organiza- tional learning by describing an organizational learning framework that incorporates the dy- namic multilevel nature of the phenomenon and captures the rich interplay between process and
  • 59. level. This framework should provide clarity, promote dialogue, foster convergence (Pfeffer, 1993), and encourage new directions in research that begin to examine organizational learning flows that enable strategic renewal. REFERENCES Anderson, I. V. 1992. Weirder than fiction: The reality and myths oi creativity. Academy of Management Executive, 6(4): 40-47. Argyris, C , & Schon, D. A. 1978. Organizationa/ learning: A theory of action perspective. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley. Argyris, C, & Schon, D. A. 1996. Organizatfona/ learning It: Theory, method, and practice. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley. Bacharach, S. 1989. Organizational theories: Some criteria for evaluation. Academy of Management Review, 14: 496-515. 536 .Academy of Management Review luly Behling, O., & Eckel, H. 1991. Making sense out oi intuition. Academy of Management Executive, 5(1): 46-54. Berger, P., 8E Luckmann, T. 1966. The social consfruc(ion of reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books. Bower, J. L. 1970. Managing the resource allocation process.
  • 60. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. Bruner, J. 1990, Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Burgelman, R. A. 1983. A model of the interaction of strategic behavior, corporate context and the concept oi strategy. Academy of Management Review, 8: 61-70. Cangelosi, V. E., & Dill, W. R. 1965. Organizational learning observations: Toward a theory. Administrative Science Quarteriy, 10: 175-203. Cohen. W. M., & Levinthal, D. A. 1990. Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on learning and innovation. Adminis- trative Science Quarterly, 35: 128-152. Conner. K., & Prahalad. C. 1998. A resource-based theory of the firm: Knowledge versus opportunism, Organizafion Science, 7: 469. Crossan. M., & Guatto, T. 1996. Organizational leaming re- search profile. Journal of Organizational Change Man- agement, ^{l): 107-112, Crossan. M.. Lane, H., White, R. E., Djurieldt, L. 1995. Organ- izational learning: Dimensions for a theory. Interna- tional Journal of Organizationai Analysis, 3: 337-360. Crossan. M., 8f Sorrenti. M. 1997. Making sense of improvisa- tion. In A. Huff & J. Walsh (Eds.), Advances in strategic management, vol. 14: 155-180. Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Dait. R. L.. 8f Huber, G. 1987. How organizations leam: A communication framework. Research in the Sociology of Organizations. 5(2): 1-36.
  • 61. Daft, R, L., & Weick, K, E. 1984. Toward a model of organiza- tions a s interpretation systems. Academy oi Manage- ment Review, 9: 284-295. Davenport, T., & Prusak, L. 1997, Informafion ecoJogy: Mas- tering the information and knowledge environment. New York: Oxford University Press. DeGeus, A. 1988. Planning as leaming. Harvard Susiness fleview, 66{March-April): 70-74. Duncan, R, B,, & Weiss, A. 1979. Organizational leaming: Implications for organizational design. Research in Or- ganizationai BehaWor, 1(4): 75-124. Edvinsson, L,, 8E Malone, M, 1997. /nteiJecfuai capital. New York: Harper Business. Fiol, C. M., & Lyles, M. A. 1985, Organizational learning. Academy of Management Review, 10: 803-813. Foss, N. 1996. Knowledge-based approaches lo the theory of the firm: Some critical comments. Organization Science. 7: 470-476. Grant, R. M. 1996. Toward a knowledge-based theory ol the firm. Strategic Management/ournal, 17: 109-122. Hambrick. D., & Mason, P. A. 1984. Upper echelons: The organization as a reflection oi its top managers. Acad- emy of Management Review, 9: 193-206, Hedberg. B. 1981. How organizations leam and unlearn. In P. C. Nystrom & W. H, Starbuck (Eds.). Handbook ol
  • 62. organizational design: 3-27. New York: Oxiord Univer- sity Press. Huber, G, P. 1991. Organizational learning: The contributing processes and the literatures. Organization Science. 2: 88-115. Huff, A. S. 1990, Mapping strategic thought. New York: Wiley. Hurst. D. K. 1995. Crisis and renewal. Boston: Harvard Busi- ness School Press. Hurst, D. K., Rush, J. C , & White, R. E. 1989. Top management teams and organizational renewal. Strategic Manage- ment Journal, 10: 87-105. Isaacs, W. H. 1993. Dialogue, collective thinking, and organizational learning. Organizational Dynamics. 22(2): 24-39, Janis, I. 1982. Groupfhinfc Psychologica/ studies of policy decisions and fiascoes. Boston: Houghton Mifilin, Kim, D. H. 1993. The link between individual and organiza- tional learning, Sloan Management Review, 33(1): 37-50. Koestler, A, 1976. The act of creation. London: Hutchinson. Kogut. B.. & Zander, U. 1992, Knowledge oi the firm, combi- native capabilities, and the replication oi technology. Organization Science. 3: 383-397. Levitt. B., & March. J. G. 1988. Organizational learning. Annuai Review ol Sociology, 14: 319-340.
  • 63. Maister. D,, & Wyckofi, D. 1974, Federal Express (A) & (B). Case No. 9-674-093-094. Boston: Harvard Business School Case Services. March, J. G. 1991. Exploration and exploitation in organiza- tion learning. Organization Science, 2: 71-87, March, J, G., & Olsen, ]. P. !975. Organizational learning under ambiguity. European Journal of Policy Review, 3(2): 147-171. Miller. D, 1990. The Icarus paradox: How exceptional compa- nies bring about their own downfall. New York: Harper Business, Mintzberg, H, 1994. The rise and fall of strategic planning. New York: Free Press, Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Free- man. Nonaka. L, & Takeuchi, H, 1995. The knowledge creating company. Oxiord, England: Oxford University Press. Pascale, R, T. 1990, Managing on the edge: How the smartest companies use conflict fo stay ahead. New York: Simon and Schuster. Pfeffer, I. 1993. Barrier to the advance of organizational sci- ence: Paradigm development as a dependent variable. Academy of Management Review. 18(4): 599-620, Pinchot, G.. & Pinchot. E. 1993. The end of bureaucracy and the rise of the intelligent organization, San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler,
  • 64. 1999 Crossan, Lane, and White 537 Polanyi, M. 1967. The tacit dimension. London: Routledge. Prietula. M. J., & Simon, H. A. 1989. The experts in your midst. Harvard Business Review, 61Ganuary-February): 120-124. Quinn. J, B, 1992, /nteiiigent enterprise: A tnowiedge and ser- vice based paradigm lor industry. New York: Free Press, Schumpeter, I. A. 1959. The theory ol economic development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, Seely-Brown, L 1993. Thinking, working, and leaming. In M, M. Crossan, H. W. Lane, J, Rush, & R. E. White (Eds.), Learning in organizations monograph: 81-99. London, Ontario: Western Business School. Seely-Brown, I. & Duguid, P, 1991. Organizational learning and communities of practice: Toward a uniiied view of working, learning and innovation. Organization Sci- ence, 2: 40-57. Senge, P. 1990. The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the learning organization. New York: Doubleday/Currency, Shrivastava, P. 1983. A typology ot organizational leaming systems. Journal ol Management Studies. 20(1): 7-28, Simon, H. A. 1991. Bounded rationality and organizational leaming. Organization Science. 2: 125-134. Simons, R. 1991, Strategic orientation and top management attention to control systems. Strategic Management
  • 65. Journal 12: 49-62. Simons, R. 1994, How new top managers use control systems as levers of strategic renewal. Strategic Management Journal 15: 169-189, Srivastava, S., & Barrett, F. J. 1988. The transforming nature oi metaphors in group development: A study in group the- ory. Human Relations. 41: 31-64. Stata, R. 1989. Organizational learning: The key to man- agement innovation. Sloan Management Review, 30{3): 63-74. Stewart, T. A. 1997. JnfeJJectua/ capital- The wealth of na- tions. New York: Doubleday/Currency. Sutton. R., & Staw, B, M. 1995. What theory is not. Adminis- trative Science QuarteWy. 40: 371-384, Tsoukas, H, 1991. The missing link: A transformational view oi metaphors in organizational science. Academy of Management Review, 16: 566-585. Underwood, B. J. 1982. Studies in learning and memory: Selected papers. New York: Praeger. Watkins, K. E., & Marsick, W. I. 1993, Scuipting the leaming organization. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Walsh. J. P. 1988. Selectivity and selective perception: An investigation of managers' beliel structures and infor- mation processing. Academy of Management Journal 31:873-896. Watson. I. D. 1969, The double helix. New York: Mentar
  • 66. Books. Weick, K. 1979. The sociai psychoiogy of organizing. Reading, MA.: Addison-Wesley. Weick, K, 1995a. What theory is not, theorizing is. Adminis- trative Science Quarterly, 40: 385-390. Weick, K. E. 1995b. Sensemaitingin organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, Weick, K.. & Roberts, K. 1993. Collective mind and organiza- tional reliability: The case of flight operations in an aircraft carrier deck. Adminisfrafiv© Science Quarterly, 38: 357-381, Weick, K.. & Van Orden, P. W. 1990. Organizing on a global scale: A research and teaching agenda. Human Re- source Management, 29: 49-61. Whetton, D. A. 1989. What constitutes a theoretical contribu- tion? Academy of Management Review, 14: 490-495. Mary M, Crossan is an associate professor in general management at the Richard Ivey School ol Business, She received her MBA and Ph.D. in business policy from the University of Western Ontario. Her research focuses on organizational learning and improvisation for strategic renewal. Henry W. Lane holds the Fred and Darla Brodsky Chair in International Business at Northeastern University in Boston. Until recently, he was at the Richard Ivey School of Business. He received his DBA in organizational behavior from
  • 67. the Harvard Business School. His research interests are intemationa] business and organizational leaming and strategic renewal, Roderick E. White is an associate professor in general management at the Richard Ivey School of Business, He received his DBA and MBA from Harvard University and his honors bachelor of arts (business) from the University of Western Ontario. His research interests focus on the renewal of mature organizations and the evolution of sociai structure. Running head: POLITICAL ROLES IN NATURAL DISASTER MANAGEMENT 1Political Roles in Natural Disaster Management Political Roles in Natural Disaster Management Student name: Course: Instructor name: Month, day, year:
  • 68. Abstract Natural disaster management is a fundamental act in the US both in economic, social-cultural, and political dimensions. Researchers and policymakers record that due to Hurricane Agnes and nuclear accidents in the United States of America, a rise to government participation at local and federal was evidence through the formation of the Federal Emergency Management Agency (FEMA) (Mener A, 2007). The purpose of this paper is to review and political analysis roles in planning, preparation, and response to a natural disaster. Introduction The impact and occurrence of natural disasters are on an administrative capacity. Despite the autonomy of genetic distance relationships with a political standpoint, human political actions display myriad ways in which political intervention leads to planning, preparation, and response to such circumstances beyond one's control (Cohen C and Weker E, 2008). Political designs and models are vital in disaster control, mitigation, and control over a given geographical jurisdiction and administration capability of political power in natural disasters such as earthquake and hurricane mitigation and monitoring. Despite an extensive literature review on the political contribution in disaster management, there is a need for further research to comprehend planning, preparation, and
  • 69. response to natural disasters from a political perspective. Planning Disaster planning is a systematic approach to natural disaster management and control. Politics play myriad roles in planning for natural disaster mitigation and control (Choudhury Z, 2013). Politics shape the systematic planning of natural disaster management prevention, alerts, response, and recovery of the losses. The federal and local government authority plays roles to control natural disaster in various approaches. Planning of inspection methods and alert preposition are vital in the preparation of the natural disaster mitigation. Administration formulated purposes and means to regulate and control potential natural hazard occurrence. Similarly, in planning to natural disaster counteraction, politics administer routinely monitor measures and precaution in the future presence of non-made made disaster. Furthermore, federal and local authorities participate in disaster planning through installation of automatic and computerized alarms in water disaster or hurricane to ensure timely evacuation of the victims. Moreover, the political setting of a region, accommodate mandate and administration privilege to create rules and regulation in planning to control natural disaster management. For instance, the creation of FEMA to respond to emergencies in managing natural disaster impacts at local and national levels of political environment establish guideline in panning to natural disaster control and mitigation. Planning creates a groundwork for natural disaster mitigation approaches and analysis. Preparation In emergency control and management, legislators are at the forefront to formulate policies and regulations for natural disaster mitigations (DeLeo R, 2018). Government institutions and provide a framework for natural disaster preparedness, influencing the participation of non-governmental organizations on the mitigation process. There are various strategies in which politics play roles in Preparation to natural disaster control.
  • 70. Firstly, the provision of policies and regulation by local and federal government establishes a ground plan for natural disaster mitigation. Secondly, astigmatic selection and alertness is a strategy useful for politics intervention and participation in natural disaster mitigation and control. Furthermore, political leadership skills and flexibility establish a significant role in predation to natural disaster control and management. Political perspective is, therefore, fundamental in preparation for natural disaster management in multiple methods; thus, a subject of concern in politics of natural disaster management across the globe. Response Response to a natural disaster is an integral segment in disaster management, showing positive response to hurricane cases in the United States of America. For instance, in Texas and Florida's example of a hurricane, the reports, analysis, and documentation indicates that the response was faster and reliable (Wilson C, Singer P, Creary M and Greer S, 2019). In the provision of safe, credible, and swift response, politics contributes to various systematic approaches to impact natural disaster management. Arousal of the disaster management team is a prominent response strategy to natural disaster control. Similarly, politics play a role in the identification and examination of the crisis in disaster management. Furthermore, identification of tasks such as alert, financial provision, and practical mechanism are variables of explicit influence from federal and regional governments. The politics role in response to natural disasters is vital since it provides a fundamental segment of recovery and compensation of the victims and resentment t in an en event of deadliest natural disasters. Conclusion Despite the absence of a direct correlation between natural disasters and politics, human factors, and involvement in controlling hazards and crises in a natural disaster are a direct influence of the political answerable and implementation. Therefore politics play fundamental roles in planning,
  • 71. preparation, and response to natural disaster occurrence, management, and control. Disaster management is a systematic approach that should simulate and implement methodical political, economic, and social perspectives on risk and natural disaster mitigation and prevention. Work Cited Choudhury, Z. (2013). Politics of Natural Disaster: How Governments Maintain Legitimacy in the Wake of Major Disasters, 1990 – 2010. Retrieved on February 15, 2020, from https://ir.uiowa.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=5618&context= etdDeLeo, R. (2018). Political Science Perspectives. Retrieved on February 15, 2020, from https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-319-92722-0_6 Mener, A. (2007). Disaster Response in the United States of America: An Analysis of the Bureaucratic and Political History of a Failing System. Retrieved from: http://www.unm.edu/~marivera/524%20readings%201/Mener_D isaster%20Response%20in%20the%20US.pdf Wilson, C., Singer, P., Creary, M, and Greer S. (2019). Quantifying inequities in US federal response to hurricane disaster in Texas and Florida compared with Puerto Rico. Retrieved from: https://gh.bmj.com/content/bmjgh/4/1/e001191.full.pdf