SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 10
Baixar para ler offline
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx



                                           Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

                                   Journal of Experimental Child
                                            Psychology
                                 journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp




‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an
invisible person inhibits cheating
Jared Piazza a,⇑, Jesse M. Bering b, Gordon Ingram c
a
    School of Psychology, University of Kent, Keynes College, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK
b
    Institute of Cognition and Culture, Queen’s University Belfast, Belfast BT7 1NN, Northern Ireland, UK
c
    Interactions Lab, School of Management, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK




a r t i c l e            i n f o                         a b s t r a c t

Article history:                                         Two child groups (5–6 and 8–9 years of age) participated in a chal-
Received 13 August 2010                                  lenging rule-following task while they were (a) told that they were
Revised 2 February 2011                                  in the presence of a watchful invisible person (‘‘Princess Alice’’), (b)
Available online xxxx
                                                         observed by a real adult, or (c) unsupervised. Children were cov-
                                                         ertly videotaped performing the task in the experimenter’s
Keywords:
                                                         absence. Older children had an easier time at following the rules
Supernatural beliefs
Cheating
                                                         but engaged in equal levels of purposeful cheating as the younger
Rule following                                           children. Importantly, children’s expressed belief in the invisible
Moral development                                        person significantly determined their cheating latency, and this
Inhibitory control                                       was true even after controlling for individual differences in tem-
Invisible                                                perament. When ‘‘skeptical’’ children were omitted from the anal-
                                                         ysis, the inhibitory effects of being told about Princess Alice were
                                                         equivalent to having a real adult present. Furthermore, skeptical
                                                         children cheated only after having first behaviorally disconfirmed
                                                         the ‘‘presence’’ of Princess Alice. The findings suggest that chil-
                                                         dren’s belief in a watchful invisible person tends to deter cheating.
                                                                                      Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.




Introduction

   A growing body of experimental research suggests that supernatural beliefs can promote altruistic
behavior and adherence to social norms, at least among adults (Bering, McLeod, & Shackelford, 2005;
Shariff & Norenzayan, 2007; Sosis & Ruffle, 2004). For example, Bering and colleagues (2005) found
that college students who were led to believe they occupied the same room as a ghost were better
than control participants at inhibiting their motivation to cheat on a competitive laboratory task.

    ⇑ Corresponding author.
      E-mail address: j.piazza@kent.ac.uk (J. Piazza).

0022-0965/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003


Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
2                         J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx


Likewise, Shariff and Norenzayan (2007) found that priming adults with religious words (e.g., God,
sacred) increased their generosity in an economic game compared with a control group who received
neutral primes. Presumably, supernatural beliefs promote prosocial behavior by eliciting reputational
concerns about being watched and punished by ‘‘morally interested’’ supernatural agents (see Bering,
2006; Boyer, 2003; Johnson & Bering, 2006; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008).
    Despite the accumulating experimental evidence that supernatural beliefs promote altruism and
rule following in adults, almost no evidence exists concerning the role of supernatural beliefs in chil-
dren’s rule following. To fill this gap, in the current experiment we sought to test the hypothesis that
belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating among children in an unsupervised context. Children
were introduced to a novel invisible person (‘‘Princess Alice’’) and were explicitly asked whether they
believed she was real. They then performed a challenging rule-based game in the absence of the
experimenter. We hypothesized that children who professed belief in the invisible person would be
less likely to cheat in the game compared with a control group of unsupervised children.
    However, we also had reason to believe that the mere exposure of information about a watchful
invisible person would inhibit cheating to a certain extent among ‘‘skeptical’’ children (i.e., children
who reported not believing in Princess Alice) as well insofar as these children may harbor an unex-
pressed ambivalence about the invisible person’s watchful presence. Thus, we assessed children’s
exploratory behavior aimed at testing the ‘‘presence’’ of Princess Alice (e.g., waving one’s hand over
a chair where she is said to be seated) in addition to their explicit beliefs. Based on observations drawn
from a previous pilot study, we expected that skeptical children would attempt to test their unex-
pressed ambivalence about the existence of the invisible person prior to cheating on the task and that
this disconfirmation of Princess Alice’s presence would serve to undermine the inhibitory benefits of
their exposure to information about the invisible person. Such a hypothesis is consistent with research
suggesting that children actively test their assumptions about the natural world rather than blindly
accepting every proposition presented to them by adults (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 1999; Harris,
2007). For example, young children reject information from adult informants who repeatedly present
falsifiably inaccurate information (Koenig & Harris, 2005). Thus, as our second hypothesis, we pre-
dicted that children who expressed disbelief in the invisible person would engage in agent-directed
exploratory behavior prior to cheating on the task.
    In addition to comparing a group of unsupervised children exposed to an invisible person with an
unsupervised control group, in the current design we included a comparison group of actually super-
vised children. Research suggests that children’s rule compliance in the absence of adult supervision
improves as children’s inhibitory control develops (Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Kochanska, Mur-
ray, & Coy, 1997; Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996). Inhibitory control involves
the capacity to suppress inappropriate responses or impulses. According to Kochanska (2002), chil-
dren pass through at least two stages of behavioral compliance: from ‘‘situational compliance’’ (i.e.,
halfhearted compliance that is contingent on sustained parental control) to ‘‘committed compliance’’
(i.e., wholehearted self-regulated compliance with parental norms). This developmental shift from sit-
uational to committed compliance usually occurs within the first 4 years of life (Kochanska et al.,
2001), although improvements continue on into school age (Kochanska et al., 1997). Given these
developmental differences, we assessed several components of children’s temperament that might af-
fect their level of committed compliance, including inhibitory control and impulsivity, via parental
ratings of children’s temperament. We hypothesized that older children would have an easier time
at following the rules than younger children due to their developmental advances in inhibitory con-
trol. Most important, however, we hypothesized that belief in a watchful invisible person would inhi-
bit cheating over and above individual differences in children’s temperament.


Method

Participants

   Participants were 39 5- and 6-year-olds (M = 5.76 years, SD = 0.58) and 29 8- and 9-year-olds
(M = 8.86 years, SD = 0.74) from Belfast, Northern Ireland. Among these children, 1 6-year-old failed

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx                     3


to demonstrate comprehension of the task and, therefore, was omitted from the sample, leaving a total
of 67 children (36 female and 31 male). Regarding ethnicity, 76% were from White European families
and 24% were from mixed or other ethnic backgrounds. Although religious backgrounds of the chil-
dren were not collected, there is reason to believe that our sample was religiously diverse; many of
our participants were drawn from a university area and were from highly educated and/or ethnically
diverse families. The age groups were selected on the basis of a pilot study that found that children
younger than 5 years of age were unresponsive to our invisible person manipulation and that by
7 years of age children readily voiced their doubts about the existence of the invisible person.


Design and measures

   The design was a 3 (Condition) Â 2 (Age Group) factorial: invisible agent (n5–6 = 14, n8–9 = 9), adult
supervision (n5–6 = 10, n8–9 = 10), or no supervision (n5–6 = 14, n8–9 = 10).

Invisible agent manipulation
   To introduce the invisible person, children in the invisible agent condition received the following
information from the experimenter:
   Before we begin the game, I would like to introduce you to someone very special. Her name is Prin-
   cess Alice. Have you ever heard of Princess Alice? [No child said ‘‘yes’’.] Well, let me tell you about
   her. Princess Alice is a friendly magical princess with a special ability. [She was described as
   ‘‘friendly’’ to reduce the risk of frightening children.] Do you think you know what her special abil-
   ity is? [No child guessed right.] Well, let me tell you. Princess Alice can make herself invisible. Do
   you know what invisible means? [Most children said, ‘‘It means you can’t see her.’’] That’s right—it
   means that you cannot see her even though she’s there! And guess what? Princess Alice is in the
   room with us right now and she is sitting in that chair. [The experimenter pointed to an empty lab
   chair. See Fig. 1 for the experimental setup.]

     In the adult supervision condition, an unfamiliar female adult confederate was seated in the same
lab chair on which children from the invisible agent condition were told Princess Alice was seated.
These children were informed that the confederate was there as a silent observer and would not be
playing the game with them. The confederate was instructed to minimize interaction with children
(e.g., no talking) while continuously observing them throughout the session. In keeping with the
‘‘friendly’’ nature of Princess Alice, the confederate was instructed to maintain a natural neutral
expression throughout the session but to gently smile at children if they attempted to engage with
the confederate (so as not to frighten children). In the control condition, children did not receive any
information about the invisible agent, nor were they supervised by an actual person; they were sim-
ply led to believe that they were left alone in the room during the rule-following task.

Belief in the invisible person and exploratory behavior
   For those children randomly assigned to the invisible agent condition, belief in the invisible
agent was measured with both an open-ended question and agent-directed exploratory behavior.
As a measure of explicit belief, children in the invisible agent condition were asked, ‘‘I want to
know what you think. Do you think Princess Alice is real?’’ The question was framed in terms
of children’s personal beliefs to discourage socially desirable responses. This occurred immediately
after being introduced to the invisible person. Responses were scored in terms of degree of belief
in Princess Alice (yes = 2, maybe = 1, no = 0). For the current purposes, children who answered ‘‘no’’
are referred to as ‘‘skeptical’’ children and children who answered ‘‘yes’’ are ‘‘nonskeptical’’ chil-
dren (children who answered ‘‘maybe’’ are treated separately from both skeptical and nonskeptical
children). A trained independent rater (blind to the hypotheses) coded instances of and latency to
first instance of agent-directed exploratory behavior, which included (a) reaching for the invisible
agent or feeling over her chair (for an example, see Fig. 1) and (b) visually attending to or gazing at
Princess Alice’s chair. For obvious reasons, both of these measures were used for the invisible agent
condition only.

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
4                         J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx




Fig. 1. Experimental setup (as viewed by a hidden camera in a mounted speaker). The Velcro target is on the far wall. The white
line on the ground was the line that participants were instructed not to cross while throwing. The chair on the far right is
Princess Alice’s chair. In this scene, a child inspects the chair for evidence of the presence of Princess Alice.



Rule-following throwing game
    The throwing game was adapted from Kochanska and colleagues (1997). Children threw Velcro
balls at a target to win a prize of their choice (a toy, a diary, or art supplies). To win, at least one ball
needed to stick to the center of the target while children followed three rules. Children were in-
structed that they had an unlimited number of attempts to strike the center of the target. The three
rules were (a) throwing the balls from behind a line marked on the ground approximately 6 feet from
the target, (b) throwing with their nondominant hand only (a wristband was worn on the correct hand
as a memory aid), and (c) throwing while facing away from the target (i.e., throwing behind their
back). The rules were meant to create a virtually impossible task where children would be motivated
to break at least one of the rules. Nevertheless, violations of the three rules were inherently ambiguous
in terms of their intentional (vs. accidental) nature. However, an incontrovertible measure of inten-
tional or full-blown cheating (labeled ‘‘full cheating’’) occurred when children manually placed a ball
on the center of the target in clear violation of multiple rules. Therefore, this full cheating response
(i.e., latency to first full cheat) served as our primary measure of cheating.


Dependent measures
    The experimenter and an independent rater (blind to the hypotheses) coded videos of the 3-min
trials for frequency of rule breaking (i.e., number of wrong-handed throws, line crossing, and facing
forward) and latency of rule breaking (i.e., lapsed time to first rule violation). Interrater agreement
was high (frequencies: a = .90–1.00; latencies: a = .95–.99). Frequencies and latencies of the three rule
violations were separately aggregated into indexes of wrong throws and rule-breaking latency, respec-
tively. Full cheating latency (i.e., lapsed time to first full cheating response) served as our third and
most important measure.


Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire
   Parents completed a shortened version of the Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire
(TMCQ) (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004), a parental report questionnaire that provides a multicomponent
index of temperament during middle childhood. Only those TMCQ components deemed as relevant to
rule following and supernatural beliefs were included in the current study, and these were activation
control (capacity to perform an action when there is a strong tendency to avoid it), activity level (level
of gross motor activity, including rate and extent of locomotion), affiliation (desire for warmth and
closeness with others), attention (capacity to maintain attentional focus on tasks), fantasy/openness

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx                     5


(active imagination and curiosity), fear (negative affectivity within potentially threatening situations),
impulsivity (speed of response initiation), inhibitory control (capacity to plan and suppress inappropri-
ate approach responses under instructions or in novel situations), and shyness (slow or inhibited speed
of social approach). In particular, fantasy/openness and shyness were expected to relate to belief in
Princess Alice, based on previous findings (Taylor, 1999), whereas impulsivity and inhibitory control
were expected to relate to cheating inhibition, as found in previous research (Kochanska et al., 1997).
Internal reliabilities (Cronbach’s a) ranged from .71 to .90.

Procedure

    Children were tested individually in a campus laboratory by a male experimenter (the first author).
All sessions were covertly videotaped and lasted approximately 20 to 30 min. When children first ar-
rived with their parent(s), they performed a warm-up game with the experimenter that involved
throwing balls at the target prior to the administration of the rules. Next, parents were moved to a
separate adjacent room and filled out the TMCQ while children were instructed on the task. After
receiving the instructions, children performed a pretest to demonstrate that they comprehended
the rules of the game. This pretest entailed performing six sequential throws without violating any
of the rules. After the pretest, children performed the game on their own following a cover story where
the experimenter removed himself from the room for 3 min. In the experimental conditions, before
leaving, the experimenter reminded children that Princess Alice (or the female confederate) would
be ‘‘watching’’ them. When the experimenter returned, he revealed that he had accidentally given
children the wrong rules, and so they played an amended version of the game that guaranteed they
received a prize. At the end, children were verbally debriefed and it was made clear to them that Prin-
cess Alice was ‘‘not real’’ but rather ‘‘pretend’’.


Results

Preliminary analysis of gender

   Preliminary analyses with gender included as an independent variable revealed no main effect of
gender or interaction effect on any of the dependent measures (all ps > .12). Thus, gender was omitted
from further analysis.

Overview of main analyses

   To test the first hypothesis that children who expressed belief in Princess Alice would inhibit their
cheating more than the control group, we conducted a chi-square analysis of full cheating occurrences
by condition and a 2 (Age) Â 3 (Condition) between-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) on full
cheating latency, followed by planned contrasts. This was done first with all children in the analysis
and then with skeptical children omitted. To test the second hypothesis that skeptical children would
engage in exploratory behavior prior to cheating, we conducted a paired samples t test of skeptical
children’s exploratory behavior latency and full cheating latency. To test the hypothesis that older
children would have an easier time at following the rules than younger children, we conducted a 2
(Age) Â 3 (Condition) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on wrong throws and rule-breaking
latency scores, followed by simple effects tests. Finally, to assess the hypothesis that belief in the invis-
ible person would inhibit cheating over and above children’s temperament, we conducted a hierarchi-
cal regression of expressed belief and temperament variables on full cheating latency.

Belief in the invisible person and cheating

   With all children in the analysis, there was a nearly significant main effect of condition on occur-
rences of full cheating (invisible person = 26%, supervision = 10%, no supervision = 42%), v2(2) = 5.58,
p = .06, and a significant main effect of condition on full cheating latency, F(2, 61) = 3.44, p = .04,

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
6                          J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx


g2 = .10, but no effect of age, F(1, 61) < 1, ns (interaction F < 1, ns). Simple contrasts revealed that great-
er resistance to cheating in the adult supervision condition (M = 168.60, SD = 39.51) compared with
control (M = 122.13, SD = 75.72), t(64) = 2.58, p = .01, was the primary source of the main effect. How-
ever, full cheating latency was also greater for the invisible agent condition (M = 150.09, SD = 54.44)
compared with control, albeit not quite significantly, t(64) = 1.61, p = .11, with all children in the
analysis.
    In the invisible agent condition, 11 children (48%) professed belief in Princess Alice, 5 (22%) were
unsure, and 7 (30%) professed their disbelief. Consistent with our pilot study, older children were sig-
nificantly more skeptical of Princess Alice being real (yes = 11%, maybe = 44%, no = 44%) than younger
children (yes = 71%, maybe = 7%, no = 21%), v2(2) = 8.63, p = .01. An independent t test of skeptical ver-
sus nonskeptical children of all ages showed that children who professed belief in the invisible person
resisted cheating to a significantly greater degree (M = 164.27, SD = 52.16) than children who pro-
fessed disbelief (M = 106.43, SD = 54.71), t(16) = 2.25, p = .04. In terms of cheating frequency, only 1
of 11 children (9%) who professed belief or uncertainty manually placed the ball on the target,
whereas 5 of 7 children (71%) who professed disbelief committed a full cheating response, and this
difference was significantly greater than chance occurrence, v2(1) = 7.48, p = .006.
    Importantly, when the 7 children who professed disbelief in the invisible agent were removed from
the sample and contrasts of the three conditions were conducted a second time, the contrast between
the invisible agent and control conditions for full cheating latency was significant (Ms = 169.19 and
122.13, respectively), t(57) = 2.53, p = .01, and the means for the two experimental conditions were
equivalent (see Fig. 2); that is, as predicted, children who professed belief in the invisible agent dis-
played significantly greater resistance to cheating than control participants and at a level equal to chil-
dren in the adult supervision condition.




Fig. 2. Mean full cheating latency as a function of children’s age (in years) and condition, with skeptical children removed from
the analysis. With skeptical children removed, cheating latencies from both experimental conditions were significantly different
from control (with a = .05).


Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx                     7


Exploratory behavior and timing of cheating among skeptical children

    Among children in the invisible person condition, expressed belief correlated negatively with the
amount of exploratory reaching for Princess Alice, r(23) = –.24, p = .27, albeit not significantly. Belief
did not correlate with visual attending to her chair, r(23) = .09, ns. In other words, skeptical children
were more likely to reach for Princess Alice than nonskeptical children, but skeptical children were
equally likely to visually search for her. Of the 5 skeptical children who committed a full cheating re-
sponse, all of them engaged in some kind of exploratory behavior aimed at testing the presence of
Princess Alice (see Fig. 1). These skeptical children engaged in exploratory behavior for, on average,
63.00 s (SD = 63.94) prior to engaging in a full cheating response. The paired samples t test confirmed
that this duration of time was nearly significant, t(5) = 2.41, p = .06. Thus, as we hypothesized, skep-
tical children’s full cheating occurred only after they first obtained disconfirming evidence for Princess
Alice’s existence (e.g., reaching for Princess Alice and feeling nothing there).

Age and rule breaking

    As expected, there was a significant main effect of age on rule-breaking latency, F(1, 61) = 9.00,
p < .01, g2 = .13, with older children violating rules later (M = 144.29, SD = 38.30) than younger chil-
dren (M = 106.66, SD = 56.32), but no effect of condition, F(2, 61) = 1.65, p = .20, g2 = .05 (interaction
F < 1, ns). There was no main effect of age or condition on wrong throws (ps > .20). However, there
was a significant interaction of age and condition, F(2, 61) = 3.48, p = .04, g2 = .10. Simple effects tests
revealed that for older children, adult supervision led to fewer wrong throws (M = 0.80, SD = 1.32)
compared with the invisible agent (M = 3.89, SD = 3.82), t(26) = 2.06, p = .05, and control (M = 3.90,
SD = 4.01), t(26) = 2.12, p = .04. Unsupervised children and children in the invisible agent condition
did not differ (t = 0). For younger children, information about an invisible person led to fewer wrong
throws (M = 4.64, SD = 4.73) compared with adult supervision (M = 10.80, SD = 10.45), t(35) = 2.07,
p = .05, but not significantly fewer than control (M = 7.14, SD = 6.38), t(35) = 1.23, p > .22. The contrast
of control and supervision was not significant (p > .35). A simple effects test of the effect of age on
wrong throws within the adult supervision group revealed that younger children made significantly
more wrong throws (M = 10.80, SD = 10.45) than older children (M = 0.80, SD = 1.31), F(1, 18) = 9.01,
p = .008, g2 = .33. However, there was no difference due to age within the other two conditions
(Fs < 2.01, ps > .16).
    Thus, in sum, older children were able to resist breaking the rules longer than younger children.
Moreover, the presence of a watchful adult led to fewer wrong throws for older children than imag-
ining an invisible person or no supervision, whereas the presence of a watchful adult led to more
wrong throws among younger children compared with the imagined presence of a watchful invisible
person (see Discussion for possible explanations for this latter unexpected finding).

Belief in the invisible person and individual differences in temperament

    A preliminary ANOVA revealed that there were no differences in any of the temperament variables
across conditions (all ps > .20). Simple correlations revealed three components of temperament that
correlated with full cheating latency: activation control, r(67) = .26, p = .04, fear, r(67) = –.27, p = .03,
and shyness, r(67) = –.24, p = .05 (all other ps > .07). Although inhibitory control had a positive rela-
tionship with cheating inhibition, r(23) = .16, p = .20, it was not significant. The only component of
temperament that correlated with expressed belief in Princess Alice was shyness, r(23) = –.43,
p = .04, an inverse relationship (all other ps > .14). Unexpectedly, fantasy/openness failed to correlate
significantly with expressed belief, although the correlation was in the expected direction, r(23) = .29,
p = .18.
    To test whether individual differences in children’s temperament could not explain the role of ex-
pressed belief in inhibiting cheating; a hierarchical regression was conducted on full cheating latency
for children in the invisible agent condition. Degree of belief in Princess Alice (yes = 2, maybe = 1,
no = 0) was entered into the regression first (step 1), followed by the relevant temperament variables
at step 2. As depicted in Table 1, belief explained a significant proportion of the variance at step 1 and

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
8                          J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx


Table 1
Hierarchical regression analysis of full cheating latency in the invisible agent condition.

    Model                                                                              b                                t(21)
    Step 1                             Belief                                          .44                              2.21*
    Step 2                             Belief                                          .65                              3.41**
                                       Activation control                              À.30                             À1.69
                                       Fear                                            À.43                             À2.24*
                                       Impulsivity                                     À.78                             À3.41**
                                       Inhibitory control                              À.52                             À2.10
                                       Shyness                                         À.09                             <1.00

Note: Belief was scored in terms of responses to the question, ‘‘Do you believe that Princess Alice is real?’’ (yes = 2, maybe = 1,
no = 0). Belief was entered at step 1, and temperament variables were entered at step 2. Belief remained a significant predictor
of full cheating latency even after controlling for children’s temperament.
 *
   p < .05.
**
   p < .01.



remained a significant predictor of cheating latency at step 2. In addition, impulsivity and fear
emerged as significant negative predictors of full cheating latency; the more impulsive and fearful
children were, the quicker they engaged in a full cheating response. In summary, although impulsivity
and fear predicted cheating, these components were unrelated to belief in the invisible person, and
expressed belief explained a significant proportion of cheating inhibition not explained by individual
differences in children’s temperament.


Discussion

   As hypothesized, children who professed belief in the invisible person were less likely to cheat on a
challenging rule-based game than children who simply performed the task in the absence of adult
supervision. Moreover, children who professed belief in the invisible person resisted cheating equally
to children who performed the same task in the presence of an actual watchful adult. In contrast, the
majority of skeptical children fully cheated on the task at some point in the experiment. However,
investigation of the timing of skeptical children’s transgressions and their exploration of the alleged
invisible person revealed that most of these skeptical children were secretly ambivalent about her
presence. In fact, it was not until they confirmed their null hypothesis about the existence of Princess
Alice that they went on to cheat on the task. In short, belief in the invisible person (whether expressed
or unexpressed) helped children to resist cheating in the absence of the experimenter’s supervision.
Moreover, belief in the invisible person predicted cheating inhibition over and above the influence
of children’s temperament, including individual differences in impulsivity and fearfulness.
   Our findings are consistent with previous findings demonstrating the prosocial effects of priming
adults with supernatural agents (Bering et al., 2005) or religious concepts (Shariff & Norenzayan,
2007) and, more broadly, with an evolutionary rationale (Bering, 2006; Boyer, 2003; Johnson & Bering,
2006; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008) arguing that the representation of watchful supernatural agents
afforded ancestral humans with adaptive social benefits (i.e., inhibiting selfish behavior under condi-
tions in which the threat of actual human detection was underestimated and, therefore, may have pro-
tected an individual’s reputation). Our findings are also consistent with theory and research
suggesting that children’s ability to reason about supernatural agents develops naturally alongside
their ability to reason about natural human agents (Barrett, 2000; Kelemen, 2004; Lane, Wellman,
& Evans, 2010).
   Importantly, the current findings demonstrate that the prosocial effects of imagining morally con-
cerned supernatural agents are not limited to adults (e.g., Bering et al., 2005) but rather extend to chil-
dren as young as 5 or 6 years of age. Second, they demonstrate that the prosocial effects of priming
thoughts about supernatural agents are not limited to culturally based religious concepts (e.g., God,
ancestral spirits) but rather are easily generalized to novel supernatural agents to which children have
had no previous exposure. Finally, our findings suggest that it is not just ‘‘believers’’ who benefit from

Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx                         9


the cognitive capacity to represent supernatural agency; even skeptical children, who professed their
disbelief in the invisible person, were reluctant to cheat until they had gathered enough empirical evi-
dence that Princess Alice was indeed not real. This suggests the intriguing hypothesis that supernat-
ural agent concepts may, under certain conditions, motivate even skeptics to refrain from antisocial
behavior.
    It is currently unclear, however, which specific aspects of our manipulation motivated rule compli-
ance. Were those children who followed the rules in the Princess Alice condition doing so simply to
please her, because they were afraid of her punishing them for cheating, or because they were afraid
that she would somehow communicate their misdeeds to the experimenter? Future work in this area,
therefore, would benefit from controlled manipulations of specific aspects of the supernatural agent
(e.g., her character, perceptual capacities, and communicative abilities). In addition, it is unclear
why being primed with a supernatural agent concept led to fewer wrong throws among younger chil-
dren than having an actual watchful adult present in the room (although not less so than the control
condition). One possibility is that young children interpreted the watchful adult’s gentle smiling and
ostensible reluctance to reprimand them (social cues that were absent in the invisible agent condition)
as a tacit endorsement of their rule breaking while failing to consider the confederate as a potential
confidant of the experimenter. In contrast, older children may have been more concerned with the
watchful adult or invisible agent reporting their rule violations to the experimenter. In short, develop-
mental advances in the understanding of tattling and gossip (see Hill & Pillow, 2006; Ingram & Bering,
2010) might explain why an adult’s watchful presence promoted rule adherence among older children
but not among younger children. However, it is important to point out that these differences due to
age are limited to acts of rule breaking that were potential accidents. In contrast, the two age groups
performed at equivalent levels with respect to full-blown cheating.
    Finally, although Princess Alice was presented as a ‘‘real’’ agent rather than as a pretend character,
it is still possible that some children inferred such a make-believe context. However, nothing in the
experimenter’s tone or speech indicated that it was a play context. The experimenter presented Prin-
cess Alice in a factual manner and solicited children’s personal beliefs on the matter. Regardless of the
reasons behind children’s expression of belief, those who stated that they believed in the supernatural
agent adhered more closely to the rules. Of course, because we could not experimentally manipulate
children’s belief in Princess Alice, the possibility remains that another variable might better explain
the observed findings; for example, it may be that children who expressed belief in Princess Alice have
internalized the importance of following rules to a greater extent than children who expressed disbe-
lief, or perhaps nonskeptics were more motivated to please the experimenter. Future studies should
address these issues by comparing the inhibitory effects of supernatural agent primes in a context
of pretend play versus one of serious instruction while controlling for individual differences in chil-
dren’s commitment to following rules.


Acknowledgments

    This research is an output from a project undertaken as part of the Cognition, Religion, and Theol-
ogy Project at the University of Oxford, funded by the John Templeton Foundation via a grant to Jared
Piazza. The views expressed are not necessarily those of the Cognition, Religion, and Theology Project,
the University of Oxford, or the John Templeton Foundation. We also thank Sarah Allen and Amy Puri-
foy for their assistance with data collection, serving in the confederate role, and recruiting participants
for the study.


References

Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29–34.
Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 29, 453–462.
Bering, J. M., McLeod, K., & Shackelford, T. K. (2005). Reasoning about dead agents reveals possible adaptive trends. Human
    Nature, 16, 360–381.
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behavior as by-products of the brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119–124.
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind. New York:
    William Morrow.


Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
10                         J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx


Harris, P. L. (2007). Trust. Developmental Science, 10, 135–138.
Hill, V., & Pillow, B. H. (2006). Children’s understanding of reputations. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 137–157.
Ingram, G. P. D., & Bering, J. M. (2010). Children’s tattling: The reporting of everyday norm violations in preschool settings. Child
     Development, 81, 945–957.
Johnson, D., & Bering, J. (2006). Hand of god, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation.
     Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 219–233.
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children ‘‘intuitive theists’’? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15,
     295–301.
Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology,
     38, 339–351.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child
     Development, 72, 1091–1111.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to
     early school age. Child Development, 68, 263–277.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role
     in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.
Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261–1277.
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Evans, E. M. (2010). Children’s understanding of ordinary and extraordinary minds. Child
     Development, 81, 1475–1489.
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 58–62.
Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous
     economic game. Psychological Science, 18, 803–809.
Simonds, J., & Rothbart, M.K. (2004). The Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ): A computerized self-report
     instrument for ages 7–10. Poster session presented at the Occasional Temperament Conference, Athens, GA. Retrieved from
     http://www.bowdoin.edu/$sputnam/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/pdf/TMCQ_Poster_Oct04.pdf on 7 November
     2010.
Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2004). Ideology, religion, and the evolution of cooperation: Field tests on Israeli kibbutzim. Research in
     Economic Anthropology, 23, 89–117.
Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. New York: Oxford University Press.




Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible
person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Semelhante a ''Princess alice'' is watching you (piazza et al. 2011)

Lifespan development early childhood
Lifespan development   early childhoodLifespan development   early childhood
Lifespan development early childhoodClaudia Megele
 
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docx
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docxNature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docx
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docxgemaherd
 
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying Children
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying ChildrenTopic 5 - Research Methods for Studying Children
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying ChildrenDaniel Bigler
 
Anomalistic psychology - Superstitions
Anomalistic psychology - SuperstitionsAnomalistic psychology - Superstitions
Anomalistic psychology - SuperstitionsGemma_Talbot
 

Semelhante a ''Princess alice'' is watching you (piazza et al. 2011) (8)

Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god cross-cultural evidenc...
Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god   cross-cultural evidenc...Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god   cross-cultural evidenc...
Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god cross-cultural evidenc...
 
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
 
Lifespan development early childhood
Lifespan development   early childhoodLifespan development   early childhood
Lifespan development early childhood
 
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docx
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docxNature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docx
Nature VS NurtureResearch writing 310Joi Tucker.docx
 
Joint Family
Joint FamilyJoint Family
Joint Family
 
Attachment Theory
Attachment TheoryAttachment Theory
Attachment Theory
 
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying Children
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying ChildrenTopic 5 - Research Methods for Studying Children
Topic 5 - Research Methods for Studying Children
 
Anomalistic psychology - Superstitions
Anomalistic psychology - SuperstitionsAnomalistic psychology - Superstitions
Anomalistic psychology - Superstitions
 

Mais de SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A.

Mais de SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A. (20)

Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete bookReligion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
 
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
 
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete bookFaces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
 
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
 
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
 
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
 
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
 
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
 
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
 
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
 
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
 
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
 
The relative unnaturalness of atheism on why geertz and markússon are both ...
The relative unnaturalness of atheism   on why geertz and markússon are both ...The relative unnaturalness of atheism   on why geertz and markússon are both ...
The relative unnaturalness of atheism on why geertz and markússon are both ...
 
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the selfThe sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
 
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
 
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
 
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
 
The evolution of religion how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
The evolution of religion   how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...The evolution of religion   how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
The evolution of religion how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
 
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
 
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
 

Último

Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...
Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...
Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...Igalia
 
How to convert PDF to text with Nanonets
How to convert PDF to text with NanonetsHow to convert PDF to text with Nanonets
How to convert PDF to text with Nanonetsnaman860154
 
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and Myths
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and MythsArtificial Intelligence: Facts and Myths
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and MythsJoaquim Jorge
 
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024Rafal Los
 
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptx
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptxEIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptx
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptxEarley Information Science
 
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law Developments
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law DevelopmentsTrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law Developments
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law DevelopmentsTrustArc
 
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI Solutions
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI SolutionsIAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI Solutions
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI SolutionsEnterprise Knowledge
 
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path Mount
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path MountBreaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path Mount
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path MountPuma Security, LLC
 
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?Igalia
 
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your Business
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your BusinessAdvantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your Business
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your BusinessPixlogix Infotech
 
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking MenDelhi Call girls
 
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processors
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone ProcessorsExploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processors
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processorsdebabhi2
 
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdf
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdfUnderstanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdf
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdfUK Journal
 
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time Automation
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time AutomationFrom Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time Automation
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time AutomationSafe Software
 
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slide
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slideHistor y of HAM Radio presentation slide
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slidevu2urc
 
GenCyber Cyber Security Day Presentation
GenCyber Cyber Security Day PresentationGenCyber Cyber Security Day Presentation
GenCyber Cyber Security Day PresentationMichael W. Hawkins
 
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreter
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreterPresentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreter
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreternaman860154
 
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024The Digital Insurer
 
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking MenDelhi Call girls
 
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024The Digital Insurer
 

Último (20)

Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...
Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...
Raspberry Pi 5: Challenges and Solutions in Bringing up an OpenGL/Vulkan Driv...
 
How to convert PDF to text with Nanonets
How to convert PDF to text with NanonetsHow to convert PDF to text with Nanonets
How to convert PDF to text with Nanonets
 
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and Myths
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and MythsArtificial Intelligence: Facts and Myths
Artificial Intelligence: Facts and Myths
 
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024
The 7 Things I Know About Cyber Security After 25 Years | April 2024
 
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptx
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptxEIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptx
EIS-Webinar-Prompt-Knowledge-Eng-2024-04-08.pptx
 
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law Developments
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law DevelopmentsTrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law Developments
TrustArc Webinar - Stay Ahead of US State Data Privacy Law Developments
 
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI Solutions
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI SolutionsIAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI Solutions
IAC 2024 - IA Fast Track to Search Focused AI Solutions
 
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path Mount
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path MountBreaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path Mount
Breaking the Kubernetes Kill Chain: Host Path Mount
 
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?
A Year of the Servo Reboot: Where Are We Now?
 
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your Business
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your BusinessAdvantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your Business
Advantages of Hiring UIUX Design Service Providers for Your Business
 
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Friends Colony Women Seeking Men
 
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processors
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone ProcessorsExploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processors
Exploring the Future Potential of AI-Enabled Smartphone Processors
 
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdf
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdfUnderstanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdf
Understanding Discord NSFW Servers A Guide for Responsible Users.pdf
 
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time Automation
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time AutomationFrom Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time Automation
From Event to Action: Accelerate Your Decision Making with Real-Time Automation
 
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slide
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slideHistor y of HAM Radio presentation slide
Histor y of HAM Radio presentation slide
 
GenCyber Cyber Security Day Presentation
GenCyber Cyber Security Day PresentationGenCyber Cyber Security Day Presentation
GenCyber Cyber Security Day Presentation
 
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreter
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreterPresentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreter
Presentation on how to chat with PDF using ChatGPT code interpreter
 
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Axa Assurance Maroc - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
 
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men
08448380779 Call Girls In Civil Lines Women Seeking Men
 
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
Bajaj Allianz Life Insurance Company - Insurer Innovation Award 2024
 

''Princess alice'' is watching you (piazza et al. 2011)

  • 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating Jared Piazza a,⇑, Jesse M. Bering b, Gordon Ingram c a School of Psychology, University of Kent, Keynes College, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK b Institute of Cognition and Culture, Queen’s University Belfast, Belfast BT7 1NN, Northern Ireland, UK c Interactions Lab, School of Management, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Two child groups (5–6 and 8–9 years of age) participated in a chal- Received 13 August 2010 lenging rule-following task while they were (a) told that they were Revised 2 February 2011 in the presence of a watchful invisible person (‘‘Princess Alice’’), (b) Available online xxxx observed by a real adult, or (c) unsupervised. Children were cov- ertly videotaped performing the task in the experimenter’s Keywords: absence. Older children had an easier time at following the rules Supernatural beliefs Cheating but engaged in equal levels of purposeful cheating as the younger Rule following children. Importantly, children’s expressed belief in the invisible Moral development person significantly determined their cheating latency, and this Inhibitory control was true even after controlling for individual differences in tem- Invisible perament. When ‘‘skeptical’’ children were omitted from the anal- ysis, the inhibitory effects of being told about Princess Alice were equivalent to having a real adult present. Furthermore, skeptical children cheated only after having first behaviorally disconfirmed the ‘‘presence’’ of Princess Alice. The findings suggest that chil- dren’s belief in a watchful invisible person tends to deter cheating. Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction A growing body of experimental research suggests that supernatural beliefs can promote altruistic behavior and adherence to social norms, at least among adults (Bering, McLeod, & Shackelford, 2005; Shariff & Norenzayan, 2007; Sosis & Ruffle, 2004). For example, Bering and colleagues (2005) found that college students who were led to believe they occupied the same room as a ghost were better than control participants at inhibiting their motivation to cheat on a competitive laboratory task. ⇑ Corresponding author. E-mail address: j.piazza@kent.ac.uk (J. Piazza). 0022-0965/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003 Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 2. 2 J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Likewise, Shariff and Norenzayan (2007) found that priming adults with religious words (e.g., God, sacred) increased their generosity in an economic game compared with a control group who received neutral primes. Presumably, supernatural beliefs promote prosocial behavior by eliciting reputational concerns about being watched and punished by ‘‘morally interested’’ supernatural agents (see Bering, 2006; Boyer, 2003; Johnson & Bering, 2006; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008). Despite the accumulating experimental evidence that supernatural beliefs promote altruism and rule following in adults, almost no evidence exists concerning the role of supernatural beliefs in chil- dren’s rule following. To fill this gap, in the current experiment we sought to test the hypothesis that belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating among children in an unsupervised context. Children were introduced to a novel invisible person (‘‘Princess Alice’’) and were explicitly asked whether they believed she was real. They then performed a challenging rule-based game in the absence of the experimenter. We hypothesized that children who professed belief in the invisible person would be less likely to cheat in the game compared with a control group of unsupervised children. However, we also had reason to believe that the mere exposure of information about a watchful invisible person would inhibit cheating to a certain extent among ‘‘skeptical’’ children (i.e., children who reported not believing in Princess Alice) as well insofar as these children may harbor an unex- pressed ambivalence about the invisible person’s watchful presence. Thus, we assessed children’s exploratory behavior aimed at testing the ‘‘presence’’ of Princess Alice (e.g., waving one’s hand over a chair where she is said to be seated) in addition to their explicit beliefs. Based on observations drawn from a previous pilot study, we expected that skeptical children would attempt to test their unex- pressed ambivalence about the existence of the invisible person prior to cheating on the task and that this disconfirmation of Princess Alice’s presence would serve to undermine the inhibitory benefits of their exposure to information about the invisible person. Such a hypothesis is consistent with research suggesting that children actively test their assumptions about the natural world rather than blindly accepting every proposition presented to them by adults (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 1999; Harris, 2007). For example, young children reject information from adult informants who repeatedly present falsifiably inaccurate information (Koenig & Harris, 2005). Thus, as our second hypothesis, we pre- dicted that children who expressed disbelief in the invisible person would engage in agent-directed exploratory behavior prior to cheating on the task. In addition to comparing a group of unsupervised children exposed to an invisible person with an unsupervised control group, in the current design we included a comparison group of actually super- vised children. Research suggests that children’s rule compliance in the absence of adult supervision improves as children’s inhibitory control develops (Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Kochanska, Mur- ray, & Coy, 1997; Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996). Inhibitory control involves the capacity to suppress inappropriate responses or impulses. According to Kochanska (2002), chil- dren pass through at least two stages of behavioral compliance: from ‘‘situational compliance’’ (i.e., halfhearted compliance that is contingent on sustained parental control) to ‘‘committed compliance’’ (i.e., wholehearted self-regulated compliance with parental norms). This developmental shift from sit- uational to committed compliance usually occurs within the first 4 years of life (Kochanska et al., 2001), although improvements continue on into school age (Kochanska et al., 1997). Given these developmental differences, we assessed several components of children’s temperament that might af- fect their level of committed compliance, including inhibitory control and impulsivity, via parental ratings of children’s temperament. We hypothesized that older children would have an easier time at following the rules than younger children due to their developmental advances in inhibitory con- trol. Most important, however, we hypothesized that belief in a watchful invisible person would inhi- bit cheating over and above individual differences in children’s temperament. Method Participants Participants were 39 5- and 6-year-olds (M = 5.76 years, SD = 0.58) and 29 8- and 9-year-olds (M = 8.86 years, SD = 0.74) from Belfast, Northern Ireland. Among these children, 1 6-year-old failed Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 3. J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx 3 to demonstrate comprehension of the task and, therefore, was omitted from the sample, leaving a total of 67 children (36 female and 31 male). Regarding ethnicity, 76% were from White European families and 24% were from mixed or other ethnic backgrounds. Although religious backgrounds of the chil- dren were not collected, there is reason to believe that our sample was religiously diverse; many of our participants were drawn from a university area and were from highly educated and/or ethnically diverse families. The age groups were selected on the basis of a pilot study that found that children younger than 5 years of age were unresponsive to our invisible person manipulation and that by 7 years of age children readily voiced their doubts about the existence of the invisible person. Design and measures The design was a 3 (Condition) Â 2 (Age Group) factorial: invisible agent (n5–6 = 14, n8–9 = 9), adult supervision (n5–6 = 10, n8–9 = 10), or no supervision (n5–6 = 14, n8–9 = 10). Invisible agent manipulation To introduce the invisible person, children in the invisible agent condition received the following information from the experimenter: Before we begin the game, I would like to introduce you to someone very special. Her name is Prin- cess Alice. Have you ever heard of Princess Alice? [No child said ‘‘yes’’.] Well, let me tell you about her. Princess Alice is a friendly magical princess with a special ability. [She was described as ‘‘friendly’’ to reduce the risk of frightening children.] Do you think you know what her special abil- ity is? [No child guessed right.] Well, let me tell you. Princess Alice can make herself invisible. Do you know what invisible means? [Most children said, ‘‘It means you can’t see her.’’] That’s right—it means that you cannot see her even though she’s there! And guess what? Princess Alice is in the room with us right now and she is sitting in that chair. [The experimenter pointed to an empty lab chair. See Fig. 1 for the experimental setup.] In the adult supervision condition, an unfamiliar female adult confederate was seated in the same lab chair on which children from the invisible agent condition were told Princess Alice was seated. These children were informed that the confederate was there as a silent observer and would not be playing the game with them. The confederate was instructed to minimize interaction with children (e.g., no talking) while continuously observing them throughout the session. In keeping with the ‘‘friendly’’ nature of Princess Alice, the confederate was instructed to maintain a natural neutral expression throughout the session but to gently smile at children if they attempted to engage with the confederate (so as not to frighten children). In the control condition, children did not receive any information about the invisible agent, nor were they supervised by an actual person; they were sim- ply led to believe that they were left alone in the room during the rule-following task. Belief in the invisible person and exploratory behavior For those children randomly assigned to the invisible agent condition, belief in the invisible agent was measured with both an open-ended question and agent-directed exploratory behavior. As a measure of explicit belief, children in the invisible agent condition were asked, ‘‘I want to know what you think. Do you think Princess Alice is real?’’ The question was framed in terms of children’s personal beliefs to discourage socially desirable responses. This occurred immediately after being introduced to the invisible person. Responses were scored in terms of degree of belief in Princess Alice (yes = 2, maybe = 1, no = 0). For the current purposes, children who answered ‘‘no’’ are referred to as ‘‘skeptical’’ children and children who answered ‘‘yes’’ are ‘‘nonskeptical’’ chil- dren (children who answered ‘‘maybe’’ are treated separately from both skeptical and nonskeptical children). A trained independent rater (blind to the hypotheses) coded instances of and latency to first instance of agent-directed exploratory behavior, which included (a) reaching for the invisible agent or feeling over her chair (for an example, see Fig. 1) and (b) visually attending to or gazing at Princess Alice’s chair. For obvious reasons, both of these measures were used for the invisible agent condition only. Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 4. 4 J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Fig. 1. Experimental setup (as viewed by a hidden camera in a mounted speaker). The Velcro target is on the far wall. The white line on the ground was the line that participants were instructed not to cross while throwing. The chair on the far right is Princess Alice’s chair. In this scene, a child inspects the chair for evidence of the presence of Princess Alice. Rule-following throwing game The throwing game was adapted from Kochanska and colleagues (1997). Children threw Velcro balls at a target to win a prize of their choice (a toy, a diary, or art supplies). To win, at least one ball needed to stick to the center of the target while children followed three rules. Children were in- structed that they had an unlimited number of attempts to strike the center of the target. The three rules were (a) throwing the balls from behind a line marked on the ground approximately 6 feet from the target, (b) throwing with their nondominant hand only (a wristband was worn on the correct hand as a memory aid), and (c) throwing while facing away from the target (i.e., throwing behind their back). The rules were meant to create a virtually impossible task where children would be motivated to break at least one of the rules. Nevertheless, violations of the three rules were inherently ambiguous in terms of their intentional (vs. accidental) nature. However, an incontrovertible measure of inten- tional or full-blown cheating (labeled ‘‘full cheating’’) occurred when children manually placed a ball on the center of the target in clear violation of multiple rules. Therefore, this full cheating response (i.e., latency to first full cheat) served as our primary measure of cheating. Dependent measures The experimenter and an independent rater (blind to the hypotheses) coded videos of the 3-min trials for frequency of rule breaking (i.e., number of wrong-handed throws, line crossing, and facing forward) and latency of rule breaking (i.e., lapsed time to first rule violation). Interrater agreement was high (frequencies: a = .90–1.00; latencies: a = .95–.99). Frequencies and latencies of the three rule violations were separately aggregated into indexes of wrong throws and rule-breaking latency, respec- tively. Full cheating latency (i.e., lapsed time to first full cheating response) served as our third and most important measure. Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire Parents completed a shortened version of the Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ) (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004), a parental report questionnaire that provides a multicomponent index of temperament during middle childhood. Only those TMCQ components deemed as relevant to rule following and supernatural beliefs were included in the current study, and these were activation control (capacity to perform an action when there is a strong tendency to avoid it), activity level (level of gross motor activity, including rate and extent of locomotion), affiliation (desire for warmth and closeness with others), attention (capacity to maintain attentional focus on tasks), fantasy/openness Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 5. J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx 5 (active imagination and curiosity), fear (negative affectivity within potentially threatening situations), impulsivity (speed of response initiation), inhibitory control (capacity to plan and suppress inappropri- ate approach responses under instructions or in novel situations), and shyness (slow or inhibited speed of social approach). In particular, fantasy/openness and shyness were expected to relate to belief in Princess Alice, based on previous findings (Taylor, 1999), whereas impulsivity and inhibitory control were expected to relate to cheating inhibition, as found in previous research (Kochanska et al., 1997). Internal reliabilities (Cronbach’s a) ranged from .71 to .90. Procedure Children were tested individually in a campus laboratory by a male experimenter (the first author). All sessions were covertly videotaped and lasted approximately 20 to 30 min. When children first ar- rived with their parent(s), they performed a warm-up game with the experimenter that involved throwing balls at the target prior to the administration of the rules. Next, parents were moved to a separate adjacent room and filled out the TMCQ while children were instructed on the task. After receiving the instructions, children performed a pretest to demonstrate that they comprehended the rules of the game. This pretest entailed performing six sequential throws without violating any of the rules. After the pretest, children performed the game on their own following a cover story where the experimenter removed himself from the room for 3 min. In the experimental conditions, before leaving, the experimenter reminded children that Princess Alice (or the female confederate) would be ‘‘watching’’ them. When the experimenter returned, he revealed that he had accidentally given children the wrong rules, and so they played an amended version of the game that guaranteed they received a prize. At the end, children were verbally debriefed and it was made clear to them that Prin- cess Alice was ‘‘not real’’ but rather ‘‘pretend’’. Results Preliminary analysis of gender Preliminary analyses with gender included as an independent variable revealed no main effect of gender or interaction effect on any of the dependent measures (all ps > .12). Thus, gender was omitted from further analysis. Overview of main analyses To test the first hypothesis that children who expressed belief in Princess Alice would inhibit their cheating more than the control group, we conducted a chi-square analysis of full cheating occurrences by condition and a 2 (Age) Â 3 (Condition) between-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) on full cheating latency, followed by planned contrasts. This was done first with all children in the analysis and then with skeptical children omitted. To test the second hypothesis that skeptical children would engage in exploratory behavior prior to cheating, we conducted a paired samples t test of skeptical children’s exploratory behavior latency and full cheating latency. To test the hypothesis that older children would have an easier time at following the rules than younger children, we conducted a 2 (Age) Â 3 (Condition) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on wrong throws and rule-breaking latency scores, followed by simple effects tests. Finally, to assess the hypothesis that belief in the invis- ible person would inhibit cheating over and above children’s temperament, we conducted a hierarchi- cal regression of expressed belief and temperament variables on full cheating latency. Belief in the invisible person and cheating With all children in the analysis, there was a nearly significant main effect of condition on occur- rences of full cheating (invisible person = 26%, supervision = 10%, no supervision = 42%), v2(2) = 5.58, p = .06, and a significant main effect of condition on full cheating latency, F(2, 61) = 3.44, p = .04, Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 6. 6 J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx g2 = .10, but no effect of age, F(1, 61) < 1, ns (interaction F < 1, ns). Simple contrasts revealed that great- er resistance to cheating in the adult supervision condition (M = 168.60, SD = 39.51) compared with control (M = 122.13, SD = 75.72), t(64) = 2.58, p = .01, was the primary source of the main effect. How- ever, full cheating latency was also greater for the invisible agent condition (M = 150.09, SD = 54.44) compared with control, albeit not quite significantly, t(64) = 1.61, p = .11, with all children in the analysis. In the invisible agent condition, 11 children (48%) professed belief in Princess Alice, 5 (22%) were unsure, and 7 (30%) professed their disbelief. Consistent with our pilot study, older children were sig- nificantly more skeptical of Princess Alice being real (yes = 11%, maybe = 44%, no = 44%) than younger children (yes = 71%, maybe = 7%, no = 21%), v2(2) = 8.63, p = .01. An independent t test of skeptical ver- sus nonskeptical children of all ages showed that children who professed belief in the invisible person resisted cheating to a significantly greater degree (M = 164.27, SD = 52.16) than children who pro- fessed disbelief (M = 106.43, SD = 54.71), t(16) = 2.25, p = .04. In terms of cheating frequency, only 1 of 11 children (9%) who professed belief or uncertainty manually placed the ball on the target, whereas 5 of 7 children (71%) who professed disbelief committed a full cheating response, and this difference was significantly greater than chance occurrence, v2(1) = 7.48, p = .006. Importantly, when the 7 children who professed disbelief in the invisible agent were removed from the sample and contrasts of the three conditions were conducted a second time, the contrast between the invisible agent and control conditions for full cheating latency was significant (Ms = 169.19 and 122.13, respectively), t(57) = 2.53, p = .01, and the means for the two experimental conditions were equivalent (see Fig. 2); that is, as predicted, children who professed belief in the invisible agent dis- played significantly greater resistance to cheating than control participants and at a level equal to chil- dren in the adult supervision condition. Fig. 2. Mean full cheating latency as a function of children’s age (in years) and condition, with skeptical children removed from the analysis. With skeptical children removed, cheating latencies from both experimental conditions were significantly different from control (with a = .05). Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 7. J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx 7 Exploratory behavior and timing of cheating among skeptical children Among children in the invisible person condition, expressed belief correlated negatively with the amount of exploratory reaching for Princess Alice, r(23) = –.24, p = .27, albeit not significantly. Belief did not correlate with visual attending to her chair, r(23) = .09, ns. In other words, skeptical children were more likely to reach for Princess Alice than nonskeptical children, but skeptical children were equally likely to visually search for her. Of the 5 skeptical children who committed a full cheating re- sponse, all of them engaged in some kind of exploratory behavior aimed at testing the presence of Princess Alice (see Fig. 1). These skeptical children engaged in exploratory behavior for, on average, 63.00 s (SD = 63.94) prior to engaging in a full cheating response. The paired samples t test confirmed that this duration of time was nearly significant, t(5) = 2.41, p = .06. Thus, as we hypothesized, skep- tical children’s full cheating occurred only after they first obtained disconfirming evidence for Princess Alice’s existence (e.g., reaching for Princess Alice and feeling nothing there). Age and rule breaking As expected, there was a significant main effect of age on rule-breaking latency, F(1, 61) = 9.00, p < .01, g2 = .13, with older children violating rules later (M = 144.29, SD = 38.30) than younger chil- dren (M = 106.66, SD = 56.32), but no effect of condition, F(2, 61) = 1.65, p = .20, g2 = .05 (interaction F < 1, ns). There was no main effect of age or condition on wrong throws (ps > .20). However, there was a significant interaction of age and condition, F(2, 61) = 3.48, p = .04, g2 = .10. Simple effects tests revealed that for older children, adult supervision led to fewer wrong throws (M = 0.80, SD = 1.32) compared with the invisible agent (M = 3.89, SD = 3.82), t(26) = 2.06, p = .05, and control (M = 3.90, SD = 4.01), t(26) = 2.12, p = .04. Unsupervised children and children in the invisible agent condition did not differ (t = 0). For younger children, information about an invisible person led to fewer wrong throws (M = 4.64, SD = 4.73) compared with adult supervision (M = 10.80, SD = 10.45), t(35) = 2.07, p = .05, but not significantly fewer than control (M = 7.14, SD = 6.38), t(35) = 1.23, p > .22. The contrast of control and supervision was not significant (p > .35). A simple effects test of the effect of age on wrong throws within the adult supervision group revealed that younger children made significantly more wrong throws (M = 10.80, SD = 10.45) than older children (M = 0.80, SD = 1.31), F(1, 18) = 9.01, p = .008, g2 = .33. However, there was no difference due to age within the other two conditions (Fs < 2.01, ps > .16). Thus, in sum, older children were able to resist breaking the rules longer than younger children. Moreover, the presence of a watchful adult led to fewer wrong throws for older children than imag- ining an invisible person or no supervision, whereas the presence of a watchful adult led to more wrong throws among younger children compared with the imagined presence of a watchful invisible person (see Discussion for possible explanations for this latter unexpected finding). Belief in the invisible person and individual differences in temperament A preliminary ANOVA revealed that there were no differences in any of the temperament variables across conditions (all ps > .20). Simple correlations revealed three components of temperament that correlated with full cheating latency: activation control, r(67) = .26, p = .04, fear, r(67) = –.27, p = .03, and shyness, r(67) = –.24, p = .05 (all other ps > .07). Although inhibitory control had a positive rela- tionship with cheating inhibition, r(23) = .16, p = .20, it was not significant. The only component of temperament that correlated with expressed belief in Princess Alice was shyness, r(23) = –.43, p = .04, an inverse relationship (all other ps > .14). Unexpectedly, fantasy/openness failed to correlate significantly with expressed belief, although the correlation was in the expected direction, r(23) = .29, p = .18. To test whether individual differences in children’s temperament could not explain the role of ex- pressed belief in inhibiting cheating; a hierarchical regression was conducted on full cheating latency for children in the invisible agent condition. Degree of belief in Princess Alice (yes = 2, maybe = 1, no = 0) was entered into the regression first (step 1), followed by the relevant temperament variables at step 2. As depicted in Table 1, belief explained a significant proportion of the variance at step 1 and Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 8. 8 J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Table 1 Hierarchical regression analysis of full cheating latency in the invisible agent condition. Model b t(21) Step 1 Belief .44 2.21* Step 2 Belief .65 3.41** Activation control À.30 À1.69 Fear À.43 À2.24* Impulsivity À.78 À3.41** Inhibitory control À.52 À2.10 Shyness À.09 <1.00 Note: Belief was scored in terms of responses to the question, ‘‘Do you believe that Princess Alice is real?’’ (yes = 2, maybe = 1, no = 0). Belief was entered at step 1, and temperament variables were entered at step 2. Belief remained a significant predictor of full cheating latency even after controlling for children’s temperament. * p < .05. ** p < .01. remained a significant predictor of cheating latency at step 2. In addition, impulsivity and fear emerged as significant negative predictors of full cheating latency; the more impulsive and fearful children were, the quicker they engaged in a full cheating response. In summary, although impulsivity and fear predicted cheating, these components were unrelated to belief in the invisible person, and expressed belief explained a significant proportion of cheating inhibition not explained by individual differences in children’s temperament. Discussion As hypothesized, children who professed belief in the invisible person were less likely to cheat on a challenging rule-based game than children who simply performed the task in the absence of adult supervision. Moreover, children who professed belief in the invisible person resisted cheating equally to children who performed the same task in the presence of an actual watchful adult. In contrast, the majority of skeptical children fully cheated on the task at some point in the experiment. However, investigation of the timing of skeptical children’s transgressions and their exploration of the alleged invisible person revealed that most of these skeptical children were secretly ambivalent about her presence. In fact, it was not until they confirmed their null hypothesis about the existence of Princess Alice that they went on to cheat on the task. In short, belief in the invisible person (whether expressed or unexpressed) helped children to resist cheating in the absence of the experimenter’s supervision. Moreover, belief in the invisible person predicted cheating inhibition over and above the influence of children’s temperament, including individual differences in impulsivity and fearfulness. Our findings are consistent with previous findings demonstrating the prosocial effects of priming adults with supernatural agents (Bering et al., 2005) or religious concepts (Shariff & Norenzayan, 2007) and, more broadly, with an evolutionary rationale (Bering, 2006; Boyer, 2003; Johnson & Bering, 2006; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008) arguing that the representation of watchful supernatural agents afforded ancestral humans with adaptive social benefits (i.e., inhibiting selfish behavior under condi- tions in which the threat of actual human detection was underestimated and, therefore, may have pro- tected an individual’s reputation). Our findings are also consistent with theory and research suggesting that children’s ability to reason about supernatural agents develops naturally alongside their ability to reason about natural human agents (Barrett, 2000; Kelemen, 2004; Lane, Wellman, & Evans, 2010). Importantly, the current findings demonstrate that the prosocial effects of imagining morally con- cerned supernatural agents are not limited to adults (e.g., Bering et al., 2005) but rather extend to chil- dren as young as 5 or 6 years of age. Second, they demonstrate that the prosocial effects of priming thoughts about supernatural agents are not limited to culturally based religious concepts (e.g., God, ancestral spirits) but rather are easily generalized to novel supernatural agents to which children have had no previous exposure. Finally, our findings suggest that it is not just ‘‘believers’’ who benefit from Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 9. J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx 9 the cognitive capacity to represent supernatural agency; even skeptical children, who professed their disbelief in the invisible person, were reluctant to cheat until they had gathered enough empirical evi- dence that Princess Alice was indeed not real. This suggests the intriguing hypothesis that supernat- ural agent concepts may, under certain conditions, motivate even skeptics to refrain from antisocial behavior. It is currently unclear, however, which specific aspects of our manipulation motivated rule compli- ance. Were those children who followed the rules in the Princess Alice condition doing so simply to please her, because they were afraid of her punishing them for cheating, or because they were afraid that she would somehow communicate their misdeeds to the experimenter? Future work in this area, therefore, would benefit from controlled manipulations of specific aspects of the supernatural agent (e.g., her character, perceptual capacities, and communicative abilities). In addition, it is unclear why being primed with a supernatural agent concept led to fewer wrong throws among younger chil- dren than having an actual watchful adult present in the room (although not less so than the control condition). One possibility is that young children interpreted the watchful adult’s gentle smiling and ostensible reluctance to reprimand them (social cues that were absent in the invisible agent condition) as a tacit endorsement of their rule breaking while failing to consider the confederate as a potential confidant of the experimenter. In contrast, older children may have been more concerned with the watchful adult or invisible agent reporting their rule violations to the experimenter. In short, develop- mental advances in the understanding of tattling and gossip (see Hill & Pillow, 2006; Ingram & Bering, 2010) might explain why an adult’s watchful presence promoted rule adherence among older children but not among younger children. However, it is important to point out that these differences due to age are limited to acts of rule breaking that were potential accidents. In contrast, the two age groups performed at equivalent levels with respect to full-blown cheating. Finally, although Princess Alice was presented as a ‘‘real’’ agent rather than as a pretend character, it is still possible that some children inferred such a make-believe context. However, nothing in the experimenter’s tone or speech indicated that it was a play context. The experimenter presented Prin- cess Alice in a factual manner and solicited children’s personal beliefs on the matter. Regardless of the reasons behind children’s expression of belief, those who stated that they believed in the supernatural agent adhered more closely to the rules. Of course, because we could not experimentally manipulate children’s belief in Princess Alice, the possibility remains that another variable might better explain the observed findings; for example, it may be that children who expressed belief in Princess Alice have internalized the importance of following rules to a greater extent than children who expressed disbe- lief, or perhaps nonskeptics were more motivated to please the experimenter. Future studies should address these issues by comparing the inhibitory effects of supernatural agent primes in a context of pretend play versus one of serious instruction while controlling for individual differences in chil- dren’s commitment to following rules. Acknowledgments This research is an output from a project undertaken as part of the Cognition, Religion, and Theol- ogy Project at the University of Oxford, funded by the John Templeton Foundation via a grant to Jared Piazza. The views expressed are not necessarily those of the Cognition, Religion, and Theology Project, the University of Oxford, or the John Templeton Foundation. We also thank Sarah Allen and Amy Puri- foy for their assistance with data collection, serving in the confederate role, and recruiting participants for the study. References Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29–34. Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 29, 453–462. Bering, J. M., McLeod, K., & Shackelford, T. K. (2005). Reasoning about dead agents reveals possible adaptive trends. Human Nature, 16, 360–381. Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behavior as by-products of the brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119–124. Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind. New York: William Morrow. Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003
  • 10. 10 J. Piazza et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Harris, P. L. (2007). Trust. Developmental Science, 10, 135–138. Hill, V., & Pillow, B. H. (2006). Children’s understanding of reputations. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 137–157. Ingram, G. P. D., & Bering, J. M. (2010). Children’s tattling: The reporting of everyday norm violations in preschool settings. Child Development, 81, 945–957. Johnson, D., & Bering, J. (2006). Hand of god, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 219–233. Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children ‘‘intuitive theists’’? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15, 295–301. Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 339–351. Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111. Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68, 263–277. Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507. Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261–1277. Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Evans, E. M. (2010). Children’s understanding of ordinary and extraordinary minds. Child Development, 81, 1475–1489. Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 58–62. Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18, 803–809. Simonds, J., & Rothbart, M.K. (2004). The Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ): A computerized self-report instrument for ages 7–10. Poster session presented at the Occasional Temperament Conference, Athens, GA. Retrieved from http://www.bowdoin.edu/$sputnam/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/pdf/TMCQ_Poster_Oct04.pdf on 7 November 2010. Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2004). Ideology, religion, and the evolution of cooperation: Field tests on Israeli kibbutzim. Research in Economic Anthropology, 23, 89–117. Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. New York: Oxford University Press. Please cite this article in press as: Piazza, J., et al. ‘‘Princess Alice is watching you’’: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (2011), doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003