SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 7
Baixar para ler offline
How We Look at Photographs as Indicated by Contrast
Discrimination Performance Versus Contrast Preference1
S. Gershoni and H. Kobayashi
Department of Information Science, Graduate School of Science and Technology, Chiba University,
1-33 Yayoi-cho, Inage-ku, Chiba 263-8522, Japan
E-mail: sharon_gershoni@hotmail.com
Abstract. Considering luminance contrast to comprise the building
blocks of the photographic language, we aimed to study the connec-
tion between viewer’s contrast discrimination performance in black-
and-white photographs with various contrast reproductions, and
their aesthetic appeal. In a previous study we examined the viewer’s
ability to discriminate contrast increments, applied to discrete re-
gions of the characteristic curves of gray scales versus photographs
of Ansel Adams. The photographs belonged to three conceptual cat-
egories: portrait, landscape, and architecture. Whereas, contrast
discrimination performance in gray scales was very poor in repro-
ductions with altered contrast in the shadows, a significant improve-
ment in performance was observed in the photographs. In the
present study subjects performed a contrast preference evaluation
task, in which, the reproductions of the photographs were rated for
their aesthetic appeal on a five-point scale. The photographs were
presented in random order, without indication as to which is the
original photograph. Nevertheless, the viewers showed a general
preference for photographs with contrast reproductions similar to the
original. The results suggest a match between the viewers’ and the
photographer’s preferences. Moreover, the preference decreased
systematically with the contrast increment for all reproductions. This
tendency seems to be independent of variations in category or spa-
tial configuration. The results are in line with the observed contrast
discrimination performance, and also consistent with the anchoring
theory and recent propositions of biologically based rules for art
creation and appreciation as manifestations of the function of the
brain. © 2006 Society for Imaging Science and Technology.
͓DOI: 10.2352/J.ImagingSci.Technol.͑2006͒50:4͑320͔͒
INTRODUCTION
The investigation of art as a means to understand the orga-
nization of the visual brain was recently proposed by Zeki1
who drew the outlines of “a theory of aesthetics that is bio-
logically based.” Ramachandran and Hirstein2
proposed uni-
versal laws for artistic experience in relation to phenomena
such as the peak shift effect and gestalt grouping principles.
Considering such suggestions led us to examine black-and-
white photography as a case of art. We chose the photo-
graphs of Ansel Adams,3
the founder of the “Zone System”4
for tone control. Assuming luminance contrast to comprise
the building blocks of the photographic language, we exam-
ined the connection between the viewers’ ability to discrimi-
nate luminance contrast in black-and-white photographs,
and their aesthetic appeal.
Early experimental studies in aesthetics used simple and
artificial stimuli. After the pioneering work by Fechner in
1876,5
measurements of the viewers moods for different
patches of color were conducted by Wexner in 1954,6
and
later repeated by Eysenck7,8
using polygons, etc. Cross-
cultural and cross-gender aesthetic studies originated with
Jahoda9
in 1956 and followed by many others. One of the
advantages of using simple displays was the accuracy of ma-
nipulation it allowed, compared to the difficulty in achieving
pictorial sameness between the reproduced stimuli and an
artwork. The conditions of pictorial sameness, under which
a reproduction is aesthetically valuable as the original, were
proposed in the transferability thesis.10
But an opposing ar-
gument stated that even pictorial sameness of a reproduction
does not necessarily grant sameness in aesthetic value.11
Thus the weakness of the early studies is that the aesthetic
response to simple artificial stimuli is likely to be weak, as
the viewers generally do not regard simple displays as pretty,
especially in the context of being participants in an experi-
ment. Moreover, most people do, in fact, experience art
through reproductions, through art books and other media.
Photography, a reproduction system in its essence, offers
more control over reproduction, and better sameness than
any other medium of art. Black-and-white photographs in
particular are a good source for faithful reproduction. They
represent subject properties as a relationship between the
correlating values of luminance (not color), with only one-
dimensional luminance gray scale. Therefore, while black-
and-white photographs still carry the conceptual attributes
as any other medium of art, they are less complex to ma-
nipulate in a controlled process.
In a previous paper12
we reported the roles and inter-
ferences of local and global visual processes in lightness per-
ception of photographs with meaningful contents; we exam-
ined whether contrast discrimination is a response to spatial
configuration properties of photographs, or a function of
conceptual contents. In three experiments we compared
contrast discrimination performances of observers, when
presented with contrast increments applied to discrete tonal
regions in gray scales versus several categories of black-and-
white photographs of natural scenes. In the first experiment
observers performed contrast discrimination in gray scales
using rank-order tasks. In the following two experiments
observers performed contrast discrimination of photographs
using sorting tasks. We found substantial differences in re-
1
Presented in part at IST’s NIP XXI, Baltimore, MD, September 2005.
Received Jun. 3, 2005; accepted for publication Oct. 30, 2005.
1062-3701/2006/50͑4͒/320/7/$20.00.
Journal of Imaging Science and Technology® 50(4): 320–326, 2006.
© Society for Imaging Science and Technology 2006
320
sponse to contrast increments, depending on where in the
characteristic curve (i.e., shadow, highlight, or midtones)
they were applied. On the other hand, we reported no sig-
nificant effect of category (portrait, landscape, and architec-
ture). In addition we found that low performances in the
shadow region of gray scales significantly improved in pho-
tograph which we related to the differences in configuration
complexity. We also reported differences in performance be-
tween photographs of light and night scenes. These results
raised an assumption that the differences in spatial configu-
ration complexity, and not the conceptual content of the
photographs, improved contrast detection performance.
A possible explanation is provided by the “anchoring
theory of lightness perception.”13
Photographs depicting
natural objects contain much more complex and articulated
information than gray scales, thus local frameworks weigh
more than global frameworks, which results in improved
lightness constancy and better performance in contrast dis-
crimination tasks. These findings are also in line with recent
psychophysical measurements of threshold versus contrast
for images of natural scenes where image whitening yielded
lower contrast discrimination threshold than high contrast
images.14
Since looking at photographs is a visual task involving
an aesthetic experience, a question of interest is: What is the
relationship between the contrast discrimination, reported
above, and the aesthetically preferred contrast reproduction
of photographs. Recent studies claimed similar to the results
reported above that there is no effect of context such as pose
and shape of objects on processes such as lightness
constancy.15
Opposing suggestions are that contextual infor-
mation does affects processes such as global form
perception.16
According to this view, one could expect contrast to
influence the content of photographs and their aesthetic ap-
peal. Therefore, the question asked in the present study was
about the relationship between the conceptual content of
photographs and their contrast preference.
Moreover, should there indeed be a common rule for
both execution of art and its appreciation, then one could
expect similarity in the preferences of viewers and
photographers,17,18
(see also: Discussion). Therefore, this
study examines the relationship between the preferred con-
trast reproduction and the “original” (unaltered) reproduc-
tion, which may reflect on the photographer’s preference
versus the viewer preference.
EXPERIMENT
Stimuli
Eight black-and-white photographs from the “Photographs
of the Southwest” book by Ansel Adams19
were used as
stimuli for the present study:
1. “Spanish Peaks, Colorado,” 1951, p. 18;
2. “Canyon de Chelly, National Monument, Arizona,”
1947, p. 30;
3. “Moonrise, Hernandez, New-Mexico,” 1944, p. 55;
4. “Navajo Woman, Wide Ruin, Arizona,” 1948, p. 39;
5. “Maynard Dixon, Painter, Tucson, Arizona,” 1944, p.
16;
6. “Martha Porter, Pioneer Woman, Ordeville, Utah,”
1961, p. 81;
7. “Adobe Dwellings, Northern New-Mexico,” 1958, p.
49; and
8. “Arches, North Court, Mission San Xavier del Bac,
Tucson, Arizona,” 1968, p. 94.
The photographs selected belong to three major themes in
photography: Landscape (photographs Nos. 1, 2, and 3), Por-
trait (Nos. 4, 5, and 6), and Architecture (Nos. 7, 8, and 9).
These themes represent Ansel Adams’ work in general, and
the selected photographs are among his well-known and re-
produced works. It is important to note that photographs
selected as stimuli were of various configuration complexi-
ties, articulation, depth, and tonal relationships, etc.
Stimuli Reproduction Process
The goal of the reproduction process was to alter the shadow
(SH), highlight (HI), and midtone (MT) contrasts of the
eight selected images. An Epson GT-9700 flatbed scanner
was used to scan the book pages. The scanning pixels-per-
inch (ppi) was 400. The ppi was chosen to match the print-
ing system output resolution (Lambda printing system, see
explanation below). For calibration of the reproduction sys-
tem, the density of a Kodak standard gray-scale and its re-
production were measured using a Sakura PDA 8112 reflec-
tion densitometer and plotted against each other to assure a
linear relationship between the reflection densities.
Image files for the various highlight, midtone and
shadow contrasts were generated using Adobe Photoshop®
Curve function in the Adjustments menu. The horizontal
axis of the curve graph represents the original brightness
values of the pixels (input levels); the vertical axis represents
the new brightness levels (output levels). The curve displays
either brightness values from 0 to 255, with the shadows on
the left, or values from 0% to 100% with shadows on the
right. The default diagonal line shows that all pixels have the
same input and output values.
In order to alter contrast, two anchor/drag points were
established on the diagonal line:
1. to alter shadow contrasts: ͑XSH=75% ,YSH=75%͒;
2. to alter highlight contrasts: ͑XHI=25% ,YHI=25%͒;
and
3. to alter midtone contrasts both highlight and shadow
anchor points were established: ͑XHI=25% ,YHI
=25%͒,͑XSH=75% ,YSH=75%͒.
A 2% contrast increment, for each of the reproduction sets,
was achieved by pulling the anchoring point in one of the
following directions:
1. shadow: ͑XSH+2=XSH+2% ,YSH+2=YSH−2%͒;
2. highlight: ͑XHI+2=XHI−2% ,YHI+2=YHI+2%͒; and
3. midtone: ͑XHI+2=XHI+2% ,YHI+2=YHI−2%͒,
͑XSH+2=XSH−2% ,YSH+2=YSH+2%͒.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 321
For each of the three sets of reproductions (“HI,” “SH,”
and “MT”) contrast increment ranged between 2% and 10%
in steps of 2%, named sample Nos. 2–10 accordingly.
The densities of the photographs are represented by
Kodak standard gray scale reproduction sets, which were
measured using a reflection densitometer, and plotted
against the original gray scale, as shown in Figs. 1(a) (HI and
SH) and 1(b) (MT).
A total of 128 photographic prints were produced by
Lambda system using a conventional black-and-white silver
process. Lambda prints are made on a Durst Lambda
printer, which uses three colored lasers to expose traditional
photographic media. These prints have the advantage of us-
ing the same rich red-green-blue (RGB) color space em-
ployed by computer monitors. In addition, these prints are
free of dots since unlike inkjet printers, the laser outputs are
continuously modulated rather than switched on and off.
Their 400 dpi resolution is comparable to 1200 dpi screened
output.
Participants
Thirty subjects male and female (in 3:1 ratio) aged between
20 and 30 participated in the experiment. 46% of the par-
ticipants were either familiar with the photographer or re-
ported to have previously seen the photographs used as
stimuli.
Illumination Source
The light source was a Toshiba natural color evaluation
lamp, 5000 K, 700 lux. Light source was overhead with re-
spect to the viewing area. Observation distance and angle:
not designated.
Procedure
Each photograph and its reproductions composed a set of 16
stimuli. The stimuli were presented on a table in a rectan-
gular arrangement of a random order of contrasts, as shown
in Fig. 2 with no indication of the unaltered original photo-
graph (OR), or any other standard.
Participants were requested to evaluate the aesthetic ap-
Figure 1. a. Characteristic curves for stimuli with 2–10% contrast increment in regions HI and SH. b.
Characteristic curve for stimuli with 2–10% contrast increment in region MT.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
322 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006
peal of each sample according to a one-dimensional prefer-
ence scale between 1 and 5, in which 1 is the lowest
evaluation—“most dislike,” 2—“dislike,” 3—“neither like
nor dislike,” 4—“like,” and 5 is the highest evaluation—“like
best.”
Results
Evaluation ratios, on the 1–5 preference scale, for the pho-
tographs of each category (portraits, landscape, and archi-
tecture), were separately calculated, as a function of the tone
regions (HI, SH, and MT). Within each region five contrast
increments of 2% were indicated as sample Nos. 2–10 as
shown in Fig. 3. The graph shows that preference was high-
est for OR, and decreased systematically with contrast incre-
ment in all regions (HI, SH, and MT) and all categories.
This pattern of decreasing preference showed some varia-
tions, which could be dependent on either category variance
or the tone regions. In order to investigate which of these
variables accounted for the observed differences, mean aver-
age preferences were calculated for the categories and for the
regions.
The mean average preferences for categories were: 2.90
for portraits, 2.88 for landscapes, and 2.88 for architecture as
shown in Fig. 4(a). These results indicated that contrast pref-
erence was similar across the different categories. In order to
confirm these observations, a two way analysis of variance
(ANOVA), with categories (portrait versus landscape versus
architecture) and contrast increments (OR vs 2% vs 4%…vs
10%) as variables, was conducted. In this and all other sta-
tistical tests, ␣=0.05 and the 95% confidence interval were
used for significance. The effect of category was not signifi-
cant ͓F͑2,58͒=0.05͔,p=0.89. This reflects that there was no
differential effect of category over preference of contrast.
Moreover, comparison between photographs within category
did not show significant difference either ͓F͑2,58͒=1.78͔,
p=0.065. Mean average preferences for regions were: 2.6 for
HI, 2.7 for MT, and 3.1 for SH as shown in Fig. 4(b). A two
way ANOVA, with regions (HI vs SH vs MT) and contrast
increments (OR vs 2% vs 4%…vs 10%) as variables, re-
vealed that although the effect of regions over preference was
not substantial, it was significant ͓F͑2,58͒=11.94͔ with p
Ͻ0.0001.
These results are in line with previous experiments in
which the ability to perform contrast discrimination was
measured for the same stimuli (see: Introduction). There,
contrast discrimination was not affected by category, but de-
termined by the discrete tonal regions in which contrast al-
teration occurred. In fact, the present results show that the
preference pattern is an inversion of the contrast discrimi-
nation pattern, the higher the contrast discrimination ratio,
the lower the preference.
It is important to note that although contrast discrimi-
nation ratio for MT was found to be significantly higher
than that for the other regions, preference for MT was not
the lowest one could expect, as shown in Fig. 5. The figure
shows the effect of tonal region (HI vs SH vs MT vs OR)
over mean contrast preference.
Paired comparisons of contrasts revealed that contrast
preference did not differ much between MT and HI regions
͓F͑1,58͒=2.14͔, pϽ0.0001. On the other hand, preference
evaluation significantly increased in SH region ͓F͑1,58͒
=21.74͔, pϽ0.0001. In addition, the mean average prefer-
ence of SH was above “3” in the evaluation scale, where
evaluation tends towards “like,” while MT and HI were be-
low the 3, where evaluation is already “dislike.” This finding
Figure 3. Average preference for stimuli 2–10%, across the three categories and three regions.
Figure 2. The arrangement of stimuli in the preference evaluation task.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 323
implies a greater degree of tolerance towards contrast incre-
ments in the shadow regions of the photographs. In such
contrast reproductions the overall impression of the lighten-
ing of the image. For an image which seems to become
darker there is a lower degree of tolerance. Figure 6 shows
the differential effect of contrast increment (2%, 6%, and
10%) over regions. For 2% contrast increment, preferences
at regions HI, SH, and MT are: 3.59, 3.60, and 3.55 respec-
tively, for 6% the preference of SH over the other regions is
already substantial: 2.52, 3.3, and 2.8, and the difference is
even greater for 10% increment: 1.58, 2.67, and 1.97.
Preference for the OR was substantially higher than all
other stimuli, independent of category or tonal region. That
is, although stimuli were displayed in a random order, with
no reference to the original photographs, observers’ prefer-
ence matched the preference of the photographer, for all
stimuli, as shown in Fig. 5, where mean average preference
for OR is 3.9, and for SH, MT, and HI: 3.1, 2.74, and 2.6,
respectively. Paired comparisons of contrasts confirmed that
preference for OR was significantly higher than all other
stimuli ͓F͑1,58͒=77.19͔, pϽ0.0001.
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to investigate the aesthetic
appeal of contrast reproductions in black-and-white photo-
graphs, and the connection between contrast preference and
contrast discrimination performance.
Results revealed an inverse and linear relationship be-
tween contrast increment and contrast preference, where
preference decreased systematically with increment. Also,
contrast preference seems to be independent of conceptual
content, but apparently also from spatial configuration char-
acteristics. Based on contrast discrimination results, we ex-
pected to find a connection between preference and regions.
Nevertheless, a uniform preference pattern was observed for
Figure 5. A comparison between average preference of the unaltered
photograph OR and regions HI vs SH vs MT.
Figure 4. a. Average preference across categories reveals no significant difference. b. Average preference
across regions HI vs SH vs MT reveals significant difference.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
324 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006
all photographs tested. Results also revealed a match be-
tween the photographer’s contrast preference, manifested in
his choice of contrasts in the photographs tested, and that of
the viewer.
One way of explaining these results is with the anchor-
ing theory of lightness perception.20–22
In mapping lumi-
nance into a lightness scale, the highest luminance is an-
chored (assigned) to white, and the rest of the values are
scaled relative to it. Other factors influencing anchoring are
the area rule and configural attributes of the image, such as
articulation and insulation. The compromise between the
anchoring rules determines whether anchoring occurs in the
local framework or the global framework. While strong an-
choring to local framework increases lightness constancy,
when the global framework is stronger, lightness constancy
decreases and assignment to white is enhanced. Perhaps, as
the contrast in the shadow region increases, insulation de-
creases and the white areas grow bigger, thus the photograph
is perceived as being lighter. Therefore, the decrease in light-
ness constancy might be the reason for what seems to be
greater tolerance to contrast increments in shadow regions
than in highlights or midtones.
In both experiments, i.e., contrast discrimination per-
formance and preference evaluation of photographs, results
reflected no influence of high vision processes, such as the
perceived depth of stimulus21
to influence perception of the
brightness of the pattern, or the context effect,23
also labeled
“object-superiority effect,” suggesting that the context of
overall structure and form influences early processing such
as features extraction.
Moreover, the argument that viewers prefer shapes and
arrangements, which are most “effectively” processed by our
visual system24,25
must also be reexamined in light of the
present results. This argument derived from a work that ex-
amined the effect of orientation on the aesthetic appeal of
Mondrian’s paintings. The results in that work reflected a
match in the preferred orientation between the artist and the
viewers. This is in line with the match in contrast preference
reported in the present paper, and also early experiments
with other mediums of art including also poetry and prose,
where sentences from well known classical writers were
modified to impair the literary effect26–28
music, where well
known pieces were modified to major mode and minor
mode,29
orientation in abstract paintings,30
and other pic-
ture alterations.31
The explanation for the tendency to prefer certain
shapes or arrangements (Mondrian), which are effectively
processed, was claimed to lie in the biological need to main-
tain a certain level of stimulation of the visual system. Like
other adaptive mechanisms, the visual system is encouraged
to function most effectively; thus it prefers the more extreme
stimuli over the weaker ones (peak shift effect32
).
According to this argument, artists should prefer to cre-
ate line art or solid-color rectangular arrangements, such as
Mondrians, and photographers should prefer high contrast
film. But, in reality it is not so. In fact even Mondrian is but
one example out of the general art creation. Even if Mon-
drian was indeed gifted in the capability to intentionally cre-
ate a faithful expression of the visual system, does that make
his art, therefore, weigh more than other art works as a
manifestation of the visual mechanisms? If so, that would be
in contradiction with the above argument itself, denying its
universal neurobiological ground, according to which all art
works are manifestations of the brain’s mechanisms and
constraints.33
If we all prefer the same shapes and orienta-
tions then we should all have been making Mondrians, with
no variance throughout art history and no effect of cultural,
geographical, and historical conditions.
If indeed the most extreme stimuli is the preferred one
by the visual system and has the highest appeal to us all, then
the results of the present experiment should have been that
the greater the contrast increment, the higher the preference
evaluation. In fact, the results reflect that both photographer
and viewer prefer the contrast of reproductions similar to
original, and not the extremes.
This leads to the suggestion that the match between
artist and viewers in constrast reproduction preference or
orientation preference are valid only within the context of
the specific artworks, and cannot be translated to other art
works.
CONCLUSION
Further investigation, using eye movement tracker, can an-
swer questions relating to the effect of contrast alteration
over meaningful details in photographs and the consequent
effect over fixation centers drifts and attention, following
earlier studies.34–37
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to express their gratitude to M. Tsukada,
Horiuchi Color, Tokyo for printing and laminating the entire
stimuli and supporting the research, and to Professor
Masako Jitsumori, Chiba University, for his extensive assis-
tance in the data analysis.
REFERENCES
1
S. Zeki, Art and the Brain (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2000).
2
V. S. Ramachandran and W. Hirstein, “The science of art—A
neurological theory of aesthetic experience”, J. Conscious. Stud. 6, 15–51
Figure 6. A comparison between average preferences at regions HI vs
SH vs MT for stimuli 2%, 6%, and 10% contrast increment.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 325
(1999).
3
A. Hammond, “Ansel Adams and the objectivism—Making a
photograph with group f/64”, History of Photography 22, 169–178
(1998).
4
I. H. Latour, “Ansel Adams, The zone system and the California School
of Fine Arts,” History of Photography 22, 147–154 (1998).
5
G. T. Fechner, Vorschule der Asthetik (Breitkopf & Hartels, Leipzig,
1876).
6
L. B. Wexner, “The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with
mood tones”, J. Appl. Psychol. 38, 432–435 (1954).
7
H. J. Eysenck, “An experimental study of aesthetic preference for
polygonal figures”, J. Gen. Psychol. 79, 3–17 (1968).
8
H. J. Eysenck and M. Castle, “Training in art as a factor in the
determination of preference judgments for polygons”, Br. J. Psychol. 61,
65–81 (1970).
9
G. Jahoda, “Sex differences in preferences for shapes: A cross-cultural
replication,” Br. J. Psychol. 47, 126–132 (1956).
10
G. Currie, “The authentic and the aesthetic”, Am. Philosophical Q. 22,
153–160 (1985).
11
J. Elkins, “What are we seeing exactly?”, Art Bull. 79, 191–198 (1997).
12
S. Gershoni and H. Kobayashi, “How we look at photographs—As
indicated by contrast discrimination performance”, J. Soc. Information
Sci. Technol. Japan, accepted.
13
A. Gilchrist and C. Kossyfidis, “An anchoring theory of lightness
perception”, Psychol. Rev. 106(4), 795–834 (1999).
14
C. A. Olman, K. Ugurbil, and D. Kersten, “Effects of image structure on
perceived contrast and cortical activity in early visual areas”, J. Vision
3(9), 47a (2003).
15
C. Ripamonti, M. Bloj, R. E. Hauck, K. Mitha, and D. H. Brainard,
“Object lightness constancy: Effects of object pose and shape”, J. Vision
3(9), 295a (2003).
16
C. Habak, F. Wilkinson, Z. Bernadette, and H. R. Wilson, “Contextual
effects in form perception”, J. Vision 3(9), 354a (2003).
17
See Ref. 1.
18
R. Latto, D. Brain, and B. Kelly, “An oblique effect in aesthetics: Homage
to Mondrian”, Perception 29, 981–987 (2000).
19
A. Adams, Photographs of the Southwest (New York Graphic Society,
Boston, MA, 1976).
20
A. L. Gilchrist, “Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial
arrangement”, Science 195, 185–187 (1977).
21
A. L. Gilchrist, “When does perceived lightness depend on perceived
spatial arrangement?”, Percept. Psychophys. 28(6), 527–538 (1980).
22
See Ref. 13.
23
N. Weisstein and C. S. Harris, “Visual detection of line segments: An
object superiority effect”, Science 186, 752–755 (1974).
24
See Ref. 18.
25
R. Latto, “The brain of the beholder”, in Artful Eye, edited by R. L. J.
Gregory, P. Harris, and D. R. Heard (Oxford University Press, Oxford,
1995), pp. 66–94.
26
C. Burt, in How the Mind Works, cited by I. Gordon and C. Gardner
(Allen & Unwin, London, 1933); “Responses to altered pictures”, Br. J.
Psychol. 65, 243–251 (1974).
27
E. D. Williams, L. Winter, and J. M. Woods, “Test of literary
appreciation”, Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 265–284 (1938).
28
C. V. Valentine, The Experimental Psychology of Beauty (Metheun,
London, 1962).
29
K. Havner, “The affective character of major and minor modes in music,
Am. Psychol. 47, 103–118 (1935).
30
M. S. Lindauer, “The orientation of form in abstract art”, Proc. Am.
Psychological Assoc. 4, 475–476 (1969).
31
N. C. Meier, The Meier Art Tests 2. Aesthetic Perception (University of
Iowa, Bureau of Educational Research and Service, Iowa City, IA, 1963).
32
See Ref. 2.
33
See Ref. 1.
34
A. L. Yarbus, Eye Movements and Vision, edited by L. A. Riggs (Plenum
Press, New York, 1967) edited by R. L. Solso, Cognition and the Visual
Arts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994), p. 145.
35
C. F. Nodine, P. J. Locher, and E. A. Krupinski, “The role of formal art
training on perception and aesthetic judgment of art compositions”,
Leonardo 26, 219–227 (1993).
36
C. F. Nodine and H. L. Kundel, “Perception and display in diagnostic
imaging”, Radio Graphs 7, 1241–1250 (1987).
37
C. F. Nodine, “Searching for NINA, 1987”, in R. L. Solso, Cognition and
the Visual Arts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994), pp. 143–147.
Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance
326 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Semelhante a How We Look at Photographs Indicated by Contrast Preferences

How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)
How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)
How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)Sharon Gershoni
 
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services https://writeessayuk.com/
 
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...Sharon Gershoni
 
Papyrus_Saxton_Lighting
Papyrus_Saxton_LightingPapyrus_Saxton_Lighting
Papyrus_Saxton_LightingNathan Saxton
 
Pupillary responses in art appreciation effects of aesthetic emotions
Pupillary responses in art appreciation  effects of aesthetic emotionsPupillary responses in art appreciation  effects of aesthetic emotions
Pupillary responses in art appreciation effects of aesthetic emotionsAhmad Faizul
 
Scientific Project Information and Lignt
Scientific Project Information and LigntScientific Project Information and Lignt
Scientific Project Information and LigntYuliya Pismennaya
 
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)samheisler
 
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)Nikolaos Ziakas
 
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...James Allen
 
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo FixationsLiu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo FixationsKalle
 
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative Research
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative ResearchCollage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative Research
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative ResearchSeha Shaharudin
 
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platforms
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platformsExamining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platforms
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platformsKim Arcand
 

Semelhante a How We Look at Photographs Indicated by Contrast Preferences (14)

How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)
How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)
How we look at photographs 1 (JSPS&TJ 2005)
 
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services
Physics lab report sample from assignmentsupport.com essay writing services
 
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...
Measuring pictorial balance perception at first glance using Japanese Calligr...
 
Papyrus_Saxton_Lighting
Papyrus_Saxton_LightingPapyrus_Saxton_Lighting
Papyrus_Saxton_Lighting
 
Pupillary responses in art appreciation effects of aesthetic emotions
Pupillary responses in art appreciation  effects of aesthetic emotionsPupillary responses in art appreciation  effects of aesthetic emotions
Pupillary responses in art appreciation effects of aesthetic emotions
 
Scientific Project Information and Lignt
Scientific Project Information and LigntScientific Project Information and Lignt
Scientific Project Information and Lignt
 
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)
Heisler sam crit_presentation(09.11.2012)
 
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)
Three-dimensional perception and aging (MSc research project)
 
Lec04 color
Lec04 colorLec04 color
Lec04 color
 
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
Is there a difference in the pattern of eye movements during visual imagery w...
 
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo FixationsLiu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations
Liu Natural Scene Statistics At Stereo Fixations
 
Seopaper
SeopaperSeopaper
Seopaper
 
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative Research
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative ResearchCollage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative Research
Collage Portraits as a Method of Analysis in Qualitative Research
 
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platforms
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platformsExamining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platforms
Examining perceptions of astronomy images across mobile platforms
 

Mais de Sharon Gershoni

Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre Project
Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre ProjectIsraeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre Project
Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre ProjectSharon Gershoni
 
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)Sharon Gershoni
 
Report 1- The Drinker Family Papers
Report 1- The Drinker Family PapersReport 1- The Drinker Family Papers
Report 1- The Drinker Family PapersSharon Gershoni
 
Photography Empathy & Mirror Neurons
Photography Empathy & Mirror NeuronsPhotography Empathy & Mirror Neurons
Photography Empathy & Mirror NeuronsSharon Gershoni
 
Mu&Ma (無と間) in Japanese Art and Visual Perception
Mu&Ma (無と間) in  Japanese Art and Visual PerceptionMu&Ma (無と間) in  Japanese Art and Visual Perception
Mu&Ma (無と間) in Japanese Art and Visual PerceptionSharon Gershoni
 
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)Sharon Gershoni
 

Mais de Sharon Gershoni (7)

Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre Project
Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre ProjectIsraeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre Project
Israeli-Palestinian Children's Theatre Project
 
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)
Art-Gershman Y Gallery Handout(2007)
 
Report 1- The Drinker Family Papers
Report 1- The Drinker Family PapersReport 1- The Drinker Family Papers
Report 1- The Drinker Family Papers
 
Photography Empathy & Mirror Neurons
Photography Empathy & Mirror NeuronsPhotography Empathy & Mirror Neurons
Photography Empathy & Mirror Neurons
 
Mu&Ma (無と間) in Japanese Art and Visual Perception
Mu&Ma (無と間) in  Japanese Art and Visual PerceptionMu&Ma (無と間) in  Japanese Art and Visual Perception
Mu&Ma (無と間) in Japanese Art and Visual Perception
 
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)
VSS Poster (Sharon Gershoni)
 
Text and Art Brochure
Text and Art BrochureText and Art Brochure
Text and Art Brochure
 

How We Look at Photographs Indicated by Contrast Preferences

  • 1. How We Look at Photographs as Indicated by Contrast Discrimination Performance Versus Contrast Preference1 S. Gershoni and H. Kobayashi Department of Information Science, Graduate School of Science and Technology, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-cho, Inage-ku, Chiba 263-8522, Japan E-mail: sharon_gershoni@hotmail.com Abstract. Considering luminance contrast to comprise the building blocks of the photographic language, we aimed to study the connec- tion between viewer’s contrast discrimination performance in black- and-white photographs with various contrast reproductions, and their aesthetic appeal. In a previous study we examined the viewer’s ability to discriminate contrast increments, applied to discrete re- gions of the characteristic curves of gray scales versus photographs of Ansel Adams. The photographs belonged to three conceptual cat- egories: portrait, landscape, and architecture. Whereas, contrast discrimination performance in gray scales was very poor in repro- ductions with altered contrast in the shadows, a significant improve- ment in performance was observed in the photographs. In the present study subjects performed a contrast preference evaluation task, in which, the reproductions of the photographs were rated for their aesthetic appeal on a five-point scale. The photographs were presented in random order, without indication as to which is the original photograph. Nevertheless, the viewers showed a general preference for photographs with contrast reproductions similar to the original. The results suggest a match between the viewers’ and the photographer’s preferences. Moreover, the preference decreased systematically with the contrast increment for all reproductions. This tendency seems to be independent of variations in category or spa- tial configuration. The results are in line with the observed contrast discrimination performance, and also consistent with the anchoring theory and recent propositions of biologically based rules for art creation and appreciation as manifestations of the function of the brain. © 2006 Society for Imaging Science and Technology. ͓DOI: 10.2352/J.ImagingSci.Technol.͑2006͒50:4͑320͔͒ INTRODUCTION The investigation of art as a means to understand the orga- nization of the visual brain was recently proposed by Zeki1 who drew the outlines of “a theory of aesthetics that is bio- logically based.” Ramachandran and Hirstein2 proposed uni- versal laws for artistic experience in relation to phenomena such as the peak shift effect and gestalt grouping principles. Considering such suggestions led us to examine black-and- white photography as a case of art. We chose the photo- graphs of Ansel Adams,3 the founder of the “Zone System”4 for tone control. Assuming luminance contrast to comprise the building blocks of the photographic language, we exam- ined the connection between the viewers’ ability to discrimi- nate luminance contrast in black-and-white photographs, and their aesthetic appeal. Early experimental studies in aesthetics used simple and artificial stimuli. After the pioneering work by Fechner in 1876,5 measurements of the viewers moods for different patches of color were conducted by Wexner in 1954,6 and later repeated by Eysenck7,8 using polygons, etc. Cross- cultural and cross-gender aesthetic studies originated with Jahoda9 in 1956 and followed by many others. One of the advantages of using simple displays was the accuracy of ma- nipulation it allowed, compared to the difficulty in achieving pictorial sameness between the reproduced stimuli and an artwork. The conditions of pictorial sameness, under which a reproduction is aesthetically valuable as the original, were proposed in the transferability thesis.10 But an opposing ar- gument stated that even pictorial sameness of a reproduction does not necessarily grant sameness in aesthetic value.11 Thus the weakness of the early studies is that the aesthetic response to simple artificial stimuli is likely to be weak, as the viewers generally do not regard simple displays as pretty, especially in the context of being participants in an experi- ment. Moreover, most people do, in fact, experience art through reproductions, through art books and other media. Photography, a reproduction system in its essence, offers more control over reproduction, and better sameness than any other medium of art. Black-and-white photographs in particular are a good source for faithful reproduction. They represent subject properties as a relationship between the correlating values of luminance (not color), with only one- dimensional luminance gray scale. Therefore, while black- and-white photographs still carry the conceptual attributes as any other medium of art, they are less complex to ma- nipulate in a controlled process. In a previous paper12 we reported the roles and inter- ferences of local and global visual processes in lightness per- ception of photographs with meaningful contents; we exam- ined whether contrast discrimination is a response to spatial configuration properties of photographs, or a function of conceptual contents. In three experiments we compared contrast discrimination performances of observers, when presented with contrast increments applied to discrete tonal regions in gray scales versus several categories of black-and- white photographs of natural scenes. In the first experiment observers performed contrast discrimination in gray scales using rank-order tasks. In the following two experiments observers performed contrast discrimination of photographs using sorting tasks. We found substantial differences in re- 1 Presented in part at IST’s NIP XXI, Baltimore, MD, September 2005. Received Jun. 3, 2005; accepted for publication Oct. 30, 2005. 1062-3701/2006/50͑4͒/320/7/$20.00. Journal of Imaging Science and Technology® 50(4): 320–326, 2006. © Society for Imaging Science and Technology 2006 320
  • 2. sponse to contrast increments, depending on where in the characteristic curve (i.e., shadow, highlight, or midtones) they were applied. On the other hand, we reported no sig- nificant effect of category (portrait, landscape, and architec- ture). In addition we found that low performances in the shadow region of gray scales significantly improved in pho- tograph which we related to the differences in configuration complexity. We also reported differences in performance be- tween photographs of light and night scenes. These results raised an assumption that the differences in spatial configu- ration complexity, and not the conceptual content of the photographs, improved contrast detection performance. A possible explanation is provided by the “anchoring theory of lightness perception.”13 Photographs depicting natural objects contain much more complex and articulated information than gray scales, thus local frameworks weigh more than global frameworks, which results in improved lightness constancy and better performance in contrast dis- crimination tasks. These findings are also in line with recent psychophysical measurements of threshold versus contrast for images of natural scenes where image whitening yielded lower contrast discrimination threshold than high contrast images.14 Since looking at photographs is a visual task involving an aesthetic experience, a question of interest is: What is the relationship between the contrast discrimination, reported above, and the aesthetically preferred contrast reproduction of photographs. Recent studies claimed similar to the results reported above that there is no effect of context such as pose and shape of objects on processes such as lightness constancy.15 Opposing suggestions are that contextual infor- mation does affects processes such as global form perception.16 According to this view, one could expect contrast to influence the content of photographs and their aesthetic ap- peal. Therefore, the question asked in the present study was about the relationship between the conceptual content of photographs and their contrast preference. Moreover, should there indeed be a common rule for both execution of art and its appreciation, then one could expect similarity in the preferences of viewers and photographers,17,18 (see also: Discussion). Therefore, this study examines the relationship between the preferred con- trast reproduction and the “original” (unaltered) reproduc- tion, which may reflect on the photographer’s preference versus the viewer preference. EXPERIMENT Stimuli Eight black-and-white photographs from the “Photographs of the Southwest” book by Ansel Adams19 were used as stimuli for the present study: 1. “Spanish Peaks, Colorado,” 1951, p. 18; 2. “Canyon de Chelly, National Monument, Arizona,” 1947, p. 30; 3. “Moonrise, Hernandez, New-Mexico,” 1944, p. 55; 4. “Navajo Woman, Wide Ruin, Arizona,” 1948, p. 39; 5. “Maynard Dixon, Painter, Tucson, Arizona,” 1944, p. 16; 6. “Martha Porter, Pioneer Woman, Ordeville, Utah,” 1961, p. 81; 7. “Adobe Dwellings, Northern New-Mexico,” 1958, p. 49; and 8. “Arches, North Court, Mission San Xavier del Bac, Tucson, Arizona,” 1968, p. 94. The photographs selected belong to three major themes in photography: Landscape (photographs Nos. 1, 2, and 3), Por- trait (Nos. 4, 5, and 6), and Architecture (Nos. 7, 8, and 9). These themes represent Ansel Adams’ work in general, and the selected photographs are among his well-known and re- produced works. It is important to note that photographs selected as stimuli were of various configuration complexi- ties, articulation, depth, and tonal relationships, etc. Stimuli Reproduction Process The goal of the reproduction process was to alter the shadow (SH), highlight (HI), and midtone (MT) contrasts of the eight selected images. An Epson GT-9700 flatbed scanner was used to scan the book pages. The scanning pixels-per- inch (ppi) was 400. The ppi was chosen to match the print- ing system output resolution (Lambda printing system, see explanation below). For calibration of the reproduction sys- tem, the density of a Kodak standard gray-scale and its re- production were measured using a Sakura PDA 8112 reflec- tion densitometer and plotted against each other to assure a linear relationship between the reflection densities. Image files for the various highlight, midtone and shadow contrasts were generated using Adobe Photoshop® Curve function in the Adjustments menu. The horizontal axis of the curve graph represents the original brightness values of the pixels (input levels); the vertical axis represents the new brightness levels (output levels). The curve displays either brightness values from 0 to 255, with the shadows on the left, or values from 0% to 100% with shadows on the right. The default diagonal line shows that all pixels have the same input and output values. In order to alter contrast, two anchor/drag points were established on the diagonal line: 1. to alter shadow contrasts: ͑XSH=75% ,YSH=75%͒; 2. to alter highlight contrasts: ͑XHI=25% ,YHI=25%͒; and 3. to alter midtone contrasts both highlight and shadow anchor points were established: ͑XHI=25% ,YHI =25%͒,͑XSH=75% ,YSH=75%͒. A 2% contrast increment, for each of the reproduction sets, was achieved by pulling the anchoring point in one of the following directions: 1. shadow: ͑XSH+2=XSH+2% ,YSH+2=YSH−2%͒; 2. highlight: ͑XHI+2=XHI−2% ,YHI+2=YHI+2%͒; and 3. midtone: ͑XHI+2=XHI+2% ,YHI+2=YHI−2%͒, ͑XSH+2=XSH−2% ,YSH+2=YSH+2%͒. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 321
  • 3. For each of the three sets of reproductions (“HI,” “SH,” and “MT”) contrast increment ranged between 2% and 10% in steps of 2%, named sample Nos. 2–10 accordingly. The densities of the photographs are represented by Kodak standard gray scale reproduction sets, which were measured using a reflection densitometer, and plotted against the original gray scale, as shown in Figs. 1(a) (HI and SH) and 1(b) (MT). A total of 128 photographic prints were produced by Lambda system using a conventional black-and-white silver process. Lambda prints are made on a Durst Lambda printer, which uses three colored lasers to expose traditional photographic media. These prints have the advantage of us- ing the same rich red-green-blue (RGB) color space em- ployed by computer monitors. In addition, these prints are free of dots since unlike inkjet printers, the laser outputs are continuously modulated rather than switched on and off. Their 400 dpi resolution is comparable to 1200 dpi screened output. Participants Thirty subjects male and female (in 3:1 ratio) aged between 20 and 30 participated in the experiment. 46% of the par- ticipants were either familiar with the photographer or re- ported to have previously seen the photographs used as stimuli. Illumination Source The light source was a Toshiba natural color evaluation lamp, 5000 K, 700 lux. Light source was overhead with re- spect to the viewing area. Observation distance and angle: not designated. Procedure Each photograph and its reproductions composed a set of 16 stimuli. The stimuli were presented on a table in a rectan- gular arrangement of a random order of contrasts, as shown in Fig. 2 with no indication of the unaltered original photo- graph (OR), or any other standard. Participants were requested to evaluate the aesthetic ap- Figure 1. a. Characteristic curves for stimuli with 2–10% contrast increment in regions HI and SH. b. Characteristic curve for stimuli with 2–10% contrast increment in region MT. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance 322 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006
  • 4. peal of each sample according to a one-dimensional prefer- ence scale between 1 and 5, in which 1 is the lowest evaluation—“most dislike,” 2—“dislike,” 3—“neither like nor dislike,” 4—“like,” and 5 is the highest evaluation—“like best.” Results Evaluation ratios, on the 1–5 preference scale, for the pho- tographs of each category (portraits, landscape, and archi- tecture), were separately calculated, as a function of the tone regions (HI, SH, and MT). Within each region five contrast increments of 2% were indicated as sample Nos. 2–10 as shown in Fig. 3. The graph shows that preference was high- est for OR, and decreased systematically with contrast incre- ment in all regions (HI, SH, and MT) and all categories. This pattern of decreasing preference showed some varia- tions, which could be dependent on either category variance or the tone regions. In order to investigate which of these variables accounted for the observed differences, mean aver- age preferences were calculated for the categories and for the regions. The mean average preferences for categories were: 2.90 for portraits, 2.88 for landscapes, and 2.88 for architecture as shown in Fig. 4(a). These results indicated that contrast pref- erence was similar across the different categories. In order to confirm these observations, a two way analysis of variance (ANOVA), with categories (portrait versus landscape versus architecture) and contrast increments (OR vs 2% vs 4%…vs 10%) as variables, was conducted. In this and all other sta- tistical tests, ␣=0.05 and the 95% confidence interval were used for significance. The effect of category was not signifi- cant ͓F͑2,58͒=0.05͔,p=0.89. This reflects that there was no differential effect of category over preference of contrast. Moreover, comparison between photographs within category did not show significant difference either ͓F͑2,58͒=1.78͔, p=0.065. Mean average preferences for regions were: 2.6 for HI, 2.7 for MT, and 3.1 for SH as shown in Fig. 4(b). A two way ANOVA, with regions (HI vs SH vs MT) and contrast increments (OR vs 2% vs 4%…vs 10%) as variables, re- vealed that although the effect of regions over preference was not substantial, it was significant ͓F͑2,58͒=11.94͔ with p Ͻ0.0001. These results are in line with previous experiments in which the ability to perform contrast discrimination was measured for the same stimuli (see: Introduction). There, contrast discrimination was not affected by category, but de- termined by the discrete tonal regions in which contrast al- teration occurred. In fact, the present results show that the preference pattern is an inversion of the contrast discrimi- nation pattern, the higher the contrast discrimination ratio, the lower the preference. It is important to note that although contrast discrimi- nation ratio for MT was found to be significantly higher than that for the other regions, preference for MT was not the lowest one could expect, as shown in Fig. 5. The figure shows the effect of tonal region (HI vs SH vs MT vs OR) over mean contrast preference. Paired comparisons of contrasts revealed that contrast preference did not differ much between MT and HI regions ͓F͑1,58͒=2.14͔, pϽ0.0001. On the other hand, preference evaluation significantly increased in SH region ͓F͑1,58͒ =21.74͔, pϽ0.0001. In addition, the mean average prefer- ence of SH was above “3” in the evaluation scale, where evaluation tends towards “like,” while MT and HI were be- low the 3, where evaluation is already “dislike.” This finding Figure 3. Average preference for stimuli 2–10%, across the three categories and three regions. Figure 2. The arrangement of stimuli in the preference evaluation task. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 323
  • 5. implies a greater degree of tolerance towards contrast incre- ments in the shadow regions of the photographs. In such contrast reproductions the overall impression of the lighten- ing of the image. For an image which seems to become darker there is a lower degree of tolerance. Figure 6 shows the differential effect of contrast increment (2%, 6%, and 10%) over regions. For 2% contrast increment, preferences at regions HI, SH, and MT are: 3.59, 3.60, and 3.55 respec- tively, for 6% the preference of SH over the other regions is already substantial: 2.52, 3.3, and 2.8, and the difference is even greater for 10% increment: 1.58, 2.67, and 1.97. Preference for the OR was substantially higher than all other stimuli, independent of category or tonal region. That is, although stimuli were displayed in a random order, with no reference to the original photographs, observers’ prefer- ence matched the preference of the photographer, for all stimuli, as shown in Fig. 5, where mean average preference for OR is 3.9, and for SH, MT, and HI: 3.1, 2.74, and 2.6, respectively. Paired comparisons of contrasts confirmed that preference for OR was significantly higher than all other stimuli ͓F͑1,58͒=77.19͔, pϽ0.0001. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to investigate the aesthetic appeal of contrast reproductions in black-and-white photo- graphs, and the connection between contrast preference and contrast discrimination performance. Results revealed an inverse and linear relationship be- tween contrast increment and contrast preference, where preference decreased systematically with increment. Also, contrast preference seems to be independent of conceptual content, but apparently also from spatial configuration char- acteristics. Based on contrast discrimination results, we ex- pected to find a connection between preference and regions. Nevertheless, a uniform preference pattern was observed for Figure 5. A comparison between average preference of the unaltered photograph OR and regions HI vs SH vs MT. Figure 4. a. Average preference across categories reveals no significant difference. b. Average preference across regions HI vs SH vs MT reveals significant difference. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance 324 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006
  • 6. all photographs tested. Results also revealed a match be- tween the photographer’s contrast preference, manifested in his choice of contrasts in the photographs tested, and that of the viewer. One way of explaining these results is with the anchor- ing theory of lightness perception.20–22 In mapping lumi- nance into a lightness scale, the highest luminance is an- chored (assigned) to white, and the rest of the values are scaled relative to it. Other factors influencing anchoring are the area rule and configural attributes of the image, such as articulation and insulation. The compromise between the anchoring rules determines whether anchoring occurs in the local framework or the global framework. While strong an- choring to local framework increases lightness constancy, when the global framework is stronger, lightness constancy decreases and assignment to white is enhanced. Perhaps, as the contrast in the shadow region increases, insulation de- creases and the white areas grow bigger, thus the photograph is perceived as being lighter. Therefore, the decrease in light- ness constancy might be the reason for what seems to be greater tolerance to contrast increments in shadow regions than in highlights or midtones. In both experiments, i.e., contrast discrimination per- formance and preference evaluation of photographs, results reflected no influence of high vision processes, such as the perceived depth of stimulus21 to influence perception of the brightness of the pattern, or the context effect,23 also labeled “object-superiority effect,” suggesting that the context of overall structure and form influences early processing such as features extraction. Moreover, the argument that viewers prefer shapes and arrangements, which are most “effectively” processed by our visual system24,25 must also be reexamined in light of the present results. This argument derived from a work that ex- amined the effect of orientation on the aesthetic appeal of Mondrian’s paintings. The results in that work reflected a match in the preferred orientation between the artist and the viewers. This is in line with the match in contrast preference reported in the present paper, and also early experiments with other mediums of art including also poetry and prose, where sentences from well known classical writers were modified to impair the literary effect26–28 music, where well known pieces were modified to major mode and minor mode,29 orientation in abstract paintings,30 and other pic- ture alterations.31 The explanation for the tendency to prefer certain shapes or arrangements (Mondrian), which are effectively processed, was claimed to lie in the biological need to main- tain a certain level of stimulation of the visual system. Like other adaptive mechanisms, the visual system is encouraged to function most effectively; thus it prefers the more extreme stimuli over the weaker ones (peak shift effect32 ). According to this argument, artists should prefer to cre- ate line art or solid-color rectangular arrangements, such as Mondrians, and photographers should prefer high contrast film. But, in reality it is not so. In fact even Mondrian is but one example out of the general art creation. Even if Mon- drian was indeed gifted in the capability to intentionally cre- ate a faithful expression of the visual system, does that make his art, therefore, weigh more than other art works as a manifestation of the visual mechanisms? If so, that would be in contradiction with the above argument itself, denying its universal neurobiological ground, according to which all art works are manifestations of the brain’s mechanisms and constraints.33 If we all prefer the same shapes and orienta- tions then we should all have been making Mondrians, with no variance throughout art history and no effect of cultural, geographical, and historical conditions. If indeed the most extreme stimuli is the preferred one by the visual system and has the highest appeal to us all, then the results of the present experiment should have been that the greater the contrast increment, the higher the preference evaluation. In fact, the results reflect that both photographer and viewer prefer the contrast of reproductions similar to original, and not the extremes. This leads to the suggestion that the match between artist and viewers in constrast reproduction preference or orientation preference are valid only within the context of the specific artworks, and cannot be translated to other art works. CONCLUSION Further investigation, using eye movement tracker, can an- swer questions relating to the effect of contrast alteration over meaningful details in photographs and the consequent effect over fixation centers drifts and attention, following earlier studies.34–37 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors wish to express their gratitude to M. Tsukada, Horiuchi Color, Tokyo for printing and laminating the entire stimuli and supporting the research, and to Professor Masako Jitsumori, Chiba University, for his extensive assis- tance in the data analysis. REFERENCES 1 S. Zeki, Art and the Brain (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2000). 2 V. S. Ramachandran and W. Hirstein, “The science of art—A neurological theory of aesthetic experience”, J. Conscious. Stud. 6, 15–51 Figure 6. A comparison between average preferences at regions HI vs SH vs MT for stimuli 2%, 6%, and 10% contrast increment. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006 325
  • 7. (1999). 3 A. Hammond, “Ansel Adams and the objectivism—Making a photograph with group f/64”, History of Photography 22, 169–178 (1998). 4 I. H. Latour, “Ansel Adams, The zone system and the California School of Fine Arts,” History of Photography 22, 147–154 (1998). 5 G. T. Fechner, Vorschule der Asthetik (Breitkopf & Hartels, Leipzig, 1876). 6 L. B. Wexner, “The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood tones”, J. Appl. Psychol. 38, 432–435 (1954). 7 H. J. Eysenck, “An experimental study of aesthetic preference for polygonal figures”, J. Gen. Psychol. 79, 3–17 (1968). 8 H. J. Eysenck and M. Castle, “Training in art as a factor in the determination of preference judgments for polygons”, Br. J. Psychol. 61, 65–81 (1970). 9 G. Jahoda, “Sex differences in preferences for shapes: A cross-cultural replication,” Br. J. Psychol. 47, 126–132 (1956). 10 G. Currie, “The authentic and the aesthetic”, Am. Philosophical Q. 22, 153–160 (1985). 11 J. Elkins, “What are we seeing exactly?”, Art Bull. 79, 191–198 (1997). 12 S. Gershoni and H. Kobayashi, “How we look at photographs—As indicated by contrast discrimination performance”, J. Soc. Information Sci. Technol. Japan, accepted. 13 A. Gilchrist and C. Kossyfidis, “An anchoring theory of lightness perception”, Psychol. Rev. 106(4), 795–834 (1999). 14 C. A. Olman, K. Ugurbil, and D. Kersten, “Effects of image structure on perceived contrast and cortical activity in early visual areas”, J. Vision 3(9), 47a (2003). 15 C. Ripamonti, M. Bloj, R. E. Hauck, K. Mitha, and D. H. Brainard, “Object lightness constancy: Effects of object pose and shape”, J. Vision 3(9), 295a (2003). 16 C. Habak, F. Wilkinson, Z. Bernadette, and H. R. Wilson, “Contextual effects in form perception”, J. Vision 3(9), 354a (2003). 17 See Ref. 1. 18 R. Latto, D. Brain, and B. Kelly, “An oblique effect in aesthetics: Homage to Mondrian”, Perception 29, 981–987 (2000). 19 A. Adams, Photographs of the Southwest (New York Graphic Society, Boston, MA, 1976). 20 A. L. Gilchrist, “Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement”, Science 195, 185–187 (1977). 21 A. L. Gilchrist, “When does perceived lightness depend on perceived spatial arrangement?”, Percept. Psychophys. 28(6), 527–538 (1980). 22 See Ref. 13. 23 N. Weisstein and C. S. Harris, “Visual detection of line segments: An object superiority effect”, Science 186, 752–755 (1974). 24 See Ref. 18. 25 R. Latto, “The brain of the beholder”, in Artful Eye, edited by R. L. J. Gregory, P. Harris, and D. R. Heard (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1995), pp. 66–94. 26 C. Burt, in How the Mind Works, cited by I. Gordon and C. Gardner (Allen & Unwin, London, 1933); “Responses to altered pictures”, Br. J. Psychol. 65, 243–251 (1974). 27 E. D. Williams, L. Winter, and J. M. Woods, “Test of literary appreciation”, Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 265–284 (1938). 28 C. V. Valentine, The Experimental Psychology of Beauty (Metheun, London, 1962). 29 K. Havner, “The affective character of major and minor modes in music, Am. Psychol. 47, 103–118 (1935). 30 M. S. Lindauer, “The orientation of form in abstract art”, Proc. Am. Psychological Assoc. 4, 475–476 (1969). 31 N. C. Meier, The Meier Art Tests 2. Aesthetic Perception (University of Iowa, Bureau of Educational Research and Service, Iowa City, IA, 1963). 32 See Ref. 2. 33 See Ref. 1. 34 A. L. Yarbus, Eye Movements and Vision, edited by L. A. Riggs (Plenum Press, New York, 1967) edited by R. L. Solso, Cognition and the Visual Arts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994), p. 145. 35 C. F. Nodine, P. J. Locher, and E. A. Krupinski, “The role of formal art training on perception and aesthetic judgment of art compositions”, Leonardo 26, 219–227 (1993). 36 C. F. Nodine and H. L. Kundel, “Perception and display in diagnostic imaging”, Radio Graphs 7, 1241–1250 (1987). 37 C. F. Nodine, “Searching for NINA, 1987”, in R. L. Solso, Cognition and the Visual Arts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994), pp. 143–147. Gershoni and Kobayashi: How we look at photographs as indicated by contrast discrimination performance 326 J. Imaging Sci. Technol. 50͑4͒/Jul.-Aug. 2006