SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 12
Nathaniel AllenHevenstone
ProfessorDetwiler
Primate Behavior
Spring2013
Primate Aggression
1
Throughout the animal kingdom, aggression seems to be a dominating factor of the
competition for survival. Numerous hypotheses have been offered to explain why this is, but
nearly all of them focus on food and mating. Aggression seems to be mainly about gaining
access to territories with abundant food and mates. However, is that the only part of it, or is there
more to aggression and what determines whether or not an aggressive encounter will occur? I
want to look at primate aggression and the causes, and hope to find analogies to certain forms of
human aggression.
Pan troglodytes, common chimpanzees, are our most closely related evolutionary
cousins. We share around 98.4% of our DNA with chimpanzees, along with a number of social
behaviors, including very similar forms of aggression. In 1991, Joseph H. Manson and Richard
W. Wrangham published a paper comparing intergroup aggression amongst Chimpanzees and
humans. Their hypothesis was that the inalienability of females and of resources important to
female reproduction is extremely important to the development of the sometimes fatal
competition between males in both chimpanzees and humans. They note that with Chimpanzees,
as with humans, males tend to be the more aggressive sex, while females usually avoid
aggressive encounters. They also note that this is rare in primate aggression; in rhesus macaques
at Cayo Santiago, for example, females tend to be the more aggressive sex, along with younger
males, while older males try to stay out of harm’s way (Manson & Wrangham, 1991).
In 1999, Ned H. Kalin published a paper attempting to determine different models of
aggression in primates and how those models might be used to understand human aggression.
Kalin and his colleagues focused mainly on Rhesus monkeys because they “show behavioral and
NAH(Primate Aggression)
2
emotional responses that resemble those in humans”. Kalin looked at three categories of
aggression: defensive, offensive, and self-injurious. Kalin didn’t seem to focus on the third one
at all, but did spend a long time dealing with the defensive and offensive models. Both models
are relatively self-explanatory. The defensive model looks at responses to perceived threats,
while the offensive model looks at monkeys actually making threats. Kalin and his colleagues
studied both environmental factors and biological factors. The latter entailed reading a monkey’s
brain waves to see what happens when the monkey creates or responds to a threat. They found
that, in Rhesus macaques, defensive (fear-based) responses correlated with “increased electrical
activity in the right frontal region of the brain and that rhesus monkeys with extreme right frontal
activity have higher plasma cortisol concentrations”, while offensive aggression correlated with
“decreased serotonin and increased testosterone”. They concluded that further study is needed
(Kalin, 1999).
In December 2007, the journal Behaviour published an entire issue (Volume 144, Issue
12) devoted to Intergroup Aggression in Wild Primates. With one introduction and eight articles,
this issue focused on non-human primates in general, but also had one article devoted to white-
faced capuchin monkeys (“Mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchins (Cebus
capucinus): the importance of short and long-term strategies”), one article devoted to gorillas
(“Intergroup encounters in mountain gorillas of Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda”),
one article devoted to blue monkeys (“Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding
0territories by blue monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya”), one article devoted to the
Thomas langur (“Familiarity and threat of opponents determine variation in Thomas langur
(Presbytis thomasi) male behaviour during between-group encounters”), and one article devoted
to chimpanzees (“Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in
NAH(Primate Aggression)
3
response to location-specific risk”). Aside from a small mention in the introduction, human
aggression is not considered in this issue. However, it does provide many fascinating insights
into the nature of primate aggression ("Intergroup aggression in," 2007).
The first article examines mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchin
monkeys. In the study, Margaret C. Crofoot suggests that intergroup aggression was not
motivated by short-term reproductive strategies, but by long-term reproductive strategies. She
also suggests that a good percentage of the intergroup aggression observed in these capuchins
could be males of one group attempting to overthrow the alpha male of the targeted group, while
a smaller percentage were mainly just “scope-outs” (the males of one group assessing the gender
make-up and size of another group). However, neither explanation accounted for all observed
periods of intergroup aggression. She concludes that there may be more explanations than mating
and even access to resources for intergroup competition (Crofoot, 2007).
The next article, written by Martha M. Robbins and Sarah C. Sawyer, looks at the
intergroup encounters of gorillas at Bwindi Impenetrable National Park in Uganda. Robbins and
Sawyer looked at “the influence of frugivory and social factors on behaviour during intergroup
encounters”. They both suggest that while encounters themselves did occur at limited fruit
patches, frugivory had no impact on the length of the encounter, or on the type of behaviour
exhibited, suggesting that while neighbouring groups may be attracted to limited fruit patches,
there was no indication of between group feeding competition.” The number of migrant females
and the presence of silverbacks also had no impact on this. How a group behaved depended on
the make-up of the second group in the encounter. If one group of gorillas encountered a solitary
male, they were more likely to herd females away from the male, and practice avoidance at all
costs, suggesting that solitary males were more likely to take more risks in a takeover than
NAH(Primate Aggression)
4
groups including females. Their conclusion is that “between group competition is linked more to
mate defence and acquisition than resource defence” (Robbins & Sawyer, 2007).
The third article in the issue, written by Tara R. Harris, is a much more generalized study
of primate aggression. Harris looks at three proposed functions of intergroup aggression: mate
defense, infant defense, and resource defense. Her goal here is to raise some issues with the
predictions and methods used to study these and make suggestions that she feels could improve
future studies (Harris, 2007). The fourth article, written by Marina Cords, looks at the defense of
communal feeding territories by blue monkeys. She is questioning why group members did not
participate equally in the defense. Using data spanning five years and five wild groups, Cords
found that adult females participated the most, while males participated less and less as they
matured and female participation increased with maturation. She also found that females with
juveniles participated less, while higher-ranking females participated more. She was surprised at
these last two, though especially the latter, having expected the opposite (Cords, 2007).
The fifth article in the issue, written by Dawn M. Kitchen and Jacinta C. Beehner, looks
at individual participation amongst non-human primate aggression. They suggest that benefits
such as group defense are not monopolizable, and as such, there is an economic problem: “who
should contribute to public goods and who should freeload?” This is mainly a review paper, in
which Kitchen and Beehner looked at over 20 years of data and focused on individual
participation in intergroup encounters. They found that between-sex variation is explainable
mainly by food-defense and mate-defense, while within-sex variation is explainable mainly by
rank and reproductive success. They also suggest that some individuals within a primate group
may use intergroup encounters to see if there are any possibilities for transfer and mating
NAH(Primate Aggression)
5
success. Overall, they find that the question of who participates in intergroup encounters has a lot
of answers and depends largely on individual factors (Kitchen & Beehner, 2007).
The sixth article in the issue, written by Serge A.Wich and Elisabeth H.M. Sterck, looks
at whether or not familiarity and the kind of threat opponents present determine the intensity of a
between-group encounter in Thomas langurs. Perhaps not surprisingly, they found that less
familiar groups posing greater threats caused more intense encounters. Thomas langurs use both
familiarity and the level of a threat to determine the intensity of their responses, which were
measured in the number and type of calls given towards a possible threat (Wich & Sterck, 2007).
The seventh article in the issue, written by Richard Wrangham, Meg Crofoot, Rochelle
Lundy, and Ian Gilby, tests the risk hypothesis. Specifically, they look at ranging data from three
species of group-living primates (redtail monkeys, white-faced capuchins, and chimpanzees) in
order to see if “overlap zones are smaller or used less in species that are subject to a higher risk
of lethal aggression in intergroup encounters.” They assessed how high the risk of encounters
was for each species: redtail monkeys have a low risk of encounters, white-faced capuchins have
an intermediate risk, and chimpanzees have a very high risk. Rather surprisingly, these risks did
not seem to be a factor in the low use or small size of overlap zones, since the overlaps zones
were small and little-used for all three species (Wrangham, Crofoot, Lundy & Gilby, 2007).
The eighth and final article in the issue, written by Michael L. Wilson, Marc D. Hauser,
and Richard W. Wrangham, looks at whether or not chimpanzees they modify their vocal
responses and group behavior based on the different risks associated with different locations.
They looked at the Kanyawara chimpanzees, comparing behavior in the territory core with
behavior in two different contexts: the periphery and the cropland; two potentially dangerous
areas of the range. They found that when male chimpanzees visited the periphery, their groups
NAH(Primate Aggression)
6
had nearly twice as many males as they did in the core, and when they visited the croplands, they
were much more silent than in the core (as an aside, it’d be interesting to see what they did do
individuals who broke that silence in the croplands, alerting any potential dangers to their
presence, and whether this reaction would be different or the same if the individual causing the
disturbance was part of the group or an outsider, and a chimpanzee or not a chimpanzee). They
could be more vocal in the periphery, however, perhaps in order to advertise (and maybe
exaggerate?) coalition strength and their ownership of the territory (Wilson, Hauser &
Wrangham, 2007).
Other factors besides defense of mates, juveniles, and resources might contribute to
primate aggression, including high levels of testosterone and stress. In a test of the former,
Martin N. Muller and Richard W. Wrangham submitted a paper to Animal Behaviour in 2004
called Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge
hypothesis’. The challenge hypothesis basically says that “variation in male testosterone levels is
more closely associated with aggression in reproductive contexts than it is with changes in
reproductive physiology.” They tested this by conducting behavioral observations and
noninvasive hormone sampling of eleven male Kanyawara chimpanzees. They found that male
testosterone level increases when parous females show maximally tumescent sexual swellings,
and aggressive behavior amongst males also increases during this time. Although male
testosterone levels do not increase in the presence of nulliparous females when they were
maximally tumescent, they do mate with both parous and nulliparous males equally, “suggesting
that testosterone increases in the presence of cycling parous females are associated with
aggression rather than sexual behaviour.” Therefore, the challenge hypothesis was largely
NAH(Primate Aggression)
7
upheld, and might actually have greater implications for aggressive behavior in primates,
including humans (Muller & Wrangham, 2004).
In 2006, P.E. Honess and C.M. Marin submitted a paper to the Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews journal called Behavioural and physiological aspects of stress and
aggression in nonhuman primates. In it, they study the effects of stress on aggression in non-
human primates. Although it is a generalized review paper, they focus primarily on macaques
since they are held in biomedical research facilities (this actually suggests, to me, a problem with
this paper, as I’m not sure how much studies on captive animals can say about their wild
counterparts). They mainly found that “the relationships between stress and aggression and in
particular the hormonal changes that are associated with them are highly complex in primates.”
Their study was frequently confounded by “variation in these between studies”, including
numerous sources of variation and inconsistencies in definitions used by different authors.
Honess and Marin do suggest that, despite this, a number of relationships are already very clear,
not least the consequences for animals of elevated stress levels in terms of their health,
development and reproductive success and the need to manage this stress is important in
determining social and reproductive strategies.” The role that stress plays in intergroup
aggression is still a question, and one that needs to be answered. However, perhaps studies of
wild primates would yield better data and results (Honess & Marin, 2006).
So why are intergroup encounters so prevalent? A paper published in 2012 by Richard
W. Wrangham and Luke Glowacki, comparing intergroup aggression in Chimpanzees with war
in Nomadic Hunter-Gatherers, suggests that individual natural selection may be the explanation.
In the discussion, Wrangham and Glowacki ask if the chimpanzee model is compatible with
evolutionary theory. They discuss three different adaptive explanations: individual selection,
NAH(Primate Aggression)
8
genetic group selection, and cultural group selection. After ruling out both genetic and cultural
group selection, they suggest that “[n]atural selection acting at the individual level is… the only
adaptive process that can explain the evolution of the predisposition for intergroup killing in
chimpanzees. This might seem surprising given that individuals apparently do not receive
benefits in proportion to their investment. Furthermore the costs of territorial defense and
intergroup aggression, while low, are positive, including not only the marginal risk of being
wounded but also the energetic costs of patrolling the territorial boundary, which can be high
(Amsler 2010). However direct individual benefits could come from an increased effectiveness
of attack when more aggressors cooperate: for example, a greater likelihood of damaging or
killing the opponent and a reduced likelihood of any members of the killing party suffering
injury, with the concomitant benefits of reduced enemy threat and increased access to resources
(Clutton-Brock 2009).” After determining this, they ask whether the chimpanzee model of
intergroup aggression can apply to nomadic hunter-gatherer warfare. They aren’t looking at
variations in war practices, but whether or not “nomadic hunter-gatherers neighboring other
nomadic hunter-gatherer societies tend to show the three key elements of the chimpanzee model:
consistent intergroup hostility, safe killing, and benefits from intergroup dominance” (Wrangham
& Glowacki, 2012).
Wrangham and Glowacki do find a context that fits the chimpanzee model; in the
Andaman Islands. “Because multiple societies were isolated in the Andaman Islands in the
absence of agriculture, Kelly (2000:96) regarded them as an ideal natural experiment.” However,
they suggest that the Andamanese were unusual in their warfare. Not because of limited access to
resources, but because of their failure to negotiate intersocietal cooperation. They found that
hunter-gather intrasocietal warfare “oscillate[s] with periods of relaxed peace and therefore do
NAH(Primate Aggression)
9
not conform to the chimpanzee model. Peacemaking is readily explained by bands being
members of the same society, with opportunities for linguistically negotiated intermarriage,
trade, alliances, and migration between bands (Chapais 2008; Rodseth et al. 1991).” However,
they did find that warfare between societies did conform to the chimpanzee model because the
aggression did, in fact, have the three key elements of the chimpanzee model (Wrangham &
Glowacki, 2012).
So now a picture of primate aggression begins to form. Rather unsurprisingly, it is mainly
the way primates, like all other life on this planet, survive; through competition. That is, the
aggression seems to be largely about obtaining greater access to resources and mates, and then
defending those resources and mates from others. This suggests that some form of competition
drives survival almost entirely. But what does this say for humans?
Do we still have to compete in order to survive? Do we still have to fight for mates and
resources? If we do, then human aggression may be explainable mainly by the same mechanisms
that explain non-human animal aggression. But even if we don’t need to compete anymore in
order to survive, that drive still exists within us, which means our aggression can still be
explained that way. We may not have to, but we still do compete, and we compete for survival…
for resources and, perhaps, mates. However, there is one form of human aggression that remains
mysterious: fanaticism.
Fanaticism is a unique form of human aggression. It is mainly associated with religion.
We recognize fanatics as those who stand on street corners and knock on doors, intent on
“spreading the good news”. They believe that the world is only between 5500 and 10,000 years
old. We also look at them as a “fringe element”. However, fanaticism is much more ubiquitous
than that. For starters, fully 46% of US-Americans believe that the world is young instead of old
NAH(Primate Aggression)
10
(Newport, 2012). There’s nothing “fringe” about 46% (in fact, only 15% actually accept the
scientific explanation for the origin of species; so one could argue, at least in terms of numbers
and percentages, those of us who do accept the science as is are the “fringe”). For another thing,
fanaticism reaches far beyond religion, and is not necessarily overtly aggressive. Nearly any
social gathering around a common theme can have its fanatics. Some would argue that most
politicians are fanatics. Patriotism itself is, in fact, a form of fanaticism. Fanaticism even shows
up in a much more benign way: social groupings/gatherings around celebrities, musicians, bands,
artists, movies, books, TV shows, and games (including sports); after all, the word “fan” is short
for “fanatic”. Though even in this case it is not always benign, as the infamous rivalry between
Yankees fans and Red Sox fans can show.
So what causes fanaticism? As I said above, survival seems to mainly be a… I guess…
“competitive sport”. One could suggest that even plants compete to survive. However, for the
human species, we may no longer need to compete. Despite this, that drive will not have gone
anywhere. We seem to still be driven to compete, as the drive for survival still seems to exist for
us. I suggest that, if we really do not need to compete with each other and with non-human
animals in order to survive, yet that drive still exists for us, then perhaps fanaticism may be at
least partially explainable as a manifestation of that drive. It has to manifest somehow, and
perhaps defending our beliefs and ideas, even in the face of contradictory evidence, is one way
that competitive drive manifests itself. Which, sadly, means that fanaticism may be impossible to
get rid of.
NAH(Primate Aggression)
11
Bibliography
(2007). Intergroup aggression in wild primates. Behaviour, 144(12), 1469-1653.
Cords, M. (2007). Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding territories by blue
monkeys in the kakamega forest, kenya. Behaviour, 144(12), 1537-1550.
Crofoot, M. C. (2007). Mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchins (cebus
capucinus): the importance of shortand long-term strategies . Behaviour, 144(12), 1473-
1495.
Harris, T. R. (2007). Testing mate, resource and infant defence functions of intergroup
aggression in nonhuman primates: issues and methodology. Behaviour, 144(12), 1521-
1535.
Honess, P. E., & Marin, C. M. (2006). Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. Behavioural and
physiological aspects of stress and aggression in nonhuman primates, 30, 390-412. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.003
Kalin, N. H. (1999). Primate models to understand human aggression. The journal of clinical
psychiatry, 60, 29-32.
Kitchen, D. M., & Beehner, J. C. (2007). Factors affecting individual participation in group-level
aggression among non-human primates. Behaviour, 144(12), 1551-1581.
Manson, J. H., & Wrangham, R. W. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans.
Current Anthropology, 32(4), 369-377. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2743814
Muller, M. M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2004). Animal behaviour. Dominance, aggression and
testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’, 67, 113-123. doi:
10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.013
Newport, F. (2012, June 01). [Web log message]. Retrieved from
http://www.gallup.com/poll/155003/Hold-Creationist-View-Human-Origins.aspx
Robbins, M. M., & Sawyer, S. C. (2007). Intergroup encounters in mountain gorillas of Bwindi
impenetrable national park, uganda. Behaviour, 144(12), 1497-1519.
Wich, S. A., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Familiarity and threat of opponents determine variation
in thomas langur (presbytis thomasi) male behaviour during between-group encounters.
Behaviour, 144(12), 1583-1598.
Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Chimpanzees (pan troglodytes)
modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour,
144(12), 1599-1619.
NAH(Primate Aggression)
12
Wrangham, R., Crofoot, M., Lundy, R., & Gilby, I. (2007). Use of overlap zones among group-
living primates: a test of the risk hypothesis. Behaviour, 144(12), 1599-1619.
Wrangham, R. W., & Glowacki, L. (2012). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and war in
nomadic hunter-gatherers: Evaluating the chimpanzee model. Human Nature, 23(1), 05-
29. doi: 10.1007/s12110-012-9132-1

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis
Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis
Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis kimlyman
 
The Flow of Energy Through Ecosystems
The Flow of Energy Through EcosystemsThe Flow of Energy Through Ecosystems
The Flow of Energy Through Ecosystemskstepping
 
Theory of evolution by natural selection
Theory of evolution by natural selectionTheory of evolution by natural selection
Theory of evolution by natural selectionLekhan Lodhi
 
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireos
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireosSigma xi showcase - competition in vireos
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireoskpeiman
 

Mais procurados (6)

Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis
Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis
Healey sdal social dynamics in living systems from microbe to metropolis
 
The Flow of Energy Through Ecosystems
The Flow of Energy Through EcosystemsThe Flow of Energy Through Ecosystems
The Flow of Energy Through Ecosystems
 
Evol. Ecol. Paper
Evol. Ecol. PaperEvol. Ecol. Paper
Evol. Ecol. Paper
 
Theory of evolution by natural selection
Theory of evolution by natural selectionTheory of evolution by natural selection
Theory of evolution by natural selection
 
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireos
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireosSigma xi showcase - competition in vireos
Sigma xi showcase - competition in vireos
 
Eastern Poster 2016
Eastern Poster 2016Eastern Poster 2016
Eastern Poster 2016
 

Semelhante a Primate Aggression Factors

Annand Edited Thesis
Annand Edited ThesisAnnand Edited Thesis
Annand Edited ThesisChloe Annand
 
Why are Humans so Afraid of Insects
Why are Humans so Afraid of InsectsWhy are Humans so Afraid of Insects
Why are Humans so Afraid of Insectsijtsrd
 
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui Hymes
 
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happens
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happensOCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happens
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happensBaltazarRosales1
 
darwin for psychologists
darwin for psychologistsdarwin for psychologists
darwin for psychologistsGreta Sykes
 
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_FinalAlicia Peluso
 
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered SpeciesTaxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species01archivist
 
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Jennifer Fiato
 
Animal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyAnimal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyTony Lisko
 
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...AI Publications
 
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2Wagner College
 
Attitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsAttitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsFábio Coltro
 
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docx
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docxThe Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docx
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 

Semelhante a Primate Aggression Factors (19)

Annand Edited Thesis
Annand Edited ThesisAnnand Edited Thesis
Annand Edited Thesis
 
Why are Humans so Afraid of Insects
Why are Humans so Afraid of InsectsWhy are Humans so Afraid of Insects
Why are Humans so Afraid of Insects
 
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster finalWangui mid-SURE poster final
Wangui mid-SURE poster final
 
Apesian Mimicry
Apesian MimicryApesian Mimicry
Apesian Mimicry
 
dissertation complete
dissertation completedissertation complete
dissertation complete
 
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happens
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happensOCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happens
OCCURENCE OF EVOLUTION.pptx how does evolution happens
 
darwin for psychologists
darwin for psychologistsdarwin for psychologists
darwin for psychologists
 
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
04.Peluso_Anderson_Final
 
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered SpeciesTaxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species
Taxonomy and the Conservation of Endangered Species
 
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
Homosexuality, 1800's-2015
 
Zoonotic Diseases: Ebola in Africa
Zoonotic Diseases: Ebola in AfricaZoonotic Diseases: Ebola in Africa
Zoonotic Diseases: Ebola in Africa
 
Why do we study Animal Behavior
Why do we study Animal Behavior Why do we study Animal Behavior
Why do we study Animal Behavior
 
Animal Cruelty
Animal CrueltyAnimal Cruelty
Animal Cruelty
 
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...
The Investigation of Snake-phobia Management by the Inhabitants of the City o...
 
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol 13 No 2
 
Attitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animalsAttitudes toward animals
Attitudes toward animals
 
Twin Studies Essay
Twin Studies EssayTwin Studies Essay
Twin Studies Essay
 
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docx
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docxThe Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docx
The Development of Evolutionary TheoryAnthropology 1 Fall.docx
 
Abstract book talks
Abstract book talksAbstract book talks
Abstract book talks
 

Primate Aggression Factors

  • 1. Nathaniel AllenHevenstone ProfessorDetwiler Primate Behavior Spring2013 Primate Aggression 1 Throughout the animal kingdom, aggression seems to be a dominating factor of the competition for survival. Numerous hypotheses have been offered to explain why this is, but nearly all of them focus on food and mating. Aggression seems to be mainly about gaining access to territories with abundant food and mates. However, is that the only part of it, or is there more to aggression and what determines whether or not an aggressive encounter will occur? I want to look at primate aggression and the causes, and hope to find analogies to certain forms of human aggression. Pan troglodytes, common chimpanzees, are our most closely related evolutionary cousins. We share around 98.4% of our DNA with chimpanzees, along with a number of social behaviors, including very similar forms of aggression. In 1991, Joseph H. Manson and Richard W. Wrangham published a paper comparing intergroup aggression amongst Chimpanzees and humans. Their hypothesis was that the inalienability of females and of resources important to female reproduction is extremely important to the development of the sometimes fatal competition between males in both chimpanzees and humans. They note that with Chimpanzees, as with humans, males tend to be the more aggressive sex, while females usually avoid aggressive encounters. They also note that this is rare in primate aggression; in rhesus macaques at Cayo Santiago, for example, females tend to be the more aggressive sex, along with younger males, while older males try to stay out of harm’s way (Manson & Wrangham, 1991). In 1999, Ned H. Kalin published a paper attempting to determine different models of aggression in primates and how those models might be used to understand human aggression. Kalin and his colleagues focused mainly on Rhesus monkeys because they “show behavioral and
  • 2. NAH(Primate Aggression) 2 emotional responses that resemble those in humans”. Kalin looked at three categories of aggression: defensive, offensive, and self-injurious. Kalin didn’t seem to focus on the third one at all, but did spend a long time dealing with the defensive and offensive models. Both models are relatively self-explanatory. The defensive model looks at responses to perceived threats, while the offensive model looks at monkeys actually making threats. Kalin and his colleagues studied both environmental factors and biological factors. The latter entailed reading a monkey’s brain waves to see what happens when the monkey creates or responds to a threat. They found that, in Rhesus macaques, defensive (fear-based) responses correlated with “increased electrical activity in the right frontal region of the brain and that rhesus monkeys with extreme right frontal activity have higher plasma cortisol concentrations”, while offensive aggression correlated with “decreased serotonin and increased testosterone”. They concluded that further study is needed (Kalin, 1999). In December 2007, the journal Behaviour published an entire issue (Volume 144, Issue 12) devoted to Intergroup Aggression in Wild Primates. With one introduction and eight articles, this issue focused on non-human primates in general, but also had one article devoted to white- faced capuchin monkeys (“Mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus): the importance of short and long-term strategies”), one article devoted to gorillas (“Intergroup encounters in mountain gorillas of Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda”), one article devoted to blue monkeys (“Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding 0territories by blue monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya”), one article devoted to the Thomas langur (“Familiarity and threat of opponents determine variation in Thomas langur (Presbytis thomasi) male behaviour during between-group encounters”), and one article devoted to chimpanzees (“Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in
  • 3. NAH(Primate Aggression) 3 response to location-specific risk”). Aside from a small mention in the introduction, human aggression is not considered in this issue. However, it does provide many fascinating insights into the nature of primate aggression ("Intergroup aggression in," 2007). The first article examines mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchin monkeys. In the study, Margaret C. Crofoot suggests that intergroup aggression was not motivated by short-term reproductive strategies, but by long-term reproductive strategies. She also suggests that a good percentage of the intergroup aggression observed in these capuchins could be males of one group attempting to overthrow the alpha male of the targeted group, while a smaller percentage were mainly just “scope-outs” (the males of one group assessing the gender make-up and size of another group). However, neither explanation accounted for all observed periods of intergroup aggression. She concludes that there may be more explanations than mating and even access to resources for intergroup competition (Crofoot, 2007). The next article, written by Martha M. Robbins and Sarah C. Sawyer, looks at the intergroup encounters of gorillas at Bwindi Impenetrable National Park in Uganda. Robbins and Sawyer looked at “the influence of frugivory and social factors on behaviour during intergroup encounters”. They both suggest that while encounters themselves did occur at limited fruit patches, frugivory had no impact on the length of the encounter, or on the type of behaviour exhibited, suggesting that while neighbouring groups may be attracted to limited fruit patches, there was no indication of between group feeding competition.” The number of migrant females and the presence of silverbacks also had no impact on this. How a group behaved depended on the make-up of the second group in the encounter. If one group of gorillas encountered a solitary male, they were more likely to herd females away from the male, and practice avoidance at all costs, suggesting that solitary males were more likely to take more risks in a takeover than
  • 4. NAH(Primate Aggression) 4 groups including females. Their conclusion is that “between group competition is linked more to mate defence and acquisition than resource defence” (Robbins & Sawyer, 2007). The third article in the issue, written by Tara R. Harris, is a much more generalized study of primate aggression. Harris looks at three proposed functions of intergroup aggression: mate defense, infant defense, and resource defense. Her goal here is to raise some issues with the predictions and methods used to study these and make suggestions that she feels could improve future studies (Harris, 2007). The fourth article, written by Marina Cords, looks at the defense of communal feeding territories by blue monkeys. She is questioning why group members did not participate equally in the defense. Using data spanning five years and five wild groups, Cords found that adult females participated the most, while males participated less and less as they matured and female participation increased with maturation. She also found that females with juveniles participated less, while higher-ranking females participated more. She was surprised at these last two, though especially the latter, having expected the opposite (Cords, 2007). The fifth article in the issue, written by Dawn M. Kitchen and Jacinta C. Beehner, looks at individual participation amongst non-human primate aggression. They suggest that benefits such as group defense are not monopolizable, and as such, there is an economic problem: “who should contribute to public goods and who should freeload?” This is mainly a review paper, in which Kitchen and Beehner looked at over 20 years of data and focused on individual participation in intergroup encounters. They found that between-sex variation is explainable mainly by food-defense and mate-defense, while within-sex variation is explainable mainly by rank and reproductive success. They also suggest that some individuals within a primate group may use intergroup encounters to see if there are any possibilities for transfer and mating
  • 5. NAH(Primate Aggression) 5 success. Overall, they find that the question of who participates in intergroup encounters has a lot of answers and depends largely on individual factors (Kitchen & Beehner, 2007). The sixth article in the issue, written by Serge A.Wich and Elisabeth H.M. Sterck, looks at whether or not familiarity and the kind of threat opponents present determine the intensity of a between-group encounter in Thomas langurs. Perhaps not surprisingly, they found that less familiar groups posing greater threats caused more intense encounters. Thomas langurs use both familiarity and the level of a threat to determine the intensity of their responses, which were measured in the number and type of calls given towards a possible threat (Wich & Sterck, 2007). The seventh article in the issue, written by Richard Wrangham, Meg Crofoot, Rochelle Lundy, and Ian Gilby, tests the risk hypothesis. Specifically, they look at ranging data from three species of group-living primates (redtail monkeys, white-faced capuchins, and chimpanzees) in order to see if “overlap zones are smaller or used less in species that are subject to a higher risk of lethal aggression in intergroup encounters.” They assessed how high the risk of encounters was for each species: redtail monkeys have a low risk of encounters, white-faced capuchins have an intermediate risk, and chimpanzees have a very high risk. Rather surprisingly, these risks did not seem to be a factor in the low use or small size of overlap zones, since the overlaps zones were small and little-used for all three species (Wrangham, Crofoot, Lundy & Gilby, 2007). The eighth and final article in the issue, written by Michael L. Wilson, Marc D. Hauser, and Richard W. Wrangham, looks at whether or not chimpanzees they modify their vocal responses and group behavior based on the different risks associated with different locations. They looked at the Kanyawara chimpanzees, comparing behavior in the territory core with behavior in two different contexts: the periphery and the cropland; two potentially dangerous areas of the range. They found that when male chimpanzees visited the periphery, their groups
  • 6. NAH(Primate Aggression) 6 had nearly twice as many males as they did in the core, and when they visited the croplands, they were much more silent than in the core (as an aside, it’d be interesting to see what they did do individuals who broke that silence in the croplands, alerting any potential dangers to their presence, and whether this reaction would be different or the same if the individual causing the disturbance was part of the group or an outsider, and a chimpanzee or not a chimpanzee). They could be more vocal in the periphery, however, perhaps in order to advertise (and maybe exaggerate?) coalition strength and their ownership of the territory (Wilson, Hauser & Wrangham, 2007). Other factors besides defense of mates, juveniles, and resources might contribute to primate aggression, including high levels of testosterone and stress. In a test of the former, Martin N. Muller and Richard W. Wrangham submitted a paper to Animal Behaviour in 2004 called Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’. The challenge hypothesis basically says that “variation in male testosterone levels is more closely associated with aggression in reproductive contexts than it is with changes in reproductive physiology.” They tested this by conducting behavioral observations and noninvasive hormone sampling of eleven male Kanyawara chimpanzees. They found that male testosterone level increases when parous females show maximally tumescent sexual swellings, and aggressive behavior amongst males also increases during this time. Although male testosterone levels do not increase in the presence of nulliparous females when they were maximally tumescent, they do mate with both parous and nulliparous males equally, “suggesting that testosterone increases in the presence of cycling parous females are associated with aggression rather than sexual behaviour.” Therefore, the challenge hypothesis was largely
  • 7. NAH(Primate Aggression) 7 upheld, and might actually have greater implications for aggressive behavior in primates, including humans (Muller & Wrangham, 2004). In 2006, P.E. Honess and C.M. Marin submitted a paper to the Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal called Behavioural and physiological aspects of stress and aggression in nonhuman primates. In it, they study the effects of stress on aggression in non- human primates. Although it is a generalized review paper, they focus primarily on macaques since they are held in biomedical research facilities (this actually suggests, to me, a problem with this paper, as I’m not sure how much studies on captive animals can say about their wild counterparts). They mainly found that “the relationships between stress and aggression and in particular the hormonal changes that are associated with them are highly complex in primates.” Their study was frequently confounded by “variation in these between studies”, including numerous sources of variation and inconsistencies in definitions used by different authors. Honess and Marin do suggest that, despite this, a number of relationships are already very clear, not least the consequences for animals of elevated stress levels in terms of their health, development and reproductive success and the need to manage this stress is important in determining social and reproductive strategies.” The role that stress plays in intergroup aggression is still a question, and one that needs to be answered. However, perhaps studies of wild primates would yield better data and results (Honess & Marin, 2006). So why are intergroup encounters so prevalent? A paper published in 2012 by Richard W. Wrangham and Luke Glowacki, comparing intergroup aggression in Chimpanzees with war in Nomadic Hunter-Gatherers, suggests that individual natural selection may be the explanation. In the discussion, Wrangham and Glowacki ask if the chimpanzee model is compatible with evolutionary theory. They discuss three different adaptive explanations: individual selection,
  • 8. NAH(Primate Aggression) 8 genetic group selection, and cultural group selection. After ruling out both genetic and cultural group selection, they suggest that “[n]atural selection acting at the individual level is… the only adaptive process that can explain the evolution of the predisposition for intergroup killing in chimpanzees. This might seem surprising given that individuals apparently do not receive benefits in proportion to their investment. Furthermore the costs of territorial defense and intergroup aggression, while low, are positive, including not only the marginal risk of being wounded but also the energetic costs of patrolling the territorial boundary, which can be high (Amsler 2010). However direct individual benefits could come from an increased effectiveness of attack when more aggressors cooperate: for example, a greater likelihood of damaging or killing the opponent and a reduced likelihood of any members of the killing party suffering injury, with the concomitant benefits of reduced enemy threat and increased access to resources (Clutton-Brock 2009).” After determining this, they ask whether the chimpanzee model of intergroup aggression can apply to nomadic hunter-gatherer warfare. They aren’t looking at variations in war practices, but whether or not “nomadic hunter-gatherers neighboring other nomadic hunter-gatherer societies tend to show the three key elements of the chimpanzee model: consistent intergroup hostility, safe killing, and benefits from intergroup dominance” (Wrangham & Glowacki, 2012). Wrangham and Glowacki do find a context that fits the chimpanzee model; in the Andaman Islands. “Because multiple societies were isolated in the Andaman Islands in the absence of agriculture, Kelly (2000:96) regarded them as an ideal natural experiment.” However, they suggest that the Andamanese were unusual in their warfare. Not because of limited access to resources, but because of their failure to negotiate intersocietal cooperation. They found that hunter-gather intrasocietal warfare “oscillate[s] with periods of relaxed peace and therefore do
  • 9. NAH(Primate Aggression) 9 not conform to the chimpanzee model. Peacemaking is readily explained by bands being members of the same society, with opportunities for linguistically negotiated intermarriage, trade, alliances, and migration between bands (Chapais 2008; Rodseth et al. 1991).” However, they did find that warfare between societies did conform to the chimpanzee model because the aggression did, in fact, have the three key elements of the chimpanzee model (Wrangham & Glowacki, 2012). So now a picture of primate aggression begins to form. Rather unsurprisingly, it is mainly the way primates, like all other life on this planet, survive; through competition. That is, the aggression seems to be largely about obtaining greater access to resources and mates, and then defending those resources and mates from others. This suggests that some form of competition drives survival almost entirely. But what does this say for humans? Do we still have to compete in order to survive? Do we still have to fight for mates and resources? If we do, then human aggression may be explainable mainly by the same mechanisms that explain non-human animal aggression. But even if we don’t need to compete anymore in order to survive, that drive still exists within us, which means our aggression can still be explained that way. We may not have to, but we still do compete, and we compete for survival… for resources and, perhaps, mates. However, there is one form of human aggression that remains mysterious: fanaticism. Fanaticism is a unique form of human aggression. It is mainly associated with religion. We recognize fanatics as those who stand on street corners and knock on doors, intent on “spreading the good news”. They believe that the world is only between 5500 and 10,000 years old. We also look at them as a “fringe element”. However, fanaticism is much more ubiquitous than that. For starters, fully 46% of US-Americans believe that the world is young instead of old
  • 10. NAH(Primate Aggression) 10 (Newport, 2012). There’s nothing “fringe” about 46% (in fact, only 15% actually accept the scientific explanation for the origin of species; so one could argue, at least in terms of numbers and percentages, those of us who do accept the science as is are the “fringe”). For another thing, fanaticism reaches far beyond religion, and is not necessarily overtly aggressive. Nearly any social gathering around a common theme can have its fanatics. Some would argue that most politicians are fanatics. Patriotism itself is, in fact, a form of fanaticism. Fanaticism even shows up in a much more benign way: social groupings/gatherings around celebrities, musicians, bands, artists, movies, books, TV shows, and games (including sports); after all, the word “fan” is short for “fanatic”. Though even in this case it is not always benign, as the infamous rivalry between Yankees fans and Red Sox fans can show. So what causes fanaticism? As I said above, survival seems to mainly be a… I guess… “competitive sport”. One could suggest that even plants compete to survive. However, for the human species, we may no longer need to compete. Despite this, that drive will not have gone anywhere. We seem to still be driven to compete, as the drive for survival still seems to exist for us. I suggest that, if we really do not need to compete with each other and with non-human animals in order to survive, yet that drive still exists for us, then perhaps fanaticism may be at least partially explainable as a manifestation of that drive. It has to manifest somehow, and perhaps defending our beliefs and ideas, even in the face of contradictory evidence, is one way that competitive drive manifests itself. Which, sadly, means that fanaticism may be impossible to get rid of.
  • 11. NAH(Primate Aggression) 11 Bibliography (2007). Intergroup aggression in wild primates. Behaviour, 144(12), 1469-1653. Cords, M. (2007). Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding territories by blue monkeys in the kakamega forest, kenya. Behaviour, 144(12), 1537-1550. Crofoot, M. C. (2007). Mating and feeding competition in white-faced capuchins (cebus capucinus): the importance of shortand long-term strategies . Behaviour, 144(12), 1473- 1495. Harris, T. R. (2007). Testing mate, resource and infant defence functions of intergroup aggression in nonhuman primates: issues and methodology. Behaviour, 144(12), 1521- 1535. Honess, P. E., & Marin, C. M. (2006). Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. Behavioural and physiological aspects of stress and aggression in nonhuman primates, 30, 390-412. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.003 Kalin, N. H. (1999). Primate models to understand human aggression. The journal of clinical psychiatry, 60, 29-32. Kitchen, D. M., & Beehner, J. C. (2007). Factors affecting individual participation in group-level aggression among non-human primates. Behaviour, 144(12), 1551-1581. Manson, J. H., & Wrangham, R. W. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology, 32(4), 369-377. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2743814 Muller, M. M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2004). Animal behaviour. Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’, 67, 113-123. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.013 Newport, F. (2012, June 01). [Web log message]. Retrieved from http://www.gallup.com/poll/155003/Hold-Creationist-View-Human-Origins.aspx Robbins, M. M., & Sawyer, S. C. (2007). Intergroup encounters in mountain gorillas of Bwindi impenetrable national park, uganda. Behaviour, 144(12), 1497-1519. Wich, S. A., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Familiarity and threat of opponents determine variation in thomas langur (presbytis thomasi) male behaviour during between-group encounters. Behaviour, 144(12), 1583-1598. Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour, 144(12), 1599-1619.
  • 12. NAH(Primate Aggression) 12 Wrangham, R., Crofoot, M., Lundy, R., & Gilby, I. (2007). Use of overlap zones among group- living primates: a test of the risk hypothesis. Behaviour, 144(12), 1599-1619. Wrangham, R. W., & Glowacki, L. (2012). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and war in nomadic hunter-gatherers: Evaluating the chimpanzee model. Human Nature, 23(1), 05- 29. doi: 10.1007/s12110-012-9132-1