SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 493
Final Presentation 
Part 2
Sections 
• Studies (3) 
• Research Papers (209) 
• Other (395)
Studies
A Message From Your Brain: I'm Not 
Good At Remembering What I Hear 
"Our auditory memory isn't as robust as we might like to think it is," says Poremba. "We think that we are great 
at integrating all the senses," but the experiment shows that tactile and visual memory easily trumped auditory 
memory. 
The results further suggest that the brain processes tactile and visual memories through a similar mechanism, 
but that auditory memory is processed differently. This has potential implications for understanding the 
evolution of the human brain, says Bigelow, since the auditory memory of monkeys and chimpanzees also lags 
behind their tactile and visual memory.
Shared brain activity predicts audience preferences 
The researchers also used official television viewing figures and publicly available 
data from Facebook and Twitter to gauge how popular each clip was at the time 
it was first shown. As in earlier studies of shared brain activity, they found that 
some of the clips produced a greater degree of synchronised brain activity in the 
participants that others. 
Remarkably, though, the participants’ shared brain activity accurately predicted 
audience reactions to each at the time they were aired, with the most popular 
scenes and commercials producing the greatest degree of brain synchronisation 
in the participants. 
Thus, the extent of shared brain activity within the small group of participants 
was closely linked to the collective behaviour of a much larger group of people, 
suggesting that brain scanning technology could eventually be used to predict 
peoples’ reactions to a forthcoming film, their product preferences, or perhaps 
even how they might vote in an upcoming election 
New research published in the journal Nature Communications adding some hope to the neuromarketing hype, by 
showing that the brain activity shared by small groups of people in response to film clips can accurately predict how 
popular those clips will be among larger groups. 
Ten years ago, Uri Hasson and his colleagues recruited five participants and used functional magnetic resonance imaging 
(fMRI) to scan their brains while each one watched the same 30-minute clip of Sergio Leone’s classic spaghetti western, 
The Good, the Bad and the Ugly. They noticed that the film produced remarkably similar patterns of brain activity in all 
the participants, synchronising the activity across multiple regions, such that their brains “ticked collectively” while they 
viewed it. 
The researchers went on to show that films differ in their ability to induce this shared brain activity, with the more 
engaging ones producing a greater degree of synchrony, and more recently others have shown that the stereoscopic 
effects used in 3D films make viewing more enjoyable by creating a more immersive experience.
The Neuroscience Of Emoticons 
Shaded areas indicate the activity areas in (1) right fusiform gyrus (2) 
right inferior frontal gyrus (3) right middle frontal gyrus, and (4) right 
inferior parietal lobule. 
More recent work, led by Churches, conducted a similar test using a different scientific approach. With electrodes 
hooked to their scalp, 20 test participants looked at pictures of actual faces and Western-style emoticons--with :-) for a 
smile and :-( for a frown. They also looked at these same items in inverted form: an upside-down face, for instance, or 
an emoticon like (-: facing the wrong way. 
The reason for this setup requires some technical explanation. Rather than measuring brain images, Churches and 
company measured electrical brain responses known as event-related potentials, or ERPs. The brain produces distinct 
ERPs for various events. When it sees faces, for instance, a reliable negative ERP occurs 170 milliseconds later--known 
as the N170. When it sees inverted faces, the amplitude of that N170 gets larger 
In simple terms, the researchers wanted to know if emoticons, both normal and inverted, produce a similar N170 
effect. Sure doesn't seem like it. While normal emoticons did produce a large N170 signal, inverted emoticons 
produced a smaller one (below), the researchers report in a 2014 issue of Social Neuroscience. They suspect that 
flipped emoticons lose their cohesive symbolic meaning--becoming just a loose collection of punctuation.
Online trolls are narcissists, sadists and 
psychopaths, says study 
In two online studies (total N = 1215), respondents completed personality inventories and a survey of their 
Internet commenting styles. Overall, strong positive associations emerged among online commenting 
frequency, trolling enjoyment, and troll identity, pointing to a common construct underlying the measures. 
Both studies revealed similar patterns of relations between trolling and the Dark Tetrad of personality: 
trolling correlated positively with sadism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism, using both enjoyment 
ratings and identity scores. 
Of all personality measures, sadism showed the most robust associations with trolling and, importantly, 
the relationship was specific to trolling behavior. Enjoyment of other online activities, such as chatting and 
debating, was unrelated to sadism. Thus cyber-trolling appears to be an Internet manifestation of everyday 
sadism
Product Psychology Explains How To Get Users Hooked 
On Products Designed Around Habit And Context 
Last year, tens of thousands of hackers signed up to receive Hack 
Design, a newsletter teaching the principles of design so that those used 
to focusing on how things work behind the scenes could start at a point 
that also considers aesthetics and user-friendliness. 
Today, a group of product-focused creators — “Hooked” author Nir Eyal, 
Product Hunt‘s Ryan Hoover, and Greylock Partners‘s Josh Elman, 
among others — are hoping to follow up on that newsletter’s success 
with the launch of Product Psychology, a weekly course on the 
psychology of user behavior. 
Courses will arrive in the form of link-blog like posts delivered to your 
email inbox. Each week, one curator from the 17 psychologists, 
entrepreneurs, and designers involved in the project focuses on a single 
topic they’re especially versed in, with posts ranging from “How Scarcity 
& Impatience Drive User Behavior” to “Does Your Product Rely On 
Intuition Or Deliberation?” 
http://www.productpsychology.com/category/user-behavior/
How Multitasking Reshapes Your Brain 
Into A Constantly Distracted State 
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=PriSFBu5CLs 
People who frequently use several media devices at the 
same time have lower grey-matter density in one particular 
region of the brain compared to those who use just one 
device occasionally 
Media multitasking, or the concurrent consumption of multiple media forms, is increasingly prevalent in today’s society 
and has been associated with negative psychosocial and cognitive impacts. Individuals who engage in heavier media-multitasking 
are found to perform worse on cognitive control tasks and exhibit more socio-emotional difficulties. However, 
the neural processes associated with media multi-tasking remain unexplored. 
The present study investigated relationships between media multitasking activity and brain structure. Research has 
demonstrated that brain structure can be altered upon prolonged exposure to novel environments and experience. Thus, 
we expected differential engagements in media multitasking to correlate with brain structure variability. 
This was confirmed via Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) analyses: Individuals with higher Media Multitasking Index (MMI) 
scores had smaller gray matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Functional connectivity between this ACC 
region and the precuneus was negatively associated with MMI. Our findings suggest a possible structural correlate for the 
observed decreased cognitive control performance and socio-emotional regulation in heavy media-multitaskers. 
While the cross-sectional nature of our study does not allow us to specify the direction of causality, our results brought to 
light novel associations between individual media multitasking behaviors and ACC structure differences.
The Psychology of Your Future Self and How Your 
Present Illusions Hinder Your Future Happiness 
Harvard psychologist Daniel Gilbert 
http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_you_are_always_changing 
http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_asks_why_are_we_happy 
http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_researches_happiness 
“Human beings are works in progress that mistakenly think they’re finished. The person you are right now is as transient, as fleeting and as 
temporary as all the people you’re ever been. The one constant in our lives is change.” 
The things we do when we expect our lives to continue are naturally and properly different than the things we might do if we expected them to 
end abruptly. We go easy on the lard and tobacco, smile dutifully at yet another of our supervisor’s witless jokes, read books like this one when 
we could be wearing paper hats and eating pistachio macaroons in the bathtub, and we do each of these things in the charitable service of the 
people we will soon become. We treat our future selves as though they were our children, spending most of the hours of most of our days 
constructing tomorrows that we hope will make them happy. Rather than indulging in whatever strikes our momentary fancy, we take 
responsibility for the welfare of our future selves, squirreling away portions of our paychecks each month so they can enjoy their retirements on 
a putting green, jogging and flossing with some regularity so they can avoid coronaries and gum grafts, enduring dirty diapers and mind-numbing 
repetitions of The Cat in the Hat so that someday they will have fat-cheeked grandchildren to bounce on their laps. Even plunking down a dollar 
at the convenience store is an act of charity intended to ensure that the person we are about to become will enjoy the Twinkie we are paying for 
now. In fact, just about any time we want something — a promotion, a marriage, an automobile, a cheeseburger — we are expecting that if we 
get it, then the person who has our fingerprints a second, minute, day, or decade from now will enjoy the world they inherit from us, honoring 
our sacrifices as they reap the harvest of our shrewd investment decisions and dietary forbearance. 
[But] our temporal progeny are often thankless. We toil and sweat to give them just what we think they will like, and they quit their jobs, grow 
their hair, move to or from San Francisco, and wonder how we could ever have been stupid enough to think they’d like that. We fail to achieve 
the accolades and rewards that we consider crucial to their well-being, and they end up thanking God that things didn’t work out according to 
our shortsighted, misguided plan. Even that person who takes a bite of the Twinkie we purchased a few minutes earlier may make a sour face 
and accuse us of having bought the wrong snack.
Curiosity improves memory by tapping 
into the brain’s reward system 
Ranganath said the findings are in line with 
theories that give dopamine a key role in 
stabilising or consolidating memories. The 
research is published in the journal, Neuron. 
Brain scans of college students have shed light on why people learn more effectively when their curiosity is piqued than 
when they are bored stiff. 
Researchers in the US found evidence that curiosity ramped up the activity of a brain chemical called dopamine, which in 
turn seemed to strengthen people’s memories. 
Students who took part in the study were better at remembering answers to trivia questions when they were curious, but 
their memories also improved for unrelated information they were shown at the same time. 
The findings suggest that while grades may have their place in motivating students, stimulating their natural curiosity 
could help them even more. 
“There are times when people feel they can take in a lot of new information, and other times when they feel their 
memories are terrible,” said Ranganath. “This work suggests that once you light that fire of curiosity, you put the brain in 
a state that’s more conducive to learning. Once you get this ramp-up of dopamine, the brain becomes more like a sponge 
that’s ready to soak up whatever is happening.”
Your Decision-Making Processes Are a 
Lot More Random Than You Realize 
For the most part, we make decisions 
based on our prior experiences. But what 
about those situations that are completely 
new or unpredictable? 
A new study suggests that when facing an 
excessively uncertain or challenging 
scenario, the brain chooses randomness as 
the best strategy. 
When it comes to making decisions, our brains are extremely dependent on past experiences. In fact, some cognitive 
scientists speculate that the brain has a built-in mechanism for assessing (or weighing) the efficacy of a decision based on 
precedents from the past. It's also something we can be conscious of; to improve our rational decision making, it's 
important that we use new information to change our confidence in a belief. But as a recent experiment conducted by 
Alla Karpova of the Janelia Farm Research Campus shows, randomness may be the brain's preferred policy when things 
get particularly challenging or when the situation is completely without precedent. 
Karpova says the mechanism behind this change is found in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a brain structure involved 
in using experience to make decisions. And in fact, when the researchers manipulated the activity of neurons that 
released a stress hormone into the ACC, they were able to reverse the rats' behavioral strategies. When these neurons 
were stimulated, the rats abandoned the experience-based strategy and started to behave randomly in situations when 
this wasn't expected. Fascinatingly, inhibition of the stress hormone, norepinephrine, caused the rats to rely on their 
experiences — even when faced with the challenging competitor.
Study finds that women who are 
ovulating are more into kissing. 
Hormonal changes associated with the human menstrual cycle have been previously found to 
affect female mate preference, whereby women in the late follicular phase of their cycle (i.e., 
at higher risk of conception) prefer males displaying putative signals of underlying genetic 
fitness. Past research also suggests that romantic kissing is utilized in human mating contexts 
to assess potential mating partners. 
The current study examined whether women in their late follicular cycle phase place greater 
value on kissing at times when it might help serve mate assessment functions. Using an 
international online questionnaire, results showed that women in the follicular phase of their 
menstrual cycle felt that kissing was more important at initial stages of a relationship than 
women in the luteal phase of their cycle. Furthermore, it was found that estimated 
progesterone levels were a significant negative predictor for these ratings.
Researchers “Copy and Paste” Fear 
From One Memory to Another 
Now, researchers studying mice have discovered that memories 
can be broken down into component parts — the emotional part 
separated from the factual part — and that the emotions 
associated with a memory can be transferred to a totally different 
memory. 
Redondo and his team expect that this growing body of research 
may help us understand how unpleasant memories are formed in 
the first place — and eventually, maybe even aid in the 
development of clinical tools for making unpleasant memories 
less painful. 
MIT neuroscientist Roger Redondo and his team started by gathering a sample group of genetically engineered mice. The 
mice’s brains were engineered so that, if an antibiotic known as doxycycline was removed from their diet, certain neurons in 
their brains would express a protein called channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) which triggers neural activity in response to blue 
light. 
This system enabled the team to use doxycycline as a sort of “record/pause” button for neural activity in the animals’ 
brains. The investigators could remove the antibiotic from the mice’s diet, give the animals certain kinds of conditioning, 
then start giving them doxycycline again; thus creating light-sensitive groups of neurons that had activated during specific 
time periods and tasks 
This research makes it pretty clear that memories aren’t all-or-nothing recollections. They can be broken down into 
component pieces like locations and emotions; and each of those pieces can be reactivated independently, and even 
become attached to completely different memory pieces than those with which it originally formed.
This Psych Discovery Explains Why You 
Get Your Best Ideas In The Shower 
When faced with a difficult decision, it is often suggested to "sleep on it" or take a break from thinking about the decision 
in order to gain clarity. 
But new brain imaging research from Carnegie Mellon University, published in the journal "Social Cognitive and Affective 
Neuroscience," finds that the brain regions responsible for making decisions continue to be active even when the 
conscious brain is distracted with a different task. The research provides some of the first evidence showing how the 
brain unconsciously processes decision information in ways that lead to improved decision-making. 
"This research begins to chip away at the mystery of our unconscious brains and decision-making," said J. David Creswell, 
assistant professor of psychology in CMU's Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences and director of the Health 
and Human Performance Laboratory. "It shows that brain regions important for decision-making remain active even while 
our brains may be simultaneously engaged in unrelated tasks, such as thinking about a math problem. What’s most 
intriguing about this finding is that participants did not have any awareness that their brains were still working on the 
decision problem while they were engaged in an unrelated task."
Human brain subliminally judges 
'trustworthiness' of faces 
Finding from brain scans adds to evidence that we make spontaneous, largely 
unconscious judgments of strangers 
The study focused on the activity of the amygdala, a small almond-shaped 
region deep inside the brain. The amygdala is intimately involved with 
processing strong emotions, such as fear. Its central nucleus sends out the 
signals responsible for the famous and evolutionarily crucial "fight-or-flight" 
response. 
Even so, the brain scans revealed that the amygdala responded differently 
to subliminal images of trustworthy and untrustworthy faces. Some 
regions of the amygdala reacted to how untrustworthy a face was, while 
other regions seemed to register the overall strength of the judgment, 
becoming more active only when a face was either very trustworthy or 
very untrustworthy. 
The human brain can judge the apparent trustworthiness of a face from a glimpse so fleeting, the person has no idea they 
have seen it, scientists claim. 
Researchers in the US found that brain activity changed in response to how trustworthy a face appeared to be when the 
face in question had not been consciously perceived. 
Scientists made the surprise discovery during a series of experiments that were designed to shed light on the neural 
processes that underpin the snap judgments people make about others. 
The findings suggest that parts of our brains are doing more complex subconscious processing of the outside world than 
many researchers thought.
Psychologists Find that Nice People 
Are More Likely to Hurt You 
People who are agreeable are also more likely to make destructive choices, if they think doing so will help them 
conform to social expectations. That's the finding of psychologists, who suggest that disagreeable, ornery people may 
be more helpful than we think. 
Researchers recently conducted a version of Stanley Milgram's famous obedience experiments, where people were 
asked by doctors to "shock" others until they died. Only later did they discover they people they'd "killed" were just 
actors. A surprising number of otherwise kindly people "killed" others, just because they'd been given orders. 
People with more agreeable, conscientious personalities are more likely to make harmful choices. In these new 
obedience experiments, people with more social graces were the ones who complied with the experimenter's wishes 
and delivered electric shocks they believed could harm an innocent person. By contrast, people with more contrarian, 
less agreeable personalities were more likely to refuse to hurt other people when told to do so.
Proxemics Is the Science of Why You Shouldn't 
Stand So Close to Me 
Proxemics is the study of how people organize their social space. Without 
knowing it, we have very strict rules for who goes where in our culture. 
Although we don't always think about them, we know when someone's 
violating them. In the 1960s, a scientist called Edward Hall studied how 
close we let strangers come to us (no closer than twelve feet), social 
partners come to us (no closer than four feet), and how close we let close 
personal friends and relatives come to us (no closer than 1.5 feet). 
Get any closer than that and you're in intimate space - where you should 
either be kissing us at the end of a wonderful date or quietly advising us 
that we can plead the fifth at a congressional hearing. 
Hall noted, however, that these rules only applied to the proxemics of North 
Americans, and that not all cultures shared the same established distances 
when it came to personal space. 
This seating pattern is not declared. It's probably almost 
unconscious for most of the people doing the sitting. It's 
also so entirely ingrained in the culture. 
I can't even imagine what the reaction would be if there 
were one person sitting on a bus - and the second passenger 
to board the bus chose to sit down right next to them. I'd try 
it, but I worry that the first person would either clobber me 
or run.
Look Angry If You Want People to Give In 
“Until we see someone, we don’t know what makes them 
sweat, or what makes them angry or happy,” Reed told 
Today. “ 
You can do a lot of things over the Internet now, but people 
still choose to have face-to-face meetings.” 
One caveat: It won’t work if people can tell you’re faking. If 
you’ve got a serious negotiation coming up, dredge up some 
real emotion or take an acting class. 
In two experiments, people were more willing to give into demands for a larger share of $1 when the person asking for 
more money looked angry. 
“If you come in with a scowl on your face, they’re going to take your threat more seriously,” study co-author Lawrence Ian 
Reed, a researcher at Harvard University, told Today. “You might think a poker face would be better in a negotiation. But 
in a bargaining situation when you make threats, your facial expression could add credibility to what you are saying.” 
During one of the experiments, 870 participants were given the task of splitting up $1 with someone else. If they couldn’t 
agree, neither would get anything. When an actress demanded 70 cents on the dollar with an angry expression, more 
people agreed than when she used a neutral expression. 
So far, the researchers have only done the experiment with women making the demands; reactions to a threatening male 
could be different. And, threatening looks only apply in face-to-face interactions, obviously.
Blacks Seen as Darker During Tough 
Economic Times 
When the economy declines, racial minorities are hit the hardest. Although existing explanations for this effect focus on 
institutional causes, recent psychological findings suggest that scarcity may also alter perceptions of race in ways that 
exacerbate discrimination. 
We tested the hypothesis that economic resource scarcity causes decision makers to perceive African Americans as 
“Blacker” and that this visual distortion elicits disparities in the allocation of resources. Studies 1 and 2 demonstrated 
that scarcity altered perceptions of race, lowering subjects’ psychophysical threshold for seeing a mixed-race face as 
“Black” as opposed to “White.” In studies 3 and 4, scarcity led subjects to visualize African American faces as darker and 
more “stereotypically Black,” compared with a control condition. 
When presented to naïve subjects, face representations produced under scarcity elicited smaller allocations than control-condition 
representations. Together, these findings introduce a novel perceptual account for the proliferation of racial 
disparities under economic scarcity.
Does Breaking Rules Make You Cool? 
Do consumers think it’s cool when a brands 
breaks a rule? 
It all depends on the rule, researchers report in 
the Journal of Consumer Research. 
“We reasoned that brands could become cool by 
breaking rules that seemed unnecessary or 
unfair, but not by breaking legitimate rules,” the 
authors wrote. 
Studies backed up their hypothesis: In one 
experiment, researchers asked participants to 
gauge their reaction to an ad that either 
advocated breaking or following a dress code. 
When the ad was accompanied by text that gave a 
legitimate reason for the dress code (honoring veterans), 
the brand did not seem cooler. But when participants read 
that the same dress code existed for an illegitimate reason 
(honoring a corrupt dictator), breaking the rule did help the 
brand seem cooler. 
There’s one caveat, however: it’s nearly impossible to 
appear cool to everyone. While one group may tolerate 
breaking traditional dress code rules, for example, it might 
be seen as deviant by another group. 
The researchers defined coolness by autonomy. Brands may 
do well in upping their coolness quotient by highlighting 
their uniqueness, the researchers suggested. 
“Collectively, our studies find that coolness is a subjective, 
positive trait perceived in people, brands, products, and 
trends that are autonomous in an appropriate way,” the 
authors wrote.
The Science of Misheard Song Lyrics 
There is an actual official term for when you 
hear "excuse me while I kiss the sky" in Jimi 
Hendrix's "Purple Haze" as "excuse me while 
I kiss this guy." Your meaningful misheard 
lyrics are called "mondegreens," and their 
study can have real psychological 
significance. 
These little misunderstandings are common, but most people don't 
know that there is an official title for them. It came from a popular 
essay by writer Sylvia Wright 
The lyrics that defy cliché and break new ground are most likely to get 
misunderstood. "Excuse me, while I kiss this guy" might have still 
been outré in the 1960s when "Purple Haze" was written, but it was 
still more familiar than kissing the sky. 
We cobble together a semi-plausible lyric because we lack the 
experience to understand the real one. 
The people who are most likely to do this are the ones most lacking in 
experience. 
Children group words together, the way they hear them, in a stream of 
continuous syllables. They assume the meaning of "donzerly" will come 
later, when they hear a few more examples of the word. 
We enunciate for small babies, but as children grow, they are expected to 
pick up individual words, many of which they've never been exposed to, 
in a stream of noise. 
Language learners also have difficulty distinguishing one word from 
another, which can run them into real trouble in business or medical 
settings. 
Many researchers have found that mondegreens tend not to travel alone. Once people lose understanding of a sentence, 
they lose context as well, causing them to "hear" words that only resemble the actual words being uttered. People, 
especially adult English learners, are desperately trying to regain the thread of meaning, and make order out of a chaos of 
sounds. Eventually they trick themselves into hearing something that the recognize, even if it doesn't make sense. What 
most people need, scientists find, is familiar points where they can get their bearings, and enter back into the thread of the 
conversation. If they can't get regular familiar points to orient themselves in a stream of sound, mondegreens will take over 
and give them fake points of familiarity.
The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to 
Consume, Consume, Consume 
Diderot Gets a Bathrobe 
Denis Diderot was a philosopher during the Enlightenment, which meant he should have been above such petty 
things as consumerism. On the other hand, he was also an art critic, and so liked beauty. That might have led to the 
situation he described in his essay, "Regrets on Parting With My Old Dressing Gown.“ 
He had an old beat-up thing that he wore around his small apartment. One day friend gave him a present of a 
beautiful scarlet dressing gown. Diderot loved it, but felt it was out of place among his cheap old furniture. He 
replaced his straw chair with a leather armchair. He replaced his desk, and the prints on his walls. Then he started 
replacing his regular clothes. The tale ends with Diderot in debt and disconsolate, working to maintain his beautiful 
room. He used to be the "master" of his possessions, but now he is the "slave" of a dressing gown. 
The essay is fictional, and the sentiment is pure romanticism. Don't consider your physical surroundings! 
Advance your mind and let go of the physical. We understand Diderot's ideals, but we also understand his 
experiences. Who among us hasn't come home with a beautiful new possession, placed it in their home, looked 
around, and thought, "This place is a dump."
The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to 
Consume, Consume, Consume 
Just noticing the grubbiness of your regular possessions, when put next to something extraordinarily nice, isn't enough for 
you to fall under the spell of the Diderot Effect. To go full Diderot, according to sociologists, we need to start identifying with 
our possessions. Unfortunately, we tend to do that already. Most of us have picked up a piece of clothing, or sat on a piece 
of furniture, and thought, "No, this isn't me." How could that be? It's just a functional object. 
But consumption doesn't work that way. Even the most dressed-down of us uses our clothes to convey a picture of who we 
are. We identify ourselves using our possessions. When we do that, we don't just want high quality. In fact, many people 
will reject high quality. We want unity. We want to present a coherent whole. This can be a big problem for any companies 
that want us to buy things. No matter how good a product they offer, if it falls outside of the consumer's ideas of the unity 
of their lives and their looks, they will resist purchasing it. On the other hand, once we own one thing that stands out, that 
doesn't fit our current sense of unity, we go on a rampage trying to reconstruct ourselves. Either we throw away the luxury 
item, or we start to upgrade ourselves. Few people throw away the luxury item. Most people start replacing the other 
things in their lives. This involves a lot of money
The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to 
Consume, Consume, Consume 
How to Use the Diderot Effect 
Naturally, there's a lot of focus on how to get people to take that first step out of their identity. Any good that's 
considered outside the pattern of someone's regular purchases is called a "departure good." Companies market hard to 
get people to make that first move, but they also want to know how to keep people Dideroting. 
How does one sample purchase, one luxury, become a whole lifestyle? According to research journals, the key is to make 
the item not a purchase, but a replacement. You're not buying a soap dispenser, you're replacing your nasty old 
bathroom soap with a nice-looking, efficient soap dispenser. Are you aware that it can come as part of a set? And that 
that matches a shower curtain? You're not buying a new pair of shoes, you're replacing an old pair with something 
simple and timeless - that you can't wear with jeans so get some nice skirts or slacks. Much of what we buy today is a 
kind of a taster product for a complete lifestyle. People aren't supposed to be springing for one big luxury purchase, they 
are meant to be buying better selves. That takes repeat business.
Want to be happier? Skip the small talk. 
We all know those people. Maybe you’re one of them. The person who manages, despite 
all the stress and work it takes to get through a normal day, to always be happy. How do 
they do it? What is their secret? 
Although it’s likely that genetics play a major role in determining one’s happiness, it’s also 
thought that behavior can help (or hinder) you on your path to bliss. These scientists asked 
whether the content of peoples’ conversation is related to their overall happiness. 
They recorded participants’ conversations during daily life, and sure enough, they found 
that people who had deeper conversations tended to be happier than those who spent 
time on small talk.
Scientists finally explain why your 
grandma will never find “Borat” funny 
Identifying social gaffes is important for maintaining relationships. Older adults are less able than young to discriminate 
between socially appropriate and inappropriate behavior in video clips. One open question is how these social 
appropriateness ratings relate to potential age differences in the perception of what is actually funny or not. 
In the present study, young, middle-aged, and older adults were equally able to discriminate between appropriate and 
inappropriate social behavior in a diverse set of clips relevant across age groups. However, young and middle-aged adults 
rated the gaffe clips as funnier than control clips and young adults smiled more during the inappropriate clips than the 
control clips. 
Older adults did not show this pattern, suggesting that they did not find the inappropriate clips funny. Additionally, young 
adults endorsed a more aggressive humor style than middle-aged and older adults and aggressive humor style 
endorsement mediated age differences in social appropriateness ratings. Results are discussed in terms of possible 
mechanisms such as cohort differences in humor and developmental prioritization of certain humor styles, as well as the 
importance of investigating age differences in both abilities and preferences
Coffee Drinkers Have Trouble Talking 
About Emotions? 
Alexithymia refers to difficulties with identifying, describing, and regulating one's own emotions. This trait dimension has 
been linked to risky or harmful use of alcohol and illicit drugs; however, the most widely used psychoactive drug in the 
world, caffeine, has not been examined previously in relation to alexithymia. 
The present study assessed 106 male and female university students aged 18-30 years on their caffeine use in relation to 
several traits, including alexithymia. The 18 participants defined as alexithymic based on their Toronto Alexithymia Scale 
(TAS-20) scores reported consuming nearly twice as much caffeine per day as did non-alexithymic or borderline alexithymic 
participants. 
They also scored significantly higher than controls on indices of frontal lobe dysfunction as well as anxiety symptoms and 
sensitivity to punishment. In a hierarchical linear regression model, sensitivity to punishment negatively predicted daily 
caffeine intake, suggesting caffeine avoidance by trait-anxious individuals. Surprisingly, however, TAS-20 alexithymia scores 
positively predicted caffeine consumption. 
Possible reasons for the positive relationship between caffeine use and alexithymia are discussed, concluding that this 
outcome is tentatively consistent with the hypo-arousal model of alexithymia.
Could Diet Sodas Be Making Us Fatter? 
The artificial sweeteners in “diet” beverages, thought to 
help people trim their waistlines, may be having the 
opposite effect. 
A new study reveals that three of the leading artificial 
sweeteners produce an increase in blood-sugar levels in 
both mice and humans, by disrupting the balance of helpful 
gut bacteria. High blood-sugar levels, in turn, are the telltale 
sign of glucose intolerance, a condition which can evolve 
into diabetes and metabolic disease. 
To be clear, researchers aren’t certain how artificial sweeteners and gut microbes interact to 
produce glucose intolerance. 
Furthermore, not everyone in the small human trial experienced the same reaction. 
Many things influence our metabolism, and some people may be more sensitive than 
others to artificial sweeteners, as Forbes reports.
Shut up and pet me! Dogs prefer 
petting to vocal praise 
It’s probably no surprise that dogs like to be petted. But do they prefer petting over other 
types of attention? Here, two scientists from the University of Florida tested whether dogs 
would prefer to be petted or given vocal praise, and whether it mattered if the petting/praise 
came from an owner or a stranger. 
Turns out that dogs love pets, regardless of who is doing the petting, and they never seem to 
get tired of being petted. Interestingly, a previous study by the same authors found that dogs 
do like one thing even more than petting: food. Perhaps a future study will determine where 
chasing their tails ranks on the list?
Ignore IQ Tests: Your Level of 
Intelligence Is Not Fixed for Life 
Those who hang dearly onto the notion that IQ is fixed for life have managed to ignore decades of published research in the 
field of applied behavior analysis. This has reported very large IQ gains in children with autism who have been exposed to 
early intensive behavioral interventions once they have been diagnosed with learning difficulties. 
Another 2009 Norwegian study examined the effects of an increase in the duration of compulsory schooling in Norway in 
the 1960s which lengthened the time in education for Norwegians by two years. The researchers used records of cognitive 
ability taken by the military to calculate the IQ of each individual in the study. They found that IQ had increased by 3.7 
points for every extra year of education received. 
More recent studies by John Jonides and his colleagues at the University of Michigan reported improvements in objective 
measures of intelligence for those who practiced a brain-training task called the “n-back task” – a kind of computerized 
memory test. 
My own research, in the field of relational frame theory, has shown that understanding relations between words, such as 
“more than,” “less than” or “opposite” is crucial for our intellectual development. One recent pilot study showed that we 
can considerably raise standard IQ scores by training children in relational language skills tasks over a period of months. 
Again, this finding challenges the idea that intelligence is fixed for life. 
even exceed them.
Research Shows that Seven Hours of 
Sleep Might Be the Sweet Spot 
The Wall Street Journal looked at a few different studies. Here's a summation of just a few of them: 
"The lowest mortality and morbidity is with seven hours," said Shawn Youngstedt, a professor in the College of Nursing and 
Health Innovation at Arizona State University Phoenix. "Eight hours or more has consistently been shown to be hazardous," 
says Dr. Youngstedt, who researches the effects of oversleeping... 
...Daniel F. Kripke, an emeritus professor of psychiatry at the University of California San Diego, tracked over a six-year 
period data on 1.1 million people who participated in a large cancer study. People who reported they slept 6.5 to 7.4 hours 
had a lower mortality rate than those with shorter or longer sleep. The study, published in the Archives of General 
Psychiatry in 2002, controlled for 32 health factors, including medications... 
...A study in the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience last year used data from users of the cognitive-training website 
Lumosity. Researchers looked at the self-reported sleeping habits of about 160,000 users who took spatial-memory and 
matching tests and about 127,000 users who took an arithmetic test. They found that cognitive performance increased as 
people got more sleep, reaching a peak at seven hours before starting to decline.
Scientists use MRI to measure precisely 
how your butt deforms when you sit down. 
RESULTS: MRIs indicated a marked decrease in muscle thickness under the ischial tuberosity during 
loaded sitting. This change in thickness resulted from a combination of muscle displacement and 
distortion. The gluteus and hamstrings overlapped beneath the pelvis in an unloaded condition, 
enveloping the ischial tuberosity. But the overlap was removed under load. 
The hamstrings moved anteriorly, while the gluteus moved posterior-laterally. Under load, neither muscle 
was directly beneath the apex of the ischial tuberosity. Furthermore, there was a change in muscle shape, 
particularly posterior to the peak of the ischial tuberosity. 
CONCLUSION: The complex deformation of buttocks tissue seen in this case study may help explain the 
inconsistent results reported in finite element models. 3D imaging of the seated buttocks provides a 
unique opportunity to study the actual buttocks response to sitting.”
The Science Behind Why You Never 
Really Leave High School 
You also store your most vivid memories during this time (from ages 15 to 25), which can 
make traumatic experiences all the more traumatizing. 
In other words, you are already poised biologically to be deeply impressed by experiences 
around you at this time while, simultaneously, you form your first sense of identity, and then 
you’re thrown into a hard, judgmental place. Oof. 
The insecurities we form in high school are often the ones we carry with us later, even when 
those no longer have any basis in reality (if they ever did at all — further in the article they 
discuss just how skewed teenagers perception of other’s feelings towards them are).
Scientists figured out how shrooms open your mind 
The study of rapid changes in brain dynamics and functional connectivity (FC) is of increasing interest in neuroimaging. Brain 
states departing from normal waking consciousness are expected to be accompanied by alterations in the aforementioned 
dynamics. In particular, the psychedelic experience produced by psilocybin (a substance found in “magic mushrooms”) is 
characterized by unconstrained cognition and profound alterations in the perception of time, space and selfhood. 
Considering the spontaneous and subjective manifestation of these effects, we hypothesize that neural correlates of the 
psychedelic experience can be found in the dynamics and variability of spontaneous brain activity fluctuations and 
connectivity, measurable with functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). Fifteen healthy subjects were scanned 
before, during and after intravenous infusion of psilocybin and an inert placebo. 
Blood-Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) temporal variability was assessed computing the variance and total spectral power, 
resulting in increased signal variability bilaterally in the hippocampi and anterior cingulate cortex. Changes in BOLD signal 
spectral behavior (including spectral scaling exponents) affected exclusively higher brain systems such as the default mode, 
executive control, and dorsal attention networks. A novel framework enabled us to track different connectivity states 
explored by the brain during rest. This approach revealed a wider repertoire of connectivity states post-psilocybin than 
during control conditions. Together, the present results provide a comprehensive account of the effects of psilocybin on 
dynamical behavior in the human brain at a macroscopic level and may have implications for our understanding of the 
unconstrained, hyper-associative quality of consciousness in the psychedelic state
Want to improve your cognitive function? 
Try some whole body vibration! 
Have you ever seen those old movies of people using body vibration machines for “exercise” 
(see photo)? Well, apparently the principle behind those machines isn’t total BS. 
Although whole body vibration (WBV) might not make you lose weight or get in better shape, 
it can actually provide some of the same brain-stimulating benefits as exercise. 
More specifically, as shown by this study, WBV can improve one’s attention and inhibition (the 
ability to tune out irrelevant stimuli). 
The authors hypothesize that WBV improves these aspects of cognitive function by producing 
similar physiological responses as exercise, including increased oxygen uptake and heart rate. 
I’ll shake on that!
The price of love? Losing two of your closest friends 
Research reveals that, on average, having a new romantic 
partner pushes out two close friends from your inner circle 
In a separate study, Dunbar's team looked at how men 
and women maintained friendships on the social 
networking website Facebook. They found that women's 
Facebook friends were more often friends from everyday 
life that they spent time with, while men tended to collect 
as many friends as they could, even if they hardly knew 
them. 
"Boys seem to be in a competition to see who can have 
the most Faccebook friends and that could be a form of 
mate advertisting. One of the cues women use for male 
quality as a mate is the number of other girls chasing 
them, so signing up lots of girls as Facebook friends seems 
to be a good idea," said Dunbar. 
Falling in love comes at the cost of losing close friends, because romantic partners absorb time that would otherwise be 
invested in platonic relationships, researchers say. 
A new partner pushes out two close friends on average, leaving lovers with a smaller inner circle of people they can turn to 
in times of crisis, a study found. 
Previous research by Dunbar's group has shown that people typically have five very close relationships – that is, people 
whom they would turn to if they were in emotional or financial trouble. 
"If you go into a romantic relationship, it costs you two friends. Those who have romantic relationships, instead of having 
the typical five 'core set' of relationships only have four. And of those, one is the new person who's come into their life," 
said Dunbar.
How Come We're So Bad at Buying 
Stuff? 
http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/how-come-were-so-bad-at-buying-stuff-video- 
140619.htm 
We've all enjoyed a little retail therapy -- or thought we did! -- but why does 
buyer's remorse so often set in? And why do we seem to lose all control over 
logic when that next big, shiny purchase calls our name?
What Makes Cursive So Good For Your 
Brain - Video 
http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/what-makes-cursive- 
so-good-for-your-brain-video-140612.htm
The early bird catches the worm… or 
at least the boss’s good opinion 
In this research, we draw from the stereotyping literature to suggest that supervisor ratings of job performance are 
affected by employees' start times-the time of day they first arrive at work. 
Even when accounting for total work hours, objective job performance, and employees' self-ratings of conscientiousness, 
we find that a later start time leads supervisors to perceive employees as less conscientious. 
These perceptions in turn cause supervisors to rate employees as lower performers. In addition, we show that supervisor 
chronotype acts as a boundary condition of the mediated model. Supervisors who prefer eveningness (i.e., owls) are less 
likely to hold negative stereotypes of employees with late start times than supervisors who prefer morningness (i.e., larks). 
Taken together, our results suggest that supervisor ratings of job performance are susceptible to stereotypic beliefs based 
on employees' start times.
Is Laughter the Best Medicine? 
We all want to believe that laughter is good for what ails us, but the science backing that up is thin. Most studies have 
been small and have relied on self-reported assessments. 
"A good belly laugh leads to the release of endorphins from the brain," says Michael Miller, director of the Center for 
Preventive Cardiology at the University of Maryland Medical Center in Baltimore. 
That release sets off a cascade of heart-healthy biological events. Endorphins, pleasure-inducing neurotransmitters, 
activate receptors on the surface of the endothelium, the layer of flat cells lining blood vessels. That leads to the release 
of nitric oxide, which widens blood vessels—increasing blood flow, lessening inflammation, inhibiting platelet clumping, 
and reducing the formation of cholesterol plaque. 
A 2005 study by Miller measured the blood flow of 20 volunteers before and after watching a funny movie and a sad 
movie. After the sad movie, blood flow was more restricted in 14 of the 20 viewers. But after the movie that made them 
laugh, average blood flow increased by 22 percent. 
"The best laugh is one that brings tears to our eyes," says Miller, author of Heal Your Heart: The Positive Emotions 
Prescription to Prevent and Reverse Heart Disease, scheduled for publication by Rodale Press in September. His 
prescription: at least 30 minutes of exercise at least three times a week—and 15 minutes of daily laughter. 
But her study of 33 healthy women, published in 2003, showed that those who laughed at a humorous movie had higher 
levels of natural killer cell activity, which increased their ability to fight off disease. However, the effect was seen only in 
the subjects who laughed out loud, not in those who quietly watched the comedy. 
A study in Japan that also used laboratory findings found that laughter could improve anti-inflammatory factors in the 
blood of people who have rheumatoid arthritis.
Why Molly Is Especially Deadly at Summer Music Festivals 
Eugene A. Kiyatkin, the study’s lead author and a scientist at the behavioral neuroscience branch of NIDA, zeroed in on the 
physiological mechanisms involved in fatal hyperthermia (overheating of the body to the point of death). “What’s new 
about this study is that it focuses on MDMA’s effects on brain temperature and clarifies the physiological mechanisms 
involved,” he tells The Daily Beast. “By recording data from the brain, muscle, and skin, we were able to show that MDMA 
raises heat in the brain by increasing brain activity as well as causing a strong, lasting constriction of peripheral blood 
vessels that prevents the normal dissipation of excess body heat to the external environment. These combined effects may 
be enhanced in conditions when brain and body heat production are naturally increased—such as social interaction—or 
when shedding excess heat is difficult, as it is in a warm environment.” 
When the body undergoes fatal hyperthermia—internal temperature rising to the point of brain damage—it’s fluid in the 
brain that leads the march to death. After MDMA raises heat production and constricts blood vessels, the internal body 
temperature increases, damaging brain cells irreparably. While this increase in brain temperature is incredibly destructive, 
it’s not the actual killer. 
“The main mechanism leading to death from psychostimulant drugs appears to be leaking of the brain-blood barrier— 
which keeps most chemicals in the blood out of the brain—and water accumulation in the brain,” says Kiyatakin. It’s for this 
reason that concertgoers thinking hydration is the key to staying safe (“if you just drink enough water you’ll be fine”) is so 
dangerous. “The excessive use of liquids often used by people at dance clubs could be problematic if they take MDMA,” 
says Kiyatakin. “In addition to its other effects, it inhibits both sweating and urination, which expel water from the body. 
These factors could also contribute to dangerous water accumulation in the brain.”
We May Finally Know Why Sleep Improves Memory 
The results were stark: Gan and his colleagues found that the sleep-deprived mice sprouted significantly fewer dendritic 
spines than those that were permitted to rest, and the rate of spine formation was correlated with the degree of task 
improvement. Growth was shown to be most dramatic during the slow-wave, non-REM stage of sleep. What's more, the 
benefits of sleep seem to carry on well after the mice woke up, with roughly 5% of new spines in the motor cortex 
developing in the 24 hours after the mice awoke. The mice that slept were also more likely to retain the spines they grew. In 
some circumstances, it seems sleep could in fact lead to the growth of new synapses. 
The researchers went on to demonstrate that the neuronal branches involved in the rod-balancing task were reactivated 
during this period of slow-wave sleep. Neuroal reactivation during sleep has been observed in the past, but Gan's team took 
it a step further by blocking the reactivation. When they did, it impeded the formation of new spines, suggesting that 
reactivation plays some role in stabilizing the dendritic spines sprouted during sleep. 
Finally, Gan tells io9 that one of the study's most surprising findings was not directly related to sleep. In a previous study, 
Gan and his colleagues used the skull-window technique to demonstrate that teaching mice to balance atop the rod led to 
the formation of new spines along dendrites in the motor cortex. The present study corroborates those findings, but it also 
shows that teaching the mice a new motor task (for instance, balancing on the rod as it spins in the reverse direction) 
caused dendritic spines to sprout on an entirely new dendritic branch – i.e., a branch distinct from the one that shot out 
spines in reaction to learning to balance on a forward-spinning rod. In other words: learning a new motor skill won't cause 
dendritic spines to appear just anywhere. Rather, the team's findings suggest that synaptic change in the mammalian brain 
occurs in a site-specific fashion.
Cocaine addiction linked to brain abnormalities 
Scans showed cocaine users 
had enlarged grey matter in 
areas of the brain associated 
with processing reward. 
Specifically, the amount of grey matter in the orbitofrontal cortex was reduced in people with cocaine addiction, an area 
involved in decision-making and goal-directed behaviour. 
Other affected areas included the insula, an area of the brain involved in feedback processing, learning and feelings of 
cravings. The grey matter in the anterior cingulate, involved in emotional processing and being attentive, was also reduced. 
In contrast, a region deep in the brain associated with reward processing, attention and motor movements - the caudate 
nucleus - was enlarged in subjects who were addicted to the drug. This could explain why those subjects were more prone 
to addiction but the scientists cannot be sure whether the enlargement is a result of cocaine use. 
Laurence John Reed, a clinical senior lecturer in addiction neurobiology at Imperial College London, said the "most 
impressive" results were the basic comparison of controls and stimulant users, which showed how parts of the brain 
remodel themselves in response to drugs. "This is a striking and visual example of how addictive stimulant use can result in 
adaptation of very important brain systems which have a direct correlates with behaviour – specifically inattention, 
impulsivity and compulsivity – and really does underline why we need a much better neurobiological understanding of the 
processes involved." 
Ersche said that, though she found links between brain structure and cocaine use,her research was not conclusive on which 
came first. "At the moment, correlation shows me a direct relationship - but I don't know which direction the relationship is. 
Has this been caused by cocaine, or are people who have this abnormality more vulnerable?"
Why teenagers can't concentrate: 
too much grey matter 
The scans revealed an unexpected level of activity in the prefrontal cortex, a large region at the front of the brain involved 
in decision-making and multitasking. This indicated that the brain was working less effectively than that of an adult. 
"We knew that the prefrontal cortex of young children functioned in this chaotic way but we didn't realise it continued 
until the late 20s or early 30s," said Dr Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, who led the study. "What we discovered was that the part 
of the brain needed to complete this sort of process is still very much developing throughout adolescence. This means it 
continues to do a lot of needless work when making these sorts of decisions." 
Chaotic thought patterns are a result, she said, of teenagers' brains containing too much grey matter – the cell bodies and 
connections which carry messages within the brain. As we age, the amount of grey matter in our brains decreases. 
"What our research has shown is that there is simply too much going on in the brains of adolescents," said Blakemore. 
"The result is that their brain energy and resources are wasted and their decision-making process negatively affected." 
Adults, on the other hand, have less grey matter, said Blakemore. "This means that neural transmissions travel more 
efficiently between brain cells, so the brain works more effectively."
Do You Feel Like You're Not Sleeping, 
Even When You Are? 
In last week's issue of New Scientist, Ann Finkbeiner has a terrific article about the people who suffer from 
pseudoinsomnia, and a handful of scientists who are trying to figure out what their brains have in common. When they 
used conventional methods of analyzing the brain wave patterns of sleepers, it appeared that these pseudoinsomniacs 
were no different than any other sleeper. But then they tried something new. They analyzed the readouts of brain 
waves using an algorithm that's normally used for spectral analysis in physics. 
And that's when they began to see a pattern — a pattern that suggested these peoples' sleep cycles were being 
interrupted by brain wave patterns associated with fear, anxiety, and wakefulness. 
What's fascinating is that similar patterns of alpha, beta and gamma waves show up in the brain wave patterns of 
people who suffer from chronic pain and anxiety as well. It's possible that this "always-on" brain pattern leads to a 
variety of nervous symptoms, including being unable to get the benefits of a healthy night's sleep.
Do Porn Watchers Have Smaller Brains? 
If you watch a lot of porn, your brain may show it, 
new research suggests. 
German researchers looked at the brains of 64 men 
between the ages of 21 and 45 and found that one 
brain region (the striatum, linked to reward 
processing), was smaller in the brains of porn 
watchers, and that a specific part of the same region 
is also less activated when exposed to more 
pornography. The study, published in JAMA 
Psychiatry, also showed that the connection between 
the striatum and prefrontal cortex seemed to be 
weaker in brains of men who watched porn. 
“Basically everything that people do very frequently can 
shape their brain structure and function,” Simone Kühn, the 
study's lead author from the Max Planck Institute for Human 
Development told Reuters. 
Still, the study doesn't confirm whether watching porn 
causes the changes, or whether people with a certain brain 
type are inherently more apt to tune into X-rated content. 
But because the areas of the brain that appear to be 
affected are linked to rewards and motivation and decision 
making, some have suggested that porn watchers may be 
lazier and poor decision-makers. 
"Everything is going to be bad in excess and it's probably not 
terrible in moderation," Dr. Gregory Tau of the Columbia 
University/New York State Psychiatric Institute, who was not 
involved in the study, told Reuters.
The Science Behind Caffeine's Productivity- 
Enhancing Effects 
1. Caffeine comes into your body whole. 
2. Then the caffeine molecule enters your liver, where 
enzymes cut off three methyl groups to form three more 
small molecules. 
3. The molecules in question are theobromine, 
paraxanthine, and theophylline. 
4. Together with the original caffeine molecule, they 
heighten your brain activity, get nutrients flowing, increase 
your athleticism, and boost your focus.
Scientists Discover Area Of Brain 
Responsible For Loving Johnny Cash 
Music is among all cultures an important part of the live of most people. Music has psychological benefits and may 
generate strong emotional and physiological responses. 
Recently, neuroscientists have discovered that music influences the reward circuit of the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), 
even when no explicit reward is present. In this clinical case study, we describe a 60-year old patient who developed 
a sudden and distinct musical preference for Johnny Cash following deep brain stimulation (DBS) targeted at the 
NAcc. 
This case report substantiates the assumption that the NAcc is involved in musical preference, based on the 
observation of direct stimulation of the accumbens with DBS. It also shows that accumbens DBS can change musical 
preference without habituation of its rewarding properties
Does the Name-Letter Effect Prove 
That You're Soooo Into Yourself? 
And the spousal studies? Similar, or matching, names are 
also influenced by culture, but there's a more basic 
reason marrying spouses tend to have the same last 
name. A surprising amount of studies neglected to check 
whether the bride changed her name before the 
ceremony. So if two Taylors were wed, one of them might 
have had a different name, but changed it during the 
ramp up to the wedding. 
So while we do like ourselves enough to get sentimental 
about our names, we probably aren't crazy enough to 
choose our entire mode of life depending on our name. 
Studies found that woman found the letters of their own initials to be feminine and men found their initials to be 
masculine. My name, and my initials, are me, we have collectively decided. And it looks like people feel pretty good about 
themselves. Extended surveys found that people tend to settle, disproportionately, in towns that are alliterative, in jobs 
that are alliterative, and with spouses who are alliterative. Some people even choose towns and spouses with their own 
names. How much of an inducement is in a name? 
Some people, for example internet writers, might find alliterative spousal names so insufferably twee that they'd change 
their own name to get away from such an atrocity. (Seriously, that was only okay with the Roosevelts.) Others, like 
scientists, think that gravitating towards our initials is a sign of intrinsic self-esteem, or egotism. We want more of 
ourselves.
Which matters more for attractiveness: a 
woman’s face or body? 
“Women’s faces and bodies are both thought to provide cues to women’s age, health, fertility, and personality. To gain a 
stronger understanding of how these cues are utilized, we investigated the degree to which ratings of women’s faces and 
bodies independently predicted ratings of women’s full-body attractiveness. Women came into the lab not knowing they 
would be photographed. In Study 1 (N = 84), we photographed them in their street clothes; in Study 2 (N = 74), we 
photographed women in a solid-colored two-piece swimsuit that revealed their body shape, body size, and breast size. 
We cropped each woman’s original photo into an additional face-only photo and body-only photo; then, independent 
sets of raters judged women’s pictures. 
When dressed in their original clothes, women’s face-only ratings were better independent predictors of full-body 
attractiveness ratings than were their body-only ratings. When cues displayed in women’s bodies were made 
conspicuous by swimsuits, ratings of faces and bodies were similarly strong predictors of full-body attractiveness ratings. 
Moreover, women’s body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio were tied to ratings of women’s body attractiveness, with 
waist-to-hip ratio more important among women wearing swimsuits than among women wearing their original clothes. 
These results suggest that perceivers attend to cues of women’s health, fertility, and personality to the extent that they 
are visible.”
NCBI ROFL: Surprise! Men vote for the 
hotter female candidate. 
Here we reveal gender biases in the intuitive heuristics that voters use when deciding 
whom to vote for in major political elections. Our findings underscore the impact of 
gender and physical appearance on shaping voter decision-making and provide novel 
insight into the psychological foundations underlying the political gender gap.”
NCBI ROFL: The presence of an attractive woman elevates 
testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. 
“The authors report a field experiment with skateboarders that demonstrates that physical risk taking by young 
men increases in the presence of an attractive female. This increased risk taking leads to more successes but 
also more crash landings in front of a female observer. 
Mediational analyses suggest that this increase in risk taking is caused in part by elevated testosterone levels of 
men who performed in front of the attractive female. In addition, skateboarders’ risk taking was predicted by 
their performance on a reversal-learning task, reversal-learning performance was disrupted by the presence of 
the attractive female, and the female’s presence moderated the observed relationship between risk taking and 
reversal learning. 
These results suggest that men use physical risk taking as a sexual display strategy, and they provide suggestive 
evidence regarding possible hormonal and neural mechanisms.”
NCBI ROFL: Beauty week: Blond, busty, 
skinny waitresses get bigger tips. 
“Waitresses completed an on-line survey about their physical characteristics, 
self-perceived attractiveness and sexiness, and average tips. The waitresses’ 
self-rated physical attractiveness increased with their breast sizes and decreased 
with their ages, waist-to-hip ratios, and body sizes. Similar effects were 
observed on self-rated sexiness, with the exception of age, which varied with 
self-rated sexiness in a negative, quadratic relationship rather than a linear one. 
Moreover, the waitresses’ tips varied with age in a negative, quadratic 
relationship, increased with breast size, increased with having blond hair, and 
decreased with body size. 
These findings, which are discussed from an evolutionary perspective, make 
several contributions to the literature on female physical attractiveness. First, 
they replicate some previous findings regarding the determinants of female 
physical attractiveness using a larger, more diverse, and more ecologically valid 
set of stimuli than has been studied before. 
Second, they provide needed evidence that some of those determinants of 
female beauty affect interpersonal behaviors as well as attractiveness ratings. 
Finally, they indicate that some determinants of female physical attractiveness 
do not have the same effects on overt interpersonal behavior (such as tipping) 
that they have on attractiveness ratings. 
This latter contribution highlights the need for more ecologically valid tests of 
evolutionary theories about the determinants and consequences of female 
beauty.”
NCBI ROFL: Why poor, hungry men 
prefer bigger breasts. 
It has been suggested human female breast size may act as signal of fat reserves, 
which in turn indicates access to resources. Based on this perspective, two studies 
were conducted to test the hypothesis that men experiencing relative resource 
insecurity should perceive larger breast size as more physically attractive than men 
experiencing resource security. 
In Study 1, 266 men from three sites in Malaysia varying in relative socioeconomic 
status (high to low) rated a series of animated figures varying in breast size for 
physical attractiveness. 
Results showed that men from the low socioeconomic context rated larger breasts as 
more attractive than did men from the medium socioeconomic context, who in turn 
perceived larger breasts as attractive than men from a high socioeconomic context. 
Study 2 compared the breast size judgements of 66 hungry versus 58 satiated men 
within the same environmental context in Britain. 
Results showed that hungry men rated larger breasts as significantly more attractive 
than satiated men. Taken together, these studies provide evidence that resource 
security impacts upon men’s attractiveness ratings based on women’s breast size.
More Evidence That Longevity 
Depends on Your State of Mind 
Of course, correlation is not causation. Having a sense of purpose isn't 
what's making people live longer. Rather, having a sense of purpose 
can give rise to healthy habits while diminishing a number of risk 
factors; setting large and long-term goals serves as a protective shield. 
For example, people with clearly defined goals may be less apt to 
abuse alcohol and drugs, which can be seen as a distraction, escape, or 
a barrier to achieving one's goals. A sense of purpose may also result 
in a more socially engaged life, particularly if helping people is a key 
motivator; studies show that social alienation is risk factor en par with 
excessive smoking and alcoholism. 
We all know that having goals is important, but a joint US-Canadian study reveals that having a sense of purpose can 
affect our longevity. Remarkably, it doesn't matter how old we are or what we aspire to — as long as we have goals, 
we live longer. 
Psychologists have known for some time that a sense of purpose is a key indicator of healthy aging, including its 
potential for reducing mortality risk. But this new study, which now appears in Psychological Science, extends 
previous findings in two important ways. First, it shows that a sense of purpose is beneficial across a person's entire 
adult lifespan, and second, that mortality rates — and by inference health — can indeed be correlated with having a 
purpose in life. 
So a sense of purpose could be derived from a desire to climb the corporate ladder, writing a book, running for 
office, or improving one's performance in art or at the gym. These ambitions can also serve as stepping stones to 
other goals, such as financial stability and raising children. And in fact, the most frequently cited purposes had to do 
with helping other people or trying to improve the social structure.
Smart Drugs Could Be Impairing The Brains Of 
Young People 
Authors looked at two stimulants in particular, methylphenidate and modafinil. 
Sold as Ritalin and Concerta to treat ADHD, methylphenidate works by increasing the level of neurotransmitters in the 
nervous system. Trials on rats have shown that young, developing brains are particularly sensitive to methylphenidate. 
Low dosages can reduce nerve activity, working memory, and the ability to quickly switch between tasks and behaviors. 
Altering glutamate function via the use of psychostimulants may impair behavioral flexibility, leading to the development 
and/or potentiation of addictive behaviors. 
Furthermore, dopamine and norepinephrine do not display linear effects; instead, their modulation of cognitive and 
neuronal function maps on an inverted-U curve. 
Healthy individuals run the risk of pushing themselves beyond optimal levels into hyperdopaminergic and 
hypernoradrenergic states, thus vitiating the very behaviors they are striving to improve.
Are 'Lucky Streaks' Real? Science Says Yes 
Bettors are more likely to win once they're 
on a winning streak, for a precisely ironic 
reason. 
The study, published this month in the journal Cognition, also found that losses can breed 
more losses. After losing twice, the chances of winning decreased to 40 percent. After four 
losses, the chance of winning was 27 percent. After six duds, you have only a 23 percent 
chance of winning. The explanation: after each loss, gamblers on average choose bets that 
are less likely to turn out, apparently assuming that they are more likely to win than before-- 
and perhaps to make up their losses (although, on average, people gamble less after each 
loss). As you probably know, bets with a lower chance of winning have higher payouts. 
The idea that one is more likely to lose after winning, or more likely to win after losing, is 
known as the gambler's fallacy (in reality, all things being equal, one is just as likely to lose or 
win on any given bet, assuming one is betting on independent events that don't effect each 
other's outcomes, as is the case with the vast majority of sports bets). This stands in contrast 
to the "hot hand fallacy": that one is more likely to win while on a hot streak. Bettors 
apparently don't generally believe this to be true, or at least their behavior suggests they 
don't. 
"The result is ironic: Winners worried their good luck was not going to continue, so they 
selected safer odds," the researchers wrote. "By doing so, they became more likely to win. 
The losers expected the luck to turn, so they took riskier odds. However, this made them 
even more likely to lose. The gamblers’ fallacy created the hot hand." 
The study found that when a person wins a bet, they become increasingly likely to succeed after each win. The converse is 
also true: Once you lose a bet, you become progressively more likely to keep losing. 
The fascinating study looked at 565,915 sports bets made by 776 online gamblers in Europe and the United States, and 
found that, all things being equal, you're likely to win or lose 48 percent of the time (draws presumably account for the 
remaining 4 percent). After a single winning bid, the chance of winning a second goes up ever so slightly to 49 percent. 
But here's where things get interesting. After the second win, the chance of winning a third time increases to 57 percent. 
After that: 67 percent. Following a four-bet winning streak, the chances of scoring a fifth haul increase to 72 percent. The 
probability of a sixth win is then 75 percent, and finally, after six wins, bettors had a 76 percent chance of notching lucky 
No. 7.
The Othello Error Makes You Sure Everyone is Lying 
Ever heard of Othello error? It's the unfortunate 
mindset that leads questioners to believe people 
are lying, even when they're telling the truth. 
It can cause them to use understandable 
coincidences as damning evidence, and getting 
rid of it can make us better at judging honesty. 
The innocent, but incriminating, coincidence is a staple of comedy. The heroine picks up a purse that's nearly identical 
to her own, and finds she's stolen her future mother-in-law's wallet. A hero hesitates to tell his boss he was at a 
proctology appointment because the woman he has a crush on is in the room, and gets so flushed and red-faced that 
the boss figures he must be guilty of something. The audience laughs because we've all been stuck in circumstances 
that make us look guilty when we're not, and then gotten so flustered during our explanations that we look even 
more guilty. Usually these circumstances are, at least in retrospect, comic. 
This kind of misunderstanding can also have dark results, as we see in dramatic works like Othello. Desdemona is the 
victim of villainous interference rather than mere coincidence, but her increasingly frantic attempts to correct the 
situation only make her look more guilty. She looks guilty because Othello suspects she's guilty. Once he starts 
looking at everything she does with suspicion, everything she does is suspicious.
How 'Hyperpalatable' Foods Could Turn You Into 
A Food Addict 
Over a third of the global population is now 
overweight, and the percentages are increasing. 
Some neuroscientists have suggested that the rise of 
so-called "hyperpalatable foods" may partially 
explain the unprecedented rates of obesity. 
Eventually, the experience of eating impossibly delicious foods results in what Kessler describes as "conditioned 
hypereating." When we consume enjoyable sugary and fatty foods, it stimulates endorphins in our brains — chemicals 
that signal a pleasurable experience. In turn, and in Pavlovian fashion, these chemicals stimulate us to eat more of that 
type of food, while also calming us down and making us feel good. Conditioned hypereating sounds suspiciously similar to 
what we might call food addiction. And indeed, studies have shown that hyperpalatable foods may be capable of 
triggering an addictive process — one that's been postulated as a possible cause of the obesity epidemic. 
But is it fair or reasonable to categorize food — something we need to keep us alive — alongside such things as illicit 
drugs, alcohol, and gambling? Some scientists say yes. 
Last year, for example, neuroscientists from Connecticut College claimed that Oreo cookies are more addictive than 
cocaine. The researchers came to this conclusion after measuring a protein called c-Fos in the brains of rats. They found 
that the cookies activated more neurons in the accumbens — a region of the brain associated with pleasure, and studied 
for its role in addiction and reward-processing — than addictive substances like cocaine. Not surprisingly, the researchers 
were harshly criticized for suggesting that something as apparently benign as an Oreo cookie could be compared to a 
notorious party drug.
Men Without Beards Could Soon Have An 
Evolutionary Advantage 
But the context did matter. When facial hair was rare among 
faces, beards and heavy stubble were rated about 20% more 
attractive. And when beards were common, clean-shaven faces 
enjoyed a similar bump, the team reports online today in Biology 
Letters. The effect on judgment was the same for men and 
women. 
“This study breaks new ground,” says Peter Frost, an 
anthropologist at the Interuniversity Centre for Aboriginal Studies 
and Research in Quebec City, Canada. Although previous studies 
have shown that people prefer novelty for certain traits, such as 
the color of clothing, this study shows “that the novelty effect 
applies not only to colors but also to other visible features [of the 
body],” he says. But hipsters shouldn’t let their beards get too 
gnarly. “There are certainly limits to this effect,” Frost says. 
“Something can be novel but also disgusting.” 
Negative frequency-dependent sexual selection maintains striking polymorphisms in secondary sexual traits in several 
animal species. Here, we test whether frequency of beardedness modulates perceived attractiveness of men's facial hair, 
a secondary sexual trait subject to considerable cultural variation. 
We first showed participants a suite of faces, within which we manipulated the frequency of beard thicknesses and then 
measured preferences for four standard levels of beardedness. Women and men judged heavy stubble and full beards 
more attractive when presented in treatments where beards were rare than when they were common, with intermediate 
preferences when intermediate frequencies of beardedness were presented. 
Likewise, clean-shaven faces were least attractive when clean-shaven faces were most common and more attractive 
when rare. This pattern in preferences is consistent with negative frequency-dependent selection.
Over the Hill? Cognitive Speeds Peak at Age 24 
Those grim “over the hill” party favors are often deployed ironically by 
those who want to razz their friends or partners when they turn 30 or 
40. But it may be more honest than we care to admit: A new study 
suggests humans’ cognitive speed peaks at age 24, and that it’s a 
steady downhill descent from there. 
The study is limited by the fact that it only focused on video game 
players. But in analyzing a dataset of over 3,000 StarCraft 2 players 
between the ages of 16 and 44, researchers determined that in-game 
response times, or cognitive speed, peaked in players at 24 years of 
age 
The present study investigates age-related changes in cognitive motor performance through adolescence and 
adulthood in a complex real world task, the real-time strategy video game StarCraft 2. In this paper we analyze the 
influence of age on performance using a dataset of 3,305 players, aged 16-44, collected by Thompson, Blair, Chen & 
Henrey [1]. 
Using a piecewise regression analysis, we find that age-related slowing of within-game, self-initiated response times 
begins at 24 years of age. We find no evidence for the common belief expertise should attenuate domain-specific 
cognitive decline. Domain-specific response time declines appear to persist regardless of skill level. A second analysis of 
dual-task performance finds no evidence of a corresponding age-related decline. 
Finally, an exploratory analyses of other age-related differences suggests that older participants may have been 
compensating for a loss in response speed through the use of game mechanics that reduce cognitive load.
Picking Sides: How Genes Help Us 
Decide Between Left and Right 
It may surprise you that left-handed women 
were found to be twice or more likely to 
develop premenopausal breast cancer than 
right-handers. 
And a few researchers believe this effect may 
be linked to exposure to certain chemicals in 
utero, affecting your genes and then setting 
the stage for both left-handedness and cancer 
susceptibility, thus opening up another 
probability of nurture changing nature. 
So why the predominance of righties? For our species, one of the most 
important tasks is communication, which is generally processed on the 
left side of the brain. 
And some scientists think that’s the reason why we’re right-side 
dominant, because, as you’ve probably heard, the left side of the brain 
generally controls the muscles on the right side of the body (which is 
why a stroke suffered on the left side of your brain is more likely to 
result in impairment to the arm and leg on the right side of the body). 
Discovering more of the biology behind handedness, tracing it to 
genetics, exposures, or both, could give us a lot more knowledge than 
simply whether we should line up our kids on the left or right side of the 
tee-ball batter’s box. 
That’s because left-handedness has also been associated with higher 
rates of dyslexia, schizophrenia, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, 
some mood disorders, and, as we’ve discussed, even cancer. 
Indeed, adding handedness into the mix has helped Danish researchers 
identify which children who had symptoms of ADHD at the age of eight 
(when, let’s face it, just about every kid is a little bit on the rambunctious 
side) would still have it at the age of 16.
Can You Control Your Metabolism With Your Mind? 
Crum designed an experiment to investigate the link between what we think we're eating, how full we feel and how our 
bodies respond. First, she made some milkshakes. Then, she labeled each shake as either "Sensishake: Fat Free, Guilt 
Free, 104 Calories" or "Indulgence: Decadence You Deserve, 620 Calories." In reality, all the shakes were around 300 
calories. Test subjects were asked to drink the milkshakes while nurses monitored their blood levels of ghrelin, a 
hormone that increases with hunger, decreases after a meal, and affects the rate of our metabolism. 
"Crum discovered that those who believed they were drinking the indulgent shake responded as if their bodies had 
eaten three times more," explains Spiegel. "So what people believed about their milkshake came true. If they thought it 
was fattening, they felt they'd eaten more and their digestion was affected. Their ghrelin levels dropped three times 
more." 
As the NPR video points out, Crum's findings suggest that while it may not matter as much as what we eat, how we 
think about what we eat could play a significant role in how our bodies process food. There's obviously something very 
appealing about that, but it warrants a good measure of skepticism. For one thing, Crum's study apparently lacked a 
control group – that is, a group of test subjects who were not intentionally deceived with either rhetoric or calorie 
counts.
Extract Money From People Using the Ambiguity Effect 
Here's a quick game. I have a jar full of ninety marbles. There are thirty red marbles for sure, but there's a mix of black 
and white marbles, and I can't be sure how many of each is in there. You can choose between one of two games. The 
first game awards you a hundred dollars if you pick a red marble. The second game forces you to pick either black or 
white as the "winning color," and awards you a hundred dollars if you pick the marble of your choice. 
Most people will pick the red marble game. Knowing their exact odds of winning comforts them. They even pick the 
red game if it's explained to them that the games have an equal probability of winning them the hundred bucks. To 
see why the black and white game wins, let's change the game slightly. Instead of picking which marble color wins 
before you draw, you draw and then flip a coin to see which color wins. So if you've just drawn a black marble, the 
odds of winning with that marble are fifty percent - the likelihood of either side of a coin flip. And if you've just drawn 
a white marble, the odds of winning with that marble are fifty percent. Considering the overall odds of drawing either 
black or white are two-thirds, fifty percent of two thirds is one third - or exactly the odds of winning with a red marble. 
Why is this important? Because of their dislike of uncertainty, people will pay to move from an uncertain game to a 
more certain one. So you can either charge slightly more money for the red marble game, or work the crowd with a 
partner. People get assigned on game or the other, and your partner, with the more-desirable red marble game will 
trade their tickets for a cash bonus to people who don't like their odds (even though they should).
Continuity fields, and why we miss subtle visual changes 
Continuity fields: Even when we fix our gaze on 
something, our eyes are actually shifting microscopically 
several times per second 
Our eyes are continuously bombarded by visual 
information – millions of colors, shapes and ever-changing 
motion – yet seeing never feels like work. 
Researchers have discovered one reason: our brains 
perform automatic visual smoothing over time. 
A study has found that our visual perception of things 
is influenced by what we saw up to 15 seconds ago. 
This helps create a stable environment, despite 
sacrificing some accuracy. 
It also means that what you see around you – that cup 
of coffee, the face of your co-worker, your computer 
screen – may be a time-averaged composite of now 
and the past. 
Fischer, who did the work as a doctoral student in the lab of 
visual scientist David Whitney, calls this filter a continuity field. 
For a real-world example of the phenomenon, think of a road 
sign on a rainy day. 
Despite the motion of hundreds of raindrops – and your retinas 
being flooded with visual fluctuations – you don't have to 
struggle too hard to read the sign. Your visual system is 
averaging over what is effectively noise. 
Fischer and Whitney also found that the filter seems to come into 
play only when we need it. Attention matters – past images had 
an influence if the subjects were paying attention to them, but 
not if they were peripheral or in a radically different location. And 
predictably, the influence of older images lessened the more time 
passed. 
Continuity fields are one type of visual insensitivity, but there are 
others. Take the failure to notice an obvious change, known as 
change blindness. One study had an experimenter ask a 
pedestrian for directions. Their conversation was then 
interrupted by a group of men walking between the two speakers 
and carrying a large, obstructive object. Behind that object, out of 
sight of the pedestrian, the initial experimenter was replaced by a 
different person. 
Despite the conversation partner being the central object in the 
scene, only half of pedestrians noticed the change. The rest 
continued giving directions as if nothing strange had happened.
Continuity fields, and why we miss 
subtle visual changes 
"We're not very good at detecting changes in our environment if the object is something we wouldn't expect to 
change," Johnson said. In the real world, we wouldn't think a person we're talking to would spontaneously 
transform. Thus, our brains often don't waste energy trying to notice these types of shifts. 
Fischer suspects that our brains learn that the world follows certain rules – objects don't change location 
spontaneously, and little changes don't matter most of the time – and adaptation of the visual system follows suit. 
Visual scientist Michele Rucci of Boston University, not involved in the study, was surprised and intrigued by the 
existence of continuity fields. 
"We have this input to our retina that is continuously jumping, but yet the world seems stable," Rucci said. He noted 
that, even when we fix our gaze on something, our eyes are actually shifting microscopically several times per 
second. "Our perception of the world is very different than what the real input to the retina is."
Change Blindness 
Change blindness is a surprising perceptual phenomenon that 
occurs when a change in a visual stimulus is introduced and 
the observer does not notice it. 
For example, observers often fail to notice major differences 
introduced into an image while it flickers off and on again. 
People's poor ability to detect changes has been argued to 
reflect fundamental limitations of human attention. 
Change blindness has become a highly researched topic and 
some have argued that it may have important practical 
implications in areas such as eyewitness testimony and 
distractions while driving. 
Additionally, further research stated that rather large changes will not be detected when they occur during saccadic movements of the eye. 
Another finding based on similar studies stated that a change was easily picked up on by participants when the eye was fixated on the point 
of change.[7] Therefore, the eye must be directly fixated on the area of change for it to be noticed. 
However, other research in the mid-1990s has indicated that individuals still have difficulty detecting change even when they are directly 
fixated on a particular scene. A study by Rensink, O’Regan, & Clarke demonstrated that change blindness can have an effect even if the eye 
was fixated on a scene. In this study, a picture was presented followed by a blank screen or “masking” stimulus, which was followed by the 
initial picture with a change. 
The masking stimulus almost acts like a saccadic movement of the eye which makes it significantly more difficult for individuals to detect the 
change.[6] This was a critical contribution to change blindness research because it demonstrated that a change can remain unnoticed with 
the smallest disruptions. 
Research on change blindness proceeded one step further into practical applications of this phenomenon. For example, there does not have 
to be a masking stimulus in order for individuals to miss a change in a scene. Individuals often take significantly longer to notice certain 
changes if there are a few small, high contrast shapes that are temporarily splattered over a picture.[8] This method for testing change 
blindness is called “mudsplashes”.[8] This method is particularly relevant to individuals driving in a car when there is a visual obstruction on 
the windshield. This obstruction may impair an individual’s ability to detect a change in their environment which could result in severe 
negative consequences while driving.
Study proves that talking on your cell 
phone makes you act like an a**hole. 
Use of a cell phone reduces attention and increases 
response times. 62 people (30 men, 32 women) were 
confronted with a confederate wearing a large leg 
brace, who dropped a stack of magazines and feigned 
difficulty retrieving them. 
Among the 33 people who talked on their cell phones 
only 9% offered their help, whereas among the 29 
people who did not talk on their cell phones, 72% 
offered help. 
The use of cell phones affects helping behavior.
Why overheard cell phone 
conversations are extra annoying. 
Overhearing someone on a cell phone means hearing only half of a conversation–a “halfalogue.” We show that merely 
overhearing a halfalogue results in decreased performance on cognitive tasks designed to reflect the attentional demands 
of daily activities. 
By contrast, overhearing both sides of a cell-phone conversation or a monologue does not result in decreased 
performance. 
This may be because the content of a halfalogue is less predictable than both sides of a conversation. In a second 
experiment, we controlled for differences in acoustic factors between these types of overheard speech, establishing that it 
is the unpredictable informational content of halfalogues that results in distraction. 
Thus, we provide a cognitive explanation for why overheard cell-phone conversations are especially irritating: Less-predictable 
speech results in more distraction for a listener engaged in other tasks.”
Need to Commit Something to 
Memory? Sleep On It 
Almost a century after scientists first began studying 
sleep, it’s clear that getting some z‘s is useful for far 
more than resting our bodies; it’s also necessary for 
retaining short- and long-term memories. 
These new findings could have important 
implications for everything from education to 
overcoming phobias, Deikelmann said. 
Remember staying up all night cramming for that statistics test in college? Turns out you 
probably would have scored higher if you had closed the books and hit the hay. 
A series of recent studies, some of which were presented at this week’s annual meeting 
of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, suggest that a good night’s sleep doesn’t just 
make you feel more rested – it’s also crucial for remembering everything from test 
answers to when to mail your mother’s birthday card.
You can't tell if someone is lying by 
reading their facial expressions 
Microexpressions, as defined by psychologist Paul Ekman (who coined the term "microexpression," 
basically wrote the book on the little bastards, and has been studying their use in detecting deception for 
going on half a century, now), are: 
...very brief facial expressions, lasting between 1/25th and 1/15th of a second. They occur when a person 
either deliberately or unconsciously conceals an emotion being felt. Any one of the seven emotions found 
to have a universal signal may appear in a micro expression: anger, fear, sadness, disgust, contempt, 
surprise and happiness. 
Microexpressions therefore fall under the umbrella of "body language" ("nonverbals," if you're one for 
parlance), and are distinguishable in that they refer explicitly to the face and specific situations in which 
they're likely to appear, viz. a situation where the emotion being felt is being either intentionally or 
unintentionally suppressed.
How Does "Valence" Help Your Brain Distinguish 
Between Good And Bad? 
Valence is the term used for the spectrum of emotions from extremely 
positive to extremely negative. 
When it comes to arousal — the intensity of an emotion — neuroscience 
indicates that valence might be less important than we think. The 
amygdala is the part of the brain that processes emotions. 
Turns out, the amygdala gets engaged based on arousal. The feeling can be negative or it can be positive. Whether a 
person is shown a picture of snuggling kittens or a severed hand, the amygdala responds. 
But it isn't the only part of the brain that responds. The prefrontal cortex is a part of the brain we really don't want to 
lose, as it deals with planning and problem-solving, and any injury to the region is devastating. It turns out, this area of 
the brain might also tell you whether you feel good or bad. 
Different areas of the prefrontal cortex get going when different stimuli are presented. The areas are scattered, but there 
are distinct sections that distinguish between what you feel when you get a free plane ticket to Costa Rica and what you 
feel when you get there and a spider drops from a tree onto your face. Although it's your amygdala that tells you that 
both events were thrilling, it's your prefrontal cortex that tells you which one you wouldn't care to repeat.
The Psychological Effect That Explains 
Why You Suck at Parties 
It doesn't just take a good brain to memorize names. It 
takes time and attention, neither of which you have in 
that situation. 
As each person is listed, we are waiting for the next 
person's name. This provokes what's known as the 
next-in-line effect. 
The next-in-line effect happens when someone is waiting for 
their turn to speak during an organized event. 
Researchers tested it by having people in small, circular groups 
take turns giving out information, then testing them to see 
which snippets of information they remembered. 
Each participant's memory was just fine, right up until they got 
to the information imparted by the person who talked just 
before they themselves were "up." 
Suddenly, they remembered nothing. Yes, they were listening 
when the person before them was talking. They just weren't 
paying attention. Their mind was already on the next task. 
When we see a group of people coming towards us with their 
hands out for handshakes, we're in a situation in which we 
know we'll have to take in a cascade of information, and we try 
to prepare for it. 
This is exactly the wrong thing to do. We try to simultaneously 
greet the person in front of us, and get ready for the next 
person in line. 
As it turns out, we can either take in and store information, or 
get ready for the next grouping of information. Since we try to 
do two things at once, we fail at both.
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits
Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Qualitative Research Methods
Qualitative Research MethodsQualitative Research Methods
Qualitative Research MethodsJukka Peltokoski
 
Princiiples of Scientific Method in Anthropology
Princiiples of Scientific Method in AnthropologyPrinciiples of Scientific Method in Anthropology
Princiiples of Scientific Method in AnthropologyPaulVMcDowell
 
Pragmatic approach to research
Pragmatic approach to researchPragmatic approach to research
Pragmatic approach to researchayoub babar
 
All you Need to Know about Deductive Reasoning
All you Need to Know about Deductive ReasoningAll you Need to Know about Deductive Reasoning
All you Need to Know about Deductive ReasoningSamantha A. Walters
 
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptx
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptxCognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptx
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptxLarry195181
 
introduction to research-2023.ppt
introduction to research-2023.pptintroduction to research-2023.ppt
introduction to research-2023.pptDoctorOkelloBen
 
Evolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary PsychologyEvolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary PsychologyRick McKinnon
 
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & Variables
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & VariablesMeaning of Constructs, Concepts & Variables
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & VariablesSahin Sahari
 
philosophy of Social science
philosophy of Social science philosophy of Social science
philosophy of Social science javeria nazeer
 
Fields of psychology
Fields of psychologyFields of psychology
Fields of psychologysherrymariani
 
Qualitative Research
Qualitative ResearchQualitative Research
Qualitative ResearchAhmad Faisal
 
Psychoanalytic Theory by Freud
Psychoanalytic Theory by FreudPsychoanalytic Theory by Freud
Psychoanalytic Theory by FreudProfiteers
 
2. qualitative research design.pdf
2. qualitative research design.pdf2. qualitative research design.pdf
2. qualitative research design.pdfRockangelJamoral1
 
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...Sally Wan
 

Mais procurados (20)

Qualitative Research Methods
Qualitative Research MethodsQualitative Research Methods
Qualitative Research Methods
 
Inductive vs deductive reasoning
Inductive vs deductive reasoningInductive vs deductive reasoning
Inductive vs deductive reasoning
 
Qualitative research
Qualitative researchQualitative research
Qualitative research
 
Princiiples of Scientific Method in Anthropology
Princiiples of Scientific Method in AnthropologyPrinciiples of Scientific Method in Anthropology
Princiiples of Scientific Method in Anthropology
 
Pragmatic approach to research
Pragmatic approach to researchPragmatic approach to research
Pragmatic approach to research
 
All you Need to Know about Deductive Reasoning
All you Need to Know about Deductive ReasoningAll you Need to Know about Deductive Reasoning
All you Need to Know about Deductive Reasoning
 
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptx
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptxCognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptx
Cognitive Psychology ppt ch1.pptx
 
Philosophy of Science
Philosophy of SciencePhilosophy of Science
Philosophy of Science
 
introduction to research-2023.ppt
introduction to research-2023.pptintroduction to research-2023.ppt
introduction to research-2023.ppt
 
Evolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary PsychologyEvolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary Psychology
 
Grounded Theory
Grounded TheoryGrounded Theory
Grounded Theory
 
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & Variables
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & VariablesMeaning of Constructs, Concepts & Variables
Meaning of Constructs, Concepts & Variables
 
philosophy of Social science
philosophy of Social science philosophy of Social science
philosophy of Social science
 
Fields of psychology
Fields of psychologyFields of psychology
Fields of psychology
 
Qualitative Research
Qualitative ResearchQualitative Research
Qualitative Research
 
Persuasive technology
Persuasive technologyPersuasive technology
Persuasive technology
 
Psychoanalytic Theory by Freud
Psychoanalytic Theory by FreudPsychoanalytic Theory by Freud
Psychoanalytic Theory by Freud
 
2. qualitative research design.pdf
2. qualitative research design.pdf2. qualitative research design.pdf
2. qualitative research design.pdf
 
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...
Using phenomenography in educational technology research from 2003 to 2017: A...
 
Exploratory research
Exploratory researchExploratory research
Exploratory research
 

Semelhante a Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits

Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?guestcf1e8d8
 
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?Micah Allen
 
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?Seismonaut
 
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsd
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsdDyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsd
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsdmdyer15
 
Persuasive Technology 2009 Summary
Persuasive Technology 2009 SummaryPersuasive Technology 2009 Summary
Persuasive Technology 2009 Summarysnyderconsulting
 
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional Development
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional DevelopmentThe Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional Development
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional DevelopmentKristen Stacey
 
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...Kyongsik Yun
 
Bonding can make us move in unison - Futurity
Bonding can make us move in unison - FuturityBonding can make us move in unison - Futurity
Bonding can make us move in unison - FuturityKyongsik Yun
 
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...Kyongsik Yun
 
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...RIKICOURSE
 
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYOD
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYODThe Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYOD
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYODMichele Chubirka
 
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in Marketing
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in MarketingFact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in Marketing
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in MarketingBjörn Persson
 
[Caltech News] Social synchronicity
[Caltech News] Social synchronicity[Caltech News] Social synchronicity
[Caltech News] Social synchronicityKyongsik Yun
 

Semelhante a Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits (17)

Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
 
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook Eating Your Brain?
 
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?
Micah Allen: Zombies or Cyborgs: Is Facebook eating your brain?
 
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsd
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsdDyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsd
Dyer portfolio essay technology and the changing brain rvsd
 
Persuasive Technology 2009 Summary
Persuasive Technology 2009 SummaryPersuasive Technology 2009 Summary
Persuasive Technology 2009 Summary
 
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional Development
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional DevelopmentThe Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional Development
The Psychology Of Childhood Social And Emotional Development
 
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...
Researchers Say Synchronized Body Movements May Help In Human Social Bonding ...
 
Neuromarketing
NeuromarketingNeuromarketing
Neuromarketing
 
Bonding can make us move in unison - Futurity
Bonding can make us move in unison - FuturityBonding can make us move in unison - Futurity
Bonding can make us move in unison - Futurity
 
Is digital technology re-wiring your brain?
Is digital technology re-wiring your brain?Is digital technology re-wiring your brain?
Is digital technology re-wiring your brain?
 
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...
Social Synchronicity: Research Finds a Connection Between Bonding and Matched...
 
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...
Researchers Discover How the Human Brain Separates, Stores, and Retrieves Mem...
 
Cognitive Psychology Essay
Cognitive Psychology EssayCognitive Psychology Essay
Cognitive Psychology Essay
 
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYOD
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYODThe Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYOD
The Homunculus Problem: Why You Will Lose the Battle of BYOD
 
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in Marketing
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in MarketingFact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in Marketing
Fact and Fiction: The Use of Neuroscience in Marketing
 
O Behave! Issue 14
O Behave! Issue 14O Behave! Issue 14
O Behave! Issue 14
 
[Caltech News] Social synchronicity
[Caltech News] Social synchronicity[Caltech News] Social synchronicity
[Caltech News] Social synchronicity
 

Último

It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 May
It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 MayIt will be International Nurses' Day on 12 May
It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 MayNZSG
 
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...anilsa9823
 
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableDipal Arora
 
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine ServiceCall Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Serviceritikaroy0888
 
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdfRenandantas16
 
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewas
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service DewasVip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewas
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewasmakika9823
 
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116 - With room Service
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116  - With room ServiceCall Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116  - With room Service
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116 - With room Servicediscovermytutordmt
 
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRL
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRLMONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRL
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRLSeo
 
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...Lviv Startup Club
 
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature Set
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature SetCreating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature Set
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature SetDenis Gagné
 
GD Birla and his contribution in management
GD Birla and his contribution in managementGD Birla and his contribution in management
GD Birla and his contribution in managementchhavia330
 
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSM
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSMMonte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSM
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSMRavindra Nath Shukla
 
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update Presentation Slides
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update  Presentation SlidesKeppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update  Presentation Slides
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update Presentation SlidesKeppelCorporation
 
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...Dipal Arora
 
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101 - Basics on Growth Marketing
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101  - Basics on Growth MarketingTech Startup Growth Hacking 101  - Basics on Growth Marketing
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101 - Basics on Growth MarketingShawn Pang
 
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...Paul Menig
 
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130 Available With Room
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130  Available With RoomVIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130  Available With Room
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130 Available With Roomdivyansh0kumar0
 

Último (20)

It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 May
It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 MayIt will be International Nurses' Day on 12 May
It will be International Nurses' Day on 12 May
 
KestrelPro Flyer Japan IT Week 2024 (English)
KestrelPro Flyer Japan IT Week 2024 (English)KestrelPro Flyer Japan IT Week 2024 (English)
KestrelPro Flyer Japan IT Week 2024 (English)
 
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...
Lucknow 💋 Escorts in Lucknow - 450+ Call Girl Cash Payment 8923113531 Neha Th...
 
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Pune Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine ServiceCall Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
Call Girls In Panjim North Goa 9971646499 Genuine Service
 
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
0183760ssssssssssssssssssssssssssss00101011 (27).pdf
 
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewas
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service DewasVip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewas
Vip Dewas Call Girls #9907093804 Contact Number Escorts Service Dewas
 
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116 - With room Service
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116  - With room ServiceCall Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116  - With room Service
Call Girls in Gomti Nagar - 7388211116 - With room Service
 
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRL
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRLMONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRL
MONA 98765-12871 CALL GIRLS IN LUDHIANA LUDHIANA CALL GIRL
 
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...
Yaroslav Rozhankivskyy: Три складові і три передумови максимальної продуктивн...
 
Best Practices for Implementing an External Recruiting Partnership
Best Practices for Implementing an External Recruiting PartnershipBest Practices for Implementing an External Recruiting Partnership
Best Practices for Implementing an External Recruiting Partnership
 
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature Set
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature SetCreating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature Set
Creating Low-Code Loan Applications using the Trisotech Mortgage Feature Set
 
GD Birla and his contribution in management
GD Birla and his contribution in managementGD Birla and his contribution in management
GD Birla and his contribution in management
 
Forklift Operations: Safety through Cartoons
Forklift Operations: Safety through CartoonsForklift Operations: Safety through Cartoons
Forklift Operations: Safety through Cartoons
 
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSM
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSMMonte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSM
Monte Carlo simulation : Simulation using MCSM
 
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update Presentation Slides
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update  Presentation SlidesKeppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update  Presentation Slides
Keppel Ltd. 1Q 2024 Business Update Presentation Slides
 
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...
Call Girls Navi Mumbai Just Call 9907093804 Top Class Call Girl Service Avail...
 
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101 - Basics on Growth Marketing
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101  - Basics on Growth MarketingTech Startup Growth Hacking 101  - Basics on Growth Marketing
Tech Startup Growth Hacking 101 - Basics on Growth Marketing
 
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...
7.pdf This presentation captures many uses and the significance of the number...
 
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130 Available With Room
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130  Available With RoomVIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130  Available With Room
VIP Kolkata Call Girl Howrah 👉 8250192130 Available With Room
 

Studies, research papers, & other interesting tid bits

  • 2. Sections • Studies (3) • Research Papers (209) • Other (395)
  • 4. A Message From Your Brain: I'm Not Good At Remembering What I Hear "Our auditory memory isn't as robust as we might like to think it is," says Poremba. "We think that we are great at integrating all the senses," but the experiment shows that tactile and visual memory easily trumped auditory memory. The results further suggest that the brain processes tactile and visual memories through a similar mechanism, but that auditory memory is processed differently. This has potential implications for understanding the evolution of the human brain, says Bigelow, since the auditory memory of monkeys and chimpanzees also lags behind their tactile and visual memory.
  • 5. Shared brain activity predicts audience preferences The researchers also used official television viewing figures and publicly available data from Facebook and Twitter to gauge how popular each clip was at the time it was first shown. As in earlier studies of shared brain activity, they found that some of the clips produced a greater degree of synchronised brain activity in the participants that others. Remarkably, though, the participants’ shared brain activity accurately predicted audience reactions to each at the time they were aired, with the most popular scenes and commercials producing the greatest degree of brain synchronisation in the participants. Thus, the extent of shared brain activity within the small group of participants was closely linked to the collective behaviour of a much larger group of people, suggesting that brain scanning technology could eventually be used to predict peoples’ reactions to a forthcoming film, their product preferences, or perhaps even how they might vote in an upcoming election New research published in the journal Nature Communications adding some hope to the neuromarketing hype, by showing that the brain activity shared by small groups of people in response to film clips can accurately predict how popular those clips will be among larger groups. Ten years ago, Uri Hasson and his colleagues recruited five participants and used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to scan their brains while each one watched the same 30-minute clip of Sergio Leone’s classic spaghetti western, The Good, the Bad and the Ugly. They noticed that the film produced remarkably similar patterns of brain activity in all the participants, synchronising the activity across multiple regions, such that their brains “ticked collectively” while they viewed it. The researchers went on to show that films differ in their ability to induce this shared brain activity, with the more engaging ones producing a greater degree of synchrony, and more recently others have shown that the stereoscopic effects used in 3D films make viewing more enjoyable by creating a more immersive experience.
  • 6. The Neuroscience Of Emoticons Shaded areas indicate the activity areas in (1) right fusiform gyrus (2) right inferior frontal gyrus (3) right middle frontal gyrus, and (4) right inferior parietal lobule. More recent work, led by Churches, conducted a similar test using a different scientific approach. With electrodes hooked to their scalp, 20 test participants looked at pictures of actual faces and Western-style emoticons--with :-) for a smile and :-( for a frown. They also looked at these same items in inverted form: an upside-down face, for instance, or an emoticon like (-: facing the wrong way. The reason for this setup requires some technical explanation. Rather than measuring brain images, Churches and company measured electrical brain responses known as event-related potentials, or ERPs. The brain produces distinct ERPs for various events. When it sees faces, for instance, a reliable negative ERP occurs 170 milliseconds later--known as the N170. When it sees inverted faces, the amplitude of that N170 gets larger In simple terms, the researchers wanted to know if emoticons, both normal and inverted, produce a similar N170 effect. Sure doesn't seem like it. While normal emoticons did produce a large N170 signal, inverted emoticons produced a smaller one (below), the researchers report in a 2014 issue of Social Neuroscience. They suspect that flipped emoticons lose their cohesive symbolic meaning--becoming just a loose collection of punctuation.
  • 7. Online trolls are narcissists, sadists and psychopaths, says study In two online studies (total N = 1215), respondents completed personality inventories and a survey of their Internet commenting styles. Overall, strong positive associations emerged among online commenting frequency, trolling enjoyment, and troll identity, pointing to a common construct underlying the measures. Both studies revealed similar patterns of relations between trolling and the Dark Tetrad of personality: trolling correlated positively with sadism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism, using both enjoyment ratings and identity scores. Of all personality measures, sadism showed the most robust associations with trolling and, importantly, the relationship was specific to trolling behavior. Enjoyment of other online activities, such as chatting and debating, was unrelated to sadism. Thus cyber-trolling appears to be an Internet manifestation of everyday sadism
  • 8. Product Psychology Explains How To Get Users Hooked On Products Designed Around Habit And Context Last year, tens of thousands of hackers signed up to receive Hack Design, a newsletter teaching the principles of design so that those used to focusing on how things work behind the scenes could start at a point that also considers aesthetics and user-friendliness. Today, a group of product-focused creators — “Hooked” author Nir Eyal, Product Hunt‘s Ryan Hoover, and Greylock Partners‘s Josh Elman, among others — are hoping to follow up on that newsletter’s success with the launch of Product Psychology, a weekly course on the psychology of user behavior. Courses will arrive in the form of link-blog like posts delivered to your email inbox. Each week, one curator from the 17 psychologists, entrepreneurs, and designers involved in the project focuses on a single topic they’re especially versed in, with posts ranging from “How Scarcity & Impatience Drive User Behavior” to “Does Your Product Rely On Intuition Or Deliberation?” http://www.productpsychology.com/category/user-behavior/
  • 9. How Multitasking Reshapes Your Brain Into A Constantly Distracted State http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=PriSFBu5CLs People who frequently use several media devices at the same time have lower grey-matter density in one particular region of the brain compared to those who use just one device occasionally Media multitasking, or the concurrent consumption of multiple media forms, is increasingly prevalent in today’s society and has been associated with negative psychosocial and cognitive impacts. Individuals who engage in heavier media-multitasking are found to perform worse on cognitive control tasks and exhibit more socio-emotional difficulties. However, the neural processes associated with media multi-tasking remain unexplored. The present study investigated relationships between media multitasking activity and brain structure. Research has demonstrated that brain structure can be altered upon prolonged exposure to novel environments and experience. Thus, we expected differential engagements in media multitasking to correlate with brain structure variability. This was confirmed via Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) analyses: Individuals with higher Media Multitasking Index (MMI) scores had smaller gray matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Functional connectivity between this ACC region and the precuneus was negatively associated with MMI. Our findings suggest a possible structural correlate for the observed decreased cognitive control performance and socio-emotional regulation in heavy media-multitaskers. While the cross-sectional nature of our study does not allow us to specify the direction of causality, our results brought to light novel associations between individual media multitasking behaviors and ACC structure differences.
  • 10. The Psychology of Your Future Self and How Your Present Illusions Hinder Your Future Happiness Harvard psychologist Daniel Gilbert http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_you_are_always_changing http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_asks_why_are_we_happy http://www.ted.com/talks/dan_gilbert_researches_happiness “Human beings are works in progress that mistakenly think they’re finished. The person you are right now is as transient, as fleeting and as temporary as all the people you’re ever been. The one constant in our lives is change.” The things we do when we expect our lives to continue are naturally and properly different than the things we might do if we expected them to end abruptly. We go easy on the lard and tobacco, smile dutifully at yet another of our supervisor’s witless jokes, read books like this one when we could be wearing paper hats and eating pistachio macaroons in the bathtub, and we do each of these things in the charitable service of the people we will soon become. We treat our future selves as though they were our children, spending most of the hours of most of our days constructing tomorrows that we hope will make them happy. Rather than indulging in whatever strikes our momentary fancy, we take responsibility for the welfare of our future selves, squirreling away portions of our paychecks each month so they can enjoy their retirements on a putting green, jogging and flossing with some regularity so they can avoid coronaries and gum grafts, enduring dirty diapers and mind-numbing repetitions of The Cat in the Hat so that someday they will have fat-cheeked grandchildren to bounce on their laps. Even plunking down a dollar at the convenience store is an act of charity intended to ensure that the person we are about to become will enjoy the Twinkie we are paying for now. In fact, just about any time we want something — a promotion, a marriage, an automobile, a cheeseburger — we are expecting that if we get it, then the person who has our fingerprints a second, minute, day, or decade from now will enjoy the world they inherit from us, honoring our sacrifices as they reap the harvest of our shrewd investment decisions and dietary forbearance. [But] our temporal progeny are often thankless. We toil and sweat to give them just what we think they will like, and they quit their jobs, grow their hair, move to or from San Francisco, and wonder how we could ever have been stupid enough to think they’d like that. We fail to achieve the accolades and rewards that we consider crucial to their well-being, and they end up thanking God that things didn’t work out according to our shortsighted, misguided plan. Even that person who takes a bite of the Twinkie we purchased a few minutes earlier may make a sour face and accuse us of having bought the wrong snack.
  • 11. Curiosity improves memory by tapping into the brain’s reward system Ranganath said the findings are in line with theories that give dopamine a key role in stabilising or consolidating memories. The research is published in the journal, Neuron. Brain scans of college students have shed light on why people learn more effectively when their curiosity is piqued than when they are bored stiff. Researchers in the US found evidence that curiosity ramped up the activity of a brain chemical called dopamine, which in turn seemed to strengthen people’s memories. Students who took part in the study were better at remembering answers to trivia questions when they were curious, but their memories also improved for unrelated information they were shown at the same time. The findings suggest that while grades may have their place in motivating students, stimulating their natural curiosity could help them even more. “There are times when people feel they can take in a lot of new information, and other times when they feel their memories are terrible,” said Ranganath. “This work suggests that once you light that fire of curiosity, you put the brain in a state that’s more conducive to learning. Once you get this ramp-up of dopamine, the brain becomes more like a sponge that’s ready to soak up whatever is happening.”
  • 12. Your Decision-Making Processes Are a Lot More Random Than You Realize For the most part, we make decisions based on our prior experiences. But what about those situations that are completely new or unpredictable? A new study suggests that when facing an excessively uncertain or challenging scenario, the brain chooses randomness as the best strategy. When it comes to making decisions, our brains are extremely dependent on past experiences. In fact, some cognitive scientists speculate that the brain has a built-in mechanism for assessing (or weighing) the efficacy of a decision based on precedents from the past. It's also something we can be conscious of; to improve our rational decision making, it's important that we use new information to change our confidence in a belief. But as a recent experiment conducted by Alla Karpova of the Janelia Farm Research Campus shows, randomness may be the brain's preferred policy when things get particularly challenging or when the situation is completely without precedent. Karpova says the mechanism behind this change is found in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a brain structure involved in using experience to make decisions. And in fact, when the researchers manipulated the activity of neurons that released a stress hormone into the ACC, they were able to reverse the rats' behavioral strategies. When these neurons were stimulated, the rats abandoned the experience-based strategy and started to behave randomly in situations when this wasn't expected. Fascinatingly, inhibition of the stress hormone, norepinephrine, caused the rats to rely on their experiences — even when faced with the challenging competitor.
  • 13. Study finds that women who are ovulating are more into kissing. Hormonal changes associated with the human menstrual cycle have been previously found to affect female mate preference, whereby women in the late follicular phase of their cycle (i.e., at higher risk of conception) prefer males displaying putative signals of underlying genetic fitness. Past research also suggests that romantic kissing is utilized in human mating contexts to assess potential mating partners. The current study examined whether women in their late follicular cycle phase place greater value on kissing at times when it might help serve mate assessment functions. Using an international online questionnaire, results showed that women in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle felt that kissing was more important at initial stages of a relationship than women in the luteal phase of their cycle. Furthermore, it was found that estimated progesterone levels were a significant negative predictor for these ratings.
  • 14. Researchers “Copy and Paste” Fear From One Memory to Another Now, researchers studying mice have discovered that memories can be broken down into component parts — the emotional part separated from the factual part — and that the emotions associated with a memory can be transferred to a totally different memory. Redondo and his team expect that this growing body of research may help us understand how unpleasant memories are formed in the first place — and eventually, maybe even aid in the development of clinical tools for making unpleasant memories less painful. MIT neuroscientist Roger Redondo and his team started by gathering a sample group of genetically engineered mice. The mice’s brains were engineered so that, if an antibiotic known as doxycycline was removed from their diet, certain neurons in their brains would express a protein called channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) which triggers neural activity in response to blue light. This system enabled the team to use doxycycline as a sort of “record/pause” button for neural activity in the animals’ brains. The investigators could remove the antibiotic from the mice’s diet, give the animals certain kinds of conditioning, then start giving them doxycycline again; thus creating light-sensitive groups of neurons that had activated during specific time periods and tasks This research makes it pretty clear that memories aren’t all-or-nothing recollections. They can be broken down into component pieces like locations and emotions; and each of those pieces can be reactivated independently, and even become attached to completely different memory pieces than those with which it originally formed.
  • 15. This Psych Discovery Explains Why You Get Your Best Ideas In The Shower When faced with a difficult decision, it is often suggested to "sleep on it" or take a break from thinking about the decision in order to gain clarity. But new brain imaging research from Carnegie Mellon University, published in the journal "Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience," finds that the brain regions responsible for making decisions continue to be active even when the conscious brain is distracted with a different task. The research provides some of the first evidence showing how the brain unconsciously processes decision information in ways that lead to improved decision-making. "This research begins to chip away at the mystery of our unconscious brains and decision-making," said J. David Creswell, assistant professor of psychology in CMU's Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences and director of the Health and Human Performance Laboratory. "It shows that brain regions important for decision-making remain active even while our brains may be simultaneously engaged in unrelated tasks, such as thinking about a math problem. What’s most intriguing about this finding is that participants did not have any awareness that their brains were still working on the decision problem while they were engaged in an unrelated task."
  • 16. Human brain subliminally judges 'trustworthiness' of faces Finding from brain scans adds to evidence that we make spontaneous, largely unconscious judgments of strangers The study focused on the activity of the amygdala, a small almond-shaped region deep inside the brain. The amygdala is intimately involved with processing strong emotions, such as fear. Its central nucleus sends out the signals responsible for the famous and evolutionarily crucial "fight-or-flight" response. Even so, the brain scans revealed that the amygdala responded differently to subliminal images of trustworthy and untrustworthy faces. Some regions of the amygdala reacted to how untrustworthy a face was, while other regions seemed to register the overall strength of the judgment, becoming more active only when a face was either very trustworthy or very untrustworthy. The human brain can judge the apparent trustworthiness of a face from a glimpse so fleeting, the person has no idea they have seen it, scientists claim. Researchers in the US found that brain activity changed in response to how trustworthy a face appeared to be when the face in question had not been consciously perceived. Scientists made the surprise discovery during a series of experiments that were designed to shed light on the neural processes that underpin the snap judgments people make about others. The findings suggest that parts of our brains are doing more complex subconscious processing of the outside world than many researchers thought.
  • 17. Psychologists Find that Nice People Are More Likely to Hurt You People who are agreeable are also more likely to make destructive choices, if they think doing so will help them conform to social expectations. That's the finding of psychologists, who suggest that disagreeable, ornery people may be more helpful than we think. Researchers recently conducted a version of Stanley Milgram's famous obedience experiments, where people were asked by doctors to "shock" others until they died. Only later did they discover they people they'd "killed" were just actors. A surprising number of otherwise kindly people "killed" others, just because they'd been given orders. People with more agreeable, conscientious personalities are more likely to make harmful choices. In these new obedience experiments, people with more social graces were the ones who complied with the experimenter's wishes and delivered electric shocks they believed could harm an innocent person. By contrast, people with more contrarian, less agreeable personalities were more likely to refuse to hurt other people when told to do so.
  • 18. Proxemics Is the Science of Why You Shouldn't Stand So Close to Me Proxemics is the study of how people organize their social space. Without knowing it, we have very strict rules for who goes where in our culture. Although we don't always think about them, we know when someone's violating them. In the 1960s, a scientist called Edward Hall studied how close we let strangers come to us (no closer than twelve feet), social partners come to us (no closer than four feet), and how close we let close personal friends and relatives come to us (no closer than 1.5 feet). Get any closer than that and you're in intimate space - where you should either be kissing us at the end of a wonderful date or quietly advising us that we can plead the fifth at a congressional hearing. Hall noted, however, that these rules only applied to the proxemics of North Americans, and that not all cultures shared the same established distances when it came to personal space. This seating pattern is not declared. It's probably almost unconscious for most of the people doing the sitting. It's also so entirely ingrained in the culture. I can't even imagine what the reaction would be if there were one person sitting on a bus - and the second passenger to board the bus chose to sit down right next to them. I'd try it, but I worry that the first person would either clobber me or run.
  • 19. Look Angry If You Want People to Give In “Until we see someone, we don’t know what makes them sweat, or what makes them angry or happy,” Reed told Today. “ You can do a lot of things over the Internet now, but people still choose to have face-to-face meetings.” One caveat: It won’t work if people can tell you’re faking. If you’ve got a serious negotiation coming up, dredge up some real emotion or take an acting class. In two experiments, people were more willing to give into demands for a larger share of $1 when the person asking for more money looked angry. “If you come in with a scowl on your face, they’re going to take your threat more seriously,” study co-author Lawrence Ian Reed, a researcher at Harvard University, told Today. “You might think a poker face would be better in a negotiation. But in a bargaining situation when you make threats, your facial expression could add credibility to what you are saying.” During one of the experiments, 870 participants were given the task of splitting up $1 with someone else. If they couldn’t agree, neither would get anything. When an actress demanded 70 cents on the dollar with an angry expression, more people agreed than when she used a neutral expression. So far, the researchers have only done the experiment with women making the demands; reactions to a threatening male could be different. And, threatening looks only apply in face-to-face interactions, obviously.
  • 20. Blacks Seen as Darker During Tough Economic Times When the economy declines, racial minorities are hit the hardest. Although existing explanations for this effect focus on institutional causes, recent psychological findings suggest that scarcity may also alter perceptions of race in ways that exacerbate discrimination. We tested the hypothesis that economic resource scarcity causes decision makers to perceive African Americans as “Blacker” and that this visual distortion elicits disparities in the allocation of resources. Studies 1 and 2 demonstrated that scarcity altered perceptions of race, lowering subjects’ psychophysical threshold for seeing a mixed-race face as “Black” as opposed to “White.” In studies 3 and 4, scarcity led subjects to visualize African American faces as darker and more “stereotypically Black,” compared with a control condition. When presented to naïve subjects, face representations produced under scarcity elicited smaller allocations than control-condition representations. Together, these findings introduce a novel perceptual account for the proliferation of racial disparities under economic scarcity.
  • 21. Does Breaking Rules Make You Cool? Do consumers think it’s cool when a brands breaks a rule? It all depends on the rule, researchers report in the Journal of Consumer Research. “We reasoned that brands could become cool by breaking rules that seemed unnecessary or unfair, but not by breaking legitimate rules,” the authors wrote. Studies backed up their hypothesis: In one experiment, researchers asked participants to gauge their reaction to an ad that either advocated breaking or following a dress code. When the ad was accompanied by text that gave a legitimate reason for the dress code (honoring veterans), the brand did not seem cooler. But when participants read that the same dress code existed for an illegitimate reason (honoring a corrupt dictator), breaking the rule did help the brand seem cooler. There’s one caveat, however: it’s nearly impossible to appear cool to everyone. While one group may tolerate breaking traditional dress code rules, for example, it might be seen as deviant by another group. The researchers defined coolness by autonomy. Brands may do well in upping their coolness quotient by highlighting their uniqueness, the researchers suggested. “Collectively, our studies find that coolness is a subjective, positive trait perceived in people, brands, products, and trends that are autonomous in an appropriate way,” the authors wrote.
  • 22. The Science of Misheard Song Lyrics There is an actual official term for when you hear "excuse me while I kiss the sky" in Jimi Hendrix's "Purple Haze" as "excuse me while I kiss this guy." Your meaningful misheard lyrics are called "mondegreens," and their study can have real psychological significance. These little misunderstandings are common, but most people don't know that there is an official title for them. It came from a popular essay by writer Sylvia Wright The lyrics that defy cliché and break new ground are most likely to get misunderstood. "Excuse me, while I kiss this guy" might have still been outré in the 1960s when "Purple Haze" was written, but it was still more familiar than kissing the sky. We cobble together a semi-plausible lyric because we lack the experience to understand the real one. The people who are most likely to do this are the ones most lacking in experience. Children group words together, the way they hear them, in a stream of continuous syllables. They assume the meaning of "donzerly" will come later, when they hear a few more examples of the word. We enunciate for small babies, but as children grow, they are expected to pick up individual words, many of which they've never been exposed to, in a stream of noise. Language learners also have difficulty distinguishing one word from another, which can run them into real trouble in business or medical settings. Many researchers have found that mondegreens tend not to travel alone. Once people lose understanding of a sentence, they lose context as well, causing them to "hear" words that only resemble the actual words being uttered. People, especially adult English learners, are desperately trying to regain the thread of meaning, and make order out of a chaos of sounds. Eventually they trick themselves into hearing something that the recognize, even if it doesn't make sense. What most people need, scientists find, is familiar points where they can get their bearings, and enter back into the thread of the conversation. If they can't get regular familiar points to orient themselves in a stream of sound, mondegreens will take over and give them fake points of familiarity.
  • 23. The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to Consume, Consume, Consume Diderot Gets a Bathrobe Denis Diderot was a philosopher during the Enlightenment, which meant he should have been above such petty things as consumerism. On the other hand, he was also an art critic, and so liked beauty. That might have led to the situation he described in his essay, "Regrets on Parting With My Old Dressing Gown.“ He had an old beat-up thing that he wore around his small apartment. One day friend gave him a present of a beautiful scarlet dressing gown. Diderot loved it, but felt it was out of place among his cheap old furniture. He replaced his straw chair with a leather armchair. He replaced his desk, and the prints on his walls. Then he started replacing his regular clothes. The tale ends with Diderot in debt and disconsolate, working to maintain his beautiful room. He used to be the "master" of his possessions, but now he is the "slave" of a dressing gown. The essay is fictional, and the sentiment is pure romanticism. Don't consider your physical surroundings! Advance your mind and let go of the physical. We understand Diderot's ideals, but we also understand his experiences. Who among us hasn't come home with a beautiful new possession, placed it in their home, looked around, and thought, "This place is a dump."
  • 24. The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to Consume, Consume, Consume Just noticing the grubbiness of your regular possessions, when put next to something extraordinarily nice, isn't enough for you to fall under the spell of the Diderot Effect. To go full Diderot, according to sociologists, we need to start identifying with our possessions. Unfortunately, we tend to do that already. Most of us have picked up a piece of clothing, or sat on a piece of furniture, and thought, "No, this isn't me." How could that be? It's just a functional object. But consumption doesn't work that way. Even the most dressed-down of us uses our clothes to convey a picture of who we are. We identify ourselves using our possessions. When we do that, we don't just want high quality. In fact, many people will reject high quality. We want unity. We want to present a coherent whole. This can be a big problem for any companies that want us to buy things. No matter how good a product they offer, if it falls outside of the consumer's ideas of the unity of their lives and their looks, they will resist purchasing it. On the other hand, once we own one thing that stands out, that doesn't fit our current sense of unity, we go on a rampage trying to reconstruct ourselves. Either we throw away the luxury item, or we start to upgrade ourselves. Few people throw away the luxury item. Most people start replacing the other things in their lives. This involves a lot of money
  • 25. The Diderot Effect Makes You Want to Consume, Consume, Consume How to Use the Diderot Effect Naturally, there's a lot of focus on how to get people to take that first step out of their identity. Any good that's considered outside the pattern of someone's regular purchases is called a "departure good." Companies market hard to get people to make that first move, but they also want to know how to keep people Dideroting. How does one sample purchase, one luxury, become a whole lifestyle? According to research journals, the key is to make the item not a purchase, but a replacement. You're not buying a soap dispenser, you're replacing your nasty old bathroom soap with a nice-looking, efficient soap dispenser. Are you aware that it can come as part of a set? And that that matches a shower curtain? You're not buying a new pair of shoes, you're replacing an old pair with something simple and timeless - that you can't wear with jeans so get some nice skirts or slacks. Much of what we buy today is a kind of a taster product for a complete lifestyle. People aren't supposed to be springing for one big luxury purchase, they are meant to be buying better selves. That takes repeat business.
  • 26. Want to be happier? Skip the small talk. We all know those people. Maybe you’re one of them. The person who manages, despite all the stress and work it takes to get through a normal day, to always be happy. How do they do it? What is their secret? Although it’s likely that genetics play a major role in determining one’s happiness, it’s also thought that behavior can help (or hinder) you on your path to bliss. These scientists asked whether the content of peoples’ conversation is related to their overall happiness. They recorded participants’ conversations during daily life, and sure enough, they found that people who had deeper conversations tended to be happier than those who spent time on small talk.
  • 27. Scientists finally explain why your grandma will never find “Borat” funny Identifying social gaffes is important for maintaining relationships. Older adults are less able than young to discriminate between socially appropriate and inappropriate behavior in video clips. One open question is how these social appropriateness ratings relate to potential age differences in the perception of what is actually funny or not. In the present study, young, middle-aged, and older adults were equally able to discriminate between appropriate and inappropriate social behavior in a diverse set of clips relevant across age groups. However, young and middle-aged adults rated the gaffe clips as funnier than control clips and young adults smiled more during the inappropriate clips than the control clips. Older adults did not show this pattern, suggesting that they did not find the inappropriate clips funny. Additionally, young adults endorsed a more aggressive humor style than middle-aged and older adults and aggressive humor style endorsement mediated age differences in social appropriateness ratings. Results are discussed in terms of possible mechanisms such as cohort differences in humor and developmental prioritization of certain humor styles, as well as the importance of investigating age differences in both abilities and preferences
  • 28. Coffee Drinkers Have Trouble Talking About Emotions? Alexithymia refers to difficulties with identifying, describing, and regulating one's own emotions. This trait dimension has been linked to risky or harmful use of alcohol and illicit drugs; however, the most widely used psychoactive drug in the world, caffeine, has not been examined previously in relation to alexithymia. The present study assessed 106 male and female university students aged 18-30 years on their caffeine use in relation to several traits, including alexithymia. The 18 participants defined as alexithymic based on their Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) scores reported consuming nearly twice as much caffeine per day as did non-alexithymic or borderline alexithymic participants. They also scored significantly higher than controls on indices of frontal lobe dysfunction as well as anxiety symptoms and sensitivity to punishment. In a hierarchical linear regression model, sensitivity to punishment negatively predicted daily caffeine intake, suggesting caffeine avoidance by trait-anxious individuals. Surprisingly, however, TAS-20 alexithymia scores positively predicted caffeine consumption. Possible reasons for the positive relationship between caffeine use and alexithymia are discussed, concluding that this outcome is tentatively consistent with the hypo-arousal model of alexithymia.
  • 29. Could Diet Sodas Be Making Us Fatter? The artificial sweeteners in “diet” beverages, thought to help people trim their waistlines, may be having the opposite effect. A new study reveals that three of the leading artificial sweeteners produce an increase in blood-sugar levels in both mice and humans, by disrupting the balance of helpful gut bacteria. High blood-sugar levels, in turn, are the telltale sign of glucose intolerance, a condition which can evolve into diabetes and metabolic disease. To be clear, researchers aren’t certain how artificial sweeteners and gut microbes interact to produce glucose intolerance. Furthermore, not everyone in the small human trial experienced the same reaction. Many things influence our metabolism, and some people may be more sensitive than others to artificial sweeteners, as Forbes reports.
  • 30. Shut up and pet me! Dogs prefer petting to vocal praise It’s probably no surprise that dogs like to be petted. But do they prefer petting over other types of attention? Here, two scientists from the University of Florida tested whether dogs would prefer to be petted or given vocal praise, and whether it mattered if the petting/praise came from an owner or a stranger. Turns out that dogs love pets, regardless of who is doing the petting, and they never seem to get tired of being petted. Interestingly, a previous study by the same authors found that dogs do like one thing even more than petting: food. Perhaps a future study will determine where chasing their tails ranks on the list?
  • 31. Ignore IQ Tests: Your Level of Intelligence Is Not Fixed for Life Those who hang dearly onto the notion that IQ is fixed for life have managed to ignore decades of published research in the field of applied behavior analysis. This has reported very large IQ gains in children with autism who have been exposed to early intensive behavioral interventions once they have been diagnosed with learning difficulties. Another 2009 Norwegian study examined the effects of an increase in the duration of compulsory schooling in Norway in the 1960s which lengthened the time in education for Norwegians by two years. The researchers used records of cognitive ability taken by the military to calculate the IQ of each individual in the study. They found that IQ had increased by 3.7 points for every extra year of education received. More recent studies by John Jonides and his colleagues at the University of Michigan reported improvements in objective measures of intelligence for those who practiced a brain-training task called the “n-back task” – a kind of computerized memory test. My own research, in the field of relational frame theory, has shown that understanding relations between words, such as “more than,” “less than” or “opposite” is crucial for our intellectual development. One recent pilot study showed that we can considerably raise standard IQ scores by training children in relational language skills tasks over a period of months. Again, this finding challenges the idea that intelligence is fixed for life. even exceed them.
  • 32. Research Shows that Seven Hours of Sleep Might Be the Sweet Spot The Wall Street Journal looked at a few different studies. Here's a summation of just a few of them: "The lowest mortality and morbidity is with seven hours," said Shawn Youngstedt, a professor in the College of Nursing and Health Innovation at Arizona State University Phoenix. "Eight hours or more has consistently been shown to be hazardous," says Dr. Youngstedt, who researches the effects of oversleeping... ...Daniel F. Kripke, an emeritus professor of psychiatry at the University of California San Diego, tracked over a six-year period data on 1.1 million people who participated in a large cancer study. People who reported they slept 6.5 to 7.4 hours had a lower mortality rate than those with shorter or longer sleep. The study, published in the Archives of General Psychiatry in 2002, controlled for 32 health factors, including medications... ...A study in the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience last year used data from users of the cognitive-training website Lumosity. Researchers looked at the self-reported sleeping habits of about 160,000 users who took spatial-memory and matching tests and about 127,000 users who took an arithmetic test. They found that cognitive performance increased as people got more sleep, reaching a peak at seven hours before starting to decline.
  • 33. Scientists use MRI to measure precisely how your butt deforms when you sit down. RESULTS: MRIs indicated a marked decrease in muscle thickness under the ischial tuberosity during loaded sitting. This change in thickness resulted from a combination of muscle displacement and distortion. The gluteus and hamstrings overlapped beneath the pelvis in an unloaded condition, enveloping the ischial tuberosity. But the overlap was removed under load. The hamstrings moved anteriorly, while the gluteus moved posterior-laterally. Under load, neither muscle was directly beneath the apex of the ischial tuberosity. Furthermore, there was a change in muscle shape, particularly posterior to the peak of the ischial tuberosity. CONCLUSION: The complex deformation of buttocks tissue seen in this case study may help explain the inconsistent results reported in finite element models. 3D imaging of the seated buttocks provides a unique opportunity to study the actual buttocks response to sitting.”
  • 34. The Science Behind Why You Never Really Leave High School You also store your most vivid memories during this time (from ages 15 to 25), which can make traumatic experiences all the more traumatizing. In other words, you are already poised biologically to be deeply impressed by experiences around you at this time while, simultaneously, you form your first sense of identity, and then you’re thrown into a hard, judgmental place. Oof. The insecurities we form in high school are often the ones we carry with us later, even when those no longer have any basis in reality (if they ever did at all — further in the article they discuss just how skewed teenagers perception of other’s feelings towards them are).
  • 35. Scientists figured out how shrooms open your mind The study of rapid changes in brain dynamics and functional connectivity (FC) is of increasing interest in neuroimaging. Brain states departing from normal waking consciousness are expected to be accompanied by alterations in the aforementioned dynamics. In particular, the psychedelic experience produced by psilocybin (a substance found in “magic mushrooms”) is characterized by unconstrained cognition and profound alterations in the perception of time, space and selfhood. Considering the spontaneous and subjective manifestation of these effects, we hypothesize that neural correlates of the psychedelic experience can be found in the dynamics and variability of spontaneous brain activity fluctuations and connectivity, measurable with functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). Fifteen healthy subjects were scanned before, during and after intravenous infusion of psilocybin and an inert placebo. Blood-Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) temporal variability was assessed computing the variance and total spectral power, resulting in increased signal variability bilaterally in the hippocampi and anterior cingulate cortex. Changes in BOLD signal spectral behavior (including spectral scaling exponents) affected exclusively higher brain systems such as the default mode, executive control, and dorsal attention networks. A novel framework enabled us to track different connectivity states explored by the brain during rest. This approach revealed a wider repertoire of connectivity states post-psilocybin than during control conditions. Together, the present results provide a comprehensive account of the effects of psilocybin on dynamical behavior in the human brain at a macroscopic level and may have implications for our understanding of the unconstrained, hyper-associative quality of consciousness in the psychedelic state
  • 36. Want to improve your cognitive function? Try some whole body vibration! Have you ever seen those old movies of people using body vibration machines for “exercise” (see photo)? Well, apparently the principle behind those machines isn’t total BS. Although whole body vibration (WBV) might not make you lose weight or get in better shape, it can actually provide some of the same brain-stimulating benefits as exercise. More specifically, as shown by this study, WBV can improve one’s attention and inhibition (the ability to tune out irrelevant stimuli). The authors hypothesize that WBV improves these aspects of cognitive function by producing similar physiological responses as exercise, including increased oxygen uptake and heart rate. I’ll shake on that!
  • 37. The price of love? Losing two of your closest friends Research reveals that, on average, having a new romantic partner pushes out two close friends from your inner circle In a separate study, Dunbar's team looked at how men and women maintained friendships on the social networking website Facebook. They found that women's Facebook friends were more often friends from everyday life that they spent time with, while men tended to collect as many friends as they could, even if they hardly knew them. "Boys seem to be in a competition to see who can have the most Faccebook friends and that could be a form of mate advertisting. One of the cues women use for male quality as a mate is the number of other girls chasing them, so signing up lots of girls as Facebook friends seems to be a good idea," said Dunbar. Falling in love comes at the cost of losing close friends, because romantic partners absorb time that would otherwise be invested in platonic relationships, researchers say. A new partner pushes out two close friends on average, leaving lovers with a smaller inner circle of people they can turn to in times of crisis, a study found. Previous research by Dunbar's group has shown that people typically have five very close relationships – that is, people whom they would turn to if they were in emotional or financial trouble. "If you go into a romantic relationship, it costs you two friends. Those who have romantic relationships, instead of having the typical five 'core set' of relationships only have four. And of those, one is the new person who's come into their life," said Dunbar.
  • 38. How Come We're So Bad at Buying Stuff? http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/how-come-were-so-bad-at-buying-stuff-video- 140619.htm We've all enjoyed a little retail therapy -- or thought we did! -- but why does buyer's remorse so often set in? And why do we seem to lose all control over logic when that next big, shiny purchase calls our name?
  • 39. What Makes Cursive So Good For Your Brain - Video http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/what-makes-cursive- so-good-for-your-brain-video-140612.htm
  • 40. The early bird catches the worm… or at least the boss’s good opinion In this research, we draw from the stereotyping literature to suggest that supervisor ratings of job performance are affected by employees' start times-the time of day they first arrive at work. Even when accounting for total work hours, objective job performance, and employees' self-ratings of conscientiousness, we find that a later start time leads supervisors to perceive employees as less conscientious. These perceptions in turn cause supervisors to rate employees as lower performers. In addition, we show that supervisor chronotype acts as a boundary condition of the mediated model. Supervisors who prefer eveningness (i.e., owls) are less likely to hold negative stereotypes of employees with late start times than supervisors who prefer morningness (i.e., larks). Taken together, our results suggest that supervisor ratings of job performance are susceptible to stereotypic beliefs based on employees' start times.
  • 41. Is Laughter the Best Medicine? We all want to believe that laughter is good for what ails us, but the science backing that up is thin. Most studies have been small and have relied on self-reported assessments. "A good belly laugh leads to the release of endorphins from the brain," says Michael Miller, director of the Center for Preventive Cardiology at the University of Maryland Medical Center in Baltimore. That release sets off a cascade of heart-healthy biological events. Endorphins, pleasure-inducing neurotransmitters, activate receptors on the surface of the endothelium, the layer of flat cells lining blood vessels. That leads to the release of nitric oxide, which widens blood vessels—increasing blood flow, lessening inflammation, inhibiting platelet clumping, and reducing the formation of cholesterol plaque. A 2005 study by Miller measured the blood flow of 20 volunteers before and after watching a funny movie and a sad movie. After the sad movie, blood flow was more restricted in 14 of the 20 viewers. But after the movie that made them laugh, average blood flow increased by 22 percent. "The best laugh is one that brings tears to our eyes," says Miller, author of Heal Your Heart: The Positive Emotions Prescription to Prevent and Reverse Heart Disease, scheduled for publication by Rodale Press in September. His prescription: at least 30 minutes of exercise at least three times a week—and 15 minutes of daily laughter. But her study of 33 healthy women, published in 2003, showed that those who laughed at a humorous movie had higher levels of natural killer cell activity, which increased their ability to fight off disease. However, the effect was seen only in the subjects who laughed out loud, not in those who quietly watched the comedy. A study in Japan that also used laboratory findings found that laughter could improve anti-inflammatory factors in the blood of people who have rheumatoid arthritis.
  • 42. Why Molly Is Especially Deadly at Summer Music Festivals Eugene A. Kiyatkin, the study’s lead author and a scientist at the behavioral neuroscience branch of NIDA, zeroed in on the physiological mechanisms involved in fatal hyperthermia (overheating of the body to the point of death). “What’s new about this study is that it focuses on MDMA’s effects on brain temperature and clarifies the physiological mechanisms involved,” he tells The Daily Beast. “By recording data from the brain, muscle, and skin, we were able to show that MDMA raises heat in the brain by increasing brain activity as well as causing a strong, lasting constriction of peripheral blood vessels that prevents the normal dissipation of excess body heat to the external environment. These combined effects may be enhanced in conditions when brain and body heat production are naturally increased—such as social interaction—or when shedding excess heat is difficult, as it is in a warm environment.” When the body undergoes fatal hyperthermia—internal temperature rising to the point of brain damage—it’s fluid in the brain that leads the march to death. After MDMA raises heat production and constricts blood vessels, the internal body temperature increases, damaging brain cells irreparably. While this increase in brain temperature is incredibly destructive, it’s not the actual killer. “The main mechanism leading to death from psychostimulant drugs appears to be leaking of the brain-blood barrier— which keeps most chemicals in the blood out of the brain—and water accumulation in the brain,” says Kiyatakin. It’s for this reason that concertgoers thinking hydration is the key to staying safe (“if you just drink enough water you’ll be fine”) is so dangerous. “The excessive use of liquids often used by people at dance clubs could be problematic if they take MDMA,” says Kiyatakin. “In addition to its other effects, it inhibits both sweating and urination, which expel water from the body. These factors could also contribute to dangerous water accumulation in the brain.”
  • 43. We May Finally Know Why Sleep Improves Memory The results were stark: Gan and his colleagues found that the sleep-deprived mice sprouted significantly fewer dendritic spines than those that were permitted to rest, and the rate of spine formation was correlated with the degree of task improvement. Growth was shown to be most dramatic during the slow-wave, non-REM stage of sleep. What's more, the benefits of sleep seem to carry on well after the mice woke up, with roughly 5% of new spines in the motor cortex developing in the 24 hours after the mice awoke. The mice that slept were also more likely to retain the spines they grew. In some circumstances, it seems sleep could in fact lead to the growth of new synapses. The researchers went on to demonstrate that the neuronal branches involved in the rod-balancing task were reactivated during this period of slow-wave sleep. Neuroal reactivation during sleep has been observed in the past, but Gan's team took it a step further by blocking the reactivation. When they did, it impeded the formation of new spines, suggesting that reactivation plays some role in stabilizing the dendritic spines sprouted during sleep. Finally, Gan tells io9 that one of the study's most surprising findings was not directly related to sleep. In a previous study, Gan and his colleagues used the skull-window technique to demonstrate that teaching mice to balance atop the rod led to the formation of new spines along dendrites in the motor cortex. The present study corroborates those findings, but it also shows that teaching the mice a new motor task (for instance, balancing on the rod as it spins in the reverse direction) caused dendritic spines to sprout on an entirely new dendritic branch – i.e., a branch distinct from the one that shot out spines in reaction to learning to balance on a forward-spinning rod. In other words: learning a new motor skill won't cause dendritic spines to appear just anywhere. Rather, the team's findings suggest that synaptic change in the mammalian brain occurs in a site-specific fashion.
  • 44. Cocaine addiction linked to brain abnormalities Scans showed cocaine users had enlarged grey matter in areas of the brain associated with processing reward. Specifically, the amount of grey matter in the orbitofrontal cortex was reduced in people with cocaine addiction, an area involved in decision-making and goal-directed behaviour. Other affected areas included the insula, an area of the brain involved in feedback processing, learning and feelings of cravings. The grey matter in the anterior cingulate, involved in emotional processing and being attentive, was also reduced. In contrast, a region deep in the brain associated with reward processing, attention and motor movements - the caudate nucleus - was enlarged in subjects who were addicted to the drug. This could explain why those subjects were more prone to addiction but the scientists cannot be sure whether the enlargement is a result of cocaine use. Laurence John Reed, a clinical senior lecturer in addiction neurobiology at Imperial College London, said the "most impressive" results were the basic comparison of controls and stimulant users, which showed how parts of the brain remodel themselves in response to drugs. "This is a striking and visual example of how addictive stimulant use can result in adaptation of very important brain systems which have a direct correlates with behaviour – specifically inattention, impulsivity and compulsivity – and really does underline why we need a much better neurobiological understanding of the processes involved." Ersche said that, though she found links between brain structure and cocaine use,her research was not conclusive on which came first. "At the moment, correlation shows me a direct relationship - but I don't know which direction the relationship is. Has this been caused by cocaine, or are people who have this abnormality more vulnerable?"
  • 45. Why teenagers can't concentrate: too much grey matter The scans revealed an unexpected level of activity in the prefrontal cortex, a large region at the front of the brain involved in decision-making and multitasking. This indicated that the brain was working less effectively than that of an adult. "We knew that the prefrontal cortex of young children functioned in this chaotic way but we didn't realise it continued until the late 20s or early 30s," said Dr Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, who led the study. "What we discovered was that the part of the brain needed to complete this sort of process is still very much developing throughout adolescence. This means it continues to do a lot of needless work when making these sorts of decisions." Chaotic thought patterns are a result, she said, of teenagers' brains containing too much grey matter – the cell bodies and connections which carry messages within the brain. As we age, the amount of grey matter in our brains decreases. "What our research has shown is that there is simply too much going on in the brains of adolescents," said Blakemore. "The result is that their brain energy and resources are wasted and their decision-making process negatively affected." Adults, on the other hand, have less grey matter, said Blakemore. "This means that neural transmissions travel more efficiently between brain cells, so the brain works more effectively."
  • 46. Do You Feel Like You're Not Sleeping, Even When You Are? In last week's issue of New Scientist, Ann Finkbeiner has a terrific article about the people who suffer from pseudoinsomnia, and a handful of scientists who are trying to figure out what their brains have in common. When they used conventional methods of analyzing the brain wave patterns of sleepers, it appeared that these pseudoinsomniacs were no different than any other sleeper. But then they tried something new. They analyzed the readouts of brain waves using an algorithm that's normally used for spectral analysis in physics. And that's when they began to see a pattern — a pattern that suggested these peoples' sleep cycles were being interrupted by brain wave patterns associated with fear, anxiety, and wakefulness. What's fascinating is that similar patterns of alpha, beta and gamma waves show up in the brain wave patterns of people who suffer from chronic pain and anxiety as well. It's possible that this "always-on" brain pattern leads to a variety of nervous symptoms, including being unable to get the benefits of a healthy night's sleep.
  • 47. Do Porn Watchers Have Smaller Brains? If you watch a lot of porn, your brain may show it, new research suggests. German researchers looked at the brains of 64 men between the ages of 21 and 45 and found that one brain region (the striatum, linked to reward processing), was smaller in the brains of porn watchers, and that a specific part of the same region is also less activated when exposed to more pornography. The study, published in JAMA Psychiatry, also showed that the connection between the striatum and prefrontal cortex seemed to be weaker in brains of men who watched porn. “Basically everything that people do very frequently can shape their brain structure and function,” Simone Kühn, the study's lead author from the Max Planck Institute for Human Development told Reuters. Still, the study doesn't confirm whether watching porn causes the changes, or whether people with a certain brain type are inherently more apt to tune into X-rated content. But because the areas of the brain that appear to be affected are linked to rewards and motivation and decision making, some have suggested that porn watchers may be lazier and poor decision-makers. "Everything is going to be bad in excess and it's probably not terrible in moderation," Dr. Gregory Tau of the Columbia University/New York State Psychiatric Institute, who was not involved in the study, told Reuters.
  • 48. The Science Behind Caffeine's Productivity- Enhancing Effects 1. Caffeine comes into your body whole. 2. Then the caffeine molecule enters your liver, where enzymes cut off three methyl groups to form three more small molecules. 3. The molecules in question are theobromine, paraxanthine, and theophylline. 4. Together with the original caffeine molecule, they heighten your brain activity, get nutrients flowing, increase your athleticism, and boost your focus.
  • 49. Scientists Discover Area Of Brain Responsible For Loving Johnny Cash Music is among all cultures an important part of the live of most people. Music has psychological benefits and may generate strong emotional and physiological responses. Recently, neuroscientists have discovered that music influences the reward circuit of the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), even when no explicit reward is present. In this clinical case study, we describe a 60-year old patient who developed a sudden and distinct musical preference for Johnny Cash following deep brain stimulation (DBS) targeted at the NAcc. This case report substantiates the assumption that the NAcc is involved in musical preference, based on the observation of direct stimulation of the accumbens with DBS. It also shows that accumbens DBS can change musical preference without habituation of its rewarding properties
  • 50. Does the Name-Letter Effect Prove That You're Soooo Into Yourself? And the spousal studies? Similar, or matching, names are also influenced by culture, but there's a more basic reason marrying spouses tend to have the same last name. A surprising amount of studies neglected to check whether the bride changed her name before the ceremony. So if two Taylors were wed, one of them might have had a different name, but changed it during the ramp up to the wedding. So while we do like ourselves enough to get sentimental about our names, we probably aren't crazy enough to choose our entire mode of life depending on our name. Studies found that woman found the letters of their own initials to be feminine and men found their initials to be masculine. My name, and my initials, are me, we have collectively decided. And it looks like people feel pretty good about themselves. Extended surveys found that people tend to settle, disproportionately, in towns that are alliterative, in jobs that are alliterative, and with spouses who are alliterative. Some people even choose towns and spouses with their own names. How much of an inducement is in a name? Some people, for example internet writers, might find alliterative spousal names so insufferably twee that they'd change their own name to get away from such an atrocity. (Seriously, that was only okay with the Roosevelts.) Others, like scientists, think that gravitating towards our initials is a sign of intrinsic self-esteem, or egotism. We want more of ourselves.
  • 51. Which matters more for attractiveness: a woman’s face or body? “Women’s faces and bodies are both thought to provide cues to women’s age, health, fertility, and personality. To gain a stronger understanding of how these cues are utilized, we investigated the degree to which ratings of women’s faces and bodies independently predicted ratings of women’s full-body attractiveness. Women came into the lab not knowing they would be photographed. In Study 1 (N = 84), we photographed them in their street clothes; in Study 2 (N = 74), we photographed women in a solid-colored two-piece swimsuit that revealed their body shape, body size, and breast size. We cropped each woman’s original photo into an additional face-only photo and body-only photo; then, independent sets of raters judged women’s pictures. When dressed in their original clothes, women’s face-only ratings were better independent predictors of full-body attractiveness ratings than were their body-only ratings. When cues displayed in women’s bodies were made conspicuous by swimsuits, ratings of faces and bodies were similarly strong predictors of full-body attractiveness ratings. Moreover, women’s body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio were tied to ratings of women’s body attractiveness, with waist-to-hip ratio more important among women wearing swimsuits than among women wearing their original clothes. These results suggest that perceivers attend to cues of women’s health, fertility, and personality to the extent that they are visible.”
  • 52. NCBI ROFL: Surprise! Men vote for the hotter female candidate. Here we reveal gender biases in the intuitive heuristics that voters use when deciding whom to vote for in major political elections. Our findings underscore the impact of gender and physical appearance on shaping voter decision-making and provide novel insight into the psychological foundations underlying the political gender gap.”
  • 53. NCBI ROFL: The presence of an attractive woman elevates testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. “The authors report a field experiment with skateboarders that demonstrates that physical risk taking by young men increases in the presence of an attractive female. This increased risk taking leads to more successes but also more crash landings in front of a female observer. Mediational analyses suggest that this increase in risk taking is caused in part by elevated testosterone levels of men who performed in front of the attractive female. In addition, skateboarders’ risk taking was predicted by their performance on a reversal-learning task, reversal-learning performance was disrupted by the presence of the attractive female, and the female’s presence moderated the observed relationship between risk taking and reversal learning. These results suggest that men use physical risk taking as a sexual display strategy, and they provide suggestive evidence regarding possible hormonal and neural mechanisms.”
  • 54. NCBI ROFL: Beauty week: Blond, busty, skinny waitresses get bigger tips. “Waitresses completed an on-line survey about their physical characteristics, self-perceived attractiveness and sexiness, and average tips. The waitresses’ self-rated physical attractiveness increased with their breast sizes and decreased with their ages, waist-to-hip ratios, and body sizes. Similar effects were observed on self-rated sexiness, with the exception of age, which varied with self-rated sexiness in a negative, quadratic relationship rather than a linear one. Moreover, the waitresses’ tips varied with age in a negative, quadratic relationship, increased with breast size, increased with having blond hair, and decreased with body size. These findings, which are discussed from an evolutionary perspective, make several contributions to the literature on female physical attractiveness. First, they replicate some previous findings regarding the determinants of female physical attractiveness using a larger, more diverse, and more ecologically valid set of stimuli than has been studied before. Second, they provide needed evidence that some of those determinants of female beauty affect interpersonal behaviors as well as attractiveness ratings. Finally, they indicate that some determinants of female physical attractiveness do not have the same effects on overt interpersonal behavior (such as tipping) that they have on attractiveness ratings. This latter contribution highlights the need for more ecologically valid tests of evolutionary theories about the determinants and consequences of female beauty.”
  • 55. NCBI ROFL: Why poor, hungry men prefer bigger breasts. It has been suggested human female breast size may act as signal of fat reserves, which in turn indicates access to resources. Based on this perspective, two studies were conducted to test the hypothesis that men experiencing relative resource insecurity should perceive larger breast size as more physically attractive than men experiencing resource security. In Study 1, 266 men from three sites in Malaysia varying in relative socioeconomic status (high to low) rated a series of animated figures varying in breast size for physical attractiveness. Results showed that men from the low socioeconomic context rated larger breasts as more attractive than did men from the medium socioeconomic context, who in turn perceived larger breasts as attractive than men from a high socioeconomic context. Study 2 compared the breast size judgements of 66 hungry versus 58 satiated men within the same environmental context in Britain. Results showed that hungry men rated larger breasts as significantly more attractive than satiated men. Taken together, these studies provide evidence that resource security impacts upon men’s attractiveness ratings based on women’s breast size.
  • 56. More Evidence That Longevity Depends on Your State of Mind Of course, correlation is not causation. Having a sense of purpose isn't what's making people live longer. Rather, having a sense of purpose can give rise to healthy habits while diminishing a number of risk factors; setting large and long-term goals serves as a protective shield. For example, people with clearly defined goals may be less apt to abuse alcohol and drugs, which can be seen as a distraction, escape, or a barrier to achieving one's goals. A sense of purpose may also result in a more socially engaged life, particularly if helping people is a key motivator; studies show that social alienation is risk factor en par with excessive smoking and alcoholism. We all know that having goals is important, but a joint US-Canadian study reveals that having a sense of purpose can affect our longevity. Remarkably, it doesn't matter how old we are or what we aspire to — as long as we have goals, we live longer. Psychologists have known for some time that a sense of purpose is a key indicator of healthy aging, including its potential for reducing mortality risk. But this new study, which now appears in Psychological Science, extends previous findings in two important ways. First, it shows that a sense of purpose is beneficial across a person's entire adult lifespan, and second, that mortality rates — and by inference health — can indeed be correlated with having a purpose in life. So a sense of purpose could be derived from a desire to climb the corporate ladder, writing a book, running for office, or improving one's performance in art or at the gym. These ambitions can also serve as stepping stones to other goals, such as financial stability and raising children. And in fact, the most frequently cited purposes had to do with helping other people or trying to improve the social structure.
  • 57. Smart Drugs Could Be Impairing The Brains Of Young People Authors looked at two stimulants in particular, methylphenidate and modafinil. Sold as Ritalin and Concerta to treat ADHD, methylphenidate works by increasing the level of neurotransmitters in the nervous system. Trials on rats have shown that young, developing brains are particularly sensitive to methylphenidate. Low dosages can reduce nerve activity, working memory, and the ability to quickly switch between tasks and behaviors. Altering glutamate function via the use of psychostimulants may impair behavioral flexibility, leading to the development and/or potentiation of addictive behaviors. Furthermore, dopamine and norepinephrine do not display linear effects; instead, their modulation of cognitive and neuronal function maps on an inverted-U curve. Healthy individuals run the risk of pushing themselves beyond optimal levels into hyperdopaminergic and hypernoradrenergic states, thus vitiating the very behaviors they are striving to improve.
  • 58. Are 'Lucky Streaks' Real? Science Says Yes Bettors are more likely to win once they're on a winning streak, for a precisely ironic reason. The study, published this month in the journal Cognition, also found that losses can breed more losses. After losing twice, the chances of winning decreased to 40 percent. After four losses, the chance of winning was 27 percent. After six duds, you have only a 23 percent chance of winning. The explanation: after each loss, gamblers on average choose bets that are less likely to turn out, apparently assuming that they are more likely to win than before-- and perhaps to make up their losses (although, on average, people gamble less after each loss). As you probably know, bets with a lower chance of winning have higher payouts. The idea that one is more likely to lose after winning, or more likely to win after losing, is known as the gambler's fallacy (in reality, all things being equal, one is just as likely to lose or win on any given bet, assuming one is betting on independent events that don't effect each other's outcomes, as is the case with the vast majority of sports bets). This stands in contrast to the "hot hand fallacy": that one is more likely to win while on a hot streak. Bettors apparently don't generally believe this to be true, or at least their behavior suggests they don't. "The result is ironic: Winners worried their good luck was not going to continue, so they selected safer odds," the researchers wrote. "By doing so, they became more likely to win. The losers expected the luck to turn, so they took riskier odds. However, this made them even more likely to lose. The gamblers’ fallacy created the hot hand." The study found that when a person wins a bet, they become increasingly likely to succeed after each win. The converse is also true: Once you lose a bet, you become progressively more likely to keep losing. The fascinating study looked at 565,915 sports bets made by 776 online gamblers in Europe and the United States, and found that, all things being equal, you're likely to win or lose 48 percent of the time (draws presumably account for the remaining 4 percent). After a single winning bid, the chance of winning a second goes up ever so slightly to 49 percent. But here's where things get interesting. After the second win, the chance of winning a third time increases to 57 percent. After that: 67 percent. Following a four-bet winning streak, the chances of scoring a fifth haul increase to 72 percent. The probability of a sixth win is then 75 percent, and finally, after six wins, bettors had a 76 percent chance of notching lucky No. 7.
  • 59. The Othello Error Makes You Sure Everyone is Lying Ever heard of Othello error? It's the unfortunate mindset that leads questioners to believe people are lying, even when they're telling the truth. It can cause them to use understandable coincidences as damning evidence, and getting rid of it can make us better at judging honesty. The innocent, but incriminating, coincidence is a staple of comedy. The heroine picks up a purse that's nearly identical to her own, and finds she's stolen her future mother-in-law's wallet. A hero hesitates to tell his boss he was at a proctology appointment because the woman he has a crush on is in the room, and gets so flushed and red-faced that the boss figures he must be guilty of something. The audience laughs because we've all been stuck in circumstances that make us look guilty when we're not, and then gotten so flustered during our explanations that we look even more guilty. Usually these circumstances are, at least in retrospect, comic. This kind of misunderstanding can also have dark results, as we see in dramatic works like Othello. Desdemona is the victim of villainous interference rather than mere coincidence, but her increasingly frantic attempts to correct the situation only make her look more guilty. She looks guilty because Othello suspects she's guilty. Once he starts looking at everything she does with suspicion, everything she does is suspicious.
  • 60. How 'Hyperpalatable' Foods Could Turn You Into A Food Addict Over a third of the global population is now overweight, and the percentages are increasing. Some neuroscientists have suggested that the rise of so-called "hyperpalatable foods" may partially explain the unprecedented rates of obesity. Eventually, the experience of eating impossibly delicious foods results in what Kessler describes as "conditioned hypereating." When we consume enjoyable sugary and fatty foods, it stimulates endorphins in our brains — chemicals that signal a pleasurable experience. In turn, and in Pavlovian fashion, these chemicals stimulate us to eat more of that type of food, while also calming us down and making us feel good. Conditioned hypereating sounds suspiciously similar to what we might call food addiction. And indeed, studies have shown that hyperpalatable foods may be capable of triggering an addictive process — one that's been postulated as a possible cause of the obesity epidemic. But is it fair or reasonable to categorize food — something we need to keep us alive — alongside such things as illicit drugs, alcohol, and gambling? Some scientists say yes. Last year, for example, neuroscientists from Connecticut College claimed that Oreo cookies are more addictive than cocaine. The researchers came to this conclusion after measuring a protein called c-Fos in the brains of rats. They found that the cookies activated more neurons in the accumbens — a region of the brain associated with pleasure, and studied for its role in addiction and reward-processing — than addictive substances like cocaine. Not surprisingly, the researchers were harshly criticized for suggesting that something as apparently benign as an Oreo cookie could be compared to a notorious party drug.
  • 61. Men Without Beards Could Soon Have An Evolutionary Advantage But the context did matter. When facial hair was rare among faces, beards and heavy stubble were rated about 20% more attractive. And when beards were common, clean-shaven faces enjoyed a similar bump, the team reports online today in Biology Letters. The effect on judgment was the same for men and women. “This study breaks new ground,” says Peter Frost, an anthropologist at the Interuniversity Centre for Aboriginal Studies and Research in Quebec City, Canada. Although previous studies have shown that people prefer novelty for certain traits, such as the color of clothing, this study shows “that the novelty effect applies not only to colors but also to other visible features [of the body],” he says. But hipsters shouldn’t let their beards get too gnarly. “There are certainly limits to this effect,” Frost says. “Something can be novel but also disgusting.” Negative frequency-dependent sexual selection maintains striking polymorphisms in secondary sexual traits in several animal species. Here, we test whether frequency of beardedness modulates perceived attractiveness of men's facial hair, a secondary sexual trait subject to considerable cultural variation. We first showed participants a suite of faces, within which we manipulated the frequency of beard thicknesses and then measured preferences for four standard levels of beardedness. Women and men judged heavy stubble and full beards more attractive when presented in treatments where beards were rare than when they were common, with intermediate preferences when intermediate frequencies of beardedness were presented. Likewise, clean-shaven faces were least attractive when clean-shaven faces were most common and more attractive when rare. This pattern in preferences is consistent with negative frequency-dependent selection.
  • 62. Over the Hill? Cognitive Speeds Peak at Age 24 Those grim “over the hill” party favors are often deployed ironically by those who want to razz their friends or partners when they turn 30 or 40. But it may be more honest than we care to admit: A new study suggests humans’ cognitive speed peaks at age 24, and that it’s a steady downhill descent from there. The study is limited by the fact that it only focused on video game players. But in analyzing a dataset of over 3,000 StarCraft 2 players between the ages of 16 and 44, researchers determined that in-game response times, or cognitive speed, peaked in players at 24 years of age The present study investigates age-related changes in cognitive motor performance through adolescence and adulthood in a complex real world task, the real-time strategy video game StarCraft 2. In this paper we analyze the influence of age on performance using a dataset of 3,305 players, aged 16-44, collected by Thompson, Blair, Chen & Henrey [1]. Using a piecewise regression analysis, we find that age-related slowing of within-game, self-initiated response times begins at 24 years of age. We find no evidence for the common belief expertise should attenuate domain-specific cognitive decline. Domain-specific response time declines appear to persist regardless of skill level. A second analysis of dual-task performance finds no evidence of a corresponding age-related decline. Finally, an exploratory analyses of other age-related differences suggests that older participants may have been compensating for a loss in response speed through the use of game mechanics that reduce cognitive load.
  • 63. Picking Sides: How Genes Help Us Decide Between Left and Right It may surprise you that left-handed women were found to be twice or more likely to develop premenopausal breast cancer than right-handers. And a few researchers believe this effect may be linked to exposure to certain chemicals in utero, affecting your genes and then setting the stage for both left-handedness and cancer susceptibility, thus opening up another probability of nurture changing nature. So why the predominance of righties? For our species, one of the most important tasks is communication, which is generally processed on the left side of the brain. And some scientists think that’s the reason why we’re right-side dominant, because, as you’ve probably heard, the left side of the brain generally controls the muscles on the right side of the body (which is why a stroke suffered on the left side of your brain is more likely to result in impairment to the arm and leg on the right side of the body). Discovering more of the biology behind handedness, tracing it to genetics, exposures, or both, could give us a lot more knowledge than simply whether we should line up our kids on the left or right side of the tee-ball batter’s box. That’s because left-handedness has also been associated with higher rates of dyslexia, schizophrenia, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, some mood disorders, and, as we’ve discussed, even cancer. Indeed, adding handedness into the mix has helped Danish researchers identify which children who had symptoms of ADHD at the age of eight (when, let’s face it, just about every kid is a little bit on the rambunctious side) would still have it at the age of 16.
  • 64. Can You Control Your Metabolism With Your Mind? Crum designed an experiment to investigate the link between what we think we're eating, how full we feel and how our bodies respond. First, she made some milkshakes. Then, she labeled each shake as either "Sensishake: Fat Free, Guilt Free, 104 Calories" or "Indulgence: Decadence You Deserve, 620 Calories." In reality, all the shakes were around 300 calories. Test subjects were asked to drink the milkshakes while nurses monitored their blood levels of ghrelin, a hormone that increases with hunger, decreases after a meal, and affects the rate of our metabolism. "Crum discovered that those who believed they were drinking the indulgent shake responded as if their bodies had eaten three times more," explains Spiegel. "So what people believed about their milkshake came true. If they thought it was fattening, they felt they'd eaten more and their digestion was affected. Their ghrelin levels dropped three times more." As the NPR video points out, Crum's findings suggest that while it may not matter as much as what we eat, how we think about what we eat could play a significant role in how our bodies process food. There's obviously something very appealing about that, but it warrants a good measure of skepticism. For one thing, Crum's study apparently lacked a control group – that is, a group of test subjects who were not intentionally deceived with either rhetoric or calorie counts.
  • 65. Extract Money From People Using the Ambiguity Effect Here's a quick game. I have a jar full of ninety marbles. There are thirty red marbles for sure, but there's a mix of black and white marbles, and I can't be sure how many of each is in there. You can choose between one of two games. The first game awards you a hundred dollars if you pick a red marble. The second game forces you to pick either black or white as the "winning color," and awards you a hundred dollars if you pick the marble of your choice. Most people will pick the red marble game. Knowing their exact odds of winning comforts them. They even pick the red game if it's explained to them that the games have an equal probability of winning them the hundred bucks. To see why the black and white game wins, let's change the game slightly. Instead of picking which marble color wins before you draw, you draw and then flip a coin to see which color wins. So if you've just drawn a black marble, the odds of winning with that marble are fifty percent - the likelihood of either side of a coin flip. And if you've just drawn a white marble, the odds of winning with that marble are fifty percent. Considering the overall odds of drawing either black or white are two-thirds, fifty percent of two thirds is one third - or exactly the odds of winning with a red marble. Why is this important? Because of their dislike of uncertainty, people will pay to move from an uncertain game to a more certain one. So you can either charge slightly more money for the red marble game, or work the crowd with a partner. People get assigned on game or the other, and your partner, with the more-desirable red marble game will trade their tickets for a cash bonus to people who don't like their odds (even though they should).
  • 66. Continuity fields, and why we miss subtle visual changes Continuity fields: Even when we fix our gaze on something, our eyes are actually shifting microscopically several times per second Our eyes are continuously bombarded by visual information – millions of colors, shapes and ever-changing motion – yet seeing never feels like work. Researchers have discovered one reason: our brains perform automatic visual smoothing over time. A study has found that our visual perception of things is influenced by what we saw up to 15 seconds ago. This helps create a stable environment, despite sacrificing some accuracy. It also means that what you see around you – that cup of coffee, the face of your co-worker, your computer screen – may be a time-averaged composite of now and the past. Fischer, who did the work as a doctoral student in the lab of visual scientist David Whitney, calls this filter a continuity field. For a real-world example of the phenomenon, think of a road sign on a rainy day. Despite the motion of hundreds of raindrops – and your retinas being flooded with visual fluctuations – you don't have to struggle too hard to read the sign. Your visual system is averaging over what is effectively noise. Fischer and Whitney also found that the filter seems to come into play only when we need it. Attention matters – past images had an influence if the subjects were paying attention to them, but not if they were peripheral or in a radically different location. And predictably, the influence of older images lessened the more time passed. Continuity fields are one type of visual insensitivity, but there are others. Take the failure to notice an obvious change, known as change blindness. One study had an experimenter ask a pedestrian for directions. Their conversation was then interrupted by a group of men walking between the two speakers and carrying a large, obstructive object. Behind that object, out of sight of the pedestrian, the initial experimenter was replaced by a different person. Despite the conversation partner being the central object in the scene, only half of pedestrians noticed the change. The rest continued giving directions as if nothing strange had happened.
  • 67. Continuity fields, and why we miss subtle visual changes "We're not very good at detecting changes in our environment if the object is something we wouldn't expect to change," Johnson said. In the real world, we wouldn't think a person we're talking to would spontaneously transform. Thus, our brains often don't waste energy trying to notice these types of shifts. Fischer suspects that our brains learn that the world follows certain rules – objects don't change location spontaneously, and little changes don't matter most of the time – and adaptation of the visual system follows suit. Visual scientist Michele Rucci of Boston University, not involved in the study, was surprised and intrigued by the existence of continuity fields. "We have this input to our retina that is continuously jumping, but yet the world seems stable," Rucci said. He noted that, even when we fix our gaze on something, our eyes are actually shifting microscopically several times per second. "Our perception of the world is very different than what the real input to the retina is."
  • 68. Change Blindness Change blindness is a surprising perceptual phenomenon that occurs when a change in a visual stimulus is introduced and the observer does not notice it. For example, observers often fail to notice major differences introduced into an image while it flickers off and on again. People's poor ability to detect changes has been argued to reflect fundamental limitations of human attention. Change blindness has become a highly researched topic and some have argued that it may have important practical implications in areas such as eyewitness testimony and distractions while driving. Additionally, further research stated that rather large changes will not be detected when they occur during saccadic movements of the eye. Another finding based on similar studies stated that a change was easily picked up on by participants when the eye was fixated on the point of change.[7] Therefore, the eye must be directly fixated on the area of change for it to be noticed. However, other research in the mid-1990s has indicated that individuals still have difficulty detecting change even when they are directly fixated on a particular scene. A study by Rensink, O’Regan, & Clarke demonstrated that change blindness can have an effect even if the eye was fixated on a scene. In this study, a picture was presented followed by a blank screen or “masking” stimulus, which was followed by the initial picture with a change. The masking stimulus almost acts like a saccadic movement of the eye which makes it significantly more difficult for individuals to detect the change.[6] This was a critical contribution to change blindness research because it demonstrated that a change can remain unnoticed with the smallest disruptions. Research on change blindness proceeded one step further into practical applications of this phenomenon. For example, there does not have to be a masking stimulus in order for individuals to miss a change in a scene. Individuals often take significantly longer to notice certain changes if there are a few small, high contrast shapes that are temporarily splattered over a picture.[8] This method for testing change blindness is called “mudsplashes”.[8] This method is particularly relevant to individuals driving in a car when there is a visual obstruction on the windshield. This obstruction may impair an individual’s ability to detect a change in their environment which could result in severe negative consequences while driving.
  • 69. Study proves that talking on your cell phone makes you act like an a**hole. Use of a cell phone reduces attention and increases response times. 62 people (30 men, 32 women) were confronted with a confederate wearing a large leg brace, who dropped a stack of magazines and feigned difficulty retrieving them. Among the 33 people who talked on their cell phones only 9% offered their help, whereas among the 29 people who did not talk on their cell phones, 72% offered help. The use of cell phones affects helping behavior.
  • 70. Why overheard cell phone conversations are extra annoying. Overhearing someone on a cell phone means hearing only half of a conversation–a “halfalogue.” We show that merely overhearing a halfalogue results in decreased performance on cognitive tasks designed to reflect the attentional demands of daily activities. By contrast, overhearing both sides of a cell-phone conversation or a monologue does not result in decreased performance. This may be because the content of a halfalogue is less predictable than both sides of a conversation. In a second experiment, we controlled for differences in acoustic factors between these types of overheard speech, establishing that it is the unpredictable informational content of halfalogues that results in distraction. Thus, we provide a cognitive explanation for why overheard cell-phone conversations are especially irritating: Less-predictable speech results in more distraction for a listener engaged in other tasks.”
  • 71. Need to Commit Something to Memory? Sleep On It Almost a century after scientists first began studying sleep, it’s clear that getting some z‘s is useful for far more than resting our bodies; it’s also necessary for retaining short- and long-term memories. These new findings could have important implications for everything from education to overcoming phobias, Deikelmann said. Remember staying up all night cramming for that statistics test in college? Turns out you probably would have scored higher if you had closed the books and hit the hay. A series of recent studies, some of which were presented at this week’s annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, suggest that a good night’s sleep doesn’t just make you feel more rested – it’s also crucial for remembering everything from test answers to when to mail your mother’s birthday card.
  • 72. You can't tell if someone is lying by reading their facial expressions Microexpressions, as defined by psychologist Paul Ekman (who coined the term "microexpression," basically wrote the book on the little bastards, and has been studying their use in detecting deception for going on half a century, now), are: ...very brief facial expressions, lasting between 1/25th and 1/15th of a second. They occur when a person either deliberately or unconsciously conceals an emotion being felt. Any one of the seven emotions found to have a universal signal may appear in a micro expression: anger, fear, sadness, disgust, contempt, surprise and happiness. Microexpressions therefore fall under the umbrella of "body language" ("nonverbals," if you're one for parlance), and are distinguishable in that they refer explicitly to the face and specific situations in which they're likely to appear, viz. a situation where the emotion being felt is being either intentionally or unintentionally suppressed.
  • 73. How Does "Valence" Help Your Brain Distinguish Between Good And Bad? Valence is the term used for the spectrum of emotions from extremely positive to extremely negative. When it comes to arousal — the intensity of an emotion — neuroscience indicates that valence might be less important than we think. The amygdala is the part of the brain that processes emotions. Turns out, the amygdala gets engaged based on arousal. The feeling can be negative or it can be positive. Whether a person is shown a picture of snuggling kittens or a severed hand, the amygdala responds. But it isn't the only part of the brain that responds. The prefrontal cortex is a part of the brain we really don't want to lose, as it deals with planning and problem-solving, and any injury to the region is devastating. It turns out, this area of the brain might also tell you whether you feel good or bad. Different areas of the prefrontal cortex get going when different stimuli are presented. The areas are scattered, but there are distinct sections that distinguish between what you feel when you get a free plane ticket to Costa Rica and what you feel when you get there and a spider drops from a tree onto your face. Although it's your amygdala that tells you that both events were thrilling, it's your prefrontal cortex that tells you which one you wouldn't care to repeat.
  • 74. The Psychological Effect That Explains Why You Suck at Parties It doesn't just take a good brain to memorize names. It takes time and attention, neither of which you have in that situation. As each person is listed, we are waiting for the next person's name. This provokes what's known as the next-in-line effect. The next-in-line effect happens when someone is waiting for their turn to speak during an organized event. Researchers tested it by having people in small, circular groups take turns giving out information, then testing them to see which snippets of information they remembered. Each participant's memory was just fine, right up until they got to the information imparted by the person who talked just before they themselves were "up." Suddenly, they remembered nothing. Yes, they were listening when the person before them was talking. They just weren't paying attention. Their mind was already on the next task. When we see a group of people coming towards us with their hands out for handshakes, we're in a situation in which we know we'll have to take in a cascade of information, and we try to prepare for it. This is exactly the wrong thing to do. We try to simultaneously greet the person in front of us, and get ready for the next person in line. As it turns out, we can either take in and store information, or get ready for the next grouping of information. Since we try to do two things at once, we fail at both.

Notas do Editor

  1. http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2014/03/140312-auditory-memory-visual-learning-brain-research-science/ http://now.uiowa.edu/2014/02/one-ear-and-out-other http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0089914
  2. http://www.theguardian.com/science/neurophilosophy/2014/jul/31/brain-activity-predicts-audience-preferences http://www.nature.com/ncomms/2014/140729/ncomms5567/full/ncomms5567.html http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2875927/ http://dcl.wustl.edu/~jzacks/Workshop05/Speakers/Hasson/Hasson_Science.pdf
  3. http://www.fastcodesign.com/3033794/evidence/the-neuroscience-of-emoticons http://nymag.com/scienceofus/2014/07/how-to-be-a-smarter-emoticon-user.html http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2010/december-10/the-psychological-study-of-smiling.html http://www.flinders.edu.au/sabs/psychology/research/labs/brain-and-cognition-laboratory/ http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1125737 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecj.10311/abstract?deniedAccessCustomisedMessage=&userIsAuthenticated=true http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/17470919.2013.873737#.VEy0GfnF-Sp
  4. http://www.cnet.com/news/web-trolls-are-narcissists-sadists-and-psychopaths-says-study http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/your-online-secrets/201409/internet-trolls-are-narcissists-psychopaths-and-sadists http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886914000324
  5. http://techcrunch.com/2014/10/14/product-psychology-explains-how-to-get-users-hooked-on-products-designed-around-habit-and-context http://techcrunch.com/2013/01/16/hack-design-teaches-design-to-hackers-has-already-signed-up-over-20k-developers/ http://www.productpsychology.com/
  6. http://www.businessinsider.com/multitasking-changes-your-brain-2014-9 http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/09/140924144949.htm http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0106698#s4 http://www.npr.org/2013/05/10/182861382/the-myth-of-multitasking http://online.wsj.com/articles/read-slowly-to-benefit-your-brain-and-cut-stress-1410823086
  7. http://www.brainpickings.org/2014/06/18/daniel-gilbert-happiness-future-self/
  8. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2014/oct/02/curiosity-memory-brain-reward-system-dopamine
  9. http://io9.com/why-our-brain-takes-risks-in-the-face-of-uncertainty-1639030771 http://io9.com/how-bayes-rule-can-make-you-a-better-thinker-471233405 http://io9.com/why-randomness-may-not-mean-what-you-think-it-means-658318167 http://www.rebellionresearch.com/ http://www.askamathematician.com/ http://www.clearerthinking.org/
  10. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/10/23/study-finds-women-ovulating-kissing/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24078298
  11. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/08/27/researchers-copy-paste-fear-one-memory-another/ http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v513/n7518/full/nature13725.html
  12. http://www.businessinsider.com/why-you-get-ideas-in-the-shower-2014-7 http://www.psy.cmu.edu/~unsio/Sio_Ormerod_meta_analysis_incubation_PB.pdf http://qz.com/238681/the-complete-guide-to-having-a-creative-breakthrough/#/h/89710,2/ http://www.cmu.edu/news/stories/archives/2013/february/feb13_unconsciousthought.html http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=eFX5S0tpTUA
  13. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2014/aug/06/brain-subliminally-judges-trustworthiness-faces http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811904004768
  14. http://io9.com/psychologists-find-that-nice-people-are-more-likely-to-1597267257 http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-green-mind/201406/are-polite-people-more-violent-and-destructive
  15. http://io9.com/proxemics-explains-how-close-you-can-stand-to-other-peo-1588551052 http://research.archives.gov/description/532264 http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/2740724?uid=3739744&uid=2&uid=4&uid=3739256&sid=21105025599133 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1976.tb01902.x/abstract
  16. http://news.discovery.com/human/psychology/look-angry-if-you-want-people-to-give-in-140610.htm http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2014/06/03/0956797614531027.abstract
  17. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/06/09/race-blacks-seen-darker-tough-economic-times/ http://www.pnas.org/content/111/25/9079
  18. http://news.discovery.com/human/psychology/does-breaking-rules-make-you-cool-14052.htm
  19. http://news.discovery.com/human/psychology/the-science-of-misheard-song-lyrics-140522.htm http://io9.com/the-science-of-misheard-song-lyrics-1579968707
  20. http://io9.com/the-diderot-effect-makes-you-want-to-consume-consume-1578882099 https://www.marxists.org/reference/archive/diderot/1769/regrets.htm http://knowledge.sagepub.com/view/greenconsumerism/n37.xml http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=uAe6U6y-i1EC&oi=fnd&pg=PA120&dq=diderot+effect&ots=cSwd8PZHvQ&sig=0_r0gcU4IsT3CTkeUVf10Pw0wsY#v=onepage&q=diderot%20effect&f=false http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=NUZ7AAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA230&dq=diderot+effect&ots=r7oMYHhcDl&sig=RPXpajTRVq3TUdiwUwHJVoBPtuU#v=onepage&q=diderot%20effect&f=false http://www.emeraldinsight.com/doi/abs/10.1108/07363760310456946
  21. http://io9.com/the-diderot-effect-makes-you-want-to-consume-consume-1578882099
  22. http://io9.com/the-diderot-effect-makes-you-want-to-consume-consume-1578882099
  23. September 26th, 2013 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2013/09/26/want-to-be-happier-skip-the-small-talk/#.U7sV__ldWVM http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2861779/
  24. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/09/29/age-related-differences-judgments-inappropriate-behavior-related-humor-style-preferences/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25244473
  25. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/neuroskeptic/2014/09/18/coffee-drinkers-emotions-alexithymia/ http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02791072.2014.942043#tabModule http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25188705
  26. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/09/18/could-diet-sodas-be-making-us-fatter http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v514/n7521/full/nature13793.html http://www.forbes.com/sites/alicegwalton/2014/09/18/could-artificial-sweeteners-be-contributing-to-the-obesity-crisis/
  27. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/09/03/shut-pet-dogs-prefer-petting-vocal-praise http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24643871 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2011/11/11/ncbi-rofl-a-scientific-analysis-of-400-youtube-videos-of-dogs-chasing-their-tails/#.VE148vnF-So http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25173617
  28. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/crux/2014/08/27/ignore-iq-tests-intelligence-fixed-life/ http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg22329830.400-brain-drain-are-we-evolving-stupidity. http://psychology.about.com/od/psychologicaltesting/a/int-history.htm http://psychology.about.com/od/intelligence/a/wechsler-adult-intelligence-scale.htm http://books.google.co.uk/books/about/Are_We_Getting_Smarter.html?id=Z_-ykOVpRccC&redir_esc=y http://www.pnas.org/content/105/19/6791.full.pdf+html http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10803-008-0596-0#page-1 http://www.pnas.org/content/109/2/425.abstract http://www.pnas.org/content/early/2008/04/25/0801268105.full.pdf+html http://books.google.co.uk/books/about/Relational_Frame_Theory.html?id=n4RmapzrihAC http://eprints.maynoothuniversity.ie/2765/
  29. http://lifehacker.com/research-shows-that-seven-hours-of-sleep-might-be-the-s-1608963716 http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?articleid=206050 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3687527/ http://online.wsj.com/articles/sleep-experts-close-in-on-the-optimal-nights-sleep-1405984970
  30. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/07/14/ever-wonder-butt-deforms-sit http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23266211
  31. http://99u.com/workbook/28055/the-science-behind-why-you-never-really-leave-high-school http://nymag.com/news/features/high-school-2013-1/
  32. http://www.theverge.com/2014/7/3/5869465/scientists-figured-out-how-shrooms-open-your-mind http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hbm.22562/abstract http://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/wp/2014/07/03/psychedelic-drugs-put-your-brain-in-a-waking-dream-study-finds/ http://theconversation.com/magic-mushrooms-expand-your-mind-and-amplify-your-brains-dreaming-areas-heres-how-28754 http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2014-07/icl-nsd070114.php http://www.imperial.ac.uk/people/r.carhart-harris http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9236630 http://www.jneurosci.org/content/33/38/15171.short http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/133/4/1265.abstract http://www.pnas.org/content/109/6/2138.abstract
  33. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/07/03/whole-body-vibration-improves-cognition-healthy-adults/ http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0100506
  34. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2010/sep/15/price-love-close-friends-relationship http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/10.1111/(ISSN)1475-6811
  35. http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/how-come-were-so-bad-at-buying-stuff-video-140619.htm
  36. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/06/11/want-seen-better-employee-take-early-shift/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24911178
  37. http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2014/06/140606-laughter-jokes-medicine-health-science-laughing-yoga/ http://umm.edu/news-and-events/news-releases/2005/school-of-medicine-study-shows-laughter-helps-blood-vessels-function-better
  38. http://www.thedailybeast.com/articles/2014/06/07/why-molly-is-especially-deadly-at-summer-music-festivals.html http://www.nih.gov/news/health/jun2014/nida-03.htm http://www.jneurosci.org/
  39. http://io9.com/we-may-finally-know-why-sleep-improves-your-memory-1586579175 http://www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6188/1173 http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v14/n11/abs/nn.2934.html
  40. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2011/jun/21/cocaine-addiction-linked-brain-abnormalities
  41. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2010/may/31/why-teenagers-cant-concentrate-brains
  42. http://io9.com/do-you-feel-like-youre-not-sleeping-even-when-you-are-1583034277 http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg22229690.400-awake-asleep-insomniac-brains-that-cant-switch-off.html#.U4aa6VhdWDE
  43. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/do-porn-watchers-have-smaller-brains-140529.htm http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?articleid=1874574#Conclusions
  44. http://www.businessinsider.com/science-of-why-caffeine-makes-you-productive-2014-5 http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YuJOhpNS0IY
  45. http://io9.com/scientists-discover-area-of-brain-responsible-for-lovin-1580839952 http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00152/full http://www.neatorama.com/2014/05/23/Brain-Implant-Turns-Man-into-Passionate-Johnny-Cash-Fan/#!PSS7K
  46. http://io9.com/does-the-name-letter-effect-prove-that-youre-soooo-into-1579784534 http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/15298868.2012.655897#.VEstQvnF-Sp http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00223891.2013.825622#.VEstRPnF-Sp http://opim.wharton.upenn.edu/~uws/papers/spurious_inpress.pdf
  47. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/05/28/matters-attractiveness-womans-face-body/ http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2010/10/25/ncbi-rofl-surprise-men-vote-for-the-hotter-female-candidate/ http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2013/03/15/ncbi-rofl-why-poor-hungry-men-prefer-bigger-breasts/
  48. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2010/10/25/ncbi-rofl-surprise-men-vote-for-the-hotter-female-candidate/#.VEswWfnF-So
  49. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2010/03/30/ncbi-rofl-the-presence-of-an-attractive-woman-elevates-testosterone-and-physical-risk-taking-in-young-men/#.VEswtfnF-So
  50. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2010/08/02/ncbi-rofl-beauty-week-blond-busty-skinny-waitresses-get-bigger-tips
  51. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2013/03/15/ncbi-rofl-why-poor-hungry-men-prefer-bigger-breasts http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0057623
  52. http://io9.com/why-lacking-a-sense-of-direction-is-hazardous-to-your-h-1580644691 http://io9.com/why-being-alone-is-hazardous-to-your-health-459061985 http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2014/05/06/0956797614531799.abstract http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/news/releases/having-a-sense-of-purpose-in-life-may-add-years-to-your-life.html http://www.pnas.org/content/110/15/5797.abstract
  53. http://io9.com/smart-drugs-could-be-impairing-the-brains-of-young-peop-1576908559 http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00038/full
  54. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/are-lucky-streaks-real-science-says-yes http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027714000031 http://www.economist.com/news/science-and-technology/21601810-lucky-streaks-happen-they-are-not-result-luck-hot-trot http://www.nizkor.org/features/fallacies/gamblers-fallacy.html
  55. http://io9.com/the-othello-error-makes-you-sure-everyone-is-lying-1576531014 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1987.tb00759.x/abstract;jsessionid=3DB6A498DDEDF5FFA47B6A0F34B0D2B6.f02t02?deniedAccessCustomisedMessage=&userIsAuthenticated=false http://moritzlaw.osu.edu/osjcl/Articles/Volume5_1/Herbert-PDF.pdf
  56. http://io9.com/how-hyperpalatable-foods-could-turn-you-into-a-food-add-1575144399 http://io9.com/5958880/how-flavor-chemists-make-your-food-so-addictively-good http://io9.com/is-it-true-that-oreos-are-more-addictive-to-lab-rats-th-1446363299 http://jn.nutrition.org/content/139/3/623 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0149763407000589 http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v13/n5/full/nn.2519.html http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03373.x/full http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3207274/
  57. http://io9.com/men-without-beards-could-soon-have-an-evolutionary-adva-1563813172 http://news.sciencemag.org/biology/2014/04/are-beards-about-die-out http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/10/4/20130958
  58. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/04/15/over-the-hill-cognitive-speeds-peak-at-age-24 http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0094215
  59. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/crux/2014/04/15/picking-sides-genes-help-us-decide-left-right http://www.amazon.com/gp/product/1455549444?pldnSite=1
  60. http://io9.com/can-you-control-your-metabolism-with-your-mind-1563802509 http://www.npr.org/blogs/health/2014/04/14/299179468/mind-over-milkshake-how-your-thoughts-fool-your-stomach
  61. http://io9.com/extract-money-from-people-using-the-ambiguity-effect-1563289677 http://www.sas.upenn.edu/~baron/papers/prob.pdf
  62. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2014/apr/11/visual-perception-eyes-brain-neuroscience http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Change_blindness
  63. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2014/apr/11/visual-perception-eyes-brain-neuroscience
  64. http://laplab.ucsd.edu/articles/Pashler_P&P1988.pdf http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Change_blindness
  65. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2014/04/09/study-proves-talking-cell-phone-makes-act-like-ahole/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24693829
  66. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2011/10/13/ncbi-rofl-why-overheard-cell-phone-conversations-are-extra-annoying http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20817912
  67. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/04/08/need-to-commit-something-to-memory-sleep-on-it
  68. http://io9.com/no-you-cant-tell-someone-is-lying-by-reading-their-mi-1454953226/all http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17221309
  69. http://io9.com/how-does-valence-help-your-brain-distinguish-between-1574967603 https://www2.bc.edu/~kensinel/Kensinger_CABN06.pdf http://affective-science.org/pubs/2006/Barrett2006valence.pdf
  70. http://io9.com/the-psychological-effect-that-explains-why-you-suck-at-1550972798 http://books.google.com/books?id=Vv1vvlIEXG0C&pg=PA260#v=onepage&q&f=false
  71. http://www.wired.com/2014/03/ear-nanopores-hearing/ http://www.rle.mit.edu/people/directory/dennis-freeman/ http://www.cell.com/biophysj/abstract/S0006-3495(14)00189-1
  72. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/why-we-cant-stop-gambling-140407.htm
  73. http://www.economist.com/news/science-and-technology/21598948-neural-seat-compulsive-gambling-may-have-been-found-slotting
  74. http://io9.com/this-fascinating-auditory-illusion-transforms-normal-sp-1544554261 http://aeon.co/magazine/culture/why-we-love-repetition-in-music/ http://music.uark.edu/7472.php http://io9.com/5929667/why-do-we-hate-seeing-photos-of-ourselves http://deutsch.ucsd.edu/psychology/pages.php?i=212 http://io9.com/5837976/what-happens-to-your-brain-under-the-influence-of-music
  75. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/how-trick-others-doing-your-bidding
  76. http://io9.com/twist-peoples-minds-to-make-them-think-disgusting-word-1545359452 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11273418
  77. http://io9.com/clap-if-you-believe-in-the-tinkerbell-effect-1543631910 http://www.swarthmore.edu/SocSci/fdurgin1/publications/JoCS_Durgin2002.pdf
  78. http://news.discovery.com/human/videos/pretty-people-are-easily-forgotten-video.htm
  79. http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/17470919.2013.873737#.VEMiufnF-Sr http://www.popsci.com/article/science/human-brains-now-understand-smiley-emoticon-real-face
  80. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/how-memory-rewrites-the-past-140204.htm
  81. http://mentalfloss.com/article/54556/what-makes-our-brains-feel-trust
  82. http://thenextweb.com/dd/2013/12/23/science-behind-fonts-make-feel/#!si1tJ http://affect.media.mit.edu/pdfs/05.larson-picard.pdf http://99u.com/workbook/21605/according-to-science-this-font-is-giving-you-a-whole-lot-of-feelings http://blog.pickcrew.com/
  83. http://99u.com/workbook/19419/5-ways-to-train-your-brain-with-dual-n-back-tasks http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/guest-blog/2011/03/07/you-can-increase-your-intelligence-5-ways-to-maximize-your-cognitive-potential/
  84. http://lifehacker.com/how-to-harnass-your-brains-dopamine-supply-and-increas-1496989326 http://neuroscience.mssm.edu/nestler/brainRewardpathways.html http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2013/07/what_is_dopamine_love_lust_sex_addiction_gambling_motivation_reward.html
  85. http://lifehacker.com/how-to-harnass-your-brains-dopamine-supply-and-increas-1496989326 http://www.news-medical.net/health/Dopamine-Functions.aspx http://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/feb/03/dopamine-the-unsexy-truth http://lifehacker.com/5958782/four-strategies-that-build-lasting-motivation-and-how-to-use-them-to-achieve-your-goals http://news.vanderbilt.edu/2012/05/dopamine-impacts-your-willingness-to-work/
  86. http://lifehacker.com/how-to-harnass-your-brains-dopamine-supply-and-increas-1496989326 http://www.edutopia.org/blog/teacher-burnout-neurology-judy-willis-md http://lifehacker.com/accomplish-any-goal-by-doing-something-small-every-day-1491749639 http://blog.idonethis.com/the-science-of-motivation-your-brain-on-dopamine/
  87. http://www.adweek.com/news/technology/study-women-love-social-media-more-men-unless-its-linkedin-154671 http://www.adweek.com/news/press/pew-finds-nonprofit-news-sites-are-growing-fragile-150178 http://www.journalism.org/2013/06/10/nonprofit-journalism/
  88. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/taking-picture-to-remember-may-help-you-forget-131211.htm
  89. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/taking-picture-to-remember-may-help-you-forget-1312111.htm
  90. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2013/10/09/how-your-posture-might-make-you-more-likely-to-cheat-steal-and-commit-traffic-violations/#.VEGnb_l4pcQ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24068113
  91. http://lifehacker.com/the-science-behind-posture-and-how-it-affects-your-brai-1463291618 http://blog.bufferapp.com/improve-posture-good-posture-science-happiness
  92. http://lifehacker.com/the-science-behind-posture-and-how-it-affects-your-brai-1463291618 http://blog.bufferapp.com/improve-posture-good-posture-science-happiness
  93. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/sad-faces-make-people-support-social-campaigns http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103113000292 http://www.oxfam.org.uk/media-centre/press-releases/2012/12/show-africas-potential-not-just-its-problems-says-oxfam http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/11/05/0956797613496823.abstract
  94. http://freakonomics.com/2013/10/16/when-the-store-gets-crowded-the-shopper-buys-safety/ http://www.freakonomics.com/media/Crowding_JMR.PDF http://journals.ama.org/doi/abs/10.1509/jmr.12.0118?journalCode=jmkr http://journals.ama.org/doi/abs/10.1509/jmr.12.0118
  95. May 17th, 2013 http://gizmodo.com/why-your-brain-thinks-these-dots-are-a-dog-506703504 http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Gestalt_psychology
  96. September 8th, 2013 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/neuroskeptic/2013/09/08/empathy-placebo/ http://muse.jhu.edu/login?auth=0&type=summary&url=/journals/perspectives_in_biology_and_medicine/v056/56.2.mommaerts.html
  97. October 17th, 2009 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/neuroskeptic/2009/10/17/deconstructing-the-placebo/#.U7rxWPldWVN http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11939870 http://www.bmj.com/content/338/bmj.b1568.extract http://neuroskeptic.blogspot.com/2009/10/placebos-have-side-effects-too.html http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19810776 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19781854
  98. January 23rd, 2013 1. http://www.wired.com/2013/01/how-people-smell-themselves/
  99. http://www.popsci.com/article/gadgets/nighttime-smartphone-use-can-sap-next-days-energy http://bigthink.com/insights-of-genius/ego-depletion-motivation-and-attention-a-new-model-of-self-control http://www.popsci.com/article/science/should-you-stay-late http://www.popsci.com/article/science/how-science-will-help-us-sleep-better http://www.popsci.com/article/science/tips-and-hacks-get-better-zs http://www.popsci.com/article/march-2014-science-sleep
  100. Sept. 7th 2012 http://www.popsci.com/technology/article/2012-09/science-confirms-obvious-reading-literature-good-your-brain http://news.stanford.edu/news/2012/september/austen-reading-fmri-090712.html
  101. October 10th, 2012 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/80beats/2012/10/10/how-the-brains-of-bookworms-compare-to-those-of-bibliophobes/#.U7jqdfldWVM http://www.pnas.org/content/early/2012/10/04/1206792109.abstract
  102. October 9th, 2012 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/discoblog/2012/10/09/ncbi-rofl-stressed-try-forcing-your-face-to-smile-using-chopsticks/#.U7jr7_ldWVM http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23012270
  103. http://news.discovery.com/tech/gear-and-gadgets/new-ad-software-deciphers-your-personality-131112.htm http://www.technologyreview.com/news/520671/ads-could-soon-know-if-youre-an-introvert-on-twitter/ http://www.technologyreview.com/view/519766/how-your-facebook-profile-reveals-more-about-your-personality-than-you-know/ http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0073791
  104. http://io9.com/a-drug-that-could-destroy-political-movements-1579088886 http://io9.com/scientists-need-philosophers-to-solve-this-problem-1578428283 http://www.bioethics.net/articles/if-i-could-just-stop-loving-you-anti-love-biotechnology-and-the-ethics-of-a-chemical-breakup/ http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/4174519.stm http://nymag.com/scienceofus/2014/05/anti-love-drugs-will-change-how-we-end-romances.html
  105. http://www.businessinsider.com/10-life-lessons-you-can-learn-from-the-smartest-older-people-2013-11
  106. http://www.inc.com/will-yakowicz/5-leadership-equations-for-success.html
  107. http://www.inc.com/will-yakowicz/5-leadership-equations-for-success.html
  108. http://www.inc.com/will-yakowicz/5-leadership-equations-for-success.html
  109. http://www.inc.com/will-yakowicz/5-leadership-equations-for-success.html
  110. http://www.inc.com/kevin-daum/7-things-really-amazing-communicators-do.html
  111. September 17th, 2013 http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2013-09/science-confirms-obvious-tv-pharmaceutical-ads-are-often-misleading http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11606-013-2604-0 http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11606-013-2622-y
  112. January 18th,2013 1. http://www.fastcodesign.com/1671627/study-shows-how-classroom-design-affects-student-learning
  113. January 18th, 2013 http://www.entrepreneur.com/blog/225548 http://www.entrepreneur.com/article/225519
  114. January 14th, 2013 1. http://www.entrepreneur.com/blog/225451
  115. May 23rd, 2012 http://www.entrepreneur.com/topic/creativity http://www.entrepreneur.com/blog/223588
  116. April 26th, 2012 1. http://www.entrepreneur.com/article/223432
  117. August 27th, 2013 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22285424 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2013/08/27/have-a-difficult-problem-to-solve-try-vodka/#.U7rWqvldWVM
  118. January 13th, 2013 http://www.inc.com/jessica-stillman/mission-possible-how-to-finally-wake-up-earlier.html http://www.businessinsider.com/how-to-wake-up-earlier-2013-1 http://www.businessinsider.com/the-3-rules-of-deep-sleep-2011-8
  119. September 26th, 2013 1. http://io9.com/the-science-behind-power-naps-and-why-theyre-so-damne-1401366016
  120. September 19th, 2013 http://www.amazon.com/dp/B00AQ1V66A http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/sleeping-angels/201004/why-is-it-so-hard-get-out-bed-in-the-morning http://io9.com/why-is-it-so-impossible-to-get-out-of-bed-in-the-mornin-1348209324 http://www.sleepcenters.org/
  121. September 26th, 2013 http://io9.com/the-science-behind-power-naps-and-why-theyre-so-damne-1401366016 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00622.x/full http://io9.com/so-it-s-true-the-full-moon-does-mess-with-our-sleep-922918960 http://io9.com/10-theories-that-explain-why-we-dream-897195110 http://www.alertsol.com/downloads/sb_alertsol/strat_naps.pdf
  122. September 2013 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0196064406002393 http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Abstract/2012/10000/The_Effects_of_a_Mid_Day_Nap_on_the_Neurocognitive.31.aspx http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17053484 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0166432808002416 http://blogs.plos.org/obesitypanacea/files/2013/05/BJN-editorial.pdf http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17296887 http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/02/110228105308.htm
  123. June 2012 http://pss.sagepub.com/content/17/8/670.short http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19840054 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00962.x/full
  124. February 28th, 2011 http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/02/110228105308.htm http://blogs.plos.org/obesitypanacea/files/2013/05/BJN-editorial.pdf
  125. November 23rd, 2012 http://io9.com/5962870/10-supplements-you-can-take-today-to-enhance-your-intelligence http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1691485/pdf/14561278.pdf http://jtoomim.org/files/Ling_2009-Cognitive_effects_of_creatine_ethyl_ester_supplementation.pdf http://iospress.metapress.com/content/lw1630245745v088/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18681988 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-2125.2012.04378.x/abstract http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/826948 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/785952 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0197458081900075 http://www.seriousnutritionsolutions.com/products/core/Focus-XT.php http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD005379.pub2/abstract%20http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD005379.pub2/abstract http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0952327807001421 http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0046832 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0944711304001461 http://online.liebertpub.com/doi/abs/10.1089/acm.2008.0018 http://examine.com/supplements/Bacopa+Monnieri/#howtotake http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/gps.2662/abstract;jsessionid=85DE62ABC45FDB67365C4EB3D3762C38.d01t01?deniedAccessCustomisedMessage=&userIsAuthenticated=false http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11026748 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hup.544/abstract http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10192826 http://psi.sagepub.com/content/3/1/2.abstract http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hup.544/abstract http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12020739 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20737519 http://researchbank.swinburne.edu.au/vital/access/manager/Repository/swin:10462 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0944711300800550 http://headshaker.eu/en/rhodiola1.pdf http://www.curador.net/indexfr/rosea/Shevtsov.SHR-5.2003.pdf http://www.aromamd.net/edu_memory_mood.pdf http://www.aromamd.net/edu_alzheimers_dementia.pdf
  126. September 25th, 2013 http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/seriouslyscience/2013/09/25/physical-order-produces-healthy-choices-generosity-and-conventionality-whereas-disorder-produces-creativity/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23907542
  127. February 18th, 2014 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24549296 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22121888
  128. July 16th, 2011 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21140315 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed?linkname=pubmed_pubmed&from_uid=12814291
  129. January 17th, 2013 http://blogs.hbr.org/2013/01/three-elements-of-great-communication-according/ http://classics.mit.edu/Aristotle/rhetoric.1.i.html
  130. October 18th, 2012 http://lifehacker.com/5952978/if-youre-too-busy-to-meditate-read-this http://lifehacker.com/5924792/meditation-can-improve-your-memory-focus-and-productivity-at-work?tag=meditation http://lifehacker.com/5800249/why-meditation-relieves-chronic-pain-and-stress http://lifehacker.com/5591576/a-guide-to-meditation-for-the-rest-of-us http://lifehacker.com/5766703/avoid-information-overload-with-regular-meditation http://lifehacker.com/137692/build-a-better-brain-with-meditation http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn8317#.U7j1hvldWVM http://faculty.washington.edu/wobbrock/pubs/gi-12.02.pdf http://lifehacker.com/5900732/develop-a-two-minute-meditation-habit-and-make-it-stick http://lifehacker.com/5855964/buddhify-teaches-you-to-meditate-and-relax-a-little-on-the-go http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/money/jobcenter/workplace/bruzzese/story/2012-07-08/meditation-helps-your-work/56071024/1 http://www.nytimes.com/2012/04/29/technology/google-course-asks-employees-to-take-a-deep-breath.html?_r=0 http://philosophistry.com/archives/2011/02/benefits-of-meditation.html
  131. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/10/01/sense-smell-predict-mortality http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0107541
  132. http://discovermagazine.com/2013/nov/05-sit-down http://cpr.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/12/10/2047487312471759.abstract
  133. http://adage.com/article/the-media-guy/truth-advertising-congress-regulate-photoshopping/292619/
  134. December 2nd, 2012 http://www.themarysue.com/aspergers-declassified-as-mental-disorder/
  135. http://adage.com/article/campaign-trail/world-campaign-tech-list-rental-a-force/293714/
  136. http://io9.com/the-u-s-has-the-most-expensive-least-effective-health-1591403515 http://www.commonwealthfund.org/~/media/files/publications/fund-report/2014/jun/1755_davis_mirror_mirror_2014.pdf
  137. http://www.pcworld.com/article/2451793/the-u-s-military-is-not-so-shockingly-studying-your-facebook-and-twitter-habits.html http://www.techhive.com/article/2449449/facebook-s-news-feed-experiment-not-the-end-of-the-world.html http://www.theguardian.com/world/2014/jul/08/darpa-social-networks-research-twitter-influence-studies
  138. https://gigaom.com/2014/08/18/researchers-get-access-to-behavioral-data-from-reddit-imgur-and-twitch-via-the-derp-consortium/
  139. http://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/jul/01/facebook-cornell-study-emotional-contagion-ethics-breach?
  140. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/d-brief/2014/03/11/genes-for-hearing-linked-with-musical-aptitude/#.VENpZvnF-So http://www.nature.com/mp/journal/vaop/ncurrent/abs/mp20148a.html
  141. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.frc.ri.cmu.edu/~hpm/book97/ch3/retina.comment.html
  142. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational
  143. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.sciencemag.org/content/328/5984/1408.short http://www.pnas.org/content/108/4/1262.short
  144. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.sas.upenn.edu/~baron/journal/06001/jdm06001.htm
  145. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/3150288?uid=3739448&uid=2&uid=3737720&uid=4&sid=21101636332447
  146. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://well.blogs.nytimes.com/2007/10/31/how-scared-should-we-be/ http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/15/6/322.short http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/1562234?uid=3739744&uid=2&uid=4&uid=3739256&sid=21105023341373
  147. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational
  148. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2009/08/31/status-quo-anxiety
  149. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.amazon.ca/The-Better-Angels-Our-Nature/dp/0670022950
  150. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://psychology.about.com/od/classicpsychologystudies/p/conformity.htm
  151. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/bul/102/1/72/ http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2FBF00987179?LI=true
  152. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9831521
  153. http://io9.com/5974468/the-most-common-cognitive-biases-that-prevent-you-from-being-rational http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/73/3/437/ http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.1812/abstract http://digest.bps.org.uk/2005/03/last-but-definitely-not-least.html http://digest.bps.org.uk/2006/12/satisfaction-guaranteed.html http://digest.bps.org.uk/2006/01/mind-where-you-sit-how-being-in-middle.html http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749597805000841 http://pss.sagepub.com/content/17/12/1027 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/00016918 http://digest.bps.org.uk/2012/04/people-prefer-middle-option.html
  154. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases 8. Baron 1994, p. 372 9. Zhang, Yu; Lewis, Mark; Pellon, Michael; Coleman, Phillip (2007). "A Preliminary Research on Modeling Cognitive Agents for Social Environments in Multi-Agent Systems". pp. 116–123. 10. Iverson, Grant; Brooks, Brian; Holdnack, James (2008). "Misdiagnosis of Cognitive Impairment in Forensic Neuropsychology". In Heilbronner, Robert L. Neuropsychology in the Courtroom: Expert Analysis of Reports and Testimony. New York: Guilford Press. p. 248. ISBN 9781593856342. 11. (Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin. 12. Schwarz, N.; Bless, Herbert; Strack, Fritz; Klumpp, G.; Rittenauer-Schatka, Helga; Simons, Annette (1991). "Ease of Retrieval as Information: Another Look at the Availability Heuristic" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 (2): 195–202.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.195. Retrieved 19 Oct 2014. 13. Kuran, Timur; Cass R Sunstein (1998). "Availability Cascades and Risk Regulation".Stanford Law Review 51: 683. doi:10.2307/1229439. 14. Sanna, Lawrence J.; Schwarz, Norbert; Stocker, Shevaun L. (2002). "When debiasing backfires: Accessible content and accessibility experiences in debiasing hindsight.".Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3): 497–502.doi:10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.497. ISSN 0278-7393. 15. Colman, Andrew (2003). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 77. ISBN 0-19-280632-7. 16. Baron 1994, pp. 224–228 17. Klauer, K. C.; Musch, J; Naumer, B (2000). "On belief bias in syllogistic reasoning".Psychological Review 107 (4): 852–884. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.4.852.PMID 11089409.
  155. 18. Pronin, Emily; Matthew B. Kugler (July 2007). "Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (Elsevier) 43 (4): 565–578. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011. ISSN 0022-1031. 19. Walker, Drew; Vul, Edward (2013-10-25). "Hierarchical Encoding Makes Individuals in a Group Seem More Attractive". Psychological Science 20 (11).doi:10.1177/0956797613497969. 20. Mather, M.; Shafir, E.; Johnson, M.K. (2000). "Misrememberance of options past: Source monitoring and choice". Psychological Science 11: 132–138. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00228. PMID 11273420. 21. Oswald, Margit E.; Grosjean, Stefan (2004). "Confirmation Bias". In Pohl, Rüdiger F.Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 79–96. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4.OCLC 55124398. 22. Fisk, John E. (2004). "Conjunction fallacy". In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 23–42. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398. 23. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g Martin Hilbert (2012) ""Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: How noisy information processing can bias human decision making"". Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 211–237; Also at http://www.martinhilbert.net/HilbertPsychBull.pdf 24. ^ Jump up to:a b Attneave, F. (1953). "Psychological probability as a function of experienced frequency". Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (2): 81–86. doi:10.1037/h0057955.PMID 13084849. 25. ^ Jump up to:a b Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S. (1977). "Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3 (4): 552–564. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552. 26. DuCharme, W. M. (1970). "Response bias explanation of conservative human inference".Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (1): 66–74. doi:10.1037/h0029546. 27. ^ Jump up to:a b Edwards, W. (1968). "Conservatism in human information processing". In Kleinmuntz, B. Formal representation of human judgment. New York: Wiley. pp. 17–52. 28. Plous 1993, pp. 38–41
  156. 29. Ackerman, Mark S., ed. (2003). Sharing expertise beyond knowledge management(online ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. p. 7. ISBN 9780262011952. 30. Why We Spend Coins Faster Than Bills by Chana Joffe-Walt. All Things Considered, 12 May 2009. 31. Hsee, Christopher K.; Zhang, Jiao (2004). "Distinction bias: Misprediction and mischoice due to joint evaluation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86 (5): 680–695.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.680. PMID 15161394. 32. (Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193) Richard Thaler coined the term "endowment effect." 33. [1] 34. Wagenaar, W. A.; Keren, G. B. (1985). "Calibration of probability assessments by professional blackjack dealers, statistical experts, and lay people". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 36 (3): 406–416. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(85)90008-1. 35. Jeng, M. (2006). "A selected history of expectation bias in physics". American Journal of Physics 74 (7): 578–583. doi:10.1119/1.2186333.
  157. 36. Kahneman, Daniel; Alan B. Krueger; David Schkade; Norbert Schwarz; Arthur A. Stone (2006-06-30). "Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion". Science312 (5782): 1908–10. doi:10.1126/science.1129688. PMID 16809528. 37. Zwicky, Arnold (2005-08-07). "Just Between Dr. Language and I". Language Log. 38. "There’s a Name for That: The Baader-Meinhof Phenomenon". 39. Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1977). Do those who know more also know more about how much they know? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20(2), 159–183.doi:10.1016/0030-5073(77)90001-0 40. Merkle, E. C. (2009). "The disutility of the hard-easy effect in choice confidence".Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16 (1): 204–213. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.1.204. 41. Juslin, P; Winman, A.; Olsson, H. (2000). "Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect". Psychological Review107 (2): 384–396. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.384. 42. Pohl, Rüdiger F. (2004). "Hindsight Bias". In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 363–378. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398.
  158. 43. Laibson, David (1997). "Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discounting". Quarterly Journal of Economics 112 (2): 443–477. doi:10.1162/003355397555253. 44. Kogut, Tehila; Ritov, Ilana (2005). "The 'Identified Victim' Effect: An Identified Group, or Just a Single Individual?". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making (Wiley InterScience)18: 157–167. doi:10.1002/bdm.492. Retrieved August 15, 2013. 45. Thompson, Suzanne C. (1999). "Illusions of Control: How We Overestimate Our Personal Influence". Current Directions in Psychological Science (Association for Psychological Science) 8 (6): 187–190. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00044. ISSN 0963-7214.JSTOR 20182602. 46. Dierkes, Meinolf; Antal, Ariane Berthoin; Child, John; Ikujiro Nonaka (2003). Handbook of Organizational Learning and Knowledge. Oxford University Press. p. 22. ISBN 978-0-19-829582-2. Retrieved 9 September 2013.http://books.google.se/books?id=JRd7RZzzw_wC&pg=PA22&dq=Illusion+of+validity&hl=en&sa=X&ei=lr4tUoKtOKKK4wSi3IEw&redir_esc=y#v=onepage&q=Illusion%20of%20validity&f=false 47. Tversky, Amos; Daniel Kahneman (September 27, 1974). "Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases". Science (American Association for the Advancement of Science) 185 (4157): 1124–1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124.PMID 17835457. 48. Fiedler, K. (1991). "The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An information loss account of distinctiveness-based illusory correlations". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 (1): 24–36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.24. 49. Sanna, Lawrence J.; Schwarz, Norbert (2004). "Integrating Temporal Biases: The Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences". Psychological Science(American Psychological Society) 15 (7): 474–481. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00704.x. PMID 15200632. 50. Baron 1994, pp. 258–259
  159. 51. (Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193) Daniel Kahneman, together with Amos Tversky, coined the term "loss aversion." 52. Bornstein, Robert F.; Crave-Lemley, Catherine (2004). "Mere exposure effect". In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 215–234. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398. 53. Shafir, Eldar; Diamond, Peter; Tversky, Amos (2000). "Money Illusion". In Kahneman, Daniel; Tversky, Amos. Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge University Press. pp. 335–355. ISBN 978-0-521-62749-8. 54. Haizlip, Julie et al. "Perspective: The Negativity Bias, Medical Education, and the Culture of Academic Medicine: Why Culture Change Is Hard". Retrieved October 3, 2012. 55. Baron 1994, p. 353
  160. 56. Baron 1994, p. 386 57. Baron 1994, p. 44 58. Hardman 2009, p. 104 59. Adams, P. A., & Adams, J. K. (1960). Confidence in the recognition and reproduction of words difficult to spell. The American Journal of Psychology, 73(4), 544–552. 60. Hoffrage, Ulrich (2004). "Overconfidence". In Rüdiger Pohl. Cognitive Illusions: a handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgement and memory. Psychology Press.ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. 61. Sutherland 2007, pp. 172–178 23. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g Martin Hilbert (2012) ""Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: How noisy information processing can bias human decision making"". Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 211–237; Also at http://www.martinhilbert.net/HilbertPsychBull.pdf 49. Sanna, Lawrence J.; Schwarz, Norbert (2004). "Integrating Temporal Biases: The Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences". Psychological Science(American Psychological Society) 15 (7): 474–481. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00704.x. PMID 15200632.
  161. 62. Hardman 2009, p. 137
  162. 63. Garcia, Stephen M.; Song, Hyunjin; Tesser, Abraham (November 2010). "Tainted recommendations: The social comparison bias". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 113 (2): 97–101. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.06.002. ISSN 0749-5978. Lay summary – BPS Research Digest (2010-10-30). 64. Dalton, D. & Ortegren, M. (2011). "Gender differences in ethics research: The importance of controlling for the social desirability response bias". Journal of Business Ethics 103 (1): 73–93. doi:10.1007/s10551-011-0843-8. 65. Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193 66. Baron 1994, p. 382 67. Baron, J. (in preparation). Thinking and Deciding, 4th edition. New York: Cambridge University Press. 68. "Penn Psychologists Believe 'Unit Bias' Determines The Acceptable Amount To Eat". ScienceDaily (Nov. 21, 2005) 27.Edwards, W. (1968). "Conservatism in human information processing". In Kleinmuntz, B. Formal representation of human judgment. New York: Wiley. pp. 17–52.
  163. 69. Meegan, Daniel V. (2010). "Zero-Sum Bias: Perceived Competition Despite Unlimited Resources". Frontiers in Psychology 1. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00191. ISSN 1664-1078. 70. Chernev, Alexander (2007). "Jack of All Trades or Master of One? Product Differentiation and Compensatory Reasoning in Consumer Choice". Journal of Consumer Research 33 (4): 430–444. doi:10.1086/510217. ISSN 0093-5301.
  164. 71. Morris, Errol (2010-06-20). "The Anosognosic’s Dilemma: Something’s Wrong but You’ll Never Know What It Is (Part 1)". Opinionator: Exclusive Online Commentary from theNew York Times. New York Times. Retrieved 2011-03-07. 72. Marks, Gary; Miller, Norman (1987). "Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review". Psychological Bulletin (American Psychological Association) 102 (1): 72–90. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.72.
  165. 73. Sutherland 2007, pp. 138–139 74. Baron 1994, p. 275 75. Pronin, E.; Kruger, J.; Savitsky, K.; Ross, L. (2001). "You don't know me, but I know you: the illusion of asymmetric insight". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 (4): 639–656. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.639. PMID 11642351.
  166. 76. Hoorens, Vera (1993). "Self-enhancement and Superiority Biases in Social Comparison".European Review of Social Psychology (Psychology Press) 4 (1): 113–139.doi:10.1080/14792779343000040. 77. Plous 2006, p. 206 78. Hsee, Christopher K.; Hastie, Reid (2006). "Decision and experience: why don't we choose what makes us happy?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (1): 31–37.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.007. PMID 16318925.
  167. 79. Plous 2006, p. 185 80. Forsyth, D. R. (2009). Group Dynamics (5th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. 81. Kruger, J. (1999). "Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 2. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.221. PMID 10474208.
  168. 82. Schacter, Daniel L. (1999). "The Seven Sins of Memory: Insights From Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience". American Psychologist 54 (3): 182–203. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.182. PMID 10199218. 83. Cacioppo, John (2002). Foundations in social neuroscience. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. pp. 130–132. ISBN 026253195X. 24.  Attneave, F. (1953). "Psychological probability as a function of experienced frequency". Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (2): 81–86. doi:10.1037/h0057955.PMID 13084849. 25. Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S. (1977). "Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3 (4): 552–564. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552.
  169. 22. Fisk, John E. (2004). "Conjunction fallacy". In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 23–42. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398. 48. Fiedler, K. (1991). "The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An information loss account of distinctiveness-based illusory correlations". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 (1): 24–36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.24. 84. Walker, W. Richard; John J. Skowronski; Charles P. Thompson (2003). "Life Is Pleasant—and Memory Helps to Keep It That Way!". Review of General Psychology(Educational Publishing Foundation) 7 (2): 203–210. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203. Retrieved 2009-08-27.
  170. 85. Koriat, A.; M. Goldsmith; A. Pansky (2000). "Toward a Psychology of Memory Accuracy".Annual Review of Psychology 51 (1): 481–537. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481.PMID 10751979. 86. Craik & Lockhart, 1972 87. Kinnell, Angela; Dennis, S. (2011). "The list length effect in recognition memory: an analysis of potential confounds". Memory & Cognition (Adelaide, Australia: School of Psychology, University of Adelaide) 39 (2): 348–63. doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0007-6. 88. Wayne Weiten (2010). Psychology: Themes and Variations: Themes and Variations. Cengage Learning. p. 338. ISBN 978-0-495-60197-5. 89. Wayne Weiten (2007). Psychology: Themes and Variations: Themes And Variations. Cengage Learning. p. 260. ISBN 978-0-495-09303-9. 90. Slamecka NJ (1968). "An examination of trace storage in free recall". J Exp Psychol 76(4): 504–13. doi:10.1037/h0025695. PMID 5650563.
  171. 91. Shepard, R.N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 156–163. 92. McBride, D. M., Dosher, B.A. (2002). A comparison of conscious and automatic memory processes for picture and word stimuli: a process dissociation analysis" Consciousness and Cognition 11, 423–460. 93. Defetyer, M. A., Russo, R., McPartlin, P. L. (2009). The picture superiority effect in recognition memory: a developmental study using the response signal procedure.Cognitive Development, 24, 265–273. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.05.002 94. Whitehouse, A. J., Maybery, M.T., Durkin, K. (2006). The development of the picture-superiority effect. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 767–773.doi:10.1348/026151005X74153 95. Ally, B. A., Gold, C. A., Budson, A. E. (2009). The picture superiority effect in patients with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment" Neuropsychologia 47, 595–598.doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.010 96. Curran, T., & Doyle, J. (2011). Picture superiority doubly dissociates the ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity" Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23(5), 1247–1262.doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21464 97. Martin, G. Neil; Neil R. Carlson; William Buskist (2007). Psychology (3rd ed.). Pearson Education. pp. 309–310. ISBN 978-0-273-71086-8. 98. "When comprehension difficulty improves memory for text." O'Brien, Edward J.; Myers, Jerome L. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol 11(1), Jan 1985, 12–21. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.1.12 99. Rubin, Wetzler & Nebes, 1986; Rubin, Rahhal & Poon, 1998 100. David A. Lieberman (8 December 2011). Human Learning and Memory. Cambridge University Press. p. 432. ISBN 978-1-139-50253-5.
  172. 101. Morton, Crowder & Prussin, 1971 102. Ian Pitt; Alistair D. N. Edwards (2003). Design of Speech-Based Devices: A Practical Guide. Springer. p. 26. ISBN 978-1-85233-436-9. 103. E. Bruce Goldstein. Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience. Cengage Learning. p. 231. ISBN 978-1-133-00912-2. 104. Poppenk, Walia, Joanisse, Danckert, & Köhler, 2006 105. Von Restorff, H. (1933). "Über die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im Spurenfeld (The effects of field formation in the trace field)". Psychological Research 18 (1): 299–342. 82. Schacter, Daniel L. (1999). "The Seven Sins of Memory: Insights From Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience". American Psychologist 54 (3): 182–203. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.182. PMID 10199218.
  173. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  174. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  175. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  176. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  177. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  178. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  179. http://news.discovery.com/human/life/ways-your-body-betrays-you-140912.htm
  180. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464
  181. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://searchengineland.com/bing-ends-2013-with-all-time-high-in-us-market-share-but-google-also-up-comscore-181876
  182. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464
  183. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://theweek.com/article/index/256506/how-online-dating-became-a-2-billion-dollar-industry http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=m9PiPlRuy6E
  184. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2014/feb/27/nsa-robots-algorithm-surveillance-bruce-schneier https://www.nsa.gov/ia/programs/suiteb_cryptography/
  185. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://www.amazon.ca/The-Filter-Bubble-Personalized-Changing/dp/0143121235 http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=B8ofWFx525s
  186. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464
  187. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/jul/01/how-algorithms-rule-world-nsa
  188. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464
  189. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464 http://io9.com/5930098/how-soon-before-the-police-can-predict-crimes-before-they-happen http://www-03.ibm.com/press/us/en/pressrelease/32169.wss http://io9.com/how-your-bodys-unique-biosignatures-are-used-for-surve-1521363957
  190. http://io9.com/the-10-algorithms-that-dominate-our-world-1580110464
  191. http://io9.com/watch-10-seconds-of-high-frequency-stock-trading-in-sup-1441258911 http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=NRUCWIosL_k&feature=c4-overview&list=UUmCDtaBVqAnsXsj0cAVyKSg http://www.nanex.net/company.html
  192. http://io9.com/a-new-digital-world-is-emerging-thats-too-fast-for-us-1286428447
  193. http://io9.com/a-new-digital-world-is-emerging-thats-too-fast-for-us-1286428447 http://www.nature.com/srep/2013/130911/srep02627/full/srep02627.html
  194. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/next-summer-already-better-summer
  195. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/next-summer-already-better-summer
  196. http://lifehacker.com/if-youre-under-40-stop-optimizing-your-retirement-plan-1580260622 http://financialuproar.com/2014/05/12/40-dont-worry-retirement/
  197. http://www.businessinsider.com/14-emerging-health-technologies-2014-4
  198. http://www.businessinsider.com/14-emerging-health-technologies-2014-4
  199. http://www.businessinsider.com/14-emerging-health-technologies-2014-4
  200. http://www.businessinsider.com/14-emerging-health-technologies-2014-4
  201. http://www.businessinsider.com/14-emerging-health-technologies-2014-4
  202. http://www.businessinsider.com/illumina-genome-sequencing-growth-2013-10
  203. http://adage.com/article/special-report-cannes-lions/nivea-ad-turns-kid-tracker-wins-mobile-grand-prix/293745/
  204. http://www.fastcodesign.com/3031917/thanks-to-the-supreme-court-our-food-is-getting-more-transparent
  205. REFERENCES Baddeley, A. (1995). Working memory. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 755-764). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bailey, C.H. & Kandel, E.R. (1995). Molecular and structural mechanisms underlying long-term memory. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 19-36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Black, I.B. (1995). Trophic interactions and brain plasticity. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 9-17). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cahill, L. & McGaugh, J.L. (1998). Modulation of memory storage. In L.R. Squire & S.M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Findings and current opinion in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 85-90). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Church, B.A. & Schacter, D.L. (1994). Perceptual specificity of auditory priming: Implicit memory for voice intonation and fundamental frequency. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 20, 521-533.21 Davis, M., Campeau, S., Kim, M., & Falls, W.A. (1995). Neural systems of emotion: The amygdala’s role in fear and anxiety. In J.L. McGaugh, N.M. Weinberger, & G. Lynch (Eds.), Brain and memory: Modulation and mediation of neural plasticity (pp. 3-40). Oxford: Oxford University Press Gazzaniga, M.S., Ivry, R.B., & Mangun, G.R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind. London: Norton. Graham, R.B. (1990). Physiological psychology. Belmont, Cal: Wadsworth. Jacoby, L.L., Lindsay, D.S., & Toth, J.P. (1992). Unconscious influences revealed: Attention, awareness, and control. American psychologist, 47, 802-809. Kalat, J.W. (1998). Biological psychology (6th ed.). Belmont, Cal: Wadsworth. Levine, D.S., Long, D.L., & Parks, R.W. (Eds.) (1998). Fundamentals of neural network modelling: Neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Loftus, E.F. & Klinger, M.R. (1992). Is the unconscious smart or dumb? American psychologist, 47, 761-765. Lynch, G. & Baudry, M. (1984). The biochemistry of memory: A new and specific hypothesis. Science, 224, 1057-1064.22 Matlin, M.W. (1994). Cognition (3rd ed.). Florida: Harcourt Brace. Ochsner, K.N., Chiu, C-Y.P, & Schacter, D.L. (1998). Varieties of priming. In L.R. Squire & S.M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Findings and current opinion in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 99-104). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Roediger, H.L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American psychologist, 45, 1043-1056. Rugg, M.D. (1995). Event relate potential studies of human memory. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 789-801). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schacter, D.L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 13, 501-518. Schacter, D.L. (1995). Implicit memory: A new frontier for cognitive neuroscience. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 815-824). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schacter, D.L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. New York: Basic Books. Shimamura, A.P. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe function. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 803-813). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Squire, L.R. & Alvarez, P. (1998). Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. In L.R. Squire & S.M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Findings and current opinion in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 75-83). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Squire, L.R. & Knowlton, B.J. (1995). Memory, hippocampus, and brain systems. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 825-837). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. West, R.L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological bulletin, 120, 272-292. Winson, J. (November, 1990). The meaning of dreams. Scientific American, 42-48. Yin, J.C. & Tully, T. (1998). CREB and the formation of long term memory. In L.R. Squire & S.M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Findings and current opinion in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 133-137). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
  206. References Bowden, E.M. & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). One hundred forty-four Compound Remote Associate Problems: Short insight-like problems with one-word solutions. Behavioral Research, Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 634-639. Czerwinski, M., Horvitz, E. & Cutrell, E. (2001). Subjective Duration Assessment: An Implicit Probe for Software Usability. In Proceedings of IHM-HCI 2001 Conference, Volume 2, (September, 2001, Lille, France), p. 167-170. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (5). Gugerty, L., Tyrrell, R.A., Aten, T.R., & Edmonds, K.A. (2004). The effects of sub-pixel addressing on users’ performance, ACM Transactions on Applied Perception. Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotion (p. 261-277). NY: Guilford. Isen, A.M., Daubman, K.A., Nowicki, G.P. (1987). Positive Affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1122-1131. Mednick, S.A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220-232. Weybrew, B.B. (1984). The Zeigarnik phenomenon revisited: Implications for enhancement of morale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, p. 223-226. Ziegarnik, B. (1927). Uber das Behalten von erledigten und unerledigten handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1-85.
  207. References Achen, Christopher H. 1985. Proxy Variables and Incorrect Signs on Regression Coefficients. Political Methodology 11: 299-317. Arrow, Kenneth J. 1963. Social Choice and Individual Values. 2 nd . ed. [orig. pub. 1951]. New York: Wiley. Bachrach, Peter, and Morton S. Baratz. 1962. Two Faces of Power. American Political Science Review 56(4): 947-952. Bartels, Larry M. 2008. Unequal Democracy: The Political Economy of the New Gilded Age. New York: Russell Sage Foundation and Princeton University Press. Bauer, Raymond A., Ithiel de Sola Pool, and Lewis Anthony Dexter. 1967. American Business and Public Policy: The Politics of Foreign Trade. New York: Atherton. Baumgartner, Frank R., Jeffrey M. Berry, Marie Hojnacki, David C. Kimball, and Beth L. Leech. 2009. Lobbying and Policy Change: Who Wins, Who Loses, and Why. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2009. Beard, Charles. 19`3. An Economic Interpretation of the Constitution of the United States. Bentley, Arthur F. 1908. The Process of Government: A Study of Social Pressures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Berry, Jeffrey M. 1999. The New Liberalism: The Rising Power of Citizen Groups. Washington, D.C.: Brookings. Black, Duncan. 1948. On the Rationale of Group Decision-making. Journal of Political Economy 56: 23–34. Black, Duncan. 1958. The Theory of Committees and Elections. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press. Block, Fred. 1977. The Ruling Class Does Not Rule: Notes on the Marxist Theory of the State. Socialist Revolution 7(3): 6-28. Block, Fred. 2007. Understanding the Divergent Trajectories of the United States and Western Europe: A Neo-Polanyian Analysis. Politics and Society (March): 1-31. Burch, Philip H. 1980-81. Elites in American History. 3 vols. New York: Holmes & Meier. Canes-Wrone, Brandice, Michael C. Herron, and Kenneth W. Shotts. 2001. Leadership and Pandering: A Theory of Executive Policymaking. American Journal of Political Science 45: 532-50. Dahl, Robert A. 1956. A Preface to Democratic Theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Dahl, Robert A. 1961. Who Governs? New Haven: Yale University Press. Dahl, Robert A. 1989. Democracy and its Critics. New Haven: Yale University Press. Davis, Otto A., Melvin J. Hinich, and Peter C. Ordeshook. 1970. An Expository Development of a Mathematical Model of the Electoral Process. American Political Science Review 64: 426-48. Domhoff, G. William. 2013. Who Rules America: The Triumph of the Corporate Rich. 7th. ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. Downs, Anthony. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper & Row. Erikson, Robert S., Michael B. MacKuen, and James A. Stimson. 2002. The Macro Polity. New York: Cambridge University Press. Erikson, Robert S., Gerald C. Wright, and John P. McIver. 1993. Statehouse Democracy: Public Opinion and Policy in the American States. New York: Cambridge University Press. Ferguson, Thomas. 1995. Golden Rule: The Investment Theory of Party Competition and the Logic of Money-Driven Political Systems. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Fiorina, Morris. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven: Yale University Press. Gilens, Martin. 2011. “Two-thirds full? Citizen Competence and Democratic Governance.” In New Directions in Public Opinion, ed., Adam Berinsky, pp.52-76. New York: Routledge. Gilens, Martin. Affluence and Influence: Economic Inequality and Political Power in America. 2012. New York: Russell Sage Foundation and Princeton University Press. Hacker, Jacob S., and Paul Pierson. 2010. Winner-Take-All Politics: How Washington Made the Rich Richer – and Turned its Back on the Middle Class. New York: Simon & Schuster. Hamilton, Alexander, James Madison, and John Jay. 1961 [orig. pub. 1787-88]. Ed. Clinton Rossiter. New York: New American Library. Hotelling, Harold. 1929. Stability in Competition. The Economic Journal 39: 41-57. Isaac, Jeffrey. 1987a. Beyond the Three Faces of Power: A Realist Critique. Polity 20(1): 4-31. Isaac, Jeffrey. 1987b. Power and Marxist Theory. Ithaca: Cornell University Press. Jacobs, Larry R., and Benjamin I. Page. 2005. Who Influences U.S. Foreign Policy? American Political Science Review 99: 107-123. Lukes, Steven. 1974. Power: A Radical View. London: Macmillan. Katznelson, Ira. 2013. Fear Itself: The New Deal and the Origins of Our Time. New York: Liveright. Key, V.O., Jr. 1961. Public Opinion and American Democracy. New York: Knopf. Lindblom, Charles E. 1977. Politics and Markets. New York: Basic. Lincoln, Abraham. 1863. Address at Gettysburg, Pennsylvania. In Speeches and Writings 1859-1865. The Library of America, New York, (1989), p.536. McChesney, Fred S. 1997. Money for Nothing: Politicians, Rent Extraction, and Political Extortion. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. McConnell, Grant. 1966. Private Power and American Democracy. New York: Random House. McKelvey, Richard. 1976. Intransitivities in Multidimensional Voting Models and Some Implications for Agenda Control. Journal of Economic Theory 12: 472-82. Mansbridge, Jane. 2003. Rethinking Representation. American Political Science Review 97: 515-28. Marx, Karl, and Friedrich Engels. 1972 [1848, 1888]. Manifesto of the Communist Party. In The Marx-Engels Reader, ed. Robert C. Tucker (New York, W.W. Norton), pp. 331-362. See also http://www.marxists.org/archive/marx/works/1848/communist- manifesto/ch01.htm May, Kenneth O. 1952. A Set of Independent Necessary and Sufficient Conditions for Simple Majority Decisions, Econometrica 20: 680–684. Michels, Robert. 2001. Political Parties: A Sociological Study of the Oligarchical Tendencies of Modern Democracy. Kitchener, Ontario: Batoche Books. Orig. pub. 1911. Milbrath, Lester W. 1963. The Washington Lobbyists. Chicago: Rand-McNally. Miliband, Ralph. 1969. The State in Capitalist Society. New York: Basic. Mills, C. Wright. 1959. The Power Elite. New York: Oxford University Press (Galaxy edition), orig. pub 1956. Monroe, Alan D. 1979. Consistency between Public Preferences and National Policy Decisions. American Politics Quarterly 7: 3-18. Monroe, Alan D. 1998. Public Opinion and Public Policy 1980-1993. Public Opinion Quarterly 68: 6-28. Olson, Mancur, Jr. 1965. The Logic of Collective Action: Public Goods and the Theory of Groups. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Page, Benjamin I., Larry M. Bartels, and Jason Seawright. 2013. Democracy and the Policy Preferences of Wealthy Americans. Perspectives on Politics 11(1): 51-73. Page, Benjamin I., and Cari Lynn Hennessy. 2008. What Affluent Americans Want from Politics. WP-11-08. Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University. Page, Benjamin I., and Lawrence R. Jacobs. 2009. Class War? What Americans Really Think about Economic Inequality. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Page, Benjamin I., and Robert Y. Shapiro. 1983. Effects of Public Opinion on Policy.” American Political Science Review 77: 175-90. Page, Benjamin I., and Robert Y. Shapiro. 1992. The Rational Public: Fifty Years of Trends in Americans’ Policy Preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Poole, Keith, and Howard Rosenthal. 2007. Ideology and Congress. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Przeworksi, Adam, and Michael Wallerstein. 1982. The Structure of Class Conflict in Democratic Capitalist Societies. American Political Science Review 76:215-238. Schattschneider, E.E. 1960. The Semisovereign People: A Realist’s View of Democracy in America. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Schattschneider, E.E. 1935. Politics, Pressures and the Tariff. New York: Prentice-Hall. Schlozman, Kay Lehman, Sidney Verba, and Henry E.Brady. 2012. The Unheavenly Chorus:Unequal Political Voice and the Broken Promise of American Democracy. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. Sen, Amartya K. 1970. Collective Choice and Social Welfare. San Francisco: Holden-Day. Skocpol, Theda. 1992. Protecting Soldiers and Mothers: The Political Origins of Social Policy in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Skocpol, Theda, and Kenneth Finegold. 1982. State Capacity and Economic Intervention in the Early New Deal. Political Science Quarterly 97: 255-278. Smith, Mark A. 2000. American Business and Political Power: Public Opinion, Elections, and Democracy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Stigler, George J. 1971. The Theory of Economic Regulation. The Bell Journal of Economics and Management Science 2(1): 3-21. Stimson, James A., Michael B. MacKuen, and Robert S. Erikson. 1995. Dynamic Representation. American Political Science Review 89: 543-65. de Tocqueville, Alexis. 2000 [1835, 1840]. Democracy in America. Trans. and ed. Harvey C. Mansfield and Delba Winthrop. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Truman, David B. 1971. The Governmental Process. 2nd. ed. [first ed. pub. 1951]. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Winters, Jeffrey A. 2011. Oligarchy. New York: Cambridge University Press. Winters, Jeffrey A., and Benjamin I. Page. 2009. Oligarchy in the United States? Perspectives on Politics 7(4): 731-51. Wolfinger, Raymond E., and Steven J. Rosenstone. 1080. Who Votes? New Haven: Yale University Press. Zaller, John. 2003. Coming to Terms with V.O. Key’s Concept of Latent Opinion. In Electoral Democracy, ed. Michael B. MacKuen and George Rabinowitz. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
  208. LITERATURE CITED Aitken MRF, Larkin MJW, Dickinson A. 2001. Re-examination of the role of within-compound associations in the retrospective revaluation of causal judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54B:27–51 Baeyens F, Eelen P, Van den Bergh O, Crombez G. 1990. Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans. Learn. Motiv. 21:434–55 Beckers T, De Houwer J, Pine˜no O, Miller RR. 2005. Outcome additivity and outcome maximality influence cue competition in human causal learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31:238–49 Beckers T, Miller RR, De Houwer J, Urushihara K. 2006. Reasoning rats: forward blocking in Pavlo- vian animal conditioning is sensitive to constraints of causal inference. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 135:92–102 Bellebaum C, Daum I. 2004. Effects of age and awareness on eyeblink conditional discrimination learning. Behav. Neurosci. 118:1157–65 Berry CJ, Henson RNA, Shanks DR. 2006. On the relationship between repetition priming and recognition memory: insights from a computational model. J. Mem. Lang. 55:515–33 Bouton ME. 1993. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol. Bull. 114:80–99 Bremner AJ, Mareschal D, Destrebecqz A, Cleeremans A. 2007. Cognitive control of sequential knowledge in 2-year-olds: evidence from an incidental sequence-learning and-generation task. Psychol. Sci. 18:261–66 Brewer WF. 1974. There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, ed. WB Weimer, DS Palermo, pp. 1–42. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Broadbent DE, Fitzgerald P, Broadbent MHP. 1986. Implicit and explicit knowledge in the control of complex systems. Br. J. Psychol. 77:33–50 Cheng PW. 1997. From covariation to causation: a causal power theory. Psychol. Rev. 104:367–405 Cheng PW, Holyoak KJ. 1995. Complex adaptive systems as intuitive statisticians: causality, contingency, and prediction. In Comparative Approaches to Cognitive Science, ed. HL Roitblat, J-A Meyer, pp. 271–302. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Chun MM, Jiang Y. 1998. Contextual cueing: implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cogn. Psychol. 36:28–71 Chun MM, Jiang Y. 2003. Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 29:224–34 Chun MM, Phelps EA. 1999. Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nat. Neurosci. 2:844–47 Clark RE, Squire LR. 1998. Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. Science 280:77–81 Cobos PL, L´opez FJ, Ca˜no A, Almaraz J, Shanks DR. 2002. Mechanisms of predictive and diagnostic causal induction. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 28:331–46 Cook SW, Harris RE. 1937. The verbal conditioning of the galvanic skin reflex. J. Exp. Psychol. 21:202–10 Cornwell BR, Echiverri AM, Grillon C. 2007. Sensitivity to masked conditioned stimuli predicts conditioned response magnitude under masked conditions. Psychophysiology 44:403–6 Dawson ME, Rissling AJ, Schell AM, Wilcox R. 2007. Under what conditions can human affective conditioning occur without contingency awareness? Test of the evaluative conditioning paradigm. Emotion 7:755–66 Dawson ME, Schell AM. 1982. Electrodermal responses to attended and nonattended significant stimuli during dichotic listening. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 8:315–24 De Houwer J. 2009. The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learn. Behav. 37:1–20 De Houwer J, Beckers T. 2002a. Higher-order retrospective revaluation in human causal learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55B:137–51 De Houwer J, Beckers T. 2002b. Second-order backward blocking and unovershadowing in human causal learning. Exp. Psychol. 49:27–33 De Houwer J, Beckers T, Glautier S. 2002. Outcome and cue properties modulate blocking. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55A:965–85 De Houwer J, Vandorpe S, Beckers T. 2007. Statistical contingency has a different impact on preparation judgements than on causal judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60:418–32 Dennis NA, Howard JH, Howard DV. 2006. Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults. Exp. Brain Res. 175:153–64 Dickinson A. 1980. Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. London: Cambridge Univ. Press Dickinson A. 2009. What are association formation models? Learn. Behav. 37:21–24 Dickinson A, Brown KJ. 2007. Flavor-evaluative conditioning is unaffected by contingency knowledge during training with color-flavor compounds. Learn. Behav. 35:36–42 Dickinson A, Burke J. 1996. Within-compound associations mediate the retrospective revaluation of causality judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 49B:60–80 Dickinson A, Shanks DR, Evenden JL. 1984. Judgement of act-outcome contingency: the role of selective attribution. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 36A:29–50 Evans JSBT. 2008. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59:255–78 Field AP, Davey GCL. 1999. Reevaluating evaluative conditioning: a nonassociative explanation of condition- ing effects in the visual evaluative conditioning paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 25:211–24 Fletcher PC, Anderson JM, Shanks DR, Honey R, Carpenter TA, et al. 2001. Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory. Nat. Neurosci. 4:1043–48 Fu QF, Dienes Z, Fu XL. 2009. Can unconscious knowledge allow control in sequence learning? Conscious. Cogn. In press Fu QF, Fu XL, Dienes Z. 2008. Implicit sequence learning and conscious awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 17:185– 202 Gaillard V, Destrebecqz A, Michiels S, Cleeremans A. 2009. Effects of age and practice in sequence learning: a graded account of ageing, learning and control. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 21:255–82 Gawronski B, Hofmann W, Wilbur CJ. 2006. Are “implicit” attitudes unconscious? Conscious. Cogn. 15:485–99 Ghirlanda S. 2005. Retrospective revaluation as simple associative learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 31:107–11 Griffiths O, Mitchell CJ. 2008. Selective attention in human associative learning and recognition memory. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 137:626–48 Haselgrove M. 2009. Reasoning rats or associative animals? A common-element analysis of the effects of additive and subadditive pretraining on blocking. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. In press Holland PC. 2008. Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Learn. Behav. 36:227–41 Howard JH, Howard DV, Dennis NA, Yankovich H, Vaidya CJ. 2004. Implicit spatial contextual learning in healthy aging. Neuropsychology 18:124–34 Jacoby LL. 1991. A process dissociation framework: separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. J. Mem. Lang. 30:513–41 Jim´enez L, Vaquero JMM, Lupi´a˜nez J. 2006. Qualitative differences between implicit and explicit sequence learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32:475–90 Kahneman D. 2003. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am. Psychol. 58:697– 720 Kamin LJ. 1969. Selective association and conditioning. In Fundamental Issues in Associative Learning, ed. NJ Mackintosh, WK Honig, pp. 42–64. Halifax: Dalhousie Univ. Press Kinder A, Shanks DR. 2003. Neuropsychological dissociations between priming and recognition: a single- system connectionist account. Psychol. Rev. 110:728–44 Knight DC, Nguyen HT, Bandettini PA. 2003. Expression of conditional fear with and without awareness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100:15280–83 Knight DC, Nguyen HT, Bandettini PA. 2006. The role of awareness in delay and trace fear conditioning in humans. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 6:157–62 Knight DC, Waters NS, Bandettini PA. 2009. Neural substrates of explicit and implicit fear memory. NeuroImage 45:208–14 Knuttinen M-G, Power JM, Preston AR, Disterhoft JF. 2001. Awareness in classical differential eyeblink conditioning in young and aging humans. Behav. Neurosci. 115:747–57 Koedijker JM, Oudejans RRD, Beek PJ. 2008. Table tennis performance following explicit and analogy learning over 10,000 repetitions. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 39:237–56 Kruschke JK. 2008. Bayesian approaches to associative learning: from passive to active learning. Learn. Behav. 36:210–26 Lagnado DA, Shanks DR. 2002. Probability judgment in hierarchical learning: a conflict between predictive- ness and coherence. Cognition 83:81–112 Le Pelley ME, Beesley T, Suret MB. 2007. Blocking of human causal learning involves learned changes in stimulus processing. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60:1468–76 Leow RP, Bowles MA. 2005. Attention and awareness in SLA. In External and Internal Factors in Adult SLA, ed. C Sanz, pp. 179–203. Washington, DC: Georgetown Univ. Press Livesey EJ, Boakes RA. 2004. Outcome additivity, elemental processing and blocking in human causality judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 57B:361–79 L´opez FJ, Cobos PL, Ca˜no A. 2005. Associative and causal reasoning accounts of causal induction: symmetries and asymmetries in predictive and diagnostic inferences. Mem. Cogn. 33:1388–98 L´opez FJ, Cobos PL, Ca˜no A, Shanks DR. 1998. The rational analysis of human causal and probability judgment. In Rational Models of Cognition, ed. M Oaksford, N Chater, pp. 314–52. London: Oxford Univ. Press Lovibond PF. 2003. Causal beliefs and conditioned responses: retrospective revaluation induced by experience and by instruction. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 29:97–106 Lovibond PF, Been S-L, Mitchell CJ, Bouton ME, Frohardt R. 2003. Forward and backward blocking of causal judgment is enhanced by additivity of effect magnitude. Mem. Cogn. 31:133–42 Lovibond PF, Shanks DR. 2002. The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 28:3–26 Mackintosh NJ. 1975. A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol. Rev. 82:276–98 Mackintosh NJ. 1983. Conditioning and Associative Learning. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Maia TV, Cleeremans A. 2005. Consciousness: converging insights from connectionist modeling and neuro- science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9:397–404 Maia TV, McClelland JL. 2004. A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102:16075–80 Matute H, Vegas S, De Marez PJ. 2002. Flexible use of recent information in causal and predictive judgments. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 28:714–25 McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. 1995. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102:419–57 McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE. 1986. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cogni- tion. Vol. 2: Psychological and Biological Models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press McNally RJ. 1981. Phobias and preparedness: instructional reversal of electrodermal conditioning to fear- relevant stimuli. Psychol. Rep. 48:175–80 Melchers KG, Lachnit H, Shanks DR. 2004a. Past experience influences the processing of stimulus compounds in human Pavlovian conditioning. Learn. Motiv. 35:167–88 Melchers KG, Lachnit H, Shanks DR. 2004b. Within-compound associations in retrospective revaluation and in direct learning: a challenge for comparator theory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 57B:25–53 Melchers KG, Lachnit H, Shanks DR. 2006. The comparator theory fails to account for the selective role of within-compound associations in cue selection effects. Exp. Psychol. 53:316–20 Melchers KG, Shanks DR, Lachnit H. 2008. Stimulus coding in human associative learning: flexible repre- sentations of parts and wholes. Behav. Process. 77:413–27 Melton AW. 1950. Learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1:9–30 Miller RR, Barnet RC, Grahame NJ. 1995. Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model. Psychol. Bull. 117:363– 86 Mitchell CJ, De Houwer J, Lovibond PF. 2009. The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 32:183–246 Mitchell CJ, Killedar A, Lovibond PF. 2005a. Inference-based retrospective revaluation in human causal judgments requires knowledge of within-compound relationships. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 31:418–24 Mitchell CJ, Livesey E, Lovibond PF. 2007. A dissociation between causal judgement and the ease with which a cause is categorized with its effect. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60:400–17 Mitchell CJ, Lovibond PF. 2002. Backward and forward blocking in human electrodermal condition- ing: blocking requires an assumption of outcome additivity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55B:311–29 Mitchell CJ, Lovibond PF, Condoleon M. 2005b. Evidence for deductive reasoning in blocking of causal judgments. Learn. Motiv. 36:77–87 Mitchell CJ, Lovibond PF, Gan CY. 2005c. A dissociation between causal judgment and outcome recall. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12:950–54 Mitchell CJ, Lovibond PF, Minard E, Lavis Y. 2006. Forward blocking in human learning sometimes reflects the failure to encode a cue-outcome relationship. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59:830–44 Mitchell CJ, Wardle SG, Lovibond PF, Weidemann G, Chang BPI. 2009. Is Perruchet’s dissociation between cued reaction time and outcome expectancy evidence for two separate associative learning processes? J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. In press Mowrer RR, Klein SB. 2001. The transitive nature of contemporary learning theory. In Handbook of Contem- porary Learning Theories, ed. RR Mowrer, SB Klein, pp. 1–21. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. 1977. Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev. 84:231–59 Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. 1987. Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 19:1–32 Norman E, Price MC, Duff SC. 2006. Fringe consciousness in sequence learning: the influence of individual differences. Conscious. Cogn. 15:723–60 Norman E, Price MC, Duff SC, Mentzoni RA. 2007. Gradations of awareness in a modified sequence learning task. Conscious. Cogn. 16:809–37 Pearce JM, Hall G. 1980. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol. Rev. 87:532–52 Perales JC, Shanks DR. 2007. Models of covariation-based causal judgment: a review and synthesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14:577–96 Perales JC, Shanks DR. 2008. Driven by power? Probe question and presentation format effects on causal judgment. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34:1482–94 Perruchet P. 1985. A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning. Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci. 20:163–70 Perruchet P, Amorim M-A. 1992. Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: evidence against dissociation. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 18:785–800 Perruchet P, Bigand E, Benoit-Gonin F. 1997. The emergence of explicit knowledge during the early phase of learning in sequential reaction time tasks. Psychol. Res. 60:4–13 Perruchet P, Cleeremans A, Destrebecqz A. 2006. Dissociating the effects of automatic activation and explicit expectancy on reaction times in a simple associative learning task. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32:955–65 Pleyers G, Corneille O, Luminet O, Yzerbyt V. 2007. Aware and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33:130–44 Postman L. 1962. Rewards and punishments in human learning. In Psychology in the Making: Histories of Selected Research Problems, ed. L Postman, pp. 331–401. New York: Knopf Preston AR, Gabrieli JDE. 2008. Dissociation between explicit memory and configural memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Cereb. Cortex 18:2192–207 Price PC, Yates JF. 1993. Judgmental overshadowing: further evidence of cue interaction in contingency judgment. Mem. Cogn. 21:561–72 Rachman S. 1977. The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: a critical examination. Behav. Res. Ther. 15:375– 87 Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of rein- forcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical Conditioning II: Current Theory and Research, ed. AH Black, WF Prokasy, pp. 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts Rogers RF, Matzel LD. 1996. Higher-order associative processing in Hermissenda suggests multiple sites of neuronal modulation. Learn. Mem. 2:279–98 Rumelhart DE, McClelland JL. 1986. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cogni- tion. Vol. 1: Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Schmajuk N, Larrauri J. 2008. Associative models can describe both causal learning and conditioning. Behav. Process. 77:443–45 Scully AL, Mitchell CJ. 2008. Extinction in human learning and memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61:1472–78 Shanks DR. 1985. Forward and backward blocking in human contingency judgement. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 37B:1–21 Shanks DR. 1990. On the cognitive theory of conditioning. Biol. Psychol. 30:171–79 Shanks DR, Channon S, Wilkinson L, Curran HV. 2006. Disruption of sequential priming in organic and pharmacological amnesia: a role for the medial temporal lobes in implicit contextual learning. Neuropsy- chopharmacology 31:1768–76 Shanks DR, Darby RJ. 1998. Feature- and rule-based generalization in human associative learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 24:405–15 Shanks DR, Dickinson A. 1990. Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: a comment on Baeyens, Eelen, and Van den Bergh. Cogn. Emot. 4:19–30 Shanks DR, Johnstone T. 1999. Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25:1435–51 Shanks DR, Perruchet P. 2002. Dissociation between priming and recognition in the expression of sequential knowledge. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9:362–67 Shanks DR, Rowland LA, Ranger MS. 2005. Attentional load and implicit sequence learning. Psychol. Res. 69:369–82 Shanks DR, Wilkinson L, Channon S. 2003. Relationship between priming and recognition in deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 29:248–61 Sloman SA. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 119:3–22 Smith CN, Clark RE, Manns JR, Squire LR. 2005. Acquisition of differential delay eyeblink classical condi- tioning is independent of awareness. Behav. Neurosci. 119:78–86 Smyth AC, Shanks DR. 2008. Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent tests. Mem. Cogn. 36:403–15 Stahl C, Unkelbach C, Corneille O. 2009. On the respective contributions of awareness of unconditioned stim- ulus valence and unconditioned stimulus identity in attitude formation through evaluative conditioning. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 97:404–20 Stefaniak N, Willems S, Adam S, Meulemans T. 2008. What is the impact of the explicit knowledge of sequence regularities on both deterministic and probabilistic serial reaction time task performance? Mem. Cogn. 36:1283–98 Thomas MSC, McClelland JL. 2008. Connectionist models of cognition. In The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology, ed. R Sun, pp. 23–58. London: Cambridge Univ. Press Thorndike EL. 1931. Human Learning. New York: Century Vadillo MA, Miller RR, Matute H. 2005. Causal and predictive-value judgments, but not predictions, are based on cue-outcome contingency. Learn. Behav. 33:172–83 Vaidya CJ, Huger M, Howard DV, Howard JH. 2007. Developmental differences in implicit learning of spatial context. Neuropsychology 21:497–506 Van Hamme LJ, Wasserman EA. 1994. Cue competition in causality judgments: the role of nonpresentation of compound stimulus elements. Learn. Motiv. 25:127–51 Vandenberghe M, Schmidt N, Fery P, Cleeremans A. 2006. Can amnesic patients learn without awareness? New evidence comparing deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. Neuropsychologia 44:1629–41 Vandorpe S, de Houwer J, Beckers T. 2007. The role of memory for compounds in cue competition. Learn. Motiv. 38:195–207 Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W. 2001. Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature 412:43–48 Wagner AR. 1981. SOP: a model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms, ed. NE Spear, RR Miller, pp. 5–47. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Waldmann MR. 2000. Competition among causes but not effects in predictive and diagnostic learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26:53–76 Waldmann MR, Holyoak KJ. 1990. Can causal induction be reduced to associative learning? Proceedings 12th Ann. Conf. Cogn. Sci. Soc., pp. 190–97. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Waldmann MR, Holyoak KJ. 1992. Predictive and diagnostic learning within causal models: asymme- tries in cue competition. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 121:222–36 Walther E, Nagengast B. 2006. Evaluative conditioning and the awareness issue: assessing contingency aware- ness with the four-picture recognition test. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 32:454–59 Wardle SG, Mitchell CJ, Lovibond PF. 2007. Flavor evaluative conditioning and contingency awareness. Learn. Behav. 35:233–41 Weidemann G, Tangen JM, Lovibond PF, Mitchell CJ. 2009. Is Perruchet’s dissociation between eyeblink conditioned responding and outcome expectancy evidence for two learning systems? J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 35:169–76 Weike AI, Schupp HT, Hamm AO. 2007. Fear acquisition requires awareness in trace but not delay condi- tioning. Psychophysiology 44:170–80 Wheeler DS, Beckers T, Miller RR. 2008. The effect of subadditive pretraining on blocking: limits on gen- eralization. Learn. Behav. 36:341–51 Wilkinson L, Shanks DR. 2004. Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30:354–69 Williams DA, Sagness KE, McPhee JE. 1994. Configural and elemental strategies in predictive learning. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 20:694–709 Willingham DB, Salidis J, Gabrieli JDE. 2002. Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. J. Neurophysiol. 88:1451–60 Wilson TD. 2002. Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap
  209. References and Notes 1. D. M. Wegner, in Theories of Group Behavior, B. Mullen, G. R. Goethals, Eds. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1986), pp. 185–208. 2. D. M. Wegner, A computer network model of human transactive memory. Soc Cog. 13, 1 (1995). 3. V. Peltokorpi, Transactive memory systems. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 12, 378 (2008). 4. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 5. R. A. Bjork, Theoretical implications of directed forgetting, in Coding Processes in Human Memory, A.W. Melton, E. Martin, Eds. (Winston, Washington, DC, 1972), pp. 217–235. 6. F. I. Craik, E. Tulving, Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. G. 104, 268 (1975). 7. T. S. Hyde, J. J. Jenkins, Differential effects of incidental tasks on the organization of recall of a list of highly associated words. J. Exp. Psychol. 82, 472 (1969). 8. A. Clark, Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence (Oxford University Press, New York, 2003). 9. N. Carr, Is Google making us stupid? The Atlantic 302, 56 (2008). Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1207745/DC1 Materials and Methods References 10–13
  210. References Abbey, A., Abramis, D. J., & Caplan, R. D. (1985). Effects of different sources of social support and social conflict on emotional well-being. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, 111-129. Abele, A. (1985). Thinking about thinking: Causal, evaluative, and finalistic cognitions about social situations. European Journal of Social Psychol- ogy, 15, 315-332. Acitelli, L. K., Douvan, E., & Veroff, J. (1993). Perceptions of conflict in the first year of marriage: How important are similarity and understanding? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 5-19. Adams-Webber, J. (1977). The golden section and the structure of self-concepts. Perceptual and Mo- tor Skills, 45, 703-706. Afifi, W. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (2000). The impact of violations on uncertainty and the consequences for attractiveness. Human Communication Research, 26, 203-233. Agostinelli, G., Sherman, S. J., Presson, C. C, & Chassin, L. (1992). Self-protection and self-en- hancement biases in estimates of population prev- alence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- tin, 18, 631-642. Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression forma- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 2, 1-9. Aries, P. (1981). The hour of our death. (H. Weaver, Trans.). New York: Knopf. Arnetz, B. B., Wasserman, J., Petrini, B., Brenner, S. O., & Levi L. (1987). Immune function in unemployed women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 3-12. Aronson, E., & Under, D. (1965). Gain and loss of esteem as determinants of interpersonal attractive- ness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 1, 156-171. Atthowe, J. M (1960). Types of conflict and their resolution: A reinterpretation. Journal of Experi- mental Psychology, 59, 1-9. Averill, J. R. (1980). On the paucity of positive emotions. In K. Blankstein, P. Pliner, & J. Polivy (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect (Vol. 6, p. 745). New York: Plenum. Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., Van den Bergh, O., & Crom- bez, G. (1990). Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans. Learning and Motiva- tion, 21, 434-455. Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. (1994). Affect and mem- ory in retrospective reports. In S. Sudman & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. New York: Springer-Verlag. Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The self in social contexts. In L. Porter & M. Rosenzweig (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 45, pp. 297-332). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. Bartholow, B. D., Fabiani, M, Gratton, G., & Bet- tencourt, B. A. (1999). Gazing into the cognitive toolbox: Expectancy violations and event-related brain potentials. Manuscript submitted for publi- cation. Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford Press. Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill. Baumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self-presentation: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 62, 851-862. Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1993). When ego threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences of high self-es- teem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 64, 141-156. Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1985). Self-esteem and responses to success and failure: Subsequent performance and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality, 53, 450-467. Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Hutton, D. G. (1989). Self-presentational motivations and per- sonality differences in self-esteem. Journal of Per- sonality, 57, 547-579. Benjafield, J. (1984). On the relation between the Pollyanna and golden section hypotheses. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 83-84. Benjafield, J., & Adams-Webber, J. (1976). The golden section hypothesis. British Journal of Psy- chology, 67, 11-15. Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Berry, D. S., Pennebaker, J. W., Mueller, J. S., & Hiller, W. S. (1997). Linguistic bases of socialperception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 526-537. Birnbaum, M. H. (1972). Morality judgments: Test of an averaging model. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology, 93, 35-42. Blaine, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Self-esteem and self-serving biases in reactions to positive and neg- ative events: An integrative review. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 55-85). New York: Plenum. Bless, H., Hamilton, D. L., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). Mood effects on the organization of person infor- mation. European Journal of Social Psychol- ogy, 22, 497-509. Bolster, B. I., & Springbett, B. M. (1961). The reac- tion of interviewers to favorable and unfavorable information. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45, 97-103. Boucher, J., & Osgood, C. E. (1969). The Pollyanna hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Ver- bal Behavior, 8, 1-8. Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Affective norms for English words. Gaines- ville, FL: Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention—National Institute of Mental Health, University of Florida. Brewer, W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sam- pled autobiographical events. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory (pp. 21-90). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation level theory: A sympo- sium (pp. 287-302). New York: Academic Press. Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 36, 917-927. Brown, J. S. (1940). Generalized approach and avoidance responses in relation to conflict behav- ior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale Uni- versity. Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relation- ship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401-423. Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychological Re- view, 1, 3-25. Cahill, C, Llewelyn, S. P., & Pearson, C. (1991). Long-term effects of sexual abuse which occurred in childhood: A review. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 117-130. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The quality of American life: Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell Sage. Carlson, E. R. (1966). The affective tone of psychol- ogy. Journal of General Psychology, 75, 65-78. Cassirer, E. (1955). The philosophy of symbolic forms: Vol. 1. Language. Yale University Press. (Original work published 1921) Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Bask- ing in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366-375. Cialdini, R. B., & Richardson, K. D. (1980). Two indirect tactics of image management: Basking and blasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 39, 406-415. Claeys, W., & Timmers, L. (1993). Some instantia- tions of the informational negativity effect: Posi- tive-negative asymmetry in category breadth and in estimated meaning similarity of trait adjectives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 111- 129. Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (1988). The semantics of affective lexicon. In V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, & N. H. Frijda (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on emotion and motivation. NATO ASI series D: Be- havioural and social sciences (Vol. 44, pp. 367- 397). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Aca- demic. Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social infor- mation processing. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 323- 417). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, S., & Herbert, T. B. (1996). Health psychol- ogy: Psychological factors and physical disease from the perspective of human psychoneuroimmu- nology. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 113- 142. Coleman, L. M., Jussim, L., & Abraham, J. (1987). Students' reactions to teacher evaluations: The unique impact of negative feedback. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 1051-1070. Costantini, A. F., & Hoving, K. L. (1973). The ef- fectiveness of reward and punishment contingen- cies on response inhibition. Journal of Experimen- tal Child Psychology, 16, 484-494. Czapinski, J. (1985). Negativity bias in psychology: An analysis of Polish publications. Polish Psycho- logical Bulletin, 16, 27-44. Czapinski, J. (1986). Informativeness of evaluations in interpersonal communication: Effects of va- lence, extremity of evaluations, and ego-involve- ment of evaluator. Polish Psychological Bulle- tin, 17, 155-164. David, J. P., Green, P. J., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). Differential roles of neuroticism, extraver- sion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 73, 149-159. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psycholog- ical Bulletin, 95, 542-575. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1253-1265. Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior based impression formation. Journal of Personal- ity and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-1768. Ebbesen, E. B., Kjos, G. L., & Konecni, V. J. (1976). Spatial ecology: Its effects on the choice of friends and enemies. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- chology, 12, 505-518. Eliade, M. (1982). A history of religious ideas. Vol. 2: From Gautama Buddha to the triumph of Chris- tianity (W. Trask, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Eliade, M. (1985). A history of religious ideas. Vol. 3: From Muhammad to the Age of Reforms. (A. Hiltebeitel & D. Apostolos-Cappadona, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Elliot, A., & Covington, M. (2001). Approach and avoidance motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 73-92. Esses, V. M, & Zanna, M. P. (1995). Mood and the expression of ethnic stereotypes. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 69, 1052-1068. Exline, J. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1999). [Forgive- ness in prisoner's dilemma games]. Unpublished findings, Case Western Reserve University. Feldman, S. (1966). Motivational aspects of attitudi- nal elements and their place in cognitive interac- tion. In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive consistency: Motivational antecedents and behavioral conse- quences (pp. 75-108). New York: Academic Press. Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. (1950). So- cial pressures in informal groups: A study of a housing community. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford Uni- versity Press. Fiedler, L. A. (1982). Love and death in the Ameri- can novel. New York: Stein & Day. (Original work published 1966). Finch, J. F., Okun, M. A., Barrera, M., Jr., Zautra, A. J., & Reich, J. W. (1989). Positive and negative social ties among older adults: Measurement mod- els and the prediction of psychological distress and well-being. American Journal of Community Psy- chology, 17, 585-605. Finkenauer, C, & Rime", B. (1998). Socially shared emotional experiences vs. emotional experiences kept secret: Differential characteristics and conse- quences. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol- ogy, 17, 295-318. Fiore, J., Becker, J., & Coppel, D. B. (1983). Social network interactions: A buffer or a stress. Ameri- can Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 423- 439. Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 22, 889-906. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fleming, J., Mullen, P. E., Sibthorpe, B., & Bammer, G. (1999). The long-term impact of childhood sex- ual abuse in Australian women. Child Abuse and Neglect, 23, 145-159. Forgas, J. P. (1992). Mood and the perception of unusual people: Affective asymmetry in memory and social judgments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 531-547. Francis, M. E., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1992). Putting stress into words: The impact of writing on phys- iological, absentee, and self-reported emotional well-being measures. American Journal of Health Promotion, 6, 280-287. Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man's search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy. New York: Washing- ton Square Press. French, D. C, Waas, G. A., Tarver-Behring, S. A. (1986). Nomination and rating scale sociometrics: Convergent validity and clinical utility. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 331-340. Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psy- chonomic Science, 4, 123-124. Gauggel, S., Wietasch, A., Bayer, C, & Rolko, C. (2000). The impact of positive and negative feed- back on reaction time in brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychology, 14, 125-133. Gilbert, A. N., Fridlund, A. J., & Sabini, J. (1987). Hedonic and social determinants of facial displays to odors. Chemical Senses, 12, 355-363. Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C, Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617-638. Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluation and persis- tence in gambling. Journal of Social and Personal Psychology, 44, 1110-1126. Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn't so. New York: Free Press. Glaser, R., Rice, J., Sheridan, J., Fertel, R., & Stout, J.C. (1987). Stress-related immune suppression:Health implications. Brain Behavior Immunol- ogy, 1, 7-20. Goodhart, D. E. (1986). The effects of positive and negative thinking on performance in an achieve- ment situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 117-124. Gottman, J. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Simon & Schuster. Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction. New York: Academic Press. Gottman, J. M., & Krokoff, L. J. (1989). Marital interaction and satisfaction: A longitudinal view. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- ogy, 57, 47-52. Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1986). Assess- ing the role of emotion in marriage. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 31-48. Graziano, W. G., Brothen, T., & Berscheid, E. (1980). Attention, attraction, and individual differ- ences in reaction to criticism. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 38, 193-202. Greenberg, M. A., & Stone, A. A. (1992). Writing about disclosed versus undisclosed traumas: Im- mediate and long-term effects on mood and health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 75-84. Greenwald, A.G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabri- cation and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603-618. Hamilton, D. L., & Huffman, L. J. (1971). Generality of impression formation processes for evaluative and nonevaluative judgments. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 20, 200-207. Hamilton, D. L., & Zanna, M. P. (1972). Differential weighting of favorable and unfavorable attributes in impressions of personality. Journal of Experi- mental Research in Personality, 6, 204-212. Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory: An ex- perimental and systematic approach to behavior. New York: Harper. Herbert, T. B., Cohen, S., Marsland, A. L., Bachen, E. A., & Rabin, B. S. (1994). Cardiovascular re- activity and the course of immune response to an acute psychological stressor. Psychosomatic Med- icine, 56, 337-344. Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in America. New York: Free Press. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319-340. Higgins, E. T. (1996). The "self digest": Self-knowl- edge serving self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1062-1083. Hodges, B. H. (1974). Effect of valence on relative weighting in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 378-381. Holmes, D. S. (1970). Differential change in affec- tive intensity and the forgetting of unpleasant per- sonal experiences. Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 3, 234-239. Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal attributions of marital couples: When do they search for causes? What do they conclude when they do? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1398-1412. Hoorens, V. (1995). Self-favoring biases, self-pre- sentation, and the self-other asymmetry in social comparison. Journal of Personality, 63, 793-817. Hoorens, V. (1996). Self-favoring biases for positive and negative characteristics: Independent phenom- ena? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol- ogy, 15, 53-67. Huston, T. L., Caughlin, J. P., Houts, R. M., Smith, S. E., & George, L. J. (2001). The connubial cru- cible: Newlywed years as predictors of marital delight, distress, and divorce. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 80, 281-293. Huston, T. L., & Vangelisti, A. L. (1991). Socioemo- tional behavior and satisfaction in marital relation- ships: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 721-733. Ikegami, T. (1993). Positive-negative asymmetry of priming effects on impression formation. Euro- pean Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 1-16. Ito, T. A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Lang, P. J. (1998). Eliciting affecting using the International Affec- tive Picture System: Bivariate evaluation and am- bivalence. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- letin, 24, 855-879. Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in eval- uative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887-900. Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor. Westwood, CT: Praeger. Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. C. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-handicap- ping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psy- chology Bulletin, 4, 200-206. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person per- ception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in exper- imental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219-266). New York: Academic Press. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect the- ory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econo- metrica, 47, 263-291. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychol- ogy of preferences. Scientific American, 246, 160— 173. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, val- ues, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341— 350. Kanouse, D. E., & Hanson, L. R. (1972). Negativity in evaluations. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of be- havior (pp. 47-62). Morristown, NJ: General Learning. Keinan, G. (1987). Decision making under stress: Scanning of alternatives under controllable and uncontrollable threats. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 639-644. Kellermann, K. (1984). The negativity effect and its implications for initial interaction. Communication Monographs, 51, 37-55. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Garner, W., Speicher, C. E., Penn, G. M., Holliday, J., & Glaser, R. (1984). Psychosocial modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 7-14. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., Strain, E., Stout, J., & Tarr, K. (1986). Modulation of cellular immu- nity in medical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 5-21. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., Williger, D., Stout, J., & Messick, G. (1985). Psychological enhance- ment of immunocompetence in a geriatric popula- tion. Health Psychology, 4, 25-41. Klein, W. M. (1992, March). Constructing social reality: Greater bias for negative than for positive behaviors? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston. Klinger, E., Barta, S. G., & Maxeiner, M. E. (1980). Motivational correlates of thought content fre- quency and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1222-1237. Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1968). Unhappy memo- ries of the "happy past": Studies in cognitive dis- sonance. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 157- 166. Krull, D. S., & Dill, J. C. (1998). Do smiles elicit more inferences than do frowns? The effect of emotional valence on the production of spontane- ous inferences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 289-300. Kuiper, N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1982). Depressed and nondepressed content self-reference in mild de- pression. Journal of Personality, 50, 67-79. Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool chil- dren. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1038-1045. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1986). Affect intensity and reactions to daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 803- 814. Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization ofsexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the United States: Preva- lence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 537-544. Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal mon- itor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 68, 518-530. LeDoux, J. E., Romanski, L., & Xagoraris, A. (1989). Indelibility of subcortical memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 238-243. Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 71, 1250-1267. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective ex- change. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 45, 587-597. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1985). Physio- logical and affective predictors of change in rela- tionship satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 85-94. Luu, P., Collins, P., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: Negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mech- anisms of error monitoring. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology: General, 129, 43-60. Major, B., Zubek, J. M., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C, & Richards, C. (1997). Mixed messages: Im- plications of social conflict and social support within close relationships for adjustment to a stressful life event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1349-1363. Manne, S. L., Taylor, K. L., Dougherty, J., & Ke- meny, N. (1997). Supportive and negative re- sponses in the partner relationship: Their associ- ation with psychological adjustment among in- dividuals with cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 101-125. Marshall, L. L., & Kidd, R. F. (1981): Good news or bad news first? Social Behavior and Personality, 9, 223-226. Martijn, C, Spears, R., van der Pligt, J., & Jakobs, E. (1992). Negativity and positivity effects in person perception and inference: Ability versus morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 453- 463. Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of be- ing. New York: Van Nostrand. Matlin, M. W., & Stang, D. J. (1978). The pollyanna principle. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman. Matt, J., Vasquez, C, & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stim- uli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227-255. McCarthy, B. W. (1999). Marital style and its effects on sexual desire and functioning. Journal of Fam- ily Psychotherapy, 10, 1-12. McFarlin, D. B., & Blascovich, J. (1981). Effects of self-esteem and performance feedback on future affective preferences and cognitive expectations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 521-531. Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (1997). When doing better means feeling worse: The effects of categor- ical cutoff points on counterfactual thinking and satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1284-1296. Meyer, W. J., & Offenbach, S. (1962). Effectiveness of reward and punishment as a function of task complexity. Journal of Comparative and Physio- logical Psychology, 55, 532-534. Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. McV. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behav- ior disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 431-465). New York: Ronald Press. Miltner, W. H. R., Braun, C. H., & Coles, M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evi- dence for a "generic" neural system for error de- tection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 788-798. Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1976). Determinants of selective memory about the self. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- ogy, 44, 92-103. Mullen, B., & Johnson, C. (1990). Distinctiveness- based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 11-28. Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness. New York: Avon Books. Myers, J. L., Reilly, R. E., & Taub, H. A. (1961). Differential cost, gain, and relative frequency of reward in a sequential choice situation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 357-360. Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children's peer relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controver- sial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 99-128. Nezlek, J. B., & Gable, S. L. (1999). Daily events, day-to-day psychological adjustment, and depres- sive symptoms. Manuscript submitted for publica- tion. Oehman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 381-396. Ogilvie, D. M. (1987). The undesired self: A ne- glected variable in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 379-385. Ohira, H., Winton, W. M., & Oyama, M. (1997). Effects of stimulus valence on recognition memory and endogenous eyeblinks: Further evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Personality and So- cial Psychology Bulletin, 24, 986-993. Okun, M. A., Melichar, J. F., & Hill, M. D. (1990). Negative daily events, positive and negative social ties, and psychological distress among older adults. Gerontologist, 30, 193-199. Osgood, C. E., & Tzeng, O. C. S. (1990). Language, meaning, and culture: The selected papers ofC. E. Osgood. New York: Praeger. Pagel, M. D., Erdly, W. W., & Becker, J. (1987). Social networks: We get by with (and in spite of) a little help from our friends. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 53, 793-804. Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-nega- tive asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction be- tween affective and informational negativity ef- fects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Eu- ropean review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33-60). New York: Wiley. Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539-548. Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confront- ing a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 528-537. Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J., & Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune func- tion: Health implications for psychotherapy. Jour- nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 239-245. Penney, R. K. (1968). Effect of reward and punish- ment on children's orientation and discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 140-142. Penney, R. K., & Lupton, A. A. (1961). Children's discrimination learning as a function of reward and punishment. Journal of Comparative and Physio- logical Psychology, 54, 449-451. Pittman, J. F., & Lloyd, S. A. (1988). Quality of family life, social support, and stress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 53-67. Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Procedures of the National Acad- emy of Sciences, USA, 91, 7398-7403. Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391. Quirk, G. J., Repa, J. C, & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: Parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron, 15, 1029-1039. Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Error correction time with- out external error signals. Nature, 212, 438. Reyes, G. M., Afifi, W. A., Krawchuk, A., Imperato, N., Shelley, D., & Lee, J. (1999, June). Just (don't) talk: Comparing the impact of interaction style on sexual desire and social attraction. Paper pre- sented at the conference of the Internationa] Net- work on Personal Relationships, Louisville, KY. Riskey, D. R., & Birnbaum, M. H. (1974). Compen- satory effects in moral judgment: Two rights don't make up for a wrong. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 171-173. Riskin, J., & Faunce, E. E. (1970). Family interaction scales: III. Discussion of methodology and sub- stantive findings. Archives of General Psychiatry, 22, 527-537. Robinson-Riegler, G. L., & Winton, W. M. (1996). The role of conscious recollection in recognition of affective material: Evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Journal of General Psychology, 123, 93-104. Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C, MacCallum, R. C, & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1997). Distinguishing opti- mism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1345-1353. Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1097-1108. Rothbart, M, & Park, B. (1986). On the confirmabil- ity and disconfirmability of trait concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 131— 142. Rowe, D. C, Jacobson, K. C, & Van den Oord, E. J. C. G. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as a moderator. Child Development, 70, 1151-1162. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (in press). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Per- sonality and Social Psychology Review. Ruehlman, L. S., & Wolchik, S. A. (1988). Personal goals and interpersonal support and hindrance as factors in psychological distress and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 293-301. Rusbult, C. E., Johnson, D. J., & Morrow, G. D. (1986). Impact of couple patterns of problem solv- ing on distress and nondistress in dating relation- ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 50, 744-753. Rusbult, C. E., & Zembrodt, J. M. (1983). Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Journal of Ex- perimental Social Psychology, 19, 274-293. Russell, J. A., Fernandez-Dols, J. M, Manstead, A. S. R., & Wellenkamp, J. (Eds.). (1995). Every- day conceptions of emotion: An introduction to the psychology, anthropology, and linguistics of emo- tion. Behavioral and social sciences (Vol. 81, pp. 244-297). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. Rynd, N. (1988). Incidence of psychometric symp- toms in rape victims. Journal of Sex Research, 24, 155-161. Sanghera, M. K., Rolls, E. T., & Roper-Hall, A. (1979). Visual response of neurons in the dorso- lateral amygdala of the alert monkey. Experimen- tal Neurology, 63, 610-626. Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990's: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1-19. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized expectancy on health. Health Psy- chology, 4, 219-247. Schulz, R., Bookwala, J., Knapp, J. E., Scheier, M, & Williamson, G. M. (1996). Pessimism, age, and cancer mortality. Psychology and Aging, 11, 304- 309. Schuster, T. L., Kessler, R. C, & Aseltine, R. H., Jr. (1990). Supportive interactions, negative interac- tions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 423-438. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Infor- mational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foun- dations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527-561). New York: Guilford Press. Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 65, 317-338. Seligman, M. (1999, April). Positive social science [president's column]. APA Monitor, p. 2. Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. (1999, August). The regulatory focus and goal organization: Implications for self-regulation. Pa- per presented at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston. Sheldon, K. M, Ryan, R., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270-1279. Silver, R. L., Boon, C, & Stones, M. H. (1983). Searching for meaning in misfortune: Making sense of incest. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 81- 102. Skowronski, J. J., Betz, A. L., Thompson, C. P., & Shannon, L. (1991). Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 831— 843. Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extrem- ity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 52, 689-699. Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Nega- tivity and extremity biases in impression forma- tion: A review of explanation. Psychological Re- view, 105, 131-142. Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1992). Caught in the act: When impressions based on highly diagnostic behaviours are resistant to contradic- tion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 435-452. Spence, J. T. (1966). Verbal discrimination perfor- mance as a function of instruction and verbal- reinforcement combination in normal and retarded children. Child Development, 37, 269-281. Spence, J. T., & Segner, L. L. (1967). Verbal vs. nonverbal reinforcement combinations in the dis- crimination learning of middle and lower class children. Child Development, 38, 29-38. Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic relationship type, gender, and relational characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 217-242. Sternbach, R. A. (1968). Pain: A psychophysiologi- cal analysis. New York: Academic Press. Styron, T., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1997). Childhood attachment and abuse: Long-term effects on adult attachment, depression and conflict resolution. Child Abuse and Neglect, 21, 1015-1023. Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173. Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of posi- tive and negative events: The mobilization-mini- mization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193— 210. Thomas, D.L., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accu- racy in the recall of emotions. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 59, 291-297. Thompson, L. A., Tiu, R. D., & Detterman, D. K. (1999, July). Differences in the heritability of gen- eral intelligence across levels of father's occupa- tion. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association, Vancouver, Brit- ish Columbia, Canada. Tice, D. M. (1991). Esteem protection or enhance- ment? Self-handicapping motives and attributions differ by trait self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 711-725. Tindall, R. C, & Ratliff, R. G. (1974). Interaction of reinforcement conditions and developmental level in a two-choice discrimination task with children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 183-189. Ting-Toomey, S. (1983). An analysis of verbal com- munication patterns in high and low marital adjust- ment groups. Human Communication Research, 9, 306-319. Tuohy, A. P., & Stradling, S. G. (1987). Maximum salience vs. golden section proportions in judge- mental asymmetry. British Journal of Psychol- ogy, 78, 457-464. Van Goozen, S., & Frijda, N. H. (1993). Emotion words used in six European countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 89-95. Vinokur, A. D., & van Ryn, M. (1993). Social sup- port and undermining in close relationships: Their independent effects on the mental health of unem- ployed persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 350-359. Vonk, R. (1993). The negativity effect in trait ratings and in open-ended descriptions of persons. Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 269- 278. Vonk, R. (1996). Negativity and potency effects in impression formation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26. 851-865. Vonk, R. (1999). Differential evaluations of likable and dislikeable behaviors enacted towards superi- ors and subordinates. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 139-146. Vonk, R., & Knippenberg, A. V. (1994). The sover- eignty of negative inferences: Suspicion of ulterior motives does not reduce the negativity effect. So- cial Cognition, 12, 169-186. Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (1997). Autobiographical memory: Unpleasant- ness fades faster than pleasantness over time. Ap- plied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 399-413. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a con- sensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulle- tin, 98, 219-235. Webster, E. C. (1964). Decision making in the em- ployment interview. Montreal, Quebec, Canada: McGill University Industrial Relations Centre. Weinberg, G. M. (1975). An introduction to general systems thinking. New York: Wiley. Weiner, B. (1985). "Spontaneous" causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74-84. Weinstein, N.D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 39, 806-820. Weiss, E. L., Longhurst, J. G., & Mazure, C. M. (1999). Childhood sexual abuse as a risk factor for depression in women: Psychosocial and neurobio- logical correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 816-828. Wells, J. D., Hobfoll, S. E., & Lavin, J. (1999). When it rains, it pours: The greater impact of resource loss compared to gain on psychological distress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1172-1182. Wills, T. A., Weiss, R. L., & Patterson, G. R. (1974). A behavioral analysis of the determinants of mar- ital satisfaction. Journal of Consulting and Clini- cal Psychology, 6, 802-811. Wright, B. A. (1991). Labeling: The need for greater person-environment individuation. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective (pp. 469-487). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon. Wyer, R. S., & Hinkle, R. L. (1971). Informational factors underlying inferences about hypothetical persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 34, 481-495. Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J. P. (1991). Requesting information to form an impression: The influence of valence and confirmatory status. Journal of Ex- perimental Social Psychology, 27, 337-356.
  211. References . Hatfield E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL (1993) Emotional contagion. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2(3):96–100. 2. Fowler JH, Christakis NA (2008) Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. BMJ 337:a2338. 3. Rosenquist JN, Fowler JH, Christakis NA (2011) Social network determinants of de- pression. Mol Psychiatry 16(3):273–281. 4. Cohen-Cole E, Fletcher JM (2008) Is obesity contagious? Social networks vs. environ- mental factors in the obesity epidemic. J Health Econ 27(5):1382–1387. 5. Aral S, Muchnik L, Sundararajan A (2009) Distinguishing influence-based contagion from homophily-driven diffusion in dynamic networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(51):21544–21549. 6. Turkle S (2011) Alone Together: Why We Expect More from Technology and Less from Each Other (Basic Books, New York). 7. Guillory J, et al. (2011) Upset now? Emotion contagion in distributed groups. Proc ACM CHI Conf on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Association for Computing Machinery, New York), pp 745–748. 8. Kramer ADI (2012) The spread of emotion via Facebook. Proc CHI (Association for Computing Machinery, New York), pp 767–770. 9. Pennebaker JW, Chung CK, Ireland M, Gonzales A, Booth RJ (2007) The development and psychological properties of LIWC2007. Available at http://liwc.net/howliwcworks. php. Accessed May 10, 2014. 10. Golder SA, Macy MW (2011) Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science 333(6051):1878–1881. 11. Thusoo A; Facebook Data Infrastructure Team (2009) Hive–A warehousing solution over a map-reduce framework. Proc VLDB 2(2):1626–1629. 12. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD (2001) Bad is stronger than good. Rev Gen Psychol 5(4):323–370. 13. Festinger L (1954) A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat 7(2):117–140. 14. Prentice DA, Miller DT (1992) When small effects are impressive. Psychol Bull 112(1): 160–164. 15. Bond RM, et al. (2012) A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature 489(7415):295–298.
  212. REFERENCES [1] L. Aalto, N. G¨othlin, J. Korhonen, and T. Ojala. Bluetooth and WAP Push Based Location-aware Mobile Advertising System. In Proceedings of MobiSys ’04, pages 49–58, 2004. [2] H. Akaike. A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, C-19(6):716–723, 1974. [3] S. Boccaletti, D. Valladares, L. Pecora, H. Geffert, and T. Carroll. Reconstructing embedding spaces of coupled dynamical systems from multivariate data. Physical Review E, 65(3):35204–35204, 2002. [4] D. Brockmann, L. Hufnagel, and T. Geisel. The scaling laws of human travel. Nature, 439:462–465, 2006. [5] L. Cao, A. Mees, and K. Judd. Dynamics from multivariate time series. Physica D, 121(1-2):75–88, 1998. [6] M. Casdagli. Nonlinear prediction of chaotic time series. Physica D, 35(3):335–356, 1989. [7] C. Chatfield. The Analysis of Time Series An Introduction. Chapman and Hall CRC, 2004. [8] T. M. Cover and J. A. Thomas. Elements of Information Theory. Wiley, 2006. [9] A. Fraser and H. Swinney. Independent coordinatesfor strange attractors from mutual information. Physical Review A, 33(2):1134, 1986. [10] M. C. Gonz´alez, C. A. Hidalgo, and A.-L. Barab´asi. Understanding individual human mobility patterns. Nature, 453:779–782, June 2008. [11] R. Hegger and H. Kantz. Improved false nearest neighbor method to detect determinism in time series data. Physical Review E, 60(4):4970, 1999. [12] R. Hegger, H. Kantz, and T. Schreiber. Practical implementation of nonlinear time series methods: The TISEAN package. Chaos, 9(2):413–435, 1999. [13] H. Kantz and T. Schreiber. Nonlinear time series analysis. Cambridge University Press, 1997. [14] M. Kennel, R. Brown, and H. Abarbanel. Determining embedding dimension for phase-space reconstruction using a geometrical construction. Physical Review A, 45(6):3403, 1992. [15] J. K. Laurila, D. Gatica-Perez, I. Aad, J. Blom, O. Bornet, T.-M.-T. Do, O. Dousse, J. Eberle, and M. Miettinen. The Mobile Data Challenge: Big Data for Mobile Computing Research. In Proceedings of the Mobile Data Challenge by Nokia Workshop. Colocated with Pervasive’12, 2012. [16] D. Lazer et al. Computational Social Science. Science, 323:721–723, February 2009. [17] S. Scellato, M. Musolesi, C. Mascolo, V. Latora, and A. T. Campbell. NextPlace: A Spatio-temporal Prediction Framework for Pervasive Systems. In Proceedings of Pervasive’11, San Francisco, California, USA, June 2011. [18] G. Schwarz. Estimating the Dimension of a Model. Ann. Stat., 6(2):461–464, 1978. [19] L. Su. Prediction of multivariate chaotic time series with local polynomial fitting. Computers & Mathematics with Applications, 59(2):737–744, 2010. [20] F. Takens. Detecting strange attractors in turbulence. In Dynamical Systems and Turbulence, Lecture Notes in Mathematics, volume 898, pages 366–381. Springer-Verlag, 1981. [21] I. Vlachos and D. Kugiumtzis. State space reconstruction from multiple time series. In Topics on Chaotic Systems: Selected Papers from Chaos 2008 International Conference, page 378. World Scientific Publishing, 2009.
  213. References [1] Wassily W Leontief. Quantitative input and output relations in the economic systems of the united states. The review of economic statistics, pages 105–125, 1936. [2] Thomas Wiedmann, Harry C Wilting, Manfred Lenzen, Stephan Lutter, and Viveka Palm. Quo vadis mrio? methodolog- ical, data and institutional requirements for multi-region input–output analysis. Ecological Economics, 70(11):1937–1945, 2011. [3] Arnold Tukker and Erik Dietzenbacher. Global multiregional input–output frameworks: an introduction and outlook. Economic Systems Research, 25(1):1–19, 2013. [4] Richard Baldwin and Javier Lopez-Gonzalez. Supply-chain trade: A portrait of global patterns and several testable hypotheses. Technical report, National Bureau of Economic Research, 2013. [5] Marcel P Timmer, Bart Los, Robert Stehrer, and Gaaitzen J Vries. Fragmentation, incomes and jobs: an analysis of european competitiveness. Economic Policy, 28(76):613–661, 2013. [6] Robert Koopman, Zhi Wang, and Shang-Jin Wei. Tracing value-added and double counting in gross exports. American Economic Review, 104(2):459–94, 2014. [7] M Lenzen, D Moran, K Kanemoto, B Foran, L Lobefaro, and A Geschke. International trade drives biodiversity threats in developing nations. Nature, 486(7401):109–112, 2012. [8] Manfred Lenzen, Daniel Moran, Anik Bhaduri, Keiichiro Kanemoto, Maksud Bekchanov, Arne Geschke, and Barney Foran. International trade of scarce water. Ecological Economics, 94:78–85, 2013. [9] Thomas O Wiedmann, Heinz Schandl, Manfred Lenzen, Daniel Moran, Sangwon Suh, James West, and Keiichiro Kanemoto. The material footprint of nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, page 201220362, 2013. [10] Florian Bl¨ochl, Fabian J Theis, Fernando Vega-Redondo, and Eric ON Fisher. Vertex centralities in input-output networks reveal the structure of modern economies. Physical Review E, 83(4):046127, 2011. [11] James McNerney, Brian D Fath, and Gerald Silverberg. Network structure of inter-industry flows. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 392(24):6427–6441, 2013. [12] Martha G Alatriste Contreras and Giorgio Fagiolo. Propagation of economic shocks in input-output networks: A cross- country analysis. arXiv preprint arXiv:1401.4704, 2014. [13] R Albert and A.-L. Barab´asi. Statistical mechanics of complex networks. Reviews of Modern Physics, 74(1):47–97, 2002. [14] M. E. J. Newman. The Structure and Function of Complex Networks. SIAM Review, 45:167–256, 2003. [15] Mark Buchanan, Guido Caldarelli, Paolo De Los Rios, and Vendruscolo Michele. Networks in cell biology. Cambridge University Press, 2010. [16] Maksim Kitsak, Massimo Riccaboni, Shlomo Havlin, Fabio Pammolli, and H Eugene Stanley. Scale-free models for the structure of business firm networks. Physical Review E, 81(3):036117, 2010. [17] Fabio Pammolli and Massimo Riccaboni. Technological regimes and the growth of networks: An empirical analysis. Small Business Economics, 19(3):205–215, 2002. [18] Massimo Riccaboni and Stefano Schiavo. Structure and growth of weighted networks. New Journal of Physics, 12(2):023003, 2010. [19] Massimo Riccaboni, Alessandro Rossi, and Stefano Schiavo. Global networks of trade and bits. Journal of Economic Interaction and Coordination, 8(1):33–56, 2013. [20] Alessandro Chessa, Andrea Morescalchi, Fabio Pammolli, Orion Penner, Alexander M Petersen, and Massimo Riccaboni. Is europe evolving toward an integrated research area? Science, 339(6120):650–651, 2013. [21] Guido Caldarelli, Alessandro Chessa, Fabio Pammolli, Andrea Gabrielli, and Michelangelo Puliga. Reconstructing a credit network. Nature Physics, 9:125–126, 2013. [22] Daron Acemoglu, Vasco M Carvalho, Asuman Ozdaglar, and Alireza Tahbaz-Salehi. The network origins of aggregate fluctuations. Econometrica, 80(5):1977–2016, 2012. [23] Vasco M Carvalho. A survey paper on recent developments of input-output analysis. Technical report, Complexity Research Initiative for Systemic InstabilitieS, 2013. [24] Marcel Timmer, AA Erumban, J Francois, A Genty, R Gouma, B Los, F Neuwahl, O Pindyuk, J Poeschl, JM Rueda- Cantuche, et al. The world input-output database (wiod): Contents, sources and methods. WIOD Background document available at www. wiod. org, 2012. [25] Alak Ghosh. Input-output approach in an allocation system. Economica, pages 58–64, 1958. [26] Ronald E Miller and Peter D Blair. Input-output analysis: foundations and extensions. Cambridge University Press, 2009. [27] Pan A Yotopoulos and Jeffrey B Nugent. A balanced-growth version of the linkage hypothesis: a test. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 87(2):157–171, 1973. [28] Prem S Laumas. The weighting problem in testing the linkage hypothesis. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, pages 308–312, 1976. [29] Emmerich Thorsten, Armin Bunde, and Shlomo Havlin. Structural and functional properties of spatially embedded scale- free networks. Physical Review E, 89:062806, 2014. [30] Xavier Gabaix. Power laws in economics and finance. Annual Review of Economics, 1(1):255–294, 2009. [31] Xavier Gabaix and Rustam Ibragimov. Rank- 1/2: a simple way to improve the ols estimation of tail exponents. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics, 29(1):24–39, 2011. [32] Santo Fortunato. Community detection in graphs. Physics Reports, 486(35):75 – 174, 2010. [33] Mark EJ Newman and Michelle Girvan. Finding and evaluating community structure in networks. Physical review E, 69(2):026113, 2004. [34] Simon Kuznets. Quantitative aspects of the economic growth of nations: Ii. industrial distribution of national product and labor force. Economic Development and Cultural Change, pages 1–111, 1957. [35] Simon Kuznets. Modern economic growth: findings and reflections. The American Economic Review, pages 247–258, 1973. [36] Lawrence Page, Sergey Brin, Rajeev Motwani, and Terry Winograd. The pagerank citation ranking: Bringing order to the web. 1999. [37] Vincenzo De Leo, Giovanni Santoboni, Federica Cerina, Mario Mureddu, Luca Secchi, and Alessandro Chessa. Community core detection in transportation networks. Phys. Rev. E, 88:042810, Oct 2013. [38] Manfred Lenzen, Keiichiro Kanemoto, Daniel Moran, and Arne Geschke. Mapping the structure of the world economy. Environmental science & technology, 46(15):8374–8381, 2012. [39] Manfred Lenzen, Daniel Moran, Keiichiro Kanemoto, and Arne Geschke. Building eora: A global multi-region input–output database at high country and sector resolution. Economic Systems Research, 25(1):20–49, 2013.
  214. Bartholomew, Abigail, "Behaviorism's Impact on Advertising: The n and Now" (2013). Th eses and Professional Projects from the College of Journalism and Mass Communications. Paper 37. References “A Pioneer in Marketing: John B. Watson.” Journal of Marketing, (Oct. 1961): 72-73. “Can Science Determine Your Baby’s Career Before It Can Talk?” 8 Jan. 1922. JWT Archives, Biographical Information, 1916-1998, bulk 1960s-1980s, Main Files Series (List of Entries), 1916-1998, bulk 1960s- 1980s, Box OV1, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. "JWT researches stages, not ages." Advertising Age. (June 26, 1989 ): 737 words. LexisNexis Academic. Web. Date Accessed: 2012/03/11. “Dr. John Watson, Psychologist, Dies,” 2 Apr. 1927. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Baum, William M. Understanding Behaviorism: Science, Behavior, and Culture. New York City: HarperCollins College Publishers, 1994. Print. Benjamin, Jr., Ludy T. A History of Psychology: Original Sources and Contemporary Research. New York City: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1988. Print. Buckley, K. (1989). Mechanical man : John Broadus Watson and the beginnings of behaviorism. New York: Guilford Press. Buckley, Kerry W. (1982). The Selling of a Psychologist: John Broadus Watson and the Application of Behavioral Techniques to Advertising. Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, 18 (3), 292-221. Cohen, David. J.B. Watson: The Founder of Behaviourism, A Biography. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd, 1979. Print. Coon, Deborah J. “’Not a Creature of Reason’: The Alleged Impact of Watsonian Behaviorism on Advertising in the 1920s.” Modern Perspectives on John B. Watson and Classical Behaviorism. Ed. James T. Todd and Edward K. Morris. Westport: Greenwood Press, 1994. 37-63. Print. Curti, Merle. “The Changing Concept of “Human Nature” in the Literature of American Advertising.” The Business History Review v41.4 (1967): 335-357. JStor. Web. 4 Apr. 2010. Davidow, William H. "Overconnectivity and Surprises." Overconnected: the promise and threat of the Internet. Harrison, N.Y.: Delphinium Books, 2011. 19-28. Print. Henderson, Howard Letter to James W. Young. 28 Dec. 1925. JWT Archives, Howard Henderson Papers, 1867- 1978, Correspondence Series, 1914-1978, Box 2, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Henderson, Howard Letter to John B. Watson. 28 Dec. 1925. JWT Archives, Howard Henderson Papers, 1867-1978, Correspondence Series, 1914-1978, Box 2, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Influencing the Mind of Another. 1935, JWT Archives, Publications Collection, 1887–2005, General Publications, 1887-2005 and undated, Box DG10, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Interbrands: best global brands 2013. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.interbrand.com/en/best-global- brands/2013/Coca-Cola Johnson, Arno H. Letter to Charles Winick. 16 Feb. 1956. JWT Archives, Howard Henderson Papers, 1867-1978, Correspondence Series, 1914-1978, Box 1, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. JWT News Bulletin, 15 Mar. 1921. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Kreshel, Peggy J. “John B. Watson at J. Walter Thompson: The Legitimization of ‘Science’ in Advertising.” Journal of Advertising, v19.2 (1990): 49-59. Print. Larson, Cedric A. “Highlights of Dr. John B. Watson’s Career in Advertising,” The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist v16, n3: 3-5. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. MacGowan, Kenneth. Profiles: The Adventure of the Behaviorist. The New Yorker n.d. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Marwick, Alice E., and Danah Boyd. "I tweet honestly, I tweet openly: Twitter users context collapse, and the imagined audience." New Media & Society 8 (2010): 1-20. Print. O’Donnell, John M. The Origins of Behaviorism: American Psychology, 1870-1920. New York City: New York University Press, 1985. Print. Peters, John Durham, and Peter Simonson. Mass communication and American social thought: key texts, 1919- 1968. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 2004. Print. PRSA: global presence at a glance. (n.d.). Retrieved from: http://www.prsa.org/AboutPRSA/Global_Alliance/#.UpUeHY2E7QU Psychologists in Marketing. The Marketing and Social Research Newsletter of the Psychological Corporation, 1958. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s- 1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Scientific Orphans. Daily Mirror, 7 Mar. 1928. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Staff Meeting Minutes, 9 Dec. 1930. JWT Archives, Staff Meeting Minutes, 1927-1938, Account Representatives’ Meeting Series, 1927-1932, Box 3, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “Advertising by Radio,” JWT News Bulletin, May 1923:11-16. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “Are Booklets Read and Kept?” JWT News Bulletin, Sep. 1922:1-3. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “How to Break Down Resistance to Life Insurance–An Outsider’s Viewpoint,” 25 Nov. 1921. JWT Archives, Publications Collection, 1887–2005, General Publications, 1887-2005 and undated, Box DG8, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “Personality and the Choice of Personnel: Can Psychology Help in the Selection of Personnel?” JWT News Bulletin, Apr. 1927:1-14. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “Personality and the Choice of Personnel: The Behaviorist Looks at Personality,” JWT News Bulletin, Mar. 1927:1-10. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “Thinking as Viewed by the Behaviorists,” JWT News Bulletin, Feb. 1927:17-22. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “What Cigarettes Are You Smoking, And Why?” JWT News Bulletin, Jul. 1922:1-17. JWT Archives, Newsletter Collection , 1910-2005, 1916-1922 News Bulletin, Box MN5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. “What, To Whom, Where, How are We Selling?” 14 Oct. 1924. JWT Archives, Dawkins (Colin) Papers, 1776-1986, Officers and Staff Series, 1873-1986 (bulk 1920s-1979), Box 5, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. Letter to Howard Henderson. 6 Jan. 1926. JWT Archives, Howard Henderson Papers, 1867-1978, Correspondence Series, 1914-1978, Box 2, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University. Watson, John B. Untitled Sales Lecture. 27 Apr. 1931. JWT Archives, Staff Meeting Minutes, 1927-1938, Account Representatives’ Meeting Series, 1927-1932, Box 3, J. Walter Thompson Company. Publications Collection, David M. Rubenstein Rare Book & Manuscript Library, Duke University.  
  215. References: 1. Wallace AR (1870) The limits of natural selection as applied to man. Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection: A Series of Essays, ed Wallace AR (MacMillan, New York). 2. Tooby J, DeVore I (1987) The reconstruction of hominid evolution through strategic modeling. The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models, ed Kinzey WG (SUNY Press, Albany, NY). 3. Brown DE (1991) Human Universals (McGraw-Hill, New York). 4. Kingdon J (1993) Self-Made Man: Human Evolution from Eden to Extinction? (Wiley, New York). 5. Wrangham RW (2009) Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human (Basic Books, New York) pp v. 6. Leslie AM (1994) ToMM, ToBY, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, eds Hirschfeld LA, Gelman SA (Cambridge Univ Press, New York). 7. Spelke ES, Breinlinger K, Macomber J, Jacobson K (1992) Origins of knowledge. Psychol Rev 99:605–632. 8. Carey S (2007) Origins of Concepts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 9. Pinker S (2007) The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature (Viking, New York). 10. Pinker S (1997) How the Mind Works (Norton, New York). 11. Trivers R (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57. 12. Kanwisher N, Moscovitch M (2000) The cognitive neuroscience of face processing: An introduction. Cogn Neuropsychol 17:1–13. 13. Klein SB, Cosmides L, Tooby J, Chance S (2002) Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychol Rev 109:306–329. 14. Cosmides L, Tooby J (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, eds Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J (Oxford Univ Press, New York). 15. Cosmides L, Tooby J (2010) Whence intelligence? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 16. Haidt J (2002) The moral emotions. Handbook of Affective Sciences, eds Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH (Oxford Univ Press, New York). 17. Nowak MA, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393:573–577. 18. Ridley M (1997) The Origins of Virtue: Human Instincts and the Evolution of Cooperation (Viking, New York) 1st American Ed, pp viii, 295. 19. Fiske AP (1991) Structures of Social Life: The Four Elementary Forms of Human Relations (Free Press, New York). 20. Pinker S, Nowak MA, Lee JJ (2008) The logic of indirect speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:833–838. 21. Lee JJ, Pinker S (2010) Rationales for Indirect Speech: The Theory of the Strategic Speaker Psychological Review. 22. Pinker S (1991) Rules of language. Science 253:530–535. 23. Jackendoff R (2002) Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution (Oxford Univ Press, New York). 24. Chomsky N (1972) Language and Mind (Harcourt Brace, New York) Extended ed. 25. Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic Structures (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 26. Senghas A, Kita S, Özyürek A (2004) Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305:1779–1782. 27. Nowak MA, Plotkin JB, Jansen VA (2000) The evolution of syntactic communication. Nature 404:495–498. 28. Pinker S (1999) Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language (HarperCollins, New York). 29. Miller GA, Selfridge J (1950) Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. Am J Psychol 63:176–185. 30. Chagnon NA (1992) Yanomamö: The Last Days of Eden (Harcourt Brace, New York). 31. DiRienzo A (2010) Human population diversity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 32. Bustamente C (2010) Genomic footprints of natural selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 33. Jablonski N (2010) The skin that makes us human. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 34. Tishkoff S (2010) Paleo-demography from extant genetics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 35. Hawkes K (2010) The evolution of human life history. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 36. Kaplan H, Robson AJ (2002) The emergence of humans: The coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:10221–10226. 37. Richerson P (2010) How cultures evolve. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 38. Wallace D (2010) Peopling the planet: Out of Africa? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 39. Wood B (2010) Evolution of the hominids. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 40. Lee JJ (2007) A g beyond Homo sapiens? Some hints and suggestions. Intelligence 35: 253–265. 41. Boyd R, Silk JB (2006) How Humans Evolved (Norton, New York), 4th Ed. 42. Mather JA (1995) Cognition in cephalopods. Adv Stud Behav 24:317–353. 43. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution (Princeton Univ Press, Princeton, NJ). 44. Lewontin RC (1984) Adaptation. Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Biology, ed Sober E (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 45. Kreitman M (2000) Methods to detect selection in populations with applications to the human. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet 1:539–559. 46. Przeworski M, Hudson RR, Di Rienzo A (2000) Adjusting the focus on human variation. Trends Genet 16:296–302. 47. Vargha-Khadem F, et al. (1998) Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:12695–12700. 48. Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (2001) A novel forkhead- domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519–523. 49. Enard W, et al. (2002) Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418:869–872. 50. Clark AG, et al. (2003) Inferring Nonneutral Evolution from Human-Chimp-Mouse Orthologous Gene Trios. Science 302:1960–1963. 51. Evans PD, et al. (2004) Adaptive evolution of ASPM, a major determinant of cortical size in humans. Hum Mol Genet 13:489–494. 52. Pinker S (2002) The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature (Viking, New York). 53. Carey S (2009) Origins of Concepts (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 54. McCloskey M (1983) Intuitive physics. Scientific American 248:122–130. 55. Atran S (1998) Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behav Brain Sci 21:547–609. 56. Bloom P (2003) Descartes’ Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human (Basic Books, New York). 57. Daly M, Wilson M (1988) Homicide (Aldine de Gruyter, Hawthorne, NY). 58. Fiske AP (2004) Four modes of constituting relationships: Consubstantial assimilation; space, magnitude, time, and force; concrete procedures; abstract symbolism. Relational Models Theory: A Contemporary Overview, ed Haslam N (Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ). 59. Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors We Live By (Univ of Chicago Press, Chicago). 60. Jackendoff R (1978) Grammar as evidence for conceptual structure. Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality, eds Halle M, Bresnan J, Miller GA (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 61. Talmy L (2000) Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition. Toward a Cognitive Semantics 1: Concept Structuring Systems (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 62. Reddy M (1993) The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. Metaphor and Thought, ed Ortony A (Cambridge Univ Press, New York), 2nd Ed. 63. Bowerman M (1983) Hidden meanings: The role of covert conceptual structures in children’s development of language. The Acquisition of Symbolic Skills, eds Rogers DR, Sloboda JA (Plenum, New York). 64. Pinker S (1989) Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 65. Hofstadter DR (1995) Fluid Concepts and Creative Analogies: Computer Models of the Fundamental Mechanisms of Thought (Basic Books, New York), pp ix, 518 pp. 66. Schank RC (1982) Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning in Computers and People (Cambridge Univ Press, New York). 67. GentnerD(2003)Whywe’resosmart.LanguageinMind:AdvancesintheStudyofLanguage and Thought, eds Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp 195–235. 68. Gentner D, Jeziorski M (1989) Historical shifts in the use of analogy in science. The psychology of Science: Contributions to Metascience, eds Gholson B, Shadish WR, Beimeyer RA, Houts A (Cambridge Univ Press, New York). 69. Spelke E (2003) What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, eds Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). 70. Boyd R (1993) Metaphor and Theory Change: What is “Metaphor” a Metaphor for? Metaphor and Thought, ed Ortony A (Cambridge Univ Press, New York), 2nd Ed.
  216. References: 1. Jensen AR (1969) How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Edu Rev 39:1–123. 2. Detterman DK, Sternberg RJ, eds (1982) How and How Much Can Intelligence Be Increased? (Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ). 3. Kramer AF, Willis SL (2002) Enhancing the cognitive vitality of older adults. Curr Directions Psychol Sci 11:173–177. 4. Detterman DK, Sternberg RJ, eds (1993) Transfer on Trial: Intelligence, Cognition, and Instruction (Ablex, Norwood, NJ). 5. Plomin R (2004) Nature and Nurture: An Introduction to Human Behavioral Genetics (Wadsworth, Thousand Oaks, CA). 6. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral Genetics (Worth, New York), 5th Ed. 7. Turkheimer E, Haley A, D’Onofrio B, Waldron M, Gottesman II (2003) Socioeconomic status modifies her-itability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci 14:623–628. 8. Carroll JB (1993) Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies (Cambridge Univ Press, New York). 9. Mackintosh N (1998) IQ and Human Intelligence (Ox- ford Univ Press, New York). 10. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ (2008) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:6829–6833. 11. Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway ARA (1999) Working memory, short-term memory, and gen- eral fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. J Exp Psychol General 128:309–331. 12. Kane MJ, Engle RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual differences perspective. Psychonom Bull Rev 9:637–671. 13. Jensen AR (1998) The G Factor (Praeger, Westport, CT). 14. Feuerstein R (1980) Instrumental Enrichment: An Inter- vention Program for Cognitive Modifiability (Univer- sity Park Press, Baltimore, MD). 15. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (2003) Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section 1: Gen- eral Overview (Harcourt Assessment, San Antonio, TX). 16. Anastasi A, Urbina S (1997) Psychological Testing (Pren- tice–Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ), 7th Ed. 17. Cattell RB (1971) Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action (Houghton Mifflin, Boston). 18. Binet A, Simon T (1916) The Development of Intelli- gence in Children, trans Kite ES (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, MD). 19. Gottfredson LS, Deary IJ (2004) Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Curr Directions Psychol Sci 13:1–4. 20. Spitz HH (1992) Does the Carolina Abecedarian Early Intervention Project prevent sociocultural mental re- tardation? Intelligence 16:225–237.
  217. References BARNeS, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and Relational Frame Theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91–124. BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., SMeeTS, P. M., CUllINAN, V., & leADeR, G. (2004). Relational frame theory and stimulus equivalence: Conceptual and procedural issues. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 181–214. BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., HAyDeN, e., BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., & STeWART, I. (2008). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time and event-related-potentials methodology for testing natural verbal relations. The Psychological Record, 58, 497–516. BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., BARNeS-HOlMeS D., & ROCHe, B. (2001). exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry. The Psychological Record, 51, 287–308. BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., ROCHe, B., & SMeeTS, P. (2001). exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry II. The Psychological Record, 51, 589–603. BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., ROCHe, B., HeAly, O., lyDDy, F., CUllINAN, V., & HAyeS, S. C. (2001). Psychological development. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 157–180). New york: Plenum Press. BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., SMeeTS, P., STRAND, P., & FRIMAN, P. (2004). establishing relational responding in accordance with more-than and less-than as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 531–558. BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., & SMeeTS, P. (2004). establishing relational responding in accordance with opposite as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 559–586. BeReNS, N. M., & HAyeS, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 45–71. BINDeR, C. (1996). Behavioral fluency: evolution of a new paradigm. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 163–197. CASSIDy, S. (2008). Relational Frame Theory and human intelligence: A conceptual and empirical analysis. Unpublished doctoral thesis, National University of Ireland, Maynooth. CASSIDy, S., ROCHe, B., & O’HORA, D. (2010). Relational Frame Theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 37–51. CeCI, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703–722. COHeN, J. (1988). Statistical power for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: erlbaum. COHeN J. D., MACWHINNey B., FlATT M., & PROVOST J. (1993). PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25, 257–271. CONNOR (1998). A review of behavioural early intervention programmes for children with autism. Educational Psychology in Practice, 14, 109–117. CUllINAN, V., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., & SMeeTS, P. M. (2001). A precursor to the relational evaluation procedure: The search for the contextual cues that control equivalence responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 339–349. DeARy, I. J., STRAND, S., SMITH, P., & FeRNANDeS, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35, 13–21. FIelDS, l., ADAMS, B. J., VeRHAVe, T., & NeWMAN, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358. FlyNN, J. R. (1998). WAIS III and WISC III IQ gains in the United States from 1972–1995: How to compensate for obsolete norms. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1231–1239. GARDNeR, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New york: Basic Books. GOMeZ, S., lOPeZ, F., MARTIN, C. B., BARNeS-HOlMeS, y., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, D. (2007). exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 57, 273–294. GReSHAM, F. M., & MACMIllAN, D. l. (1997). Autistic recovery? An analysis and critique of the empirical evidence on the early Intervention Project. Behavioral Disorders, 22, 185–201. HAyeS, S. C., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., & ROCHe, B. (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New york: Plenum. HAyeS, S. C., GIFFORD, e. V., TOWNSeND, R. C., JR., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, D. (2001). Thinking, problem-solving, and pragmatic verbal analysis. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (eds.), Relational Frame Theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 87–101). New york: Plenum Press. JAeGGI, S. M., BUSCHKUeHl, M., JONIDeS, J., & PeRRIG, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105, 6829–6833. lOVAAS, O. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9. MAGIATI, I. & HOWlIN, P. A. (2001). Monitoring the progress of preschool children with autism enrolled in early intervention programmes: Problems in cognitive assessment. Autism, 5, 399–406. MOFFITT, T. e., CASPI, A., HARKNeSS, R., & SIlVA, P. A. (1993). The natural history of change in intellectual performance: Who changes? How much? Is it meaningful? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 455–506. O’HORA, D., PelAeZ, M., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, D. (2005). Derived relational responding and performance on verbal sub-tests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 55, 155–175. O’HORA, D., PelAeZ, M., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., RAe, G., ROBINSON, K., & CHAUDHARy, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58, 569–584. O’KeeFe, D. J. (2003). Against familywise alpha adjustment. Human Communication Research, 29, 431–447. O’TOOle, C., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, D. (2009). Three chronometric indices of relational responding as predictors of performance on a brief intelligence test: The importance of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59, 119–132. O’TOOle, C., BARNeS-HOlMeS, D., MURPHy, C., O’CONNOR, J., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: extending the remit of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 1–17. RAUDeNBUSH, S., & BRyK, A. (2001). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. ReeD, P., OSBORNe, l., & CORNeSS, M., (2005). The effectiveness of early intervention programmes for autistic spectrum disorders. Unpublished report. ReHFelDT, R. A., & BARNeS-HOlMeS, y. (eds.) (2009). Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. ROSeNTHAl, R., & ROSNOW, R. l. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (2nd ed.). New york: McGraw-Hill. SAllOWS, G. O., & GRAUPNeR, T. D. (2005). Intensive behavioural treatment for children with autism. American Journal on Mental Retardation 110, 417-438. SCHlINGeR, H. D. (2003). The myth of intelligence. The Psychological Record, 53, 15–32. SCHMIDT, F. l., & HUNTeR, J. e. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274. SIDMAN, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13. SIDMAN, M. (2008). Symmetry and equivalence relations in behavior. Cognitive Studies, 15, 322–332. SKINNeR, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New york: Random House Inc. STeele, D. l., & HAyeS, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555. SMITH, T., eIKeSeTH, S., KleVSTRAND, M., & lOVAAS, O. (1997). Intensive behavioral treatment for preschoolers with severe mental retardation and pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 238–249. TUTZAUeR, F. (2003). On the sensible application of familywise alpha adjustment. Human Communication Research, 29, 455–463. VeRBeKe, G., & MOleNBeRGHS, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New york: Springer-Verlag. WeCHSleR, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–Third edition–UK Manual. london: Psychological Corp, europe. WeCHSleR, D. (2004). WISC-IVUK administration and scoring manual. london: Harcourt Assessment. WIllIAMS, B., MyeRSON, J., & HAle, S. (2008). Individual differences, intelligence, and behavior analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 219–231.
  218. Citation: Gagnon MA, Lexchin J (2008) The cost of pushing pills: A new estimate of pharmaceutical promotion expenditures in the united states. PLoS Med 5(1): e1. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0050001 References 1. Kefauver E (1965) In a few hands. Harmondsworth: Penguin. 2. Froud J, Johal S, Leaver A, Williams K (2006) Financialization and strategy: narrative and numbers. London and New York: Routledge. 3. General Accounting Office (2002) Prescription drugs: FDA oversight of direct-to-consumer advertising has limitations. Available: http:// www.gao.gov/new.items/d03177.pdf. Accessed 27 November 2007. 4. IMS Health (2005) Total U.S. promotional spend by type, 2004. Accessed 4 November 2006 at http://www.imshealth.com/ims/ portal/front/articleC/0,2777,6599_49695992_ 75406357,00.html. 5. Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America (2006) Pharmaceutical industry profile 2006. Available: http://www.phrma. org/files/2006%20Industry%20Profile.pdf. Accessed 27 November 2007. 6. Hensley S, Martinez B (2005 July 15) To sell their drugs, companies increasingly rely on doctors. Wall Street Journal. p. A1. 7. Quintiles Transnational (2001) Promoting drugs through physician meetings and events: Pfizer leads the way; antidepressants are top category. Accessed 3 March 2002 at http:// www.quintiles.com/products_and_services/ informatics/scott_levin/press_releases/press_ release/1,1254,209,00.html. 8. La Puma J, Seltzer J (2002) Phase IV market steams ahead. CenterWatch 9: 1, 9-13. 9. Lauzon L-P, Hasbani M (2006) Analyse socio- économique: industrie pharmaceutique mondiale pour la période de dix ans 1996-2005. Montreal: Chaire d’études socio- économiques de l’UQAM. 10. CAM Group (2005) Total U.S. promotional activity for 2004. CAM USA Newsletter. Accessed 22 July 2007 at http://csd.cam- group.com/www_assets/pages/downloads/ USNewsletterQ404.pdf. 11. IMS Health (2006) Channel distribution by U.S. sales. Available: http://www.imshealth. com/ims/portal/front/articleC/0,2777,6652_ 80408863_80411875,00.htm. Accessed 28 November 2007. 12. IMS Health (2006) Global pharmaceutical sales, 1999-2006. Available: http://www. imshealth.com/ims/portal/front/ articleC/0,2777,6652_80528184_80528202,00. html. Accessed 27 November 2007. 13. CAM Group (2005) 4th quarter 2004 promotional activity. CAM Group News. Accessed 22 July 2007 at http://csd.cam- group.com/www_assets/pages/downloads/ CAMNewsletterQ404.pdf. 14. IMS Health (2004) Total U.S. value of free product samples, 2004. Accessed 4 November 2006 at http://www.imshealth.com/ims/ portal/front/articleC/0,2777,6599_78152267_ 78152297,00.html. 15. National Science Foundation (2006) U.S. industrial R&D performers report increased expenditures for 2004. Available: http://www. nsf.gov/statistics/infbrief/nsf07304/#notes. Accessed 27 November 2007. 16. Gøtzsche PC, Hróbjartsson A, Johansen HK, Haahr MT, Altman DG, et al. (2007) Ghost authorship in industry-initiated randomised trials. PLoS Med 4: e19. doi:10.1371/journal. pmed.0040019 17. Steinman MA, Bero LA, Chren MM, Landefeld CS (2006) Narrative review: the promotion of gabapentin: an analysis of internal industry documents. Ann Intern Med 145: 284-293. 18. Gencarelli DM (2005) One pill, many prices: variation in prescription drug prices in selected government programs. National Health Policy Forum. Available: http://www.nhpf.org/pdfs_ ib/IB807_DrugPricing_08-29-05.pdf. Accessed 27 November 2007. 19. Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America (2005) America’s pharmaceutical companies increase Katrina donations; more than $100 million in medicines, cash donated so far. Available: http://www.liberty-page.com/ issues/comparison/pharm.html. Accessed 27 November 2007. 20. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (2006) OECD health data 2006. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. 21. IMS Health (2004) U.S. purchase activity by channel, 2004. Accessed 4 November 2006 at http://www.imshealth.com/ims/portal/front/ articleC/0,2777,6599_49695983_69891354,00. html. 22. Angell M (2004) The truth about the drug companies: how they deceive us and what to do about it. New York: Random House. 23. Office of Technology Assessment (1993) Pharmaceutical R&D: costs, risks and rewards. Washington (D. C.): US Government Printing Office. 24. Jacobzone S (2000) Pharmaceutical policies in OECD countries: reconciling social and industrial goals. Paris: OECD Publishing. Available: http://ideas.repec.org/p/oec/ elsaaa/40-en.html. Accessed 28 November 2007. 25. Consumers International (2006) Branding the cure: a consumer perspective on corporate social responsibility, drug promotion and the pharmaceutical industry in Europe. London: Consumers International. 26. Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (2003) Health care industry market update: pharmaceuticals. Available: http://www.fdcpa. com/CMS%20Pharmaceuticals%20Jan%20200 3.pdf. Accessed 28 November 2007. 27. Families USA (2002) Profiting from pain: where prescription drug dollars go. Available: http://www.familiesusa.org/assets/pdfs/ PPreport89a5.pdf. Accessed 28 November 2007.2.254
  219. REFERENCES Advertising Age’s History of TV Advertising ( 2004 , March 6 ). Available: www.adage.com Aubry, P. ( 2000 ). The ‘Television Without Frontiers’ Directive, Cornerstone of the European Broadcasting Policy. Available: http://www.obs.coe.int Avery, R. J., and Ferraro, R. ( 2003 ). ‘Versimilitude or Advertising? Brand Appearances on Prime-Time Television,’ Journal of Consumer Affairs, 34 , 217 – 44 . Bachman, K., and Flass, R. ( 2003 ). ‘Viacom, Tribune and Pepsi Test Cross-media Strategy,’ Mediaweek, (February 3 ), 10 . Baker, S. ( 2003 ). ‘My Son, the Ad-zapper: Will TiVo Be the Death of Commercials? If Jack is any Indication . . .’ BusinessWeek, (November 10 ), 76 . Baron, R. ( 2003 ). ‘DVR Threat Real, Growing,’ TelevisionWeek, (October 20 ), 11 . Campbell, A. J. ( 1999 ). ‘Self-regulation and the Media,’ Federal Communications Law Journal, 51 ( 3 ), 711 – 52 . Carter, M. ( 2004 ). ‘A Word from Our Sponsors . . . Again,’ Marketing, (May 12 ), 42 – 3 . Chunovic, L. ( 2002 ). ‘The PVR Revolution: Mere Myth or Nightmare to Come?’ Electronic Media, (November 18 ), 8 . Cobb-Walgren, C. J. ( 1990 ). ‘The Changing Commercial Climate,’ Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 13 , 343 – 68 . Consoli, J. ( 2003 ). ‘Nets Eye New Ways to Spice Up Summer: More Advertiser Involvement Seen as Key to Financing Fresh Programming,’ Mediaweek, (July28 ), 4 . Cowen, M. ( 2003 ). ‘Beyond the Break,’ Campaign, (November 7 ), 28 . Erdelyi, M. H., and Zizak, D. M. ( 2004 ). ‘Beyond Gizmo Subliminality,’ in L. J. Shrum (ed.), The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines Between Entertainment and Persuasion, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 13 – 43 . Ephron, E. ( 2003 ). ‘The Paradox of Product Placement,’ Mediaweek, (June 2 ), 20 . Felten, E. W. ( 2003 ). Freedom to Tinker: Aimster Loses. Available: http://www.freedom-to-tinker.com Fox, S. ( 1984 ). The Mirror Makers: A History of American Advertising and its Creators. New York: William Morrow and Company, Inc. Freys, A. ( 1993 ). ‘Naming Names,’ Managing Intellectual Property, 33 , 25 – 8 . Friestad, M., and Wright, P. ( 1994 ). ‘The Persuasion Knowledge Model: How People Cope with Persuasion Attempts,’ Journal of Consumer Research, 22 , 62 – 74 . Ganguzza, P. ( 2002 ). Integrating Programming and Promotion: How it Works in Practice. Paper presented at the annual Advertising and Consumer Psychology conference, New York, May. Green, M. C., Garst, J., and Brock, T. C. ( 2004 ). ‘The Power of Fiction: Determinants and Boundaries,’ in L. J. Shrum (ed.), The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines Between Entertainment and Persuasion, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 161 – 76 . Greenspan, R. ( 2003 ). Remote Power: Can PVRs Kill TV Spots? Available: http://www.internetnews.com In-Stat/MDR ( 2004 ). Personal Video Recorders : Worldwide Unit Shipment Forecasts. Ives, N. ( 2004 ). Commercials have Expanded into Short Films with the Story as the Focus Rather than the Product. New York Times, (April 21 ), C 6 . Jacoby, L. L. ( 1984 ). ‘Incidental versus Intentional Retrieval: Remembering and Awareness as Separate Issues,’ in L. R. Squire and N. Butters (eds.), Neuropsychology of Memory, New York: Guilford Press, pp. 145 – 56 . Kalter, A. ( 2003 ). Unnecessary Force. Adweek, (June 30 ), 18 . Kent, R. J. ( 1995 ). ‘Competitive Clutter in Network Television Advertising: Current Levels and Advertiser Responses,’ Journal of Advertising Research, 35 , 49 – 57 . Law, S., and Braun-LaTour, K. A. ( 2004 ). ‘Product Placements: How to Measure their Impact,’ in L. J. Shrum (ed.), The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines Between Entertainment and Persuasion, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 63 – 78 . Linnett, R. ( 2003 ). ‘Fox Sports Specialty: Product ‘‘Immersion,’’ ’ Advertising Age, (January 30 ), 3 – 4 . Marsh, H. ( 2001 ). ‘Does Reality TV Deliver for Advertisers?’ Marketing, (May 24 ) 18 – 19 . Martilla, J. A., and Thompson, D. L. ( 1966 ). ‘The Perceived Effects of Piggyback Television Commercials,’ Journal of Marketing Research, 3 , 365 – 71 . McCarty, J. A. ( 2004 ). ‘Product Placement: The Nature of the Practice and Potential Avenues of Inquiry,’ in L. J. Shrum (ed.), The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines Between Entertainment and Persuasion, Mahwah, NJ: Law- rence Erlbaum, pp. 45 – 61 . Misdom, L. ( 2004 ). Advertising Environment Study 2003/2004. PhaseOne Communications. Product Placement Is a Slippery Slope ( 2003 ). Television Week, (October 6 ), 8 . Riding, A. ( 2001 ). ‘Reality TV in France: A Smash Hit with Dialectics,’ The New York Times, (May 22 ), E 2 . Rust, R. T., and Oliver, R. W. ( 1994 ). ‘The Death of Advertising,’ Journal of Advertising, 23 , 71 – 7 . Schacter, D. L. ( 1987 ). ‘Implicit Memory: History and Current Status,’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13 , 501 – 18 . Shavitt, S., Lowrey, P., and Haefner, J. ( 1998 ). ‘Public Attitudes Toward Advertising: More Favorable Than You Might Think,’ Journal of Advertising Research, 38 , 7 – 22 . Sherman, J. ( 2003 ). ‘New Software Fine-tunes Product Placement Values,’ TelevisionWeek, (September 15 ), 10 . Shrum, L. J. ( 2004 ). The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines Between Entertainment and Persuasion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Song, S. ( 2003 ). ‘Queer Eye, Straight Plugs,’ Time, (August 18 ), 20 . Soriano, C. ( 2001 , February 7 ). ‘CBS, Tribe Speak Out on Lawsuit,’ USA TODAY, Available: http://www.usatoday.com Stanley, T. ( 2004 ). ‘A Kinder, Gentler Reality TV Unfolds,’ Advertising Age, (May 31 ), S 12 . Television Bureau of Advertising ( 2004 ). Network Television Commercial Activity by Length of Commercial. Available: http://www.tvb.org Whitney, D. ( 2003 ). ‘Integrating the Brand,’ TelevisionWeek, (March 31 ), 17 . Zajonc, R. B. ( 1980 ). ‘Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need no Inferences,’ Ameri- can Psychologist, 35 , 151 – 75 . ZenithOptimedia, Western Europe Market & MediaFact, 1996–2003.
  220. References Aaker, D. A., Stayman D. M., & Hagerty M. R. (1986). Warmth in Advertising: Measurement, Impact and Sequence Effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 365-381. Axelrod, J. N., (1963). Induced Moods and Attitude toward Products. Journal of Advertising Research, 3 (2), 19- 24. Bower, G. H., (1981). Mood and Memory. American Psychologist, 36 (2), 129-148. Aylesworth, A. B., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1998). Context is the Key: The Effect of Program-Induced Mood on Thoughts about the Ad. Journal of Advertising, 27 (2), 17-31. Batra, R., & Ray, M. L., (1986). Affective Responses Mediating Acceptance of Advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 234-249. Cauberghe, V., Geuens, M., & De Pelsmacker, P., (2010). Context Effects of TV Programme-Induced Interactivity and Telepresence on Advertising Responses. Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, Universiteit Gent, working paper series no. 671. Coulter, K. S., (1998). The Effects of Affective Responses to Media Context on Advertising Evaluations. Journal of Advertising, 27 (4), 41-51. De Pelsmacker, P., Geuens, M., & Ankaert, P., (2002). Media Context and Advertising Effectiveness: The Role of Context Appreciation and Context/Ad Similarity. Journal of Advertising, 31 (2), 49-61. Eysenk, M. W., (1984). A Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Gardner, M. P., (1985). Mood States and Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 281-299. Goldberg, M. E., & Gorn, G. J., (1987). Happy and Sad TV Programs: How They Affect Reactions to Commercials. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 387-403. Handley, I. M., and Lassiter, G. D. (2002). Mood and Information Processing: When Happy and Sad Look the Same. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 223-255. Hosein, N. Z., (2012). Measuring the Purchase Intention of Visitors to the Auto Show. Journal of Management & Marketing Research, 9, 1-4. Krugman, H. E., (1983). Television Program Interest and Commercial Interruption. Journal of Advertising Research, 23 (1), 21-23. Medianu, S., (2007). Mood Incongruent Recall: An Examination of Mediating Variables. Lethbridge Undergraduate Research Journal, 2, 1-5. Moorman, M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2012). Program-Involvement Effects on Commercial Attention and Recall of Successive and Embedded Advertising. Journal of Advertising, 41(2), 25-38. Moorman, M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2007). The Effects of Program Involvement on Commercial Exposure and Recall in a Naturalistic Setting. Journal of Advertising, 36 (1), 121-137. Moorman, M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2005). The Effects of Program Responses on the Processing of Commercials Placed at Various Positions in the Program and the Block. Journal of Advertising Research, 45 (1), 49-59. Murphy, J. H., Cunningham, I. C. M., & Wilcox, G. B. (1979). The Impact of Program Environment on Recall of Humorous Television Commercials. Journal of Advertising, 8, 17-21. Norris, C. E., & Colman, A. M., (1993). Context Effects on Memory for Television Advertisements. Social Behavior and Personality, 21(4), 279-296. Owolabi, A. B., (2009). Effect of Consumers Mood on Advertising Effectiveness. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 4, 118-127. Parrott, W. G., & Sabini, J., (1990). Mood and Memory under Natural Conditions: Evidence for Mood- Incongruent Recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 321–336. Rich, T., Owens, D., & Ellenbogen, I., (1978). What Canadians Dislike About Television Commercials. Journal of Advertising Research, 18 (1), 37-44. Shapiro, S., MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W., (2001). Understanding Program Induced Mood Effects: Decoupling Arousal from Valence. Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26. Silk, A. J., & Vavra, T., (1974). The Influence of Advertising’s Affective Qualities on Consumer Response. In G.D. Hughes, & M. L. Roy (Eds.), Buying Consumer Information Processing (pp. 157-186). Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press. Soldow, G. F., & Principe, V., (1981). Response to Commercials as a Function of Program Context. Journal of Advertising Research, 21 (2), 59-65. Srull, T., (1983). Affect and Memory: The Impact of Affective Reactions in Advertising on the Representation of Product Information in Memory. In R. Bagossi & A. Tybout (Eds.), Advances in Consumer Research (pp. 520-525). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research. Swaminathan, S., & Kent, R., (2013). Second-by-Second Analysis of Advertising Exposure in TV Pods: The Dynamics of Position, Length, and Timing. Journal of Advertising Research, 53(1), 91-100. Tavassoli, N. T., Shultz, C. J., & Fitzsimons, G. J., (1995). Program Involvement: Are Moderate Levels Best for Ad Memory and Attitude Toward the Ad?. Journal of Advertising Research, 35 (5), 61-72.
  221. REFERENCES Aaker, David, and Douglas M. Stayman (1990), “Measuring Audience Perceptions of Commercials and Relating them to Ad Impact,” Journal of Advertising Research, 30(4), 7-17. Aiken, Leona S., and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. Alwitt, Linda F. (2002), “Suspense and Advertising Responses,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(1), 35-49. Aylesworth, Andrew, B., and Scott B. MacKenzie (1998), “Context Is Key: The Effect of Program-Induced Mood on Thoughts about the Ad,” Journal of Advertising, 27(2), 17-31. Broach, V. Carter, Thomas J. Page, and R. Dale Wilson (1995), “Television Programming and Its Influence on Viewers‟ Perceptions of Commercials: The Role of Program Arousal and Pleasantness,” Journal of Advertising, 24(4), 45-54. Coulter, Keith S. (1998), “The Effects of Affective Responses to Media Context on Advertising Evaluations,” Journal of Advertising, 27(4), 41-51. Dahlén, Micael (2005), “The Medium as a Contextual Cue: Effects of Creative Media Choice,” Journal of Advertising, 34(3), 89-98. De Pelsmacker, Patrick, Maggie Geuens, and Pascal Anckaert (2002), “Media Context and Advertising Effectiveness: The Role of Context Appreciation and Context/Ad Similarity,” Journal of Advertising, 31(2), 49-61. Edell, Julie A., and Marian Chapman Burke (1987), “The Power of Feelings in Understanding Advertising Effects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14(December), 421-433. Gan, Su-Lin, Charles A. Tuggle, Michael A. Mitrook, Sylvere H. Coussement, and Dolf Zillmann (1997), “The Thrill of a Close Game: Who Enjoys It and Who Doesn't?” Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 21, 53-64. Goldberg, Marvin E., and Gerald J. Gorn (1987), “Happy and Sad TV Programs: How They Affect Reactions to Commercials,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (December), 387-403. Gorn, Gerald, Michel Tuan Pham, and Leo Yatming Sin (2001), “When Arousal Influences Ad Evaluation and Valence Does Not (and Vice Versa),” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 43-55. Hirt, Edward R., Dolf Zillmann, Grant A. Erickson, and Chris Kennedy (1992), “Costs and Benefits of Allegiance: Changes in Fans Self-Ascribed Competencies After Team Victory Versus Defeat,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(5), 724-738. Kamins, Michael A., Lawrence J. Marks, and Deborah Skinner (1991), “Television Commercial Evaluation In the Context of Program Induced Mood: Congruency Versus Consistency Effects,” Journal of Advertising, 20(2), 1-14. Kennedy, John R. (1971), “How Program Environment Affects TV Commercials,” Journal of Advertising Research, 11(1), 33-38. Lord, Kenneth R., and Sanjay Putrevu (1996), “Super Bowl Ad Recall: Program and Involvement Effects,” in Proceedings of the Society for Consumer Psychology, Paul. M. Herr and John Kim, eds., Boulder, CO: University of Colorado Graduate School of Business, 1-7. Madrigal, Robert (2008), “Hot vs. Cold Cognitions and Consumers‟ Reactions to Sporting Event Outcomes,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(4) 304-19. Madrigal, Robert, Colleen Bee, Johnny Chen, Monica LaBarge (2011), “The Effect of Suspense on Enjoyment Following a Desirable Outcome: The Mediating Role of Relief,” Media Psychology, 14(3), 259-288. Mattes, John and Joanne Cantor (1982), “Enhancing Responses to Television Advertisements via the Transfer of Residual Arousal from Prior Programming,” Journal of Broadcasting, 26(Spring), 553-66. Moorman, Marjolein, Peter C. Neijens, and Edith G. Smit (2002), “The Effects of Magazine Induced Psychological Responses and Thematic Congruence on Memory and Attitude Toward the Ad in a Real-Life Setting,” Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 27-40. ------, ------, and ------ (2005), “The Effects of Program Responses on the Processing of Commercials Placed at Various Positions in the Program and the Block,” Journal of Advertising Research, 45(1), 49-59. ------, ------, and ------ (2007), “The Effects of Program Involvement on Commercial Exposure and Recall in a Naturalistic Setting,” Journal of Advertising, 36(1), 121-37. Murry, John P., John L. Lastovicka, and Surendra N. Singh (1992), “Feeling and Liking Responses to Television Programs: An Examination of Two Explanations for Media-Context Effects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18(March), 441-451. Newell, Stephen J., Kenneth V. Henderson, and Bob T. Wu (2001), “The Effects of Pleasure and Arousal on Recall of Advertisements during the Super Bowl,” Psychology & Marketing, 18(11), 1135- 53. Pavelchak, Mark A., John H. Antil, and James M. Munch (1988), “The Super Bowl: An Investigation into the Relationship Among Program Context, Emotional Experience, and Ad Recall,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15(December), 360-67. Peterson, Erik M. and Arthur A. Raney (2008), “Reconceptualizing and Reexamining Suspense as a Predictor of Mediated Sports Enjoyment,” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(4), 544-62. Prasad, V. Kanti and Lois J. Smith (1994), “Television Commercials in Violent Programming: An Experimental Evaluation of Their Effects on Children,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 22(4), 340-51. Shapiro, Stewart, Deborah J. MacInnis, and C. Whan Park (2002), “Understanding Program Induced Mood Effects: Decoupling Arousal from Valence,” Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26. Singh, Surendra N. and Gilbert A. Churchill (1987), “Arousal and Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Advertising, 16(1), 4-10. Singh, Surendra N. and Jacqueline C. Hitchon (1989), “The Intensifying Effects of Exciting Television Programs on the Reception of Subsequent Commercials,” Psychology & Marketing, 6(1), 1-31. Soldow, Gary F. and Victor Principe (1981), “Response to Commercials as a Function of Program Context,” Journal of Advertising Research, 21(2), 59-65. Zillmann, Dolf (1996), “The Psychology of Suspense in Dramatic Exposition,” in Suspense: Conceptualizations, Theoretical Analyses, and Empirical Explanations, ed. Peter Vorderer, Hans J. Wulff, and Mike Friedrichsen, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 199-231.
  222. REFERENCES ACNE Film Showreel (2009), “Commercial for Visa—‘Superher- oes,’” http://www.youtube.com/watch?v p doFWpiT6qjs. Adaval, Rashmi, Linda M. Isbell, and Robert S. Wyer (2007), “The Impact of Pictures on Narrative- and List-Based Impression Formation: A Process Interference Model,” Journal of Ex- perimental Social Psychology, 43 (3), 352–64. Adaval, Rashmi, and Robert S. Wyer (1998), “The Role of Nar- ratives in Consumer Information Processing,” Journal of Con- sumer Psychology, 7 (3), 207–45. Alba, Joseph W. (2012), “In Defense of Bumbling,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (6), 981–87. Albarracı´n, Dolores, Mark P. Zanna, Blair T. Johnson, and G. Tarcan Kumkale (2005), “Attitudes: Introduction and Scope,” in The Handbook of Attitudes, ed. Dolores Albarracı´n, Blair T. Johnson, and Mark P. Zanna, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 3– 19. Ang, Ien, and Della Couling (1985), Watching Dallas: Soap Opera and the Melodramatic Imagination, London: Methuen. ∗ Appel, Markus, and Barbara Malecˇkar (2012), “The Influence of Paratext on Narrative Persuasion: Fact, Fiction, or Fake?” Human Communication Research, 38 (4), 459–84. Appel, Markus, and Tobias Richter (2007), “Persuasive Effects of Fictional Narratives Increase over Time,” Media Psychology, 10 (1), 113–34. ∗ ——— (2010), “Transportation and Need for Affect in Narrative Persuasion: A Mediated Moderation Model,” Media Psy- chology, 13 (2), 101–35. ∗ Argo, Jennifer J., Rui Zhu, and Darren W. Dahl (2008), “Fact or Fiction: An Investigation of Empathy Differences in Response to Emotional Melodramatic Entertainment,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 34 (5), 614–23. Bahl, Shalini, and George R. Milne (2010), “Talking to Ourselves: A Dialogical Exploration of Consumption Experiences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (1), 176–95. Bal, P. Matthijs, Olivia S. Butterman, and Arnold B. Bakker (2011), “The Influence of Fictional Narrative Experience on Work Outcomes: A Conceptual Analysis and Research Model,” Review of General Psychology, 15 (4), 361–70. Bandura, Albert (2004), “Health Promotion by Social Cognitive Means,” Health Education and Behavior, 31 (2), 143–65. ∗ Banerjee, Smita C., and Kathryn Greene (2012a), “‘I Quit’ versus ‘I’m Sorry I Used’: A Preliminary Investigation of Variations in Narrative Ending and Transportation,” Psychology and Health, 27 (11), 1308–22. ∗ ——— (2012b), “Role of Transportation in the Persuasion Pro- cess: Cognitive and Affective Responses to Antidrug Nar- ratives,” Journal of Health Communication, 17 (5), 564–81. Baranowski, Tom, Richard Buday, Debbe I. Thompson, and Janice Baranowski (2008), “Playing for Real: Video Games and Sto- ries for Health-Related Behavior Change,” American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 34 (1), 74–82. Barthes, Roland (1975), “An Introduction to the Structural Analysis of Narrative,” New Literary History, 6 (2), 237–72. Batra, Rajeev, and Michael L. Ray (1986), “Affective Responses Mediating Acceptance of Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (2), 234–49. Bennett, Andrew, and Nicholas Royle (2004), Introduction to Lit- erature, Criticism and Theory, Harlow, UK: Pearson. Berlyne, Daniel Ellis (1960), Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity, New York: McGraw-Hill. Berman, Ruth A., and Bracha Nir-sagiv (2007), “Comparing Nar- rative and Expository Text Construction across Adolescence: A Developmental Paradox,” Discourse Processes, 43 (2), 79–120. Betts, George Herbert (1909), The Distribution and Functions of Mental Imagery, New York: Columbia University Press. Bilandzic, Helena, and Rick W. Busselle (2008), “Transportation and Transportability in the Cultivation of Genre-Consistent Attitudes and Estimates,” Journal of Communication, 58 (3), 508–29. Borenstein, Michael, Larry V. Hedges, Julian P. T. Higgins, and Hannah R. Rothstein (2005), “Comprehensive Meta-Analy- sis,” unpublished manuscript, Biostat, Englewood, NJ. Borghini, Stefania, Nina Diamond, Robert V. Kozinets, Mary Ann McGrath, Albert M. Mun˜iz Jr., and John F. Sherry Jr. (2009), “Why Are Themed Brandstores So Powerful? Retail Brand Ideology at American Girl Place,” Journal of Retailing, 85 (3), 363–75. Boyd, Brian (2009), On the Origin of Stories: Evolution, Cogni- tion, and Fiction, Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Bozarth, Jerold D., and Ralph R. Roberts (1972), “Signifying Sig- nificant Significance,” American Psychologist, 27 (8), 774– 75. Bracken, Cheryl Campanella (2006), “Perceived Source Credibility of Local Television News: The Impact of Television Form and Presence,” Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Me- dia, 50 (4), 723–41. ∗ Braverman, Julia (2008), “Testimonials versus Informational Per- suasive Messages: The Moderating Effect of Delivery Mode and Personal Involvement,” Communication Research, 35 (5), 666–94. Brinberg, David, and Anne Hamby (2012), “Narrative Persuasion: Applications and Reflections on This Approach from Three Disciplines,” Advances in Consumer Research, 40 (1), 191–94. Brown, Stephen (1995), “Postmodern Marketing Research: No Representation without Taxation,” Journal of the Market Re- search Society, 37 (3), 287–310. Bruner, Jerome (1986), Actual Minds, Possible Worlds, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Busselle, Rick W., and Helena Bilandzic (2008), “Fictionality and Perceived Realism in Experiencing Stories: A Model of Nar- rative Comprehension and Engagement,” Communication Theory, 18 (2), 255–80. ∗ Caputo, Nicole Mossing, and Donna Rouner (2011), “Narrative Processing of Entertainment Media and Mental Illness Stigma,” Health Communication, 26 (7), 595–604. Cartier (2012), “L’Odysse´e de Cartier,” http://www.youtube.com /watch?v p yaBNjTtCxd4&feature p c4-overview&playnext p 1&list p TLwEjuL8h5I9o. Chaiken, Shelly (1980), “Heuristic versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source versus Message Cues in Persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (5), 752–66. ——— (1987), “The Heuristic Model of Persuasion,” in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 5, ed. Mark P. Zanna, James M. Olson, and C. Peter Herman, Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- baum, 3–39. ∗ Chang, Chingching (2009), “‘Being Hooked’ by Editorial Con- tent: The Implications for Processing Narrative Advertising,” Journal of Advertising, 38 (1), 21–33. Chase, Susan E. (1995), “Taking Narrative Seriously: Conse- quences for Method and Theory in Interview Studies,” in Interpreting Experience: The Narrative Study of Lives, ed. Ruthellen Josselson and Amia Lieblich, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1–26. Cheung, Mike W. L., and Wai Chan (2005), “Meta-Analytic Struc- tural Equation Modeling: A Two-Stage Approach,” Psycho- logical Methods, 10 (1), 40–64. Chronis, Anthinodoros (2008), “Co-constructing the Narrative Ex- perience: Staging and Consuming the American Civil War at Gettysburg,” Journal of Marketing Management, 24 (1/2), 5–27. Cohen, Jacob (1992), “A Power Primer,” Psychological Bulletin, 112 (1), 155–59. Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly (1992), Flow: The Psychology of Hap- piness, New York: Harper & Row. Dal Cin, Sonya, Bryan Gibson, Mark P. Zanna, Roberta Shumate, and Geoffrey T. Fong (2007), “Smoking in Movies, Implicit Associations of Smoking with the Self, and Intentions to Smoke,” Psychological Science, 18 (7), 559–63. ∗ Dal Cin, Sonya, Mark P. Zanna, and Geoffrey T. Fong (2004), “Narrative Persuasion and Overcoming Resistance,” in Re-sistance and Persuasion, ed. Eric S. Knowles and Jay A. Linn, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 175–91. David, Bruno, and Meredith Wilson (2002), “Spaces of Resistance: Graffiti and Indigenous Place Markings in the Early European Contact Period of Northern Australia,” in Inscribed Land- scapes: Marking and Making Place, ed. Bruno David and Meredith Wilson, Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Davis, Mark H. (1983), “Measuring Individual Differences in Em- pathy: Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 (1), 113–26. De Certeau, Michel Jean Emmanuel de La Barge (1984), The Prac- tice of Everyday Life, Berkeley: University of California Press. Deighton, John (1992), “The Consumption of Performance,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 19 (3), 362–72. Deighton, John, Daniel Romer, and Josh McQueen (1989), “Using Drama to Persuade,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (3), 335–43. Diamond, Nina, John F. Sherry, Albert M. Mun˜iz, Mary Ann McGrath, Robert V. Kozinets, and Stefania Borghini (2009), “American Girl and the Brand Gestalt: Closing the Loop on Sociocultural Branding Research,” Journal of Marketing, 73 (3), 118–34. Diekman, Amanda, and Sarah Murnen (2004), “Learning to Be Little Women and Little Men: The Inequitable Gender Equal- ity of Nonsexist Children’s Literature,” Sex Roles, 50 (5), 373–85. Dunlop, Sally M., Melanie Wakefield, and Yoshihisa Kashima (2008), “The Contribution of Antismoking Advertising to Quitting: Intra- and Interpersonal Processes,” Journal of Health Communication, 13 (3), 250–66. ∗ ——— (2010), “Pathways to Persuasion: Cognitive and Expe- riential Responses to Health-Promoting Mass Media Mes- sages,” Communication Research, 37 (1), 133–64. Eagle, Lynne (2007), “Commercial Media Literacy,” Journal of Advertising, 36 (2), 101–10. Eagly, Alice H., and Shelly Chaiken (1993), The Psychology of Attitudes, Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Eco, Umberto (1994), Six Walks in the Fictional Woods, Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Edell, Julie A., and Marian Chapman Burke (1987), “The Power of Feelings in Understanding Advertising Effects,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (3), 421–33. Epp, Amber M., and Linda L. Price (2008), “Family Identity: A Framework of Identity Interplay in Consumption Practices,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 50–70. Escalas, Jennifer Edson (1998), “Advertising Narratives: What Are They and How Do They Work?” in Representing Consumers: Voices, Views, and Visions, ed. Barbara B. Stern, London: Routledge, 267–89. ∗ ——— (2004a), “Imagine Yourself in the Product: Mental Sim- ulation, Narrative Transportation, and Persuasion,” Journal of Advertising, 33 (2), 37–48. ∗ ——— (2004b), “Narrative Processing: Building Consumer Con- nections to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (1/ 2), 168–80. ∗ ——— (2007), “Self-Referencing and Persuasion: Narrative Transportation versus Analytical Elaboration,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (4), 421–29. Escalas, Jennifer Edson, Marian Chapman Moore, and Julie Edell Britton (2004), “Fishing for Feelings? Hooking Viewers Helps!” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (1–2), 105–14. Escalas, Jennifer Edson, and Barbara B. Stern (2003), “Sympathyand Empathy: Emotional Responses to Advertising Dramas,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (4), 566–78. Fish, Stanley Eugene (1980), Is There a Text in This Class? The Authority of Interpretive Communities, Cambridge, MA: Har- vard University Press. Fishbein, Martin, and Marco C. Yzer (2003), “Using Theory to Design Effective Health Behavior Interventions,” Commu- nication Theory, 13 (2), 164–83. Frijda, Nico H., Andrew Ortony, Joep Sonnemans, and Gerald L. Clore (1992), “The Complexity of Intensity: Issues Concern- ing the Structure of Emotion Intensity,” in Emotion: Review of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 13, ed. Margaret S. Clark, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 60–89. Frye, Herman Northrop (1957/1973), Anatomy of Criticism: Four Essays, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Genette, Ge´rard (1979/1992), The Architext: An Introduction, Berkeley: University of California Press. Gergen, Kenneth J., and Mary M. Gergen (1988), “Narrative and the Self as Relationship,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 21, ed. Leonard Berkowitz, New York: Ac- ademic Press, 17–56. Gerrig, Richard J. (1993), Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Ginns, Paul (2005), “Meta-Analysis of the Modality Effect,” Learning and Instruction, 15 (4), 313–31. Grayson, Kent (1997), “Narrative Theory and Consumer Research: Theoretical and Methodological Perspectives,” Advances in Consumer Research, 24 (1), 67–70. ∗ Green, Melanie C. (2004), “Transportation into Narrative Worlds: The Role of Prior Knowledge and Perceived Realism,” Dis- course Processes, 38 (2), 247–67. ——— (2006), “Narratives and Cancer Communication,” Journal of Communication, 56 (S1), S163–S183. ——— (2008), “Transportation Theory,” in International Ency- clopedia of Communication, ed. Wolfgang Donsbach, Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 5170–75. ∗ Green, Melanie C., and Timothy C. Brock (2000), “The Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (5), 701– 21. ——— (2002), “In the Mind’s Eye: Transportation-Imagery Model of Narrative Persuasion,” in Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations., ed. Melanie C. Green, Jeffrey J. Strange, and Timothy C. Brock, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 315–41. Green, Melanie C., Timothy C. Brock, and Geoff F. Kaufman (2004), “Understanding Media Enjoyment: The Role of Trans- portation into Narrative Worlds,” Communication Theory, 14 (4), 311–27. ∗ Green, Melanie C., and John K. Donahue (2011), “Persistence of Belief Change in the Face of Deception: The Effect of Factual Stories Revealed to Be False,” Media Psychology, 14 (3), 312–31. Green, Melanie C., Jennifer Garst, and Timothy C. Brock (2004), “The Power of Fiction: Determinants and Boundaries,” in The Psychology of Entertainment Media: Blurring the Lines be- tween Entertainment and Persuasion, ed. L. J. Shrum, Mah- wah, NJ: Erlbaum, 161–76. ∗ Green, Melanie C., Sheryl Kass, Jana Carrey, Benjamin Herzig, Ryan Feeney, and John Sabini (2008), “Transportation across Media: Repeated Exposure to Print and Film,” Media Psy- chology, 11 (4), 512–39. Greenwood, Dara N. (2007), “Are Female Action Heroes Risky Role Models? Character Identification, Idealization, and Viewer Aggression,” Sex Roles, 57 (9–10), 725–32. Hall, Alice (2009), “Perceptions of the Authenticity of Reality Programs and Their Relationships to Audience Involvement, Enjoyment, and Perceived Learning,” Journal of Broadcast- ing and Electronic Media, 53 (4), 515–31. Head, Gabriela, Hope Jensen Schau, and Katherine Thompson (2011), “Facilitating Collective Brand Engagement and Col- laborative Production through Cultural Marketing,” in Mar- keting Management: A Cultural Perspective, ed. Lisa Pen˜a- loza, Nil Toulouse, and Luca M. Visconti, London: Routledge, 194–211. Hoffner, Cynthia A., Kenneth J. Levine, and Raiza A. Toohey (2008), “Socialization to Work in Late Adolescence: The Role of Television and Family,” Journal of Broadcasting and Elec- tronic Media, 52 (2), 282–302. Holbrook, Morris B., and Rajeev Batra (1987), “Assessing the Role of Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Ad- vertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (3), 404–20. Holt, Douglas B. (1995), “How Consumers Consume: A Typology of Consumption Practices,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (1), 1–16. ——— (1997), “Poststructuralist Lifestyle Analysis: Conceptu- alizing the Social Patterning of Consumption in Postmoder- nity,” Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (4), 326–50. ——— (2002), “Why Do Brands Cause Trouble? A Dialectical Theory of Consumer Culture and Branding,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 29 (1), 70–90. Hood, Ralph W., Jr. (1975), “The Construction and Preliminary Validation of a Measure of Reported Mystical Experience,” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 14 (1), 29–41. Howard, Daniel J., and Charles Gengler (2001), “Emotional Con- tagion Effects on Product Attitudes,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (2), 189–201. Hullett, Craig R., and Timothy R. Levine (2003), “The Overesti- mation of Effect Sizes from F Values in Meta-Analysis: The Cause and a Solution,” Communication Monographs, 70 (1), 52–67. Hunter, John Edward, and Frank L. Schmidt (2004), Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Find- ings, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Jaguar (2013), “Jaguar F-Type Presents Desire,” http://www.you tube.com/watch?feature p player_embedded&v p Trlv9Ud IheM. ∗ Jensen, Jakob D., Jennifer K. Bernat, Kari M. Wilson, and Julie Goonewardene (2011), “The Delay Hypothesis: The Mani- festation of Media Effects over Time,” Human Communica- tion Research, 37 (4), 509–28. John, Deborah Roedder (1999), “Consumer Socialization of Chil- dren: A Retrospective Look at Twenty-Five Years of Re- search,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (3), 183–213. ∗ Karapanos, Evangelos, Mary Barreto, Valentina Nisi, and Evan- gelos Niforatos (2012), “Does Locality Make a Difference? Assessing the Effectiveness of Location-Aware Narratives,” Interacting with Computers, 24 (4), 273–79. ∗ Kaufman, Geoff F., and Lisa K. Libby (2012), “Changing Beliefs and Behavior through Experience-Taking,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 103 (1), 1–19. Kozinets, Robert V. (2002), “Can Consumers Escape the Market? Emancipatory Illuminations from Burning Man,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (1), 20–38. Kozinets, Robert V., Kristine de Valck, Andrea C. Wojnicki, andSarah J. S. Wilner (2010), “Networked Narratives: Under- standing Word-of-Mouth Marketing in Online Communities,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (2), 71–89. ∗ Krakowiak, K. Maja, and Mary Beth Oliver (2012), “When Good Characters Do Bad Things: Examining the Effect of Moral Ambiguity on Enjoyment,” Journal of Communication, 62 (1), 117–35. Ku¨ntay, Aylin (2002), “Development of the Expression of Indef- initeness: Presenting New Referents in Turkish Picture-Series Stories,” Discourse Processes, 33 (1), 77–102. ∗ LaMarre, Heather L., and Kristen D. Landreville (2009), “When Is Fiction as Good as Fact? Comparing the Influence of Doc- umentary and Historical Reenactment Films on Engagement, Affect, Issue Interest, and Learning,” Mass Communication and Society, 12 (4), 537–55. Lasswell, Harold D. (1948), “The Structure and Function of Com- munication in Society,” in The Communication of Ideas: A Series of Addresses, ed. Lyman Bryson, New York: Institute for Religious and Social Studies, 37–51. ∗ Lochbuehler, Kirsten, Michiel Peters, Ron H. J. Scholte, and Rutger C. M. E. Engels (2010), “Effects of Smoking Cues in Movies on Immediate Smoking Behavior,” Nicotine and To- bacco Research, 12 (9), 913–18. Luedicke, Marius K., Craig J. Thompson, and Markus Giesler (2010), “Consumer Identity Work as Moral Protagonism: How Myth and Ideology Animate a Brand-Mediated Moral Con- flict,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (6), 1016–32. Magnum Ice Cream US (2013), “Magnum Ice Cream Presents as Good as Gold Starring Joe Manganiello, Caroline Correa & Zac Posen,” http://www.youtube.com/watch?v p reTPmnvJ3 Ng&feature p youtu.be. Mar, Raymond A., Keith Oatley, Jacob Hirsh, Jennifer de la Paz, and Jordan B. Peterson (2006), “Bookworms versus Nerds: Exposure to Fiction versus Non-fiction, Divergent Associa- tions with Social Ability, and the Simulation of Fictional So- cial Worlds,” Journal of Research in Personality, 40 (5), 694–712. ∗ Marsh, Elizabeth J., and Lisa K. Fazio (2006), “Learning Errors from Fiction: Difficulties in Reducing Reliance on Fictional Stories,” Memory and Cognition, 34 (5), 1140–49. ∗ Mazzocco, Philip J., Melanie C. Green, Jo A. Sasota, and Norman W. Jones (2010), “This Story Is Not for Everyone: Trans- portability and Narrative Persuasion,” Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1 (4), 361–68. McCracken, Grant (1986), “Culture and Consumption: A Theo- retical Account of the Structure and Movement of the Cultural Meaning of Consumer Goods,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 13 (1), 71–84. ——— (2005), Culture and Consumption II: Markets, Meaning, and Brand Management, Bloomington: Indiana University Press. McGill, Ann, Laura Peracchio, and Mary Frances Luce (2011), “Solidarity of Purpose: Building an Understanding of Con- sumers through a Community of Scholars,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 38 (1), ii–viii. Moore, Sarah G. (2012), “Some Things Are Better Left Unsaid: How Word of Mouth Influences the Storyteller,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (6), 1140–54. Morgan, Susan E., Lauren Movius, and Michael J. Cody (2009), “The Power of Narratives: The Effect of Entertainment Tele- vision Organ Donation Storylines on the Attitudes, Knowl- edge, and Behaviors of Donors and Nondonors,” Journal of Communication, 59 (1), 135–51. Moyer-Guse´, Emily (2008), “Toward a Theory of Entertainment Persuasion: Explaining the Persuasive Effects of Entertain- ment-Education Messages,” Communication Theory, 18 (3), 407–25. ∗ Moyer-Guse´, Emily, and Robin L. Nabi (2010), “Explaining the Effects of Narrative in an Entertainment Television Program: Overcoming Resistance to Persuasion,” Human Communi- cation Research, 36 (1), 26–52. Muniz, Albert M., and Thomas C. O’Guinn (2001), “Brand Com- munity,” Journal of Consumer Research, 27 (4), 412–32. Nabi, Robin L., and Marina Krcmar (2004), “Conceptualizing Me- dia Enjoyment as Attitude: Implications for Mass Media Ef- fects Research,” Communication Theory, 14 (4), 288–310. ∗ Nabi, Robin L., Carnien R. Stitt, Jeff Halford, and Keli L. Fin- nerty (2006), “Emotional and Cognitive Predictors of the En- joyment of Reality-Based and Fictional Television Program- ming: An Elaboration of the Uses and Gratifications Perspective,” Media Psychology, 8 (4), 421–47. Nell, Victor (1988), Lost in a Book: The Psychology of Reading for Pleasure, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ∗ Nielsen, Jesper H., and Jennifer Edson Escalas (2010), “Easier Is Not Always Better: The Moderating Role of Processing Type on Preference Fluency,” Journal of Consumer Psy- chology, 20 (3), 295–305. Paharia, Neeru, Anat Keinan, Jill Avery, and Juliet B. Schor (2011), “The Underdog Effect: The Marketing of Disadvan- tage and Determination through Brand Biography,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (5), 775–90. Pen˜aloza, Lisa (2001), “Consuming the American West: Animating Cultural Meaning and Memory at a Stock Show and Rodeo,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (3), 369–98. Pennington, Nancy, and Reid Hastie (1988), “Explanation-Based Decision Making: Effects of Memory Structure on Judgment,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14 (3), 521–33. Peracchio, Laura A. (1992), “How Do Young Children Learn to Be Consumers? A Script-Processing Approach,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (4), 425–40. ——— (1993), “Young Children’s Processing of a Televised Nar- rative: Is a Picture Really Worth a Thousand Words?” Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (2), 281–93. Petty, Richard E., and John T. Cacioppo (1986), Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change, New York: Springer. Petty, Richard E., and Duane T. Wegener (1998), “Attitude Change: Multiple Roles for Persuasion Variables,” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, ed. Daniel Todd Gilbert, Susan T. Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey, Boston: McGraw-Hill, 323–90. Phillips, Barbara J., and Edward F. McQuarrie (2010), “Narrative and Persuasion in Fashion Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (3), 368–92. Piaget, Jean (1972), “Intellectual Evolution from Adolescence to Adulthood,” Human Development, 15 (1), 1–12. Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Jeong-Yeon Lee, and Nathan P. Podsakoff (2003), “Common Method Biases in Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and Recommended Remedies,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (5), 879–903. Polichak, James W., and Richard J. Gerrig (2002), “‘Get Up and Win!’ Participatory Responses to Narrative,” in Narrative Im- pact: Social and Cognitive Foundations, ed. Timothy C. Brock, Melanie C. Green, and Jeffrey J. Strange, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Radway, Janice A. (1984), Reading the Romance: Women, Patri- archy and Popular Literature, Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press. Ritson, Mark, and Richard Elliott (1999), “The Social Uses of Advertising: An Ethnographic Study of Adolescent Adver- tising Audiences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (3), 260–77. Rosenthal, Robert (1991), Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ryokai, Kimiko, Vaucelle Cati, and Cassell Justine (2003), “Virtual Peers as Partners in Storytelling and Literacy Learning,” Jour- nal of Computer Assisted Learning, 19 (2), 195–208. ∗ Schlosser, Ann E. (2003), “Experiencing Products in the Virtual World: The Role of Goal and Imagery in Influencing Attitudes versus Purchase Intentions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (2), 184–98. Schrøder, Kim C. (1994), “Audience Semiotics, Interpretive Com- munities and the Ethnographic Turn in Media Research,” Me- dia, Culture and Society, 16 (2), 337–47. Scott, Linda M. (1994), “Images in Advertising: The Need for a Theory of Visual Rhetoric,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (2), 252–73. ∗ Sestir, Marc, and Melanie C. Green (2010), “You Are Who You Watch: Identification and Transportation Effects on Tempo- rary Self-Concept,” Social Influence, 5 (4), 272–88. Shankar, Avi, Richard Elliott, and Christina Goulding (2001), “Un- derstanding Consumption: Contributions from a Narrative Perspective,” Journal of Marketing Management, 17 (3/4), 429–53. Shavitt, Sharon (2011), “What’s New? Novelty in Consumer Re- search,” presidential address, Assocation for Consumer Re- search, St. Louis. Shen, Lijiang (2010), “Mitigating Psychological Reactance: The Role of Message-Induced Empathy in Persuasion,” Human Communication Research, 36 (3), 397–422. Sherry, John F., Jr., Robert V. Kozinets, Diana Storm, Adam Du- hachek, Krittinee Nuttavuthisit, and Bene´t DeBerry-Spence (2001), “Being in the Zone,” Journal of Contemporary Eth- nography, 30 (4), 465–510. Simon, Bryant (2009), Everything but the Coffee: Learning about America from Starbucks, Berkeley: University of California Press. Singhal, Arvind, and Everett M. Rogers (2002), “A Theoretical Agenda for Entertainment-Education,” Communication The- ory, 12 (2), 117–35. Slater, Michael D. (2002a), “Entertainment Education and the Per- suasive Impact of Narratives,” in Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations, ed. Timothy C. Brock, Melanie C. Green, and Jeffrey J. Strange, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 157–81. ——— (2002b), “Involvement as Goal-Directed Strategic Pro- cessing: Extending the Elaboration Likelihood Model,” in The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice, ed. James Price Dillard and Michael Pfau, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 175–94. Slater, Michael D., David B. Buller, Emily Waters, Margarita Ar- chibeque, and Michelle LeBlanc (2003), “A Test of Conver- sational and Testimonial Messages versus Didactic Presen- tations of Nutrition Information,” Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 35 (5), 255–59. Slater, Michael D., and Donna Rouner (2002), “Entertainment- Education and Elaboration Likelihood: Understanding theProcessing of Narrative Persuasion,” Communication Theory, 12 (2), 173–91. Slater, Michael D., Donna Rouner, and Marilee Long (2006), “Tele- vision Dramas and Support for Controversial Public Policies: Effects and Mechanisms,” Journal of Communication, 56 (2), 235–52. Smith, Keri (2007), The Guerilla Art Kit, New York: Princeton Architectural. Stern, Barbara B. (1992), “Feminist Literary Theory and Adver- tising Research: A New ‘Reading’ of the Text and the Con- sumer,” Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertis- ing, 14 (1), 9–21. ——— (1993), “Feminist Literary Criticism and the Deconstruc- tion of Ads: A Postmodern View of Advertising and Con- sumer Responses,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (4), 556–66. ——— (1994), “Authenticity and the Textual Persona: Postmodern Paradoxes in Advertising Narrative,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 11 (4), 387–400. ——— (1995), “Consumer Myths: Frye’s Taxonomy and the Structural Analysis of Consumption Text,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 22 (2), 165–85. ——— (1997), “Postmodern Consumer Research Narratives: Problems in Construct Definition, Structure, and Classifica- tion,” Advances in Consumer Research, 24 (1), 68–69. Stern, Barbara B., Craig J. Thompson, and Eric J. Arnould (1998), “Narrative Analysis of a Marketing Relationship: The Con- sumer’s Perspective,” Psychology and Marketing, 15 (3), 195–214. Sun, Shuyan (2011), “Meta-Analysis of Cohen’s Kappa,” Health Services and Outcomes Research Methodology, 11 (3–4), 145–63. Tellegen, Auke, and Gilbert Atkinson (1974), “Openness to Ab- sorbing and Self-Altering Experiences (‘Absorption’), a Trait Related to Hypnotic Susceptibility,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83 (3), 268–77. Thompson, Craig J. (1997), “Interpreting Consumers: A Herme- neutical Framework for Deriving Marketing Insights from the Texts of Consumers’ Consumption Stories,” Journal of Mar- keting Research, 34 (4), 438–55. Thompson, Craig, and Kelly Tian (2008), “Reconstructing the South: How Commercial Myths Compete for Identity Value through the Ideological Shaping of Popular Memories and Countermemories,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (5), 595–613. Valentine, Jeffrey C., Therese D. Pigott, and Hannah R. Rothstein (2010), “How Many Studies Do You Need?” Journal of Ed- ucational and Behavioral Statistics, 35 (2), 215–47. ∗ Van den Hende, Ellis A., Jan P. L. Schoormans, Kaj P. N. Morel, Tatiana Lashina, Evert van Loenen, and Erik I. de Boevere (2007), “Using Early Concept Narratives to Collect Valid Cus- tomer Input about Breakthrough Technologies: The Effect of Application Visualization on Transportation,” Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 74 (9), 1773–87. ∗ Van Laer, Tom (2013), “The Means to Justify the End: Combating Cyber Harassment in Social Media,” Journal of Business Ethics, 115 (3), forthcoming. ∗ Van Laer, Tom, and Ko de Ruyter (2010), “In Stories We Trust: How Narrative Apologies Provide Cover for Competitive Vul- nerability after Integrity-Violating Blog Posts,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 27 (2), 164–74. ∗ Van Laer, Tom, Ko de Ruyter, and David Cox (2013), “A Walk in Customers’ Shoes: How Attentional Bias Modification Af-fects Ownership of Integrity-Violating Social Media Posts,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 27 (1), 14–27. Visconti, Luca M., John F. Sherry Jr., Stefania Borghini, and Laurel Anderson (2010), “Street Art, Sweet Art? Reclaiming the ‘Public’ in Public Place,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (3), 511–29. ∗ Volkman, Julie E., and Roxanne L. Parrott (2012), “Expressing Emotions as Evidence in Osteoporosis Narratives: Effects on Message Processing and Intentions,” Human Communication Research, 38 (4), 429–58. Vygotsky, Lev Semenovicˇ, and Michael Cole (1978), Mind and Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ∗ Wang, Jing, and Bobby J. Calder (2006), “Media Transportation and Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (2), 151–62. Weick, Karl E. (1995), Sensemaking in Organizations, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ∗ Williams, Jessica H., Melanie C. Green, Connie Kohler, Jeroan J. Allison, and Thomas K. Houston (2011), “Stories to Com- municate Risks about Tobacco: Development of a Brief Scale to Measure Transportation into a Video Story—the Acce Proj- ect,” Health Education Journal, 70 (2), 184–91. Wood, Wendy, Carl A. Kallgren, and Rebecca M. Preisler (1985), “Access to Attitude-Relevant Information in Memory as a Determinant of Persuasion: The Role of Message Attrib- utes,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21 (1), 73–85. Yannopoulou, Natalia, and Richard Elliott (2008), “Open versus Closed Advertising Texts and Interpretive Communities,” In- ternational Journal of Advertising, 27 (1), 9–36. ∗ Zwarun, Lara, and Alice Hall (2012), “Narrative Persuasion, Transportation, and the Role of Need for Cognition in Online Viewing of Fantastical Films,” Media Psychology, 15 (3), 327–55.
  223. References Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality: Vol. 1. Psychological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press. Bachorowski, J. A. (1999). Vocal expression and perception of emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- ence, 8, 53–57. Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psy- chological Bulletin, 126, 925–945. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation e V ect. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 62, 893–912. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation e V ect: Unconditional auto- matic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104–128. Barrett, L. F. (1996). Hedonic tone, perceived arousal, and item desirability: Three components of a V ective experi- ence. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 47–68. Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimensions. The role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 579–599. Barrett, L. F. (2004). Feelings or words. Understanding the content in self-report ratings of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 266–281. Barrett, L. F. (in press, a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science. Barrett, L. F. (in press, b). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personal- ity and Social Psychology Review. Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Conner, T., & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Emotion di V erentiation and regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 713–724. Barrett, L. F., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2004). Valence focus and the perception of facial a V ect. Emotion, 4, 266–274. Barrett, L. F., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (in press). Automaticity and emotion. In J. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior. New York: Psychology Press. Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. (in preparation). Valence focus and self-esteem lability, Boston College. Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Aronson, K. R. (2004). Arousal focus and interoceptive sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 684–697. Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 563–573. Bolger, N., & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of everyday life: The role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. Journal of Personality. Special Issue: Personality and daily experi- ence, 59, 355–386. Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personality in the stress process. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 69, 890–902. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242–276). New York: Oxford University Press. Bremner, J. D. (2003). Long-term e V ects of childhood abuse on brain and neurobiology. Child Adolescence Psychi- atric Clinics of North America, 12, 271–292. Brendl, C. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1995). Principles of judging valence: What makes events positive or negative? Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 95–160. Bridges, K. (1932). Emotional development in early infancy. Child Development, 3, 324–334. Burnkrant, R. E., & Page, T. J. (1984). A modi W cation to Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss self-consciousness scales. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 629–637. Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 42, 116–131. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Klein, D. J., & Poehlmann, K. M. (1997). The psychophysiology of emotion across the lifespan. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 17, 27–74. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In R. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), The handbook of emotion (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press. Cahill, L., Haier, R. J., Fallon, F., Alkire, M. T., Tang, C., Keator, D., Wu, J., & McGaugh, J. L. (1996). Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 8016–8021. Canli, T., Sivers, H., Gotlib, I. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Amygdala activation to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science, 296, 2191. Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality in X uences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33–42. Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amyg- dala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 26, 321–352. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and a V ective responses to impending reward and punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. Conner, T., Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2005). Valence focus and automatic evaluation. Manuscript under review. Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1995). Personality and reinforcement in associative and instrumental learning. Personality Individual Di V erences, 19, 47–71. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Damasio, A. R. (2000). The Feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Trade Publishers. Davis, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 13–34. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative a V ect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1253–1265. Duckworth, K. L., Bargh, J. A., Garcia, M., & Chaiken, S. (2002). The automatic evaluation of novel stimuli. Psy- chological Science, 13, 513–519. Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural di V erences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Elzinga, B. M., Schmahl, C. G., Vermetten, E., van Dyck, R., & Bremner, J. D. (2003). Higher cortisol levels follow- ing exposure to traumatic reminders in abuse-related PTSD. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 1656–1665. Emde, R. N., Gaensbauer, T. J., & Harmon, R. J. (1976). Emotional expression in infancy: A biobehavioral study. Psychological Issues, 10, 1–200. Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115–141. Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. Feldman, L. A. (1995). Valence focus and arousal focus: Individual di V erences in the structure of a V ective experi- ence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 153–166. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527. Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations in mater- nal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 1155–1158. Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (2002). Environmental enrichment reverses the e V ects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 7840–7848. Gitau, R., Fisk, N. M., Teixeira, J., Cameron, A., & Glover, V. (2001). Fetal HPA stress responses to invasive pro- cedures are independent of maternal responses. Journal of Clinical and Endocrinological Metabolism, 86, 104– 109. Gottleib, G. (2002). A developmental psychobiological systems view: Early formulation and current status. In S. Oyama, P. E. Gri Y ths, & R. Gray (Eds.), Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution (pp. 41– 54). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Ho V man, J. M., & Kilts, C. D. (1999). Ecstasy and agony: Activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative emotion. Psychological Science, 13, 135–141. Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., Goldman, D., et al. (2002). Serotonin trans- porter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science, 297, 400–403. Harper, L. V. (2005). Epigenetic inheritance and the intergenerational transfer of experience. Psychological Bulle- tin, 131, 340–360. Heinz, A., Braus, D. F., Smolka, M. N., Wrase, J., Puls, I., Hermann, D., et al. (2005). Amygdala-prefrontal cou- pling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 20–21. Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, O. M. (1993). Stability of emotional experiences and their rela- tions to traits of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 847–860. Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (1995). Epigenetic inheritance and evolution. NewYork: Oxford University Press. Jarvis, B. J., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194. Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning Press. Joreskog, K., & Sorbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8 user’s reference guide. IL, Chicago: Scienti W c Software Interna- tional, Inc.. Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evi- dence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, 341–372. Lane, R. D., Chua, P. M. L., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Common e V ects of emotional valence, arousal, and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia, 37, 989–997. Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). A V ect intensity as an individual di V erence characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 1–39. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–141. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1986). A V ect intensity and reactions to daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 803–814. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Cropanzano, R. S. (1987). Cognitive operations associated with individual di V erences in a V ect intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 767–774. Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lewontin, R. (2000). The triple helix: Gene, organism, and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 819–835. Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the fundamental fea- tures of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 452–468. Maccari, S., Darnaudery, M., Morley-Fletcher, S., Zuena, A. R., Cinque, C., & Van Reeth, O. (2003). Prenatal stress and long-term consequences: implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 119–127. Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whit W eld, S., Wais, P., Ochsner, K., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15, 259–263. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1991). Adding Liebe und Arbeit: The full W ve-factor model and well-being. Person- ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227–232. Meany, M. J., Diorio, J., Francis, D., Widdowson, J., LaPlante, P., Caldji, C., et al. (1996). Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: Implications for adrenocortical responses to stress. Developmental Neuroscience, 18, 49–72. Messinger, D. S. (2002). Positive and negative: Infant facial expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psy- chological Science, 11, 1–6. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Mischel, W. (2004). Towards an integrative science of the person. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 1–22. Nauta, W. J. (1971). The problem of the frontal lobe: A reinterpretation. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 8, 167–187. Ongur, D., Ferry, A. T., & Price, J. L. (2003). Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefron- tal cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 460, 425–449. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The congnitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge Univer- sity. Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions. Psychological Review, 97, 315–331. Panksepp, J. (1998). A V ective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality. In L. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 277–299). New York: Guilford Press. Reisenzein, R. (1992). A structuralist reconstruction of Wundt’s three-dimensional theory of emotion. In H. West- meyer (Ed.), The structuralist program in psychology: Foundations and applications (pp. 141–189). Toronto: Hopgrefe Huber. Reisenzein, R., & Schonp X ug, W. (1992). Stumpf’s cognitive-evaluative theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 47, 34–45. Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. New York: Oxford University Press. Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships, as developed from the cli- ent-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of science (Vol. 3). New York: McGraw-Hill. Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450. Russell, J. A. (2003). Core a V ect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172. Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core a V ect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emo- tion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Matthews, K. A. (1983). Attentional factors in the perception of bodily states. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook (pp. 510–542). New York: Guilford Press. Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Schneider, M. L., Moore, C. F., Kraemer, G. W., Roberts, A. D., & DeJesus, O. T. (2002). The impact of prenatal stress, fetal alcohol exposure, or both on development: Perspectives from a primate model. Psychoneuroendo- crinology, 27, 285–298. Schneider, T. R. (2004). The role of neuroticism on psychological and physiological stress responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 795–804. Schneirla, T. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 27–58). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P. J., McMullin, K. G., et al. (2003). Di V erential Amygdala response to novel vs. newly familiar neutral faces: A functional MRI probe developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 854–862. Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Wright, J. C. (1994). Intra-individual stability in the organization and patterning of behavior: Incorporating psychological situations into the idiographic analysis of personality. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 65, 674–687. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Toward delivering on the promise of appraisal theory. In K. R. Scherer & A. Schorr (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 121–138). New York: Oxford University Press. Smith, C. A., & Pope, L. K. (1992). Appraisal and emotion: The interactional contributions of dispositional and situational factors. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 32–62. Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 1233–1243. Spitz, R. A. (1965). The W rst year of life. New York: International Universities Press. Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 462–516). New York: Wiely. Tesser, A., & Martin, L. (1996). The psychology of evaluation. In T. E. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 400–432). New York: Guilford Press. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. P. (1999). Self-consciousness and the W ve factor model of personality: Distinguish- ing re X ection from rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304. Wager, T. D., Phan, K. L., Liberzon, I., & Taylor, S. F. (2003). Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: A meta-analysis of W ndings from neuroimaging. Neuroimage, 19, 513–531. Wallace, J. F., Newman, J. P., & Bachorowski, J. (1991). Failures of response modulation: Impulsive behavior in anxious and impulsive individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 23–44. Wallace, J. F., & Newman, J. P. (1997). Neuroticism and the attentional mediation of dysregulatory psychopathol- ogy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 135–156. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and speci W c factors of emotional experi- ence and their relation to the W ve factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441–476. Wierzbicka, A. (1992). De W ning emotion concepts. Cognitive Science, 16, 539–581. Wright, J. C., & Mischel, W. (1988). Conditional hedges and the intuitive psychology of traits. Journal of Personal- ity and Social Psychology, 55, 454–469.
  224. References Bandura,A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory.American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184. Barkow, J.H. (1992). Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and social stratification. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 627–638). New York: Oxford University Press. Baumeister, R.F., & Tice, D.M. (1996). Rethinking and reclaiming the interdisciplinary role of personality psychology: The science of human nature should be the center of the social sci- ences and humanities. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 363–373. Benjamin, L.T., Bryant, W.H.M., Campbell, C., Luttrell, J., & Holtz, C. (1997). Between psoriasis and ptarmigan: American encyclopedia portrayals of psychology, 1880–1940. Review of Gen- eral Psychology, 1, 5–18. Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon. Bevan, W. (1991). Contemporary psychology: A tour inside the onion. American Psychologist, 46, 475–483. Blumberg, M.S., & Wasserman, E.A. (1996). Animals have minds? American Psychologist, 51, 59–60. Brown, D.E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill. Bunge, M. (1990). What kind of discipline is psychology: Autonomous or dependent, humanistic or scientific, biological or sociological? New Ideas in Psychology, 8, 121–137. Cook, N.D. (1989). Toward a central dogma for psychology. New Ideas in Psychology, 7, 1–18. Cronk, L. (1999). That complex whole: Culture and the evolution of human behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Crook, J.H. (1980). The evolution of human consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press. Daly, M., & Wilson, M.I. (1999). Human evolutionary psychology and animal behavior. Animal Behavior, 57, 509–519. Day, W.F., Jr., & Leigland, S. (1992). Radical behaviorism: Willard Day on psychology and phi- losophy. Reno, NV: Context Press. Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species. New York: Norton. Dess, N.K., & Chapman, C.D. (1998). “Humans and animals”? On saying what we mean. Psycho- logical Science, 9, 156–157. Domjan, M., & Purdy, J.E. (1995). Animal research in psychology: More than meets the eye of the general psychology student. American Psychologist, 50, 496–503. Eidelson, R.J., & Eidelson, J.I. (2003). Dangerous ideas: Five beliefs that propel groups toward conflict. American Psychologist, 58, 182–192. Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724. Foa, E.B., & Kozak, M.J. (1997). Beyond the efficacy ceiling? Cognitive behavior therapy in search of theory. Behavior Therapy, 28, 601–612. Gallup, G.G., Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87. Gergen, K. (2001). Psychological science in a postmodern context. American Psychologist, 56, 803–813. Haggbloom, S.J., Warnick, R., Warnick, J.E., Jones, V.K., Yarbrough, G.L., Russell, T.M., Borecky, C.M., et al. (2002). The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century. Review of Gen- eral Psychology, 6, 139–152. Hawking, S. (1998). A brief history of time (2nd ed.). New York: Bantam Books. Hebb, D.O. (1955). Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254. Henriques, G.R. (2003). The tree of knowledge system and the theoretical unification of psychol- ogy. Review of General Psychology, 7, 150–182. Henriques, G.R., & Sternberg, R.J. (in press.) Unified Professional Psychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Hishinuma, E.S. (1998). Pre-unified separatism and rapprochement between behaviorism and cog- nitive psychology: The case of the reinforcer. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 18, 1–15. Kaufman, A. (1990). Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Kaye, J. (1996). Animal minds and evolution. American Psychologist, 51, 56–57. Kimble, G.A. (1994). A frame of reference for psychology. American Psychologist, 49, 510–519. Koch, S. (1993). “Psychology” or “the psychological studies?” American Psychologist, 48, 902–904. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I.Q. (1990). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology (3rd ed.). San Fran- cisco: W.H. Freeman. Leahey, T.H. (1992). A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological thought. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Matarazzo, J.D. (1987). There is only one psychology, no specialties, but many applications. Amer- ican Psychologist, 42, 893–903. Maynard-Smith, J., & Szathmary, E. (1999). The origins of life: From the birth of life to the origin of language. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Mayr, E., & Provine, W.B. (1998). The evolutionary synthesis: Perspectives on the unification of biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Nelson, T.O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51, 102–116. Ornstein, R.E. (1972). The psychology of consciousness. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman. Reber, A.S. (1995). Dictionary of Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin. Rilling, M. (1996). The mystery of vanished citations: James McConnell’s forgotten 1960s quest for planarian learning, a biochemical engram, and celebrity.American Psychologist, 51, 589–598. Robins, R.W., Norem, J.K., & Cheek, J.M. (1999). Naturalizing the self. In L.A. Pervin & O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 443–477). New York: The Guilford Press. Rumbaugh, D.M. (2003). A perspective of human and chimpanzee cognition. Contemporary Psy- chology, 48, 5–8. Sarason, S.B. (1989). The lack of an overarching conception in psychology. Journal of Mind & Behavior, 10, 263–279. Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S., & Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The ape at the brink of the human mind. New York: Wiley. Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J.J. (1997). The symbolic self in evolutionary context. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 80–102. Skinner, B.F. (1990). Can psychology be a science of mind? American Psychologist, 45, 1206–1210. Smolin, L. (2001). Three roads to quantum gravity. New York: Basic Books. Staats, A.W. (1999). Uniting psychology requires new infrastructure, theory, method, and a research agenda. Review of General Psychology, 3, 3–13. Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 79–95. Tolman, E.C. (1978). The determiners of behavior at a choice point. In E.R. Hilgard (Ed.), Amer- ican psychology in historical perspective (pp. 337–370). Washington, DC: American Psycho- logical Association. (Original work published 1938). Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- sity Press. Uttal, W.R. (2000). The war between mentalism and behaviorism: On the accessibility of mental processes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviourist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177. Webster’s Encyclopedic Unabridged Dictionary of the English Language. (1994). New York: Gramercy Books. Wolfe, B.E. (2003). Knowing the self: Building a bridge from basic research to clinical practice. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 13, 83–95. Wrangham, R.W., & McGrew, W.C. (1994). Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- versity Press. Yanchar, S.C., & Slife, B.D. (1997). Pursuing unity in a fragmented psychology: Problems and prospects. Review of General Psychology, 1, 235–255.
  225. References Adrian, E. D. (1935). The mechanism of nervous action. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Aiken, L. R. (1993). Personality: Theories, research and applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and human af- fairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Amador, X. F., & David, A. S. (1998). Insight and psychosis. New York: Oxford University Press. Anderson, N. (1996). A functional theory of cogni- tion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Aronson, E. (1996). Persuasion via self-justification: Large commitments for small rewards. In E. Aron- son (Ed.), The social animal (7th ed., pp. 221– 236). New York: Freeman. Bahn, P. G. (1996). Ancient art. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge en- cyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 261–277). London: Oxford University Press. Bailey, K. G. (1987). Human paleopsychology: Ap- plications to aggression and pathological pro- cesses. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Barkow, J. H. (1992). Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and social stratification. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 627–638). New York: Oxford University Press. Barr, W. B. (1998). Neurobehavioral disorders of awareness and their relevance to schizophrenia. In X. F. Amador & A. S. David (Eds.), Insight and psychosis (pp. 107–141). New York: Oxford Uni- versity Press. Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emo- tional disorders. New York: Meridian. Beck, A. T. (1999). Cognitive aspects of personality disorders and their relation to syndromal disorders: A psychoevolutionary approach. In C. R. Clon- inger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 411–429). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and human behav- ior. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwinism to behaviour- ism: Psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bonner, J. T. (1980). The evolution of culture in animals. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communi- cation. New York: Pergamon Press. Bronowski, J. (1974). Ascent of man. Boston: Little, Brown. Byrne, R. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. New York: Oxford Univer- sity Press. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective re- sponses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 67, 319–333. Catania, A. C., & Harnard, S. (1988). The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skin- ner: Comments and consequences. New York: Cambridge University Press. Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Conte, H. R., & Plutchik, R. (1995). Ego defenses: Theory and measurement. New York: Wiley. Cotman, C. W., & McGaugh, J. L. (1980). Behav- ioral neuroscience. New York: Academic Press. Crick, F. (1994). The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul. New York: Simon & Schuster. Cummins, D. D. (1996a). Evidence of deontic rea- soning in 3- and 4-year-old children. Memory & Cognition, 24, 823–829. Cummins, D. D. (1996b). Evidence for innateness of deontic reasoning. Mind and Language, 11, 160– 190. Cziko, G. (2000). The things we do: Using the les- sons of Bernard and Darwin to understand the what, how, and why of our behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Damasio, A. (1998). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1989). Laterality and emotion: An electrophysiological approach. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 419–441). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (2nd ed.). Ox- ford, England: Oxford University Press. Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype: The long reach of the gene (2nd ed.). New York: Ox- ford University Press. Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species. New York: Norton. Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. New York: Simon & Schuster. Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York: Basic Books. de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. New York: Harper & Row. Dobzhansky, T. (1964). Heredity and the nature of man. New York: New American Library. Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. American Biology Teacher, 35, 125–129. Donahoe, J. W. (1999). Edward L. Thorndike: The selectionist connectionist. Journal of the Experi- mental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 451–454. Donahoe, J. W., Burgos, J. E., & Palmer, D. C. (1993). Selectionist approach to reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 17–40. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. (1972). A theory of ob- jective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press. Eagle, M. N. (1998). Freud’s legacy: Defenses, so- matic symptoms and neurophysiology. In G. Gutt- mann & I. Scholz-Strasser (Eds.), Freud and the neurosciences: From brain research to the uncon- scious (pp. 87–101). Vienna: American Psycho- logical Association. Edelman, G. (1989). The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books. Edelman, G. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psy- chologist, 49, 709–724. Ferris, T. (1997). The whole shebang: A state-of-the- art universe(s) report. New York: Simon & Schus- ter. Fossey, D. (1983). Gorillas in the mist. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Fowles, D. C. (1994). A motivational theory of psy- chopathology. In W. Spaulding (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Integrated views of mo- tivation, cognition and emotion (Vol. 41, pp. 181– 238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Freud, A. (1966). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. New York: International Universities Press. Freud, S. (1949). Outline of psychoanalysis. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1940) Freud, S. (1960). The ego and the id. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1923) Friedrich, J. (1996). On seeing oneself as less self- serving than others: The ultimate self-serving bias? Teaching of Psychology, 23, 107–109. Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recog- nition. Science, 167, 86–87. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992). Nature’s mind: The biolog- ical roots of thinking, emotion, sexuality, lan- guage, and intelligence. New York: Basic Books. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). The cognitive neuro- sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Geary, D. (1998). Male/female: The evolution of hu- man sex differences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books. Gergen, K. (1985). The social constructionist move- ment in modern psychology. American Psycholo- gist, 40, 266–275. Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnection syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237–294. Gilgen, A. R. (1987). The psychological level of organization in nature and interdependencies among major psychological concepts. In A. W. Staats & L. P. Mos (Eds.), Annals of theoretical psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 179–209). New York: Plenum. Goleman, D. (1985). Vital lies, simple truths: The psychology of self-deception and shared illusions. New York: Simon & Schuster. Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Pat- terns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap. Gould, S. J. (1987, November). The limits of adap- tation: Is language a spandrel of the human brain? Paper presented at the Cognitive Science Seminar, Cambridge, MA. Gould, S. J. (1989). Wonderful life: The Burgess Shale and the nature of history. New York: Norton. Gould, S. J. (1993). Eight little piggies. New York: Norton. Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The span- drels of San Marco and the Panglossian program: A critique of the adaptionist programme. Proceed- ings of the Royal Society of London, 205, 281–288. Gray, J. A. (1987). Perspectives on anxiety and im- pulsivity: A commentary. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493–509. Greene, B. (1999). The elegant universe: Super- strings, hidden dimensions, and the quest for the ultimate theory. New York: Norton. Greenspan, S. I. (1989). The development of the ego. Madison, CT: International Universities Press. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fab- rication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618. Gribbin, J. (1998). Q is for quantum: An encyclope- dia of particle physics. New York: Touchstone. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior (I and II). Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52. Hawking, S. (1998). A brief history of time (2nd ed.). New York: Bantam Books. Henriques, G. R. (2000). Depression: Disease or be- havioral shutdown mechanism? Journal of Science and Health Policy, 1, 152–165. Henriques, G. R. (2002). The harmful dysfunction analysis and the differentiation between mental disorder and disease. Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice, 1, 157–173. Hofstadter, D. R., & Dennett, D. C. (1981). The mind’s I: Fantasies and reflections on self and soul. New York: Basic Books. Hoyle, F. (1964). Man in the universe. New York: Columbia University Press. Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intel- lect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cam- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. James, W. J. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1890) Johnston, V. S. (1999). Why we feel: The science of human emotions. Reading, MA: Perseus Books. Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social intelligence and pri- mate intelligence. Science, 153, 501–506. Jones, E. (1955). The life and work of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 2). New York: Basic Books. Kant, I. (1996). Critique of pure reason. Indianapo- lis, IN: Hackett. (Original work published 1781) Kimble, G. (1996). Psychology: The hope of a sci- ence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Koch, S. (1993). “Psychology” or “the psychological studies”? American Psychologist, 48, 902–904. Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (1997). Social-illusions and self-deception: The evolution of biases in per- son perception. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 21– 48). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (1997). Behavioral ecology: An evolutionary approach (4th ed.). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Science. Leaky, R., & Lewin, R. (1992). Origins reconsid- ered: In search of what makes us human. New York: Doubleday. Lieberman, P. (1998). Eve spoke: Human language and human evolution. New York: Norton. Lifson, S. (1997). On the crucial stages in the origin of animate matter. Journal of Molecular Evolu- tion, 44, 1–8. Maden, B. F. (1995). No soup for starters? Autotro- phy and the origins of metabolism. Trends in Bio- chemistry, 20, 337–341. Magnusson, D. (2000). The individual as the orga- nizing principle in psychological inquiry: A holis- tic approach. In L. R. Bergman, R. B. Cairns, L. G. Nilsson, & L. Nystedt (Eds.), Developmental sci- ence and the holistic approach (pp. 33–47). Mah- wah, NJ: Erlbaum. Maj, M., & Sartorius, N. (1999). Depressive disor- ders. New York: Wiley. Maynard-Smith, J., & Szathmary, E. (1999). The origins of life: From the birth of life to the origin of language. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Mayr, E. (1983). How to carry out the adaptionist program. American Naturalist, 121, 324–334. Mayr, E., & Provine, W. B. (1998). The evolutionary synthesis: Perspectives on the unification of biol- ogy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. McWilliams, N. (1994). Psychoanalytic diagnosis. New York: Guilford Press. Meehl, P. (1992). Theoretical risks and tabular aster- isks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the soft progress of slow psychology. In R. B. Miller (Ed.), The resto- ration of dialogue (pp. 523–555). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. (Origi- nal work published 1978) Messer, S. B., & Winokur, M. (1980). Some limits to the integration of psychoanalytic and behavior therapy. American Psychologist, 35, 818–827. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt. Mitchell, R. W. (1994). Multiplicities of self. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans (pp. 81– 107). New York: Cambridge University Press. Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: A search for the origins of art, religion, and science. London: Thames & Hudson. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Nesse, R., & Lloyd, A. T. (1992). The evolution of psychodynamic defense mechanisms. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 601–626). New York: Oxford University Press. Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Ornstein, R. E. (1972). The psychology of conscious- ness. San Francisco: Freeman. Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W., & Boccia, M. L. (1994). Self-awareness in animals and humans. New York: Cambridge University Press. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: HarperCollins. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1992). Natural language and natural selection. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 451– 494). New York: Oxford University Press. Ponvinelli, D. J., & Prince, C. G. (1998). When self met other. In M. Ferrari & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Self-awareness: Its nature and development (pp. 37–107). New York: Guilford Press. Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of per- ception. Chicago: Aldine. Pribram, K. (1986). The cognitive revolution and mind/brain issues. American Psychologist, 41, 507–520. Reed, S. K. (1996). Cognition: Theory and applica- tions (4th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Rivto, L. (1990). Darwin’s influence on Freud: A tale of two sciences. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Robins, R. W., Norem, J. K., & Cheek, J. M. (1999). Naturalizing the self. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and re- search (2nd ed., pp. 443–477). New York: Guil- ford Press. Roth, G., & Wulliman, M. F. (2001). Brain evolution and cognition. New York: Wiley. Sarason, S. B. (1989). The lack of an overarching conception in psychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 10, 263–279. Schacter, D. L. (1990). Toward a cognitive neuro- psychology of awareness: Implicit knowledge and anosognosia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 155–178. Schacter, D. L. (1993). Neuropsychological evidence for a consciousness system. In A. I. Goldman (Ed.), Readings in philosophy and cognitive sci- ence (pp. 415–444). Cambridge, England: Cam- bridge University Press. Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). The sym- bolic self in evolutionary context. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 80–102. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On de- pression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman. Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning nec- essary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Skinner, B. F. (1966). The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science, 153, 1205–1213. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504. Skinner, B. F. (1984). The evolution of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav- ior, 41, 217–221. Skinner, B. F. (1988a). Reply to Gottlieb. In A. C. Catania & S. Harnard (Eds.), The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skin- ner: Comments and consequences (p. 430). New York: Cambridge University Press. Skinner, B. F. (1988b). What are the scopes and limits of radical behaviorist theory: Reply to Har- nard. In A. C. Catania & S. Harnard (Eds.), The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner: Comments and consequences (pp. 465–473). New York: Cambridge University Press. Skinner, B. F. (1990). Can psychology be a science of mind? American Psychologist, 45, 1206–1210. Slavin, M. O., & Greif, D. (1995). The evolved function of repression and the adaptive design of the human psyche. In H. R. Conte & R. Plutchik (Eds.), Ego defenses: Theory and measurement (pp. 139–175). New York: Wiley. Slavin, M. O., & Kriegman, D. (1992). The adaptive design of the human psyche: Psychoanalysis, evo- lutionary biology and the therapeutic process. New York: Guilford Press. Solomon, A. (1998, January). Anatomy of melan- choly. The New Yorker, pp. 47–61. Staats, A. W. (1963). Complex human behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Staats, A. W. (1983). Psychology’s crisis of disunity: Philosophy and method for a unified science. New York: Praeger. Staats, A. W. (1991). Unified positivism and unifica- tion psychology: Fad or new field? American Psy- chologist, 46, 899–912. Staats, A. W. (1996). Behavior and personality: Psy- chological behaviorism. New York: Springer. Staats, A. W. (1999). Uniting psychology requires new infrastructure, theory, method, and a research agenda. Review of General Psychology, 3, 3–13. Staddon, J. E. R. (2001). Adaptive dynamics: The theoretical analysis of behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Stanovich, K. (2001). How to think straight about psychology (6th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging theory. In J. R. Krebs & T. Clutton-Brock (Eds.), Monographs in behavior and ecology (pp. 354– 375). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). Uni- fied psychology. American Psychologist, 56, 1069–1079. Stringer, C., & McKie, R. (1997). African exodus: The origins of modern humanity. New York: Holt. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193– 210. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illu- sions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27. Thorndike, E. L. (1905). Elements of psychology. New York: Seiler. Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolution- ary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press. Trivers, R. (1971). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and descent of man (pp. 1871–1971). Chicago: Aldine. Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, NJ: Benjamin Cummings. Tryon, W. W. (1993). Neural networks: I. Theoretical unification through connectionism. Clinical Psy- chology Review, 13, 341–352. Wakefield, J. C. (1999). Evolutionary versus proto- type analyses of the concept of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 374–399. Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general systems of affect: Struc- tural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838. Westen, D. (1997). Towards a clinically and empir- ically sound theory of motivation. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 78, 521–548. Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371. Westen, D. (1999). The scientific status of uncon- scious processes: Is Freud really dead? Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 47, 1061–1106. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural se- lection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthe- sis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Wrangham, R. W., & McGrew, W. C. (1994). Chim- panzee cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- sity Press.
  226. REFERENCES Brooks, Lee R., Geoffrey R. Norman, and Scott W. Allen (1991), “Role of Specific Similarity in a Medical Diagnostic Task,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120 (Septem- ber), 278–87. Cesario, Joseph, Heidi Grant, and E. Tory Higgins (2004), “Reg- ulatory Fit and Persuasion: Transfer from ‘Feeling Right,’” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (3), 388– 404. Dhar, Ravi, Stephen M. Nowlis, and Steven J. Sherman (1999), “Comparison Effects on Preference Construction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), 293–306. Dhar, Ravi, and Itamar Simonson (1999), “Making Complementary Choices in Consumption Episodes: Highlighting versus Bal- ancing,” Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (February), 29–44. Etkin, Jordan, and Rebecca K. Ratner (2012), “The Dynamic Im- pact of Variety among Means on Motivation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (April), 1076–92. Fishbach, Ayelet, and Ravi Dhar (2005), “Goals as Excuses or Guides: The Liberating Effect of Perceived Goal Progress on Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (December), 370–77. Fishbach, Ayelet, James Y. Shah, and Arie W. Kruglanski (2004), “Emotional Transfer in Goal Systems,” Journal of Experi- mental Social Psychology, 40 (November), 723–38. Fo¨rster, Jens (2009), “Relations between Perceptual and Concep- tual Scope: How Global versus Local Processing Fits a Focus on Similarity versus Dissimilarity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138 (1), 383–99. Fo¨rster, Jens, E. Tory Higgins, and Lorraine Chen Idson (1998), “Approach and Avoidance Strength during Goal Attainment: Regulatory Focus and the ‘Goal Looms Larger’ Effect,” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (November), 1115–31. Fujita, Kentaro, Marlone D. Henderson, Juliana Eng, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2006), “Spatial Distance and Men- tal Construal of Social Events,” Psychological Science, 17 (April), 278–82. Gollwitzer, Peter M. (1990), “Action Phases and Mindsets,” in The Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of So- cial Behavior, ed. E. Tory Higgins and Richard M. Sorrentino, New York: Guilford, 53–92. Goodman, Joseph K., and Selin A. Malkoc (2012), “Choosing for Here and Now versus There and Later: The Moderating Role of Psychological Distance on Assortment Size Preferences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (December), electronically published March 26. Higgins, E. Tory (2000), “Making a Good Decision: Value from Fit,” American Psychologist, 55 (November), 1217–30. ——— (2005), “Value from Regulatory Fit,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 (August), 209–13. Higgins, E. Tory, Joseph Cesario, Nao Hagiwara, Scott Spiegel, and Thane Pittman (2010), “Increasing or Decreasing Interest in Activities: The Role of Regulatory Fit,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 98 (4), 559–72. Higgins, E. Tory, Lorraine Chen Idson, Antonio L. Frietas, Scott Spiegel, and Daniel C. Molden (2003), “Transfer of Value from Fit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (6), 1140–53. Khan, Uzma, and Ravi Dhar (2006), “Licensing Effect in Con-sumer Choice,” Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (May), 259–66. Kruglanski, Arie W., Antonio Pierro, and Anna Sheveland (2010), “How Many Roads Lead to Rome? Equifinality Set-Size and Commitment to Goals and Means,” European Journal of So- cial Psychology, 40 (April), 1–9. Kruglanski, Arie W., James Y. Shah, Ayelet Fishbach, Ron Fried- man, Woo Young Chun, and David Sleeth-Keppler (2002), “A Theory of Goal Systems,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 34, ed. Mark P. Zanna, San Diego: Academic Press, 331–78. Macrae, C. Neil, and Helen L. Lewis (2002), “Do I Know You? Processing Orientation and Face Recognition,” Psychological Science, 13 (March), 194–96. Merton, Robert K. (1957), Social Theory and Social Structure, Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Mogilner, Cassie, Tamar Rudnick, and Sheena Iyengar (2008), “The Mere Categorization Effect: How the Presence of Cat- egories Increases Consumers’ Perceptions of Assortment Va- riety and Outcome Satisfaction,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 35 (August), 202–15. Schwarz, Norbert, Herbert Bless, Fritz Strack, Gisela Klumpp, Helga Rittenauer-Schatka, and Anette Simons (1991), “Ease of Retrieval as Information: Another Look at the Availability Heuristic,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (August), 195–202. Simonson, Itamar (1990), “The Effect of Purchase Quantity and Timing on Variety-Seeking Behavior,” Journal of Marketing Research, 27 (May), 150–62. Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman (2000), “Temporal Construal and Time-Dependent Changes in Preferences,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 876–89. ——— (2003), “Temporal Construal,” Psychological Review, 110 (July), 403–21.
  227. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer L., and Patti Williams (1998), “Empathy versus Pride: The Influence of Emotional Appeals across Cultures,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (December), 241–61. Andrade, Eduardo B. (2005), “Behavioral Consequences of Affect: Combining Evaluative and Regulatory Mechanisms,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (December), 355–62. Andrade, Eduardo B., and Joel B. Cohen (2007), “On the Con- sumption of Negative Feelings,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 34 (October), 283–300. Ariely, Dan, Anat Bracha, and Stephan Meier (2009), “Doing Good or Doing Well? Image Motivation and Monetary Incentives in Behaving Prosocially,” American Economic Review, 99 (March), 544–55. Beaupre´, Martin G., and Ursula Hess (2005), “Cross-Cultural Emo- tion Recognition among Canadian Ethnic Groups,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36 (May), 355–70. Berger, Jonah, and Chip Heath (2007), “Where Consumers Diverge from Others: Identity-Signaling and Product Domains,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 34 (August), 121–34. Bower, Gordon H. (1981), “Mood and Memory,” American Psy- chologist, 36 (February), 129–48. Brewer, Marilynn B., and Wendi Gardner (1996), “Who Is This ‘We’? Levels of Collective Identity and Self Representations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (July), 83–93. Burke, Peter J., and Jan E. Stets (2009), Identity Theory, New York: Oxford University Press. Bushman, Brad J., Roy F. Baumeister, and Colleen M. Phillips (2001), “Do People Aggress to Improve Their Mood? Ca- tharsis Beliefs, Affect Regulation Opportunity, and Aggres- sive Responding,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 81 (July), 17–32. Cialdini, Robert B., and Douglas T. Kenrick (1976), “Altruism as Hedonism: A Social Development Perspective on the Rela- tionship of Negative Mood State and Helping,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (November), 907–14. Cohen, Joel B., and Eduardo B. Andrade (2004), “Affective Intuition and Task-Contingent Affect Regulation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (September), 358–67. Cooley, Charles Horton (1902), Human Nature and the Social Order, New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Cuddy, Amy J. C., Susan T. Fiske, and Peter Glick (2004), “When Professionals Become Mothers, Warmth Doesn’t Cut the Ice,” Journal of Social Issues, 60 (December), 701–18. Davis, Paul A., Tim Woodman, and Nichola Callow (2010), “Better Out than In: The Influence of Anger Regulation on Physical Performance,” Personality and Individual Differences, 49 (October), 457–60. Erber, Ralph, Daniel M. Wegner, and Nicole Therriault (1996), “On Being Cool and Collected: Mood Regulation in Antic- ipation of Social Interaction,” Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 70 (April), 757–66. Escalas, Jennifer Edson, and James R. Bettman (2005), “Self-Con- strual, Reference Groups, and Brand Meaning,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (December), 378–89. Forehand, Mark R., and Rohit Deshpande´ (2001), “What We See Makes Us Who We Are: Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response,” Journal of Marketing Research, 38 (August), 336–48. Frijda, Nico H. (1986), The Emotions, New York: Cambridge Uni- versity Press. Frijda, Nico H., Peter Kuipers, and Elizabeth ter Schure (1989), “Relations among Emotion, Appraisal, and Emotional Action Readiness,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (August), 212–28. Gao, Leilei, S. Christian Wheeler, and Baba Shiv (2009), “The ‘Shaken Self’: Product Choices as a Means of Restoring Self- View Confidence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (June), 29–38. Greenwald, Anthony G., and Anthony R. Pratkanis (1984), “The Self,” in Handbook of Social Cognition, ed. Robert S. Wyer and Thomas K. Srull, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 129–78. Gross, James J. (1998), “The Emerging Field of Emotion Regu- lation: An Integrative Review,” Review of General Psychol- ogy, 2 (September), 271–99. Gross, James J., and Robert W. Levenson (1993), “Emotional Sup- pression: Physiology, Self-Report, and Expressive Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64 (June), 970–86. Grunberg, Neil E., and Richard O. Straub (1992), “The Role of Gender and Taste Class in the Effects of Stress on Eating,” Health Psychology, 11 (March), 97–100. Haidt, Jonathan, Paul Rozin, Clark McCauley, and Sumio Imada (1997), “Body, Psyche and Culture: The Relationship between Disgust and Morality,” Psychology and Developing Societies, 9 (March), 107–31. Higgins, E. Tory (1987), “Self-Discrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect,” Psychological Review, 94 (July), 319–40. Hochschild, Arlie Russell (1983), The Managed Heart: Commer- cialization of Human Feeling, Los Angeles: University of California Press. Horberg, E. J., Christopher Oveis, Dacher Keltner, and Adam B. Cohen (2009), “Disgust and the Moralization of Purity,” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (December), 963–76. Hsiao, Evana T., and Robert E. Thayer (1998), “Exercising for Mood Regulation: The Importance of Experience,” Person- ality and Individual Differences, 24 (June), 829–36. Inbar, Y., D. Pizarro, and P. Bloom (2009), “Conservatives AreMore Easily Disgusted Than Liberals,” Cognition and Emo- tion, 23 (May), 714–25. Izard, Carroll E. (1977), Human Emotions, New York: Plenum. Kleine, Susan S., Robert E. Kleine III, and Jerome B. Kernan (1993), “Mundane Consumption and the Self: A Social Iden- tity Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2 (June), 209–35. Markus, Hazel, and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), “Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation,” Psychological Review, 98 (April), 224–53. Markus, Hazel, and Elissa Wurf (1987), “The Dynamic Self-Con- cept: A Social Psychological Perspective,” Annual Review of Psychology, 38 (January), 299–337. Matsumoto, David (1993), “Ethnic Differences in Affect Intensity, Emotion Judgments, Display Rule Attitudes, and Self-Re- ported Emotional Expression in an American Sample,” Mo- tivation and Emotion, 17 (June), 107–23. Mercurio, Kathryn R., and Mark R. Forehand (2011), “An Inter- pretive Frame Model of Identity-Dependent Learning: The Moderating Role of Content-State Association,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (October), 555–57. Oyserman, Daphna (2009), “Identity-Based Motivation and Con- sumer Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19 (July), 276–79. Reed, Americus, II (2004), “Activating the Self-Importance of Consumer Selves: Exploring Identity Salience Effects on Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (September), 286–95. Reed, Americus, II, Mark R. Forehand, Stefano Puntoni, and Luke Warlop (2012), “Identity-Based Consumer Behavior,” Inter- national Journal of Research in Marketing, forthcoming. Rozin, Paul, Laura Lowery, Sumio Imada, and Jonathan Haidt (1999), “The CAD Triad Hypothesis: A Mapping between Three Moral Emotions (Contempt, Anger, Disgust) and Three Moral Codes (Community, Autonomy, Divinity),” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (April), 574–86. Scott, Brent A., and Christopher M. Barnes (2011), “A Multilevel Field Investigation of Emotional Labor, Affect, Work With- drawal, and Gender,” Academy of Management Journal, 54 (February), 116–36. Simpson, Patricia A., and Linka K. Stroh (2004), “Gender Dif- ferences: Emotional Expression and Feelings of Personal In- authenticity,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 89 (August), 715–21. Small, Deborah, and Jennifer S. Lerner (2008), “Emotional Policy: Personal Sadness and Anger Shape Policy Judgment,” Polit- ical Psychology, 29 (April), 149–68. Small, Deborah A., and Nicole M. Verrochi (2009), “The Face of Need: Facial Emotion Expression on Charity Advertise- ments,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (December), 777–87. Smith, Craig A., and Phoebe C. Ellsworth (1985), “Patterns of Cognitive Appraisal in Emotion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 (April), 813–38. Smith-Lovin, Lynn (1990), “Emotion as the Confirmation and Dis- confirmation of Identity: An Affect Control Model,” in Re- search Agendas in the Sociology of Emotions, ed. Theodore D. Kemper, Albany: State University of New York Press, 238–70. Stets, Jan E., and Peter J. Burke (2005), “New Directions in Identity Control Theory,” in Advances in Group Processes: Social Identification in Groups, Vol. 22, ed. Shane R. Thye and Edward J. Lawler, Bingley, UK: Emerald House, 43–64. Stryker, Sheldon (2004), “Integrating Emotion into Identity The- ory,” inAdvances in Group Processes: Theory and Research on Human Emotions, Vol. 21, ed. J. H. Turner, Bingley, UK: Emerald House, 1–23. Swann, William B. (1983), “Self-Verification: Bringing Social Re- ality into Harmony with the Self,” in Social Psycological Perspectives on the Self, Vol. 2, ed. J. Suis and A. G. Green- wald, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 33–66. Tajfel, Henri (1978), “Social Categorization, Social Identity and Social Comparison,” in Differentiation between Social Groups, ed. Henri Tajfel, New York: Academic Press, 61–76. Tamir, Maya (2009), “What Do People Want to Feel and Why? Pleasure and Utility in Emotion Regulation,” Current Direc- tions in Psychological Science, 18 (April), 101–5. Tamir, Maya, Christopher Mitchell, and James J. Gross (2008), “Hedonic and Instrumental Motives in Anger Regulation,” Psychological Science, 19 (April), 324–28. Tice, Dianne M., Ellen Bratslavsky, and Roy F. Baumeister (2001), “Emotional Distress Regulation Takes Precedence over Im- pulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do It!” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (January), 53–67. Tsai, Jeanne L., Brian Knutson, and Helene H. Fung (2006), “Cul- tural Variation in Affect Valuation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (February), 288–307. Wheatley, Thalia, and Jonathan Haidt (2005), “Hypnotic Disgust Makes Moral Judgments More Severe,” Psychological Sci- ence, 16 (October), 780–84. White, Katherine, and Darren W. Dahl (2007), “Are All Out- Groups Created Equal? Consumer Identity and Dissociative Influence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (December), 525–36. Williams, Patti, and Aimee Drolet (2005), “Age-Related Differ- ences in Responses to Emotional Advertisements,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (December), 343–54.
  228. REFERENCES Aiken, Leona S., and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Boroditsky, Lera (2000), “Metaphoric Structuring: Understanding Time through Spatial Metaphors,” Cognition, 75 (April), 1– 28. ——— (2001), “Does Language Shape Thought? Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time,” Cognitive Psy- chology, 43 (August), 1–22. ——— (2011), “How Languages Construct Time,” in Space, Time and Number in the Brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought, ed. Stanislas Dehaene and Elizabeth Brannon, London: Elsevier, 333–41. Boroditsky, Lera, Orly Fuhrman, and Kelly McCormick (2011), “Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think about Time Dif- ferently?” Cognition, 118 (January), 123–29. Campbell, Margaret C., and Ronald C. Goodstein (2001), “The Moderating Effect of Perceived Risk on Consumers’ Evalu- ations of Product Incongruity: Preference for the Norm,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 28 (December), 439–49. Chandon, Pierre, J. Wesley Hutchinson, Eric T. Bradlow, and Scott Young (2007), “Measuring the Value of Point-of-Purchase Marketing with Commercial Eye-Tracking-Data,” in Visual Marketing: From Attention to Action, ed. Michel Wedel and Rik Pieters, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chatterjee, Anjan (2001), “Language and Space: Some Interac- tions,” Trends in Cognitive Science, 5 (February), 55–61. Chatterjee, Anjan, M. Helen Southwood, and David Basilico (1999), “Verbs, Events and Spatial Representations,” Neu- ropsychologia, 37 (April), 395–402. Christenfeld, Nicholas (1995), “Choices from Identical Options,” Psychological Science, 6 (January), 50–55. Christman, Stephen, and Katherine Pinger (1997), “Lateral Biases in Aesthetic Preferences: Pictorial Dimensions and Neural Mechanisms,” Laterality, 2 (2), 155–75. Dehaene, Stanislas, Serge Bossini, and Pascal Giraux (1993), “The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122 (Septem- ber), 371–96. Deng, Xiaoyan, and Barbara E. Kahn (2009), “Is Your Product on the Right Side? The ‘Location Effect’ on Perceived Product Heaviness and Package Evaluation,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (December), 725–38. Dobel, Christian, Gil Diesendruck, and Jens Bo¨lte (2007), “How Writing System and Age Influence Spatial Representations of Actions,” Psychological Science, 18 (June), 487–91. Drake, Roger A. (1987), “Effects of Gaze Manipulation on Aes- thetic Judgments: Hemisphere Priming of Affect,” Acta Psy- chologica, 65 (July), 91–99. Fuhrman, Orly, and Lera Boroditsky (2010), “Cross-Cultural Dif- ferences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence from an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task,” Cognitive Science, 34 (No- vember), 1430–51. Gevers, Wim, Bert Reynvoet, and Wim Fias (2004), “The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences Is Spatially Organised: Evidence from Days of the Week,” Cortex, 40 (February), 171–72. Janiszewski, Chris (1988), “Preconscious Processing Effects: The Independence of Attitude Formation and Conscious Thought,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 199–209. ——— (1990), “The Influence of Print Advertisement Organi- zation on Affect toward a Brand Name,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (June), 53–65. Kim, Hakkyun, Akshay R. Rao, and Angela Y. Lee (2009), “It’s Time to Vote: The Effect of Matching Message Orientation and Temporal Frame on Political Persuasion,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 35 (April), 877–89. Labroo, Aparna A., and Angela Y. Lee (2006), “Between Two Brands: A Goal Fluency Account of Brand Evaluation,” Jour- nal of Marketing Research, 43 (August), 374–85. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson (1980), Metaphors We Live By, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lee, Angela Y., and Jennifer L. Aaker (2004), “Bringing the Frame into Focus: The Influence of Regulatory Fit on Processing Fluency and Persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (February), 205–18. Lee, Angela Y., Punam Anand Keller, and Brian Sternthal (2010), “Value from Regulatory Construal Fit: The Persuasive Impact of Fit between Consumer Goals and Message Concreteness,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (February), 735–47. Lee, Angela Y., and Aparna A. Labroo (2004), “The Effect of Conceptual and Perceptual Fluency on Brand Evaluation,” Journal of Marketing Research, 41 (May), 151–65. Maass, Anne, and Aurore Russo (2003), “Directional Bias in the Mental Representation of Spatial Events: Nature or Culture?” Psychological Science, 14 (July), 296–301. Meier, Brian P., and Michael D. Robinson (2006), “Does ‘Feeling Down’ Mean Seeing Down? Depressive Symptoms and Ver- tical Selective Attention,” Journal of Research in Personality, 40 (August), 451–61. Meier, Brian P., Michael D. Robinson, and Gerald L. Clore (2004), “Why Good Guys Wear White: Automatic Inferences aboutStimulus Valence Based on Brightness,” Psychological Sci- ence, 15 (February), 82–87. Meier, Brian P., Martin Sellbom, and Dustin B. Wygant (2007), “Failing to Take the Moral High Ground: Psychopathy and the Vertical Representation of Morality,” Personality and In- dividual Differences, 43 (September), 757–67. Meyers-Levy, Joan, Therese A. Louie, and Mary T. Curren (1994), “How Does the Congruity of Brand Names Affect Evaluations of Brand Name Extensions?,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 79 (February), 56–53. Meyers-Levy, Joan, and Alice M. Tybout (1989), “Schema Con- gruity as a Basis for Product Evaluation,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 16 (June), 39–54. Moskowitz, Gordon B. (1993), “Individual Differences in Social Categorization: The Influence of Personal Need for Structure on Spontaneous Trait Inferences,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (July), 132–42. Muller, Dominique, Charles Judd, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2005), “When Moderation Is Mediated and Mediation Is Moderated,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (Decem- ber), 852–63. Nelson, Leif D., and Joseph P. Simmons (2009), “On Southbound Ease and Northbound Fees: Literal Consequences of the Met- aphoric Link between Vertical Position and Cardinal Direc- tion,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (December), 715–24. Neuberg, Steven L., and Jason T. Newsom (1993), “Personal Need for Structure: Individual Differences in the Desire for Simpler Structure,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (July), 113–31. Nisbett, Richard E., and Timothy D. Wilson (1977), “Telling More Than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes,” Psychological Review, 84 (March), 231–59. Novemsky, Nathan, Ravi Dhar, Norbert Schwarz, and Itamar Si- monson (2007), “Preference Fluency in Choice,” Journal of Marketing Research, 44 (August), 347–56. Ouellet, Marc, Julio Santiago, Maria Jesus Funes, and Juan Lu- pianez (2010), “Thinking about the Future Moves Attention to the Right,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36 (February), 17–24. Peracchio, Laura A., and Joan Meyers-Levy (1997), “Evaluating Persuasion-Enhancing Techniques from a Resource-Matching Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (September), 178–91. Reber, Rolf, and Norbert Schwarz (1999), “Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Judgments of Truth,” Consciousness and Cog- nition, 8 (September), 338–42. Rietzschel, Eric F., Carsten K. W. De Dreu, and Bernard A. Nijstad (2007), “Personal Need for Structure and Creative Perfor- mance: The Moderating Influence of Fear of Invalidity,” Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33 (June), 855–66. Santiago, Julio, Juan Lupia´n˜ez, Elvira Pe´rez, and Marı´a Jesu´s Funes (2007), “Time (Also) Flies from Left to Right,” Psy- chonomic Bulletin and Review, 14 (June), 512–16. Santiago, Julio, Antonio Roman, Marc Ouellet, Nieves Rodriguez, and Pilar Perez-Azor (2008), “In Hindsight, Life Flows from Left to Right,” Psychological Research, 74 (January), 59–70. Schaller, Mark, Carrie Boyd, Jonathan Yohannes, and Meredith O’Brien (1995), “The Prejudiced Personality Revisited: Per- sonal Need for Structure and Formation of Erroneous Group Stereotypes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 (March), 544–55. Schwarz, Norbert (2004), “Meta-Cognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (September), 332–48. Shaki, Samuel, and Martin H. Fischer (2008), “Reading Space into Numbers: A Cross-Linguistic Comparison of the SNARC Ef- fect,” Cognition, 108 (August), 590–99. Shen, Hao, Yuwei Jiang, and Rashmi Adaval (2010), “Contrast and Assimilation Effects of Processing Fluency,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (February), 876–89. Shubert, Thomas W. (2005), “Your Highness: Vertical Position as Perceptual Symbols of Power,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (July), 1–21. Spalek, Thomas M., and Sherief Hammad (2005), “The Left-to- Right Bias in Inhibition of Return Is Due to the Direction of Reading,” Psychological Science, 16 (January), 15–18. Tversky, Barbara, Sol Kugelmass, and Atalia Winter (1991), “Cross-Cultural and Developmental Trends in Graphic Pro- ductions,” Cognitive Psychology, 23 (October), 515–57. Valenzuela, Ana, and Priya Raghubir (2009), “Position Based Schemas: The Center-Stage Effect,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9 (April), 185–96. ——— (2010), “Are Consumers Aware of Top-Bottom but Not of Left-Right Inferences?” Unpublished manuscript, Baruch College, CUNY. Valenzuela, Ana, Priya Raghubir, and Chrissy Mitakakis (2012), “Shelf Space Schemas: Myth or Reality?” Journal of Business Research, doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.12.006. Vallesi, Antonino, Malcolm A. Binns, and Tim Shallice (2008), “An Effect of Spatial-Temporal Association of Response Codes: Understanding the Cognitive Representations of Time,” Cognition, 107 (May), 501–27. Zebian, Samar (2005), “Linkages between Number Concepts, Spa- tial Thinking, and Directionality of Writing: The SNARC Ef- fect and the REVERSE SNARC Effect in English and Arabic Monoliterates, Biliterates, and Illiterate Arabic Speakers,” Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5 (1–2), 165–90.
  229. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer L., and Patti Williams (1998), “Empathy versus Pride: The Influence of Emotional Appeals across Cultures,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (3), 241–61. Ahluwalia, Rohini (2008), “How Far Can a Brand Stretch? Un- derstanding the Role of Self-Construal,” Journal of Marketing Research, 45 (June), 337–50. Anderson, Cameron, and Adam D. Galinsky (2006), “Power, Op- timism, and Risk-Taking,” European Journal of Social Psy- chology, 36 (4), 511–36. Bargh, John A., Ronald N. Bond, Wendy J. Lombardi, and Mary E. Tota (1986), “The Additive Nature of Chronic and Tem- porary Sources of Construct Accessibility,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 50 (5), 869–78. Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator- Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Re- search: Conceptual, Strategic and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173–82. Choi, Incheol, Reeshad Dalal, Chu Kim-Prieto, and Hyekyung Park (2003), “Culture and Judgment of Causal Relevance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (1), 46–59. Choi, Incheol, Minkyung Koo, and Jongan Choi (2007), “Individ-ual Differences in Analytic versus Holistic Thinking,” Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33 (5), 691–705. Cronley, Maria L., Steven S. Posavac, Tracy Meyer, Frank R. Kardes, and James J. Kellaris (2005), “A Selective Hypothesis Testing Perspective on Price-Quality Inference and Inference- Based Choice,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15 (2), 159–69. Dawar, Niraj, and Philip Parker (1994), “Marketing Universals: Consumers Use of Brand-Name, Price, Physical Appearance and Retailer Reputation as Signals of Product Quality,” Jour- nal of Marketing, 58 (2), 81–95. Escalas, Jennifer Edson, and James R. Bettman (2005), “Self-Con- strual, Reference Groups and Brand Meaning,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 378–89. Faulds, David J., and Subhash C. Lonial (2001), “Price-Quality Relationships of Nondurable Consumer Products: A European and United States Perspective,” Journal of Economic and So- cial Research, 3 (1), 59–77. Hamilton, Rebecca W., and Gabriel J. Biehal (2005), “Achieving Your Goals or Protecting Their Future? The Effects of Self- View on Goals and Choices,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (2), 277–83. Hamilton, Ryan, and Alexander Chernev (2010), “The Impact of Product Line Extensions and Consumer Goals on the For- mation of Price Image,” Journal of Marketing Research, 47 (1), 51–62. Huddleston, Patricia, and Linda K. Good (1998), “The Price-Qual- ity Relationship: Does It Hold True for Russian and Polish Consumers?” International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, 8 (1), 33–51. Jenkins, Margaret, and Richard Griffith (2004), “Using Personality Constructs to Predict Performance: Narrow or Broad Band- width,” Journal of Business and Psychology, 19 (2), 255–69. Ji, Li-Jun, Kaiping Peng, and Richard E. Nisbett (2000), “Culture, Control and Perception of Relationships in the Environment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (5), 943– 55. Jo, Myung-Soo, and Emine Sarigollu (2007), “Cross-Cultural Dif- ferences of Price-Perceived Quality Relationships,” Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 19 (4), 59–74. Johnson, Timothy, Sharon Shavitt, and Allyson Holbrook (2011), “Culture and Response Styles in Survey Research,” in Cross- Cultural Research Methods in Psychology, ed. David Mat- sumoto and Fons J. R. van de Vijver, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 130–78. Kardes, Frank R., Maria L. Cronley, James J. Kellaris, and Steven S. Posavac (2004), “The Role of Selective Information Pro- cessing in Price-Quality Inference,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 31 (2), 368–74. Kardes, Frank R., Steven S. Posavac, and Maria L. Cronley (2004), “Consumer Inference: A Review of Processes, Bases, and Judgment Contexts,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (3), 230–56. Keller, Kevin Lane (1993), “Conceptualizing, Measuring, Man- aging Customer-Based Brand Equity,” Journal of Marketing, 57 (1), 1–22. Lalwani, Ashok K. (2009), “The Distinct Influence of Cognitive Busyness and Need for Closure on Cultural Differences in Socially Desirable Responding,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 36 (2), 305–16. Lalwani, Ashok K., and Kent B. Monroe (2005), “A Re-Exami- nation of Frequency-Depth Effects in Consumer Price Judg- ments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 480–85. Lalwani, Ashok K., and Sharon Shavitt (2009), “The ‘Me’ I Claim to Be: Cultural Self-Construal Elicits Self-Presentational Goal Pursuit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (1), 88–102. Lalwani, Ashok K., Sharon Shavitt, and Timothy Johnson (2006), “What Is the Relation between Cultural Orientation and So- cially Desirable Responding?” Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 90 (1), 165–78. Lalwani, Ashok K., L. J. Shrum, and Chi-Yue Chiu (2009), “Mo- tivated Response Styles: The Role of Cultural Values, Reg- ulatory Focus and Self-Consciousness in Socially Desirable Responding,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96 (4), 870–82. Lichtenstein, Donald R., Nancy M. Ridgway, and Richard G. Ne- temeyer (1993), “Price Perceptions and Consumer Shopping Behavior: A Field Study,” Journal of Marketing Research, 30 (2), 234–45. Mandel, Naomi (2003), “Shifting Selves and Decision Making: The Effects of Self-Construal Priming on Consumer Risk- Taking,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (1), 30–40. Masuda, Takahiko, and Richard E. Nisbett (2001), “Attending Ho- listically versus Analytically: Comparing the Context Sensi- tivity of Japanese and Americans,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (5), 922–34. Monga, Alokparna (Sonia) Basu (2004), “Cultural Differences in Brand Extension Evaluation: The Role of Holistic versus An- alytic Thinking,” unpublished dissertation, University of Min- nesota. Monga, Alokparna (Sonia) Basu, and Deborah Roedder John (2007), “Cultural Differences in Brand Extension Evaluation: The Influence of Analytic versus Holistic Thinking,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (March), 529–36. ——— (2008), “When Does Negative Brand Publicity Hurt? The Moderating Influence of Analytic versus Holistic Thinking,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18 (4), 320–32. ——— (2010), “What Makes Brands Elastic? The Influence of Brand Concept and Styles of Thinking on Brand Extension Evaluation,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (May), 80–92. Monroe, Kent B. (2003), Pricing: Making Profitable Decisions, New York: McGraw Hill. Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Noren- zayan (2001), “Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic ver- sus Analytic Cognition,” Psychological Review, 108 (2), 291–310. Ones, Deniz S., and Chockalingam Viswesvaran (1996), “Band- width-Fidelity Dilemma in Personality Measurement for Per- sonnel Selection,” Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17 (6), 609–26. Oyserman, Daphna (1993), “The Lens of Personhood: Viewing the Self and Others in a Multicultural Society,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 65 (5), 993–1009. Rao, Akshay R., and Kent B. Monroe (1989), “The Effect of Price, Brand Name, and Store Name on Buyers’ Perceptions of Prod- uct Quality: An Integrative Review,” Journal of Marketing Research, 26 (3), 351–57. Reddy, Srinivas K., Susan L. Holak, and Subodh Bhat (1994), “To Extend or Not to Extend: Success Determinants of Line Ex- tensions,” Journal of Marketing Research, 31 (May), 243– 62. Schlosser, Ann E., and Sharon Shavitt (2002), “Anticipating Dis- cussion about a Product: Rehearsing What to Say Can Affect Your Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (1), 101–15. Shavitt, Sharon (1990), “The Role of Attitude Objects in Attitude Functions,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26 (2), 124–48. ——— (1992), “Evidence for Predicting the Effectiveness of Value-Expressive versus Utilitarian Appeals: A Reply to Johar and Sirgy,” Journal of Advertising, 21 (2), 47–51. Shavitt, Sharon, Tina M. Lowrey, and Sang-pil Han (1992), “At- titude Functions in Advertising: The Interactive Role of Prod- ucts and Self-Monitoring,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1 (4), 337–64. Trafimow, David, Harry C. Triandis, and Sharon G. Goto (1991), “Some Tests of the Distinction between the Private Self and the Collective Self,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 60 (5), 649–55. Triandis, Harry C. (1995), Individualism and Collectivism, Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Triandis, Harry C., and Michele J. Gelfand (1998), “Converging Measurement of Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (1), 118–28. Troutman, C. Michael, and James Shanteau (1976), “Do Consum- ers Evaluate Products by Adding or Averaging Attribute In- formation?” Journal of Consumer Research, 3 (2), 101–6. White, Katherine, and Darren W. Dahl (2007), “Are All Out- Groups Created Equal? Consumer Identity and Dissociative Influence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (4), 525–36. Zhou, Kevin Z., Centing Su, and Yeqing Bao (2002), “A Paradox of Price-Quality and Market Efficiency: A Comparative Study of the U.S. and China Markets,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19 (4), 349–65. Zhu, Rui, and Joan Meyers-Levy (2009), “The Influence of Self- View on Context Effects: How Display Fixtures Can Affect Product Evaluations,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (1), 37–45.
  230. REFERENCES Ackerman, Joshua M., Christopher C. Nocera, and John A. Bargh (2010), “Incidental Haptic Sensations Influence Social Judg- ments and Decisions,” Science, 328 (5986), 1712–15. Bargh, John A. (2006), “What Have We Been Priming All These Years? On the Development, Mechanisms, and Ecology of Nonconscious Social Behavior,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 36 (2), 147–68. Barsalou, Lawrence W. (2008), “Grounded Cognition,” Annual Re- view of Psychology, 59 (1), 617–45. Barsalou, Lawrence W., Kyle W. Simmons, Aron K. Barbey, and Christine D. Wilson (2003), “Grounding Conceptual Knowl- edge in Modality-Specific Systems,” Trends in Cognitive Sci- ences, 7 (2), 84–91. Beike, Denise R., Laura P. Adams, and Erin T. Wirth-Beaumont (2007), “Incomplete Inhibition of Emotion in Specific Au- tobiographical Memories,” Memory, 15 (4), 375–89. Beike, Denise R., Keith D. Markman, and Figen Karadogan (2009), “What I Regret Most Are Lost Opportunities: A Theory of Regret Intensity,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35 (3), 385–97. Beike, Denise R., and Erin Wirth-Beaumont (2005), “Psycholog- ical Closure as a Memory Phenomenon,” Memory, 13 (6), 574–93. Bell, David E. (1982), “Regret in Decision Making under Uncer- tainty,” Operations Research, 30 (5), 961–81. Bettman, James R., Mary Frances Luce, and John W. Payne (1998), “Constructive Consumer Choice Processes,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 25 (3), 187–217. Botti, Simona, and Christopher K. Hsee (2010), “Dazed and Con- fused by Choice: How the Temporal Costs of Choosing Lead to Undesirable Outcomes,” Organizational Behavior and Hu- man Decision Processes, 112 (2), 161–71. Botti, Simona, and Ann L. McGill (2006), “When Choosing Is Not Deciding: The Effect of Perceived Responsibility on Sat- isfaction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (2), 211–19. Brenner, Lyle, Yuval S. Rottenstreich, and Sanjay Sood (1999), “Comparison, Grouping, and Preference,” Psychological Sci- ence, 10 (3), 225–29. Broniarczyk, Susan M. (2008), “Product Assortment,” in Hand- book of Consumer Psychology, ed. Curtis P. Haugtvedt, Paul M. Herr, and Frank R. Kardes, New York: Erlbaum, 755–79. Cacioppo, John T., Joseph R. Priester, and Gary G. Berntson (1993), “Rudimentary Determinants of Attitude. II: Arm Flex- ion and Extension Have Differential Effect on Attitudes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (1), 5–17. Carmon, Ziv, Klaus Wertenbroch, and Marcel Zeelenberg (2003), “Option Attachment: When Deliberating Makes Choosing Feel Like Losing,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (1), 15–29. Chernev, Alexander (2003), “When More Is Less and Less Is More: The Role of Ideal Point Availability and Assortment in Con- sumer Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (2), 170–83. Frey, Dieter (1981), “Reversible and Irreversible Decisions: Pref- erence for Consonant Information as a Function of Attrac- tiveness of Decision Alternatives,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7 (4), 621–26. Gilbert, Daniel T., and Jane E. J. Ebert (2002), “Decisions and Revisions: The Affective Forecasting of Changeable Out- comes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82 (4), 503–14. Hafner, Rebecca J., Matthew P. White, and Simon J. Handley (2012), “Spoilt of Choice: The Role of Counterfactual Think- ing in the Excess Choice and Reversibility Paradoxes,” Jour- nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 (1), 28–36. Hayes, Andrew F. (2013), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Ap- proach, New York: Guilford. Hsee, Christopher K., and France Leclerc (1998), “Will Products Look More Attractive When Presented Separately or To- gether?” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (2), 175–86. Hsee, Christopher K., George F. Loewenstein, Sally Blount, and Max H. Bazerman (1999), “Preference Reversals between Joint and Separate Evaluation of Options: A Review and The- oretical Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin, 125 (5), 576–90. Hung, Iris W., and Aparna A. Labroo (2011), “From Firm Muscles to Firm Willpower: Understanding the Role of Embodied Cognition in Self-Regulation,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 37 (6), 1046–64. Inbar, Yoel, Simona Botti, and Karlene Hanko (2011), “Decision Speed and Choice Regret: When Haste Feels Like Waste,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47 (3), 533–40. Iyengar, Sheena S., and Mark R. Lepper (2000), “When Choice Is Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 995– 1006. Iyengar, Sheena S., Rachael E. Wells, and Barry Schwartz (2006), “Doing Better but Feeling Worse,” Psychological Science, 17 (2), 143–50. Johnson, Mark (2007), The Meaning of the Body, Chicago: Uni- versity of Chicago Press. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky (1979), “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk,” Econometrica 47 (2), 263–91. Kruglanski, Arie W. (1989), Lay Epistemics and Human Knowl- edge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases, New York: Plenum. ——— (1990), “Lay Epistemic Theory in Social Cognitive Psy- chology,” Psychological Inquiry, 1 (3), 181–97. Lakoff, George (1993), “The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor,” in Metaphor and Thought, ed. Andrew Ortony, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 203–51. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson (1980), Metaphors We Live By, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Landau, Mark J., Brian P. Meier, and Lucas A. Keefer (2010) “A Metaphor-Enriched Social Cognition,” Psychological Bulle- tin, 136 (6), 1045–67. Li, Xiuping, Liyuan Wei, and Dilip Soman (2010), “Sealing the Emotions Genie: The Effects of Physical Enclosure on Psy- chological Closure,” Psychological Science, 21 (8), 1047–50. Litt, Ab, and Zakary L. Tormala (2010), “Fragile Enhancement of Attitudes and Intentions Following Difficult Decisions,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 37 (4), 584–98. Luce, Mary Frances (1998), “Choosing to Avoid: Coping with Negatively Emotion-Laden Consumer Decisions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (4), 409–33. Malhotra, Naresh K. (1982), “Information Load and Consumer Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (4), 419–30. Mayseless, Ofra, and Arie W. Kruglanski (1987), “What Makes You So Sure? Effects of Epistemic Motivations on Judgmental Confidence,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39 (2), 162–83. Mogilner, Cassie, Baba Shiv, and Sheena S. Iyengar (2013), “Eter- nal Quest for the Best: Sequential (vs. Simultaneous) Option Presentation Undermines Choice Commitment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (6), 1300–1312. Muller, Dominique, Charles M. Judd, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2005), “When Moderation Is Mediated and Mediation Is Moderated,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 852–63. Pham, Michel Tuan (1998), “Representativeness, Relevance, and the Use of Feelings in Decision Making,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 25 (2), 144–59. Preacher, Kristopher J., Derek D. Rucker, and Andrew F. Hayes (2007), “Addressing Moderated Mediation Hypotheses: Theory, Methods, and Prescriptions,” Multivariate Behavioral Re- search, 42 (1), 185–227. Shafir, Eldar, Itamar Simonson, and Amos Tversky (1993), “Rea- son-Based Choice,” Cognition, 49 (1–2), 11–36. Taylor, Cheryl, Charles G. Lord, and Charles F. Bond Jr. (2009), “Embodiment, Agency, and Attitude Change,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 97 (6), 946–62. Tversky, Amos, and Eldar Shafir (1992), “Choice under Conflict: The Dynamics of Deferred Decision,” Psychological Science, 3 (6), 358–61. Zeelenberg, Marcel (1999), “Anticipated Regret, Expected Feed- back, and Behavioral Decision Making,” Journal of Behav- ioral Decision Making, 12 (2), 93–106. Zhang, Meng, and Xiuping Li (2012), “From Physical Weight to Psychological Significance: The Contribution of Semantic Ac- tivations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (6), 1063–75.
  231. REFERENCES Attneave, F. & Olson, R.K. (1971). Pitch as a medium: A new approach to psychophysical scaling. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 147-166. Bachem, A. (1937). Various types of absolute pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 9, 146-151. Bernstein, L. (1976). The Unanswered Question. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Besson, M. (1997). Electrophysiological studies of music processing. In I. Deliège & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Perception and Cognition of Music (pp. 217-250). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Besson, M. & Faïta, F. (1995). An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1278-1296. Besson, M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., & Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the brain: Independence of lyrics and tunes. Psychological Science, 9, 494-498. Besson, M. & Macar, F. (1987). An event-related potential analysis of incongruity in music and other non-linguistic contexts. Psychophysiology, 24, 14-25. Bever, T. & Chiarello, R. (1974). Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians. Science, 185, 537-539. Bharucha, J.J. (1984a). Anchoring effects in music: The resolution of dissonance. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 485- 518. Bharucha, J.J. (1984b). Event hierarchies, tonal hierarchies, and assimilation: A reply to Deutsch and Dowling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 421-425. Bharucha, J.J. (1987). Music cognition and perceptual facilitation: A connectionist framework. Music Perception, 5, 1-30. Bharucha, J.J. (1996). Melodic anchoring. Music Perception, 13, 383-400. Bharucha, J.J. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1983). The representation of harmonic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of context. Cognition, 13, 63-102. Bharucha, J.J. & Mencl, W.E. (1996). Two issues in auditory cognition: Self-organization of octave categories and pitch-invariant pattern recognition. Psychological Science, 7, 142-149. Bharucha, J.J. & Pryor, J.H. (1986). Disrupting the isochrony underlying rhythm: An asymmetry in discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 137-141. Bharucha, J.J. & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Response time and musical expectancy: Priming of Chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 403-410. Bharucha, J.J. & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 519-524. Bigand, E. (1997). Perceiving musical stability: The effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 808-822. Bigand, E., Mandurell, F., Tillmann, B., & Pineau, M. (1999). Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 184- 197. Bigand, E. & Pineau, M. (1996). Context effects on melody recognition: A dynamic interpretation. Current Psychology of Cognition, 15, 121-134. Bigand, E. & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1098-1107. Bigand, E., Tillmann, B., Poulin, B., & D’Adamo, D.A. (under review). The effect of harmonic context on phoneme monitoring in vocal music. Blackmore, S. (2000). The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bolton, T.L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145-238. Boltz, M.G. (1989a). Perceiving the end: Effects of tonal relationships on melodic completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 749-761. Boltz, M.G. (1989b). Rhythm and “good endings”: Effects of temporal structure on tonality judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 9-17. Boltz, M.G. (1989c). Time judgments of musical endings: Effects of expectancies on the “filled interval effect.” Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 409-418. Boltz, M.G. (1991a). Some structural determinants of melody recall. Memory & Cognition, 19, 239-251. Boltz, M.G. (1991b). Time estimation and attentional perspective. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 422-433. Boltz, M.G. (1992). The remembering of auditory event durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 938-956. Boltz, M.G. (1993a). The generation of temporal and melodic expectancies during musical listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 585-600. Boltz, M.G. (1993b). Time estimation and expectancies. Memory & Cognition, 21, 853-863. Boltz, M.G. (1998). The processing of temporal and nontemporal information in the remembering of event durations and musical structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1087-1104. Boltz, M.G. & Jones, M.R. (1986). Does rule recursion make melodies easier to reproduce? If not, what does? Cognitive Psychology, 18, 389-431. Bregman, A.S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Brown, S. (2000). The "musilanguage" model of music evolution. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 271-300). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bundy, R.S., Colombo, J., & Singer, J. (1982). Pitch perception in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 18, 10-14. Burns, E.M. (1999). Intervals, scales, and tuning. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (2nd ed., pp. 215- 264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Burns, E.M. & Ward, W.D. (1978). Categorical perception - phenomenon or epiphenomenon: Evidence form experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 456-468. Castellano, M.A., Bharucha, J.J., & Krumhansl, C.L. (1984). Tonal hierarchies in the music of North India. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 394-412. Clarke, E.F. (1993). Imitating and evaluating real and transformed musical performances. Music Perception, 10, 317-343. Clarke, E.F. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (2nd ed., pp. 473- 500). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Clarke, E.F. & Baker-Short, C. (1987). The imitation of perceived rubato: A preliminary study. Psychology of Music, 15, 58-75. Clarkson, M.G. & Clifton, R.K (1985). Infant pitch perception: Evidence for responding to pitch categories and the missing fundamental. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 1521-1528. Clarkson, M.G. & Clifton, R.K. (1995). Infants’ pitch perception: Inharmonic tonal complexes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 1372-1379. Clarkson, M.G., Martin, R.L., & Miciek, S.G. (1996). Infants’ perception of pitch: Number of harmonics. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 191-197. Clarkson, M.G. & Rogers, E.C. (1995). Infants require low-frequency energy to hear the pitch of the missing fundamental. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 148-154. Cohen, M.A., Grossberg, S., & Wyse, L.L. (1995). A spectral network model of pitch perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 862-879. Cohen, A.J., Thorpe, L.A., & Trehub, S.E. (1987). Infants’ perception of musical relations in short transposed tone sequences. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 33-47. Collier, G.L. & Wright, C.E. (1995). Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 602-627. Cuddy, L.L. (1991). Melodic patterns and tonal structure: Converging evidence. Psychomusicology, 10, 107-126. Cuddy, L.L. & Badertscher, B. (1987). Recovery of the tonal hierarchy: Some comparisons across age and levels of musical experience. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 609-620. Cuddy, L.L. & Cohen, A.J. (1976). Recognition of transposed melodic sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 255-270. Cuddy, L.L., Cohen, A.J., & Mewhort, D.J. (1981). Perception of structures in short melodic sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 7, 869-883. Cuddy, L.L., Cohen, A.J., & Miller, J. (1979). Melody recognition: The experimental application of musical rules. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 148-157. Cuddy, L.L. & Lyons, H.I. (1981). Music pattern recognition: A comparison of listening to and studying tonal structure and tonal ambiguities. Psychomusicology, 1, 15-33. Cuddy, L.L. & Lunney, C.A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Perceptual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 451-462. Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man. London: John Murray. Davidson, J.W., Howe, M.J.A., Moore, D.G., & Sloboda, J.A. (1996). The role of parental influences in the development of musical ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 399-412. Davidson, J.W., Howe, M.J.A., & Sloboda, J.A. (1997). Environmental factors in the development of musical performance skill. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Music (pp. 188-206). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Davidson, J.W., Moore, D.G., Sloboda, J.A., & Howe, M.J.A. (1998). Characteristics of music teachers and the process of young instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 141-161. Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press. de Boer, E. (1956). On the Residue in Hearing. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam. Deliège, I. (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: An approach to Lerdahl & Jackendoff’s grouping preference rules. Music Perception, 4, 325-360. Deliège, I. & Sloboda, J. (Eds.) (1997). Perception and Cognition of Music. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Demany, L. & Armand, F. (1984). The perceptual reality of tone chroma in early infancy. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 57-66. Deutsch, D. (Ed.) (1982). The Psychology of Music (1st ed.) New York: Academic Press. Deutsch, D. (1984). Two issues concerning tonal hierarchies: Comment on Castellano, Bharucha, and Krumhansl. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 413-416. Deutsch, D. (1986). A musical paradox. Music Perception, 3, 275-280. Deutsch, D. (1987). The tritone paradox: Effects of spectral variables. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 563-575. Deutsch, D. (1991). The tritone paradox: An influence of language on music perception. Music Perception, 8, 335- 347. Deutsch, D. (1999a). Grouping mechanisms in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (2nd ed., pp. 299-348). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Deutsch, D. (Ed.) (1999b). The Psychology of Music (2nd ed.) San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Dewar, K.M., Cuddy, L.L, & Mewhort, D.J. (1977). Recognition memory for single tones with and without context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 60-67. DeWitt, L.A. & Crowder, R.G. (1987). Tonal fusion of consonant musical intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 73-84. Dowling, W.J. (1978). Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85, 341-354. Dowling, W.J. (1984). Assimilation and tonal structure: Comment on Castellano, Bharucha, and Krumhansl. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 13, 417-420. Dowling, W.J. & Fujitani, D.S. (1971). Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 524-531. Dowling, W.J. & Harwood, D.L. (1986). Music Cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Drake, C. (1998). Psychological processes involved in the temporal organization of complex auditory sequences: Universal and acquired processes. Music Perception, 16, 11-26. Drake, C. & Palmer, C. (1993). Accent structures in music performance. Music Perception, 10, 343-378. Drake, C. & Palmer, C. (2000). Skill acquisition in music performance: Relations between planning and temporal control. Cognition, 74, 1-32 Drake, C., Dowling, W.J., & Palmer, C. (1991). Accent structures in the reproduction of simple tunes by children and adult pianists. Music Perception, 8, 315-334. Essens, P.J. & Povel, D.-J. (1985). Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 37, 1-7. Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 144-125. Ferland, M.B. & Mendelson, M.J. (1989). Infants’ categorization of melodic contour. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 341-355. Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fodor, J.A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fodor, J.A. (2000). The Mind Doesn’t Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fraisse, P. (1956). Les structures rythmiques. Lovain, Belgium: Editions Universitaires. Fraisse, P. (1966). L’anticipation de stimulus rythmiques. Vitesse d’établissement et précision de la synchronisation. L’Année Psychologique, 66, 15-36. Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (1st ed., pp. 149-180). New York: Academic Press. Fraisse, P., Pichot, P., & Chairouin, G. (1949). Les aptitudes rythmiques. Etude comparée des oligophrènes et des enfants normaux. Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique, 42, 309-330. Friberg, A. & Sundberg, J. (1999). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1469- 1484. Gabrielsson, A. (1999). The performance of music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (2nd ed., pp. 501- 602). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Gardener, H., Silverman, J., Denes, G., Semenza, C, & Rosenstiel, A.K. (1977). Sensitivity to musical denotation and connotation in organic patients. Cortex, 13, 242-256. Garrett, M., Bever, T., Fodor, J. (1966). The active use of grammar in speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 30-32. Goebl, W. (in press). Melody lead in piano performance: Expressive device or artifact? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. Goldstein, J.L. (1973). An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 1496-1516. Gould, S.J. & Lewontin, R.C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 205, 581-598. Gould, S.J. & Vrba, E.S. (1982). Exaptation - a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8, 4-15. Green, D.M. (1976). An Introduction to Hearing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Halpern, A.R. & Zatorre, R.J. (1999). When that tune runs through your head: A PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 1047-3211. Handel, S. (1998). The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1546-1561. Hébert, S., Peretz, I., & Gagnon, L. (1995). Perceiving the tonal ending of tune excerpts: The roles of pre-existing representation and musical expertise. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 193-209. Heffner, H. & Whitfield, I.C. (1976). Perception of the missing fundamental by cats. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59, 915-919. Helmholtz, H. von (1954). On the Sensation of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music. New York: Dover. (Original German work published 1863) Hermann, L. (1912). Neue Versuche zur Frage der Unterbrechungstöne. Pflüger’s Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere, 146, 249-294. Hibi, S. (1983). Rhythm perception in repetitive sound sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan, 4, 83- 95. Houtsma, A.J.M. & Goldstein, J.L. (1972). The central origin of the pitch of complex tones: Evidence from musical interval recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 520-529. Howe, M.J.A., Davidson, J.W., & Sloboda, J.A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399-442. Huron, D. (1991). The avoidance of part-crossing in polyphonic music: Perceptual evidence and musical practice. Music Perception, 9, 93-104. Huron, D. (1999). An instinct for music: Is music an evolutionary adaptation? The 1999 Ernest Bloch Lectures, Department of Music, University of California, Berkeley. Janata, P. (1995). ERP measures assay the degree of expectancy violation of harmonic contexts in music. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 153-164. Janata, P. & Reisberg, D. (1988). Response-time measures as a means of exploring tonal hierarchies. Music Perception, 6, 161-172. Johnsrude, I.S., Penhune, V.B., & Zatorre, R.J. (2000). Functional specificity in the right human auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction. Brain, 23, 155-163. Jones, M.R. (1987). Dynamic pattern structure in music: Recent theory and research. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 621-634. Jones, M.R. & Boltz, M.G. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, 459-491. Jones, M.R., Boltz, M.G., & Klein, J.M. (1993). Expected endings and judged duration. Memory & Cognition, 21, 646-665. Jones, M.R., Boltz, M.G., & Kidd, G. (1982). Controlled attending as a function of melodic and temporal context. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 211-218. Jones, M.R., Kidd, G., & Wetzel, R. (1981). Evidence for rhythmic attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1059-1072. Jones, M.R. & Ralston, J.T. (1991). Some influence of accent structure on melody recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 8-20. Jones, M.R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies: A dynamic interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 89-121. Jusczyk, P.W. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1993). Pitch and rhythmic patterns affecting infants’ sensitivity to musical phrase structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 627-640. Justus, T.C. & Bharucha, J.J. (in press). Modularity in musical processing: The automaticity of harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Justus, T.C. & Hutsler, J.J. (2000). Origins of human musicality: Evolution, mind, and culture. In C. Woods, G. Luck, R. Brochard, F. Seddon, & J.A. Sloboda (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 1151-1160). Keele, UK: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music. Justus, T.C. & Hutsler, J.J. (under review). Fundamental issues in the evolutionary psychology of music: The contributions of culture and recruited cognitive domains. Kemp, A.E. (1997). Individual differences in musical behaviour. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Music (pp. 25-45). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Kessler, E.J., Hansen, C., & Shepard, R.N. (1984). Tonal schemata in the perception of music in Bali and the West. Music Perception, 2, 131-165. Koelsch, S., Gunter, T., Friederici, A., & Schröger. (2000). Brain indices of music processing: “Nonmusicians” are musical. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 520-541. Krumhansl, C.L. (1978). Concerning the applicability of geometric models to similarity data: The interrelationship between similarity and spatial density. Psychological Review, 85, 445-463. Krumhansl, C.L. (1979). The psychological representation of musical pitch in a tonal context. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 346-374. Krumhansl, C.L. (1990). Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Krumhansl, C.L. (1991). Music psychology: Tonal structures in perception and memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 277-303. Krumhansl, C.L. (1995). Music psychology and music theory: Problems and prospects. Music Theory Spectrum, 17, 53-80. Krumhansl, C.L. (1996). A perceptual analysis of Mozart’s piano sonata K. 282: Segmentation, tension, and musical ideas. Music Perception, 13, 401-432. Krumhansl, C.L. (2000a). Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 159-179. Krumhansl, C.L. (2000b). Tonality induction: A statistical approach applied cross-culturally. Music Perception, 17, 461-479. Krumhansl, C.L., Bharucha, J.J., & Castellano, M.A. (1982). Key distance effects on perceived harmonic structure in music. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 96-108. Krumhansl, C.L., Bharucha, J.J., & Kessler, E.J. (1982). Perceived harmonic structure of chords in three related musical keys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 24-36. Krumhansl, C.L. & Castellano, M.A. (1983). Dynamic processes in music perception. Memory & Cognition, 11, 325-334. Krumhansl, C.L. & Jusczyk, P.W. (1990). Infant’s perception of phrase structure in music. Psychological Science, 1, 1-4. Krumhansl, C.L. & Keil, F.C. (1982). Acquisition of the hierarchy of tonal functions in music. Memory & Cognition, 10, 243-251. Krumhansl, C.L. & Kessler, E.J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89, 334-368. Krumhansl, C.L., Louhivuori, J., Toiviainen, P., Järvinen, T., & Eerola, T. (1999). Melodic expectation in Finnish spiritual folk hymns: Convergence of statistical, behavioral, and computational approaches. Music Perception, 17, 151-195. Krumhansl, C.L. & Shepard, R.N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 579-594. Krumhansl, C.L., Toivanen, P., Eerola, T., Toiviainen, P., Järvinen, T., & Louhivuori, J. (2000). Cross-cultural music cognition: Cognitive methodology applied to North Sami yoiks. Cognition, 76, 13-58. Lehmann, A.C. & Ericsson, K.A. (1993). Sight-reading ability of expert pianists in the context of piano accompanying. Psychomusicology, 12, 182-195. Lerdahl, F. & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Levitin, D.J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 414-423. Levitin, D.J. (1996). Do absolute pitch possessors have categorical perception? International Journal of Psychology, 31, 10. Levitin, D.J. (1999). Absolute pitch: Self-reference and human memory. International Journal of Computing Anticipatory Systems, 4, 255-266. Lewontin, R.C. (1990). The evolution of cognition. In D.N. Osherson and E.E. Smith (Eds.), Thinking: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, Volume 3 (pp. 229-246). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Liberman, M. & Prince, A. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 249-336. Licklider, J.C.R. (1954). Periodicity and place pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 945. Liégeois-Chauvel, C., Peretz, I., Babaï, M., Laguitton, V., & Chauvel, P. (1998). Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music processing. Brain, 121, 1853-1867. Lynch, M.P. & Eilers, R.E. (1991). Children’s perception of native and nonnative musical scales. Music Perception, 9, 121-132. Lynch, M.P., Eilers, R.E., Oller, K.D., & Urbano, R.C. (1990). Innateness, experience, and music perception. Psychological Science, 1, 272-276. Lynch, M.P., Eilers, R.E., Oller, K.D., Urbano, R.C., & Wilson, R. (1991). Influences of acculturation and musical sophistication on perception of musical interval patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 967-975. MacDougall, R. (1903). The structure of simple rhythmic forms. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 4, 309-416. McAdams, S. & Bigand, E. (Eds.) (1993). Thinking in Sound: The Cognitive Psychology of Human Audition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Mead, M. (1964). Continuities in Cultural Evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Meyer, L.B. (1956). Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L.B. (1967). Music, the Arts, and Ideas: Patterns and Predictions in Twentieth-Century Culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L.B. (1973). Explaining Music: Essays and Explorations. Berkeley: University of California Press. Meyer, L.B. (2000). The Spheres of Music: A Gathering of Essays. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Miyazaki, K. (1988). Musical pitch identification by absolute pitch possessors. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 501-512. Miyazaki, K. (1989). Absolute pitch identification: Effects of timbre and pitch region. Music Perception, 7, 1-14. Miller, G. (2000). Evolution of human music through sexual selection. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 329-360). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. Monahan, C.B. & Carterette, E.C. (1985). Pitch and duration as determinants of musical space. Music Perception, 3, 1-32. Monahan, C.B., Kendall, R.A., & Carterette, E.C. (1987). The effect of melodic and temporal contour on recognition memory for pitch change. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 576-600. Montgomery, C.R. & Clarkson, M.G. (1997). Infants’ pitch perception: Masking by low- and high-frequency noises. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 3665-3672. Morrongiello, B.A., Trehub, S.E., Thorpe, L.A., & Capodilupo, S. (1995). Children’s perception of melodies: The role of contour, frequency, and rate of presentation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 40, 279- 292. Murphy, L.E. (1966). Absolute judgments of duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 260-263. Nagasaki, H. (1987a). Correlations of stress and timing in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 161-180. Nagasaki, H. (1987b). Frequency dependence of rhythm in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 247-256. Narmour, E. (1990). The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. O’Neill, S.A. (1997). Gender and music. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Music (pp. 46-63). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Palmer, C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 331-346. Palmer, C. (1996). On the assignment of structure in music performance. Music Perception, 14, 23-56. Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115-138. Palmer, C. & Drake, C. (1997). Monitoring and planning capacities in the acquisition of music performance skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 369-384. Palmer, C. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1987a). Independent temporal and pitch structures in perception of musical phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 116-126. Palmer, C. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1987b). Pitch and temporal contributions to musical phrase perception: Effects of harmony, performance timing, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 505-518. Palmer, C. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1990). Mental representation for musical meter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 728-741. Palmer, C. & Meyer, R.K. (2000). Conceptual and motor learning in music performance. Psychological Science, 11, 63-68. Palmer, C. & van de Sande, C. (1993). Units of knowledge in music performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 457-470. Palmer, C. & van de Sande, C. (1995). Range of planning in music performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 947-962. Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: A Psychoacoustical Approach. New York: Springer-Verlag. Patel, A.D. (1998). Syntactic processing in language and music: Different cognitive operations, similar neural resources? Music Perception, 16, 27-42. Patel, A.D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P.J. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 717-713. Patterson, R.D. (1973). The effects of relative phase and the number of components on residue pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 53, 1565-1572. Penel, A. (2000). Variations temporelles dans l'interpretation musicale: Processus perceptifs et cognitifs. Doctoral dissertation, University of Paris 6. Penel, A. & Drake, C. (1998). Sources of timing variations in music performance: A psychological segmentation model. Psychological Research, 61, 12-32. Peretz, I. (1990). Processing of local and global musical information by unilateral brain-damaged patients. Brain, 113, 1185-1205. Peretz, I. (1993). Auditory agnosia: A functional analysis. In S. McAdams and E. Bigand (Eds.), Thinking in Sound: The Cognitive Psychology of Human Audition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Peretz, I. (2001). Music perception and recognition. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal About the Human Mind (pp. 519-540). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Peretz, I. & Morais, J. (1980). Modes of processing melodies and ear asymmetry in non-musicians. Neuropsychologia, 18, 477-489. Perlman, M. & Krumhansl, C.L. (1996). An experimental study of internal interval standards in Javanese and Western musicians. Music Perception, 14, 95-116. Peters, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychological Research, 51, 38-42. Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707-754. Plomp, R. & Levelt, W.J.M. (1965). Tonal consonance and the critical bandwidth. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 548-560. Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3-18. Povel, D.-J. (1984). A theoretical framework for rhythm perception. Psychological Research, 45, 315-337. Raffman, D. (1993). Language, Music, and Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rasch, R.A. (1988). Timing and synchronization in performed ensemble music. In J.A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition (pp. 70-90). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Regnault, P., Bigand, E., & Besson, M. (2001). Different brain mechanisms mediate sensitivity to sensory consonance and harmonic context: Evidence from auditory event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 241-255. Repp, B.H. (1992a). A constraint on the expressive timing of a melodic gesture: Evidence from performance and aesthetic judgment. Music Perception, 10, 221-241. Repp, B.H. (1992b). Diversity and commonality in music performance. An analysis of timing microstructure in Schumann’s “Träumerei.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92, 2546-2568. Repp, B.H. (1992c). Probing the cognitive representation of musical time: Structural constraints on the perception of timing perturbations. Cognition, 44, 241-281. Repp, B.H. (1993). Music as motion: A synopsis of Alexander Truslit’s (1938) Gestaltung und Bewegung in der Musik. Psychology of Music, 21, 48-73. Repp, B.H. (1994). The tritone paradox and the pitch range of the speaking voice: A dubious connection. Music Perception, 12, 227-255. Repp, B.H. (1996a). Patterns of note onset asynchronies in expressive piano performance. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 3917-3932. Repp, B.H. (1996b). The art of inaccuracy: Why pianists’ errors are difficult to hear. Music Perception, 14, 161- 184. Repp, B. H. (1998a). The detectability of local deviations from a typical expressive timing pattern. Music Perception, 15, 265-290. Repp, B. (1998b). Variations on a theme by Chopin: Relations between perception and production of timing in music. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 791-811. Repp, B.H. (1999a). Control of expressive and metronomic timing in pianists. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 145- 164. Repp, B.H. (1999b). Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 529-548. Repp, B.H. (1999c). Relationships between performance timing, perception of timing perturbations, and perceptual- motor synchronization in two Chopin preludes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 188-203. Repp, B.H. (2000). Pattern typicality and dimensional interactions in pianists' imitation of expressive timing and dynamics. Music Perception, 18, 173-211. Rosch, E. (1975a). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 532-547. Rosch, E. (1975b). Universals and cultural specifics in human categorization. In R. Breslin, S. Bochner, & W. Lonner (Eds.), Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Learning (pp. 117-206). New York: Halsted Press. Schellenberg, E.G. (1996). Expectancy in melody: Tests of the implication-realization model. Cognition, 58, 75- 125. Schellenberg, E.G. (1997). Simplifying the implication-realization model of melodic expectancy. Music Perception, 14, 295-318. Schellenberg, E.G. & Trehub, S.E. (1996a). Children’s discrimination of melodic intervals. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1039-1050. Schellenberg, E.G. & Trehub, S.E. (1996b). Natural musical intervals: Evidence from infant listeners. Psychological Science, 7, 272-277. Schenker, H. (1935). Der freie Satz. Vienna: Universal Edition. Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y, & Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267, 699-701. Schmuckler, M.A. (1989). Expectation in music: Investigations of melodic and harmonic processes. Music Perception, 7, 109-150. Schouten, J.F., Ritsma, R.J., & Cardozo, B.L. (1962). Pitch of the residue. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34, 1418-1424. Schulhoff, C. & Goodglass, H. (1969). Dichotic listening, side of brain injury and cerebral dominance. Neuropsychologia, 7, 149-160. Selkirk, E. (1980). The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 563-605. Shankweiler, D. (1966). Effects of temporal-lobe damage on perception of dichotically presented melodies. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 62, 115-119. Shapiro, B.E., Grossman, M., & Gardener, H. Selective processing deficits in brain damage populations. Neuropsychologia, 19, 161-169. Shepard, R.N. (1964). Circularity in judgments of relative pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 36, 2346-2353. Shepard, R.N. (1965). Approximation to uniform gradients of generalization by monotone transformations of scale. In D.I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus Generalization. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Shepard, R.N. (1982). Geometrical approximations to the structure of musical pitch. Psychological Review, 89, 305- 333. Siegel, J.A. & Siegel, W. (1977a). Absolute identification of notes and intervals by musicians. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 143-152. Siegel, J.A. & Siegel, W. (1977b). Categorical perception of tonal intervals: Musicians can’t tell sharp from flat. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 399-407. Sloboda, J.A. (1976). The effect of item position on the likelihood of identification by inference in prose reading and music reading. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 30, 22-237. Sloboda, J.A. (1977). Phrase units as determinants of visual processing in music reading. British Journal of Psychology, 68, 117-124. Sloboda, J.A. (1983). The communication of musical metre in piano performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 377-396. Sloboda, J.A. (1984). Experimental studies of musical reading: A review. Music Perception, 2, 222-236. Sloboda, J.A. (1985). The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sloboda, J.A. (Ed.) (1988). Generative Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sloboda, J.A., Clarke, E.F., Parncutt, R., & Raekallio, M. (1998). Determinants of finger choice in piano sight- reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 185-203. Sloboda, J.A., Davidson, J.W., & Howe, M.J.A. (1994). Is everyone musical? The Psychologist, 7, 359-354. Sloboda, J.A., Davidson, J.W., Howe, M.J.A., & Moore, D.G. (1996). The role of practice in the development of expert musical performance. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 287-309. Sloboda, J.A. & Gregory, A.H. (1980). The psychological reality of musical segments. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 274-280. Smith, K.C. & Cuddy, L.L (1989). Effects of metric and harmonic rhythm on the detection of pitch alterations in melodic sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 457- 471. Speer, J.R. & Meeks, P.U. (1985). School children’s perception of pitch in music. Psychomusicology, 5, 49-56. Sternberg, S., Knoll, R.L., & Zukofsky, P. (1982). Timing by skilled musicians. In D. Deutsch (Ed.) The Psychology of Music (1st ed., pp. 181-239) New York: Academic Press. Studdert-Kennedy, M., Liberman, A.M., Harris, K., & Cooper, F.S. (1970). The motor theory of speech perception: A reply to Lane's critical review. Psychological Review, 77, 234-249. Stumpf, C. (1898). Konsonanz und Dissonanz. Beiträge zur Akustik und Musikwissenschaft, 1, 1-108. Takeuchi, A.H. & Hulse, S.H. (1991). Absolute pitch judgments of black- and white-key pitches. Music Perception, 9, 27-46. Takeuchi, A.H. & Hulse, S.H. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 345-361. Tekman, H.G. & Bharucha, J.J. (1992). Time course of chord priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 33-39. Tekman, H.G. & Bharucha, J.J. (1998). Implicit knowledge versus psychoacoustic similarity in priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 252-260. Terhardt, E. (1972). Zur Tonhöhenwahrnehmug von Klängen. Acoustica, 26, 173-199. Terhardt, E. (1974). Pitch, consonance, and harmony. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55, 1061-1069. Terhardt, E. (1979). Calculating virtual pitch. Hearing Research, 1, 155-182. Tervaniemi, M., Ilvonen, T., Karma, K., Alho, K, & Näätänen, R. (1997). The musical brain: Brain waves reveal the neurophysiological basis of musicality in human subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 226, 1-4. Thompson, W.F. & Cuddy, L.L. (1997). Music performance and the perception of key. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 116-135. Thompson, W.F., Cuddy, L.L., & Plaus, C. (1997). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Evaluation of principles of melodic implication in a melody-completion task. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1069- 1076. Thurlow, W.R. & Rawlings, I.L. (1959). Discrimination of number of simultaneously sounding tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 31, 1332-1336. Tillmann, B. & Bharucha, J.J. (under review). Effect of harmonic relatedness on the detection of temporal asynchronies. Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J.J., & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological Review, 107, 885-913. Tillmann, B. & Bigand, E. (in press). Global context effect in normal and scrambled musical sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Tillmann, B., Bigand, E., & Pineau, M. (1998). Effects of global and local contexts on harmonic expectancy. Music Perception, 16, 99-117. Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 19-136). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Trainor, L.J. (1997). Effect of frequency ratio on infants’ and adults’ discrimination of simultaneous intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1427-1438. Trainor, L.J. & Heinmiller, B.M. (1998). The development of evaluative responses to music: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 77-88. Trainor, L.J. & Trehub, S.E. (1992). A comparison of infants’ and adults’ sensitivity to Western musical structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 394-402. Trainor, L.J. & Trehub, S.E. (1993a). Musical context effects in infants and adults: Key distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 615-626. Trainor, L.J. & Trehub, S.E. (1993b). What mediates infants’ and adults’ superior processing of the major over the augmented triad? Music Perception, 11, 185-196. Trainor, L.J. & Trehub, S.E. (1994). Key membership and implied harmony in Western tonal music: Developmental perspectives. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 125-132. Trehub, S.E., Bull, D., & Thorpe, L.A. (1984). Infants’ perception of melodies: The role of melodic contour. Child Development, 55, 821-830. Trehub, S.E., Cohen, A.J., Thorpe, L.A., & Morrongiello, B.A. (1986). Development of the perception of musical relations: Semitone and diatonic structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 295-301. Trehub, S.E., Thorpe, L.A., & Morrongiello, B.A. (1985). Infants’ perception of melodies: Changes in a single tone. Infant Behavior & Development, 8, 213-223. Trehub, S.E., Thorpe, L.A., & Morrongiello, B.A. (1987). Organizational processes in infants’ perception of auditory patterns. Child Development, 58, 741-749. Trehub, S.E., Thorpe, L.A., & Trainor, L.J. (1990). Infants’ perception of good and bad melodies. Psychomusicology, 9, 5-15. Truslit, A. (1938). Gestaltung und Bewegung in der Musik. Berlin: Friedrich Vieweg. Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327-352. Van Nuys, K. & Weaver, H.E. (1943). Memory span and visual pauses in reading rhythms and melodies. Psychological Monographs, 55, 33-50. Viviani, P. & Stucchi, N. (1992). Biological movements look uniform: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 603-623. Vos, J. & van Vianen, B.G. (1984). Thresholds for discrimination between pure and tempered intervals: The relevance of nearly coinciding harmonics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 176-187. Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. & Brown, S. (Eds.) (2000). The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Weaver, H.E. (1943). A study of visual processes in reading differently constructed musical selections. Psychological Monographs, 55, 1-30. Weir, C., Jesteadt, W., & Green, D.M. (1977). Frequency discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 61, 178-184. Westbury, C.F., Zatorre, R.J., & Evans, A.C. (1999). Quantifying variability in the planum temporale: A probability map. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 392-405. Whitfield, I.C. (1980). Auditory cortex and the pitch of complex tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 644-647. Wightman, F.L. (1973a). Pitch and stimulus fine structure. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 397- 406. Wightman, F.L. (1973b). The pattern-transformation model of pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 407-416. Zatorre, R.J. (1984). Musical perception and cerebral function: A critical review. Music Perception, 2, 196-221. Zatorre, R.J. (1988). Pitch perception of complex tones and human temporal-lobe function. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 82, 566-572. Zatorre, R.J., Evans, A.C., & Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 1908-1919. Zatorre, R.J., Halpern, A., Perry, D.W., Meyer, E., & Evans, A.C. (1996). Hearing in the mind’s ear: A PET investigation of musical imagery and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 29-46. Zatorre, R.J., Perry, D.W., Beckett, C.A., Westbury, C.F., & Evans, A.C. (1998). Functional anatomy of musical processing in listeners with absolute pitch and relative pitch. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 3172-3177. Zatorre, R.J. & Samson, S. (1991). Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short-term memory. Brain, 114, 2403-2417.
  232. REFERENCES Ainsworth, Mary S. (1989), “Attachments beyond Infancy,” Amer- ican Psychologist, 44 (4), 709–16. Andrade, Eduardo B. (2005), “Behavioral Consequences of Affect: Combining Evaluative and Regulatory Mechanisms,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (December), 355–62. Baumeister, Roy F., and Mark R. Leary (1995), “The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Funda- mental Human Motivation,” Psychological Bulletin, 117 (3), 497–529. Biswas, Rahul, Daniel Riffe, and Dolf Zillmann (1994), “Mood Influence on the Appeal of Bad News,” Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 71 (3), 689–96. Bowlby, John (1977), “The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds,” British Journal of Psychiatry, 130 (November), 201–10. Bryant, Jennings, and Dolf Zillmann (1984), “Using Television to Alleviate Boredom and Stress: Selective Exposure as a Func- tion of Induced Excitational States,” Journal of Broadcasting, 28 (1), 1–20. Cohen, Joel B., Michel T. Pham, and Eduardo B. Andrade (2008), “The Nature and Role of Affect in Consumer Behavior,” in Handbook of Consumer Psychology, ed. Curt Haugtvedt, Frank Kardes, and Paul Herr, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 297– 348. Cohen, Sheldon, and Thomas A. Wills (1985), “Stress, Social Sup- port, and the Buffering Hypothesis,” Psychological Bulletin, 98 (2), 310–57. Derrick, Jaye L., Shira Gabriel, and Kurt Hugenberg (2009), “So- cial Surrogacy: How Favored Television Programs Provide the Experience of Belonging,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45 (2), 352–62. Fournier, Susan (1998), “Consumers and Their Brands: Developing Relationship Theory in Consumer Research,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 24 (4), 343–53. Gabriel, Shira, and Ariana F. Young (2011), “Becoming a Vampire without Being Bitten: The Narrative Collective-Assimilation Hypothesis,” Psychological Science, 22 (8), 990–94. Gibson, Rhonda, Charles F. Aust, and Dolf Zillmann (2000), “Loneliness of Adolescents and Their Choice and Enjoyment of Love-Celebrating versus Love-Lamenting Popular Music,” Empirical Studies of the Arts, 18 (1), 43–8. Helregel, Brenda K., and James B. Weaver (1989), “Mood-Man- agement during Pregnancy through Selective Exposure to Television,” Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 33 (1), 15–33. Hill, Craig A. (1991), “Seeking Emotional Support: The Influence of Affiliative Need and Partner Warmth,” Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 60 (1), 112–21. Holbrook, Morris B., and Elizabeth C. Hirschman (1982), “The Experiential Aspects of Consumer Consumption: Consumer Fantasies, Feelings, and Fun,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (2), 132–40. Kamins, Michael A., Lawrence J. Marks, and Deborah Skinner (1991), “Television Commercial Evaluation in the Context of Program Induced Mood: Congruency versus Consistency Ef- fects,” Journal of Advertising, 20 (2), 1–14. Knobloch, Silvia, and Dolf Zillmann (2002), “Mood Management via the Digital Jukebox,” Journal of Communication, 52 (2), 351–66. ——— (2003), “Appeal of Love Themes in Popular Music,” Psy- chological Reports, 93 (3), 653–58. Knobloch-Westerwick, Silva, Yuan Gong, Holly Hagner, and Laura Kerbeykian (2012), “Tragedy Viewers Count Their Blessings: Feeling Low on Fiction Leads to Feeling High on Life,” Com- munication Research, 20 (5), 1–20. Lehman, Darrin R., John H. Ellard, and Camille B. Wortman (1986), “Social Support for the Bereaved: Recipients’ and Providers’ Perspectives on What Is Helpful,” Journal of Con- sulting and Clinical Psychology, 54 (4), 438–46. Mares, Marie-Louise, and Joanne Cantor (1992), “Empathy and Mood Management versus Social Comparison: Elderly Viewers’ Responses to Televised Portrayals of Old Age,” Communication Research, 19 (4), 459–78. Martin, Leonard L., Teresa Abend, Constantine Sedikides, and Jeffrey D. Green (1997), “How Would I Feel If . . .? Mood as Input to a Role Fulfillment Evaluation Process,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (2), 242–53. Meadowcroft, Jeanne M., and Dolf Zillmann (1987), “Women’s Comedy Preferences during the Menstrual Cycle,” Commu- nication Research, 14 (2), 204–18. Nabi, Robin L., Keli Finnerty, Tricia Domschke, and Shawnika Hull (2006), “Does Misery Love Company? Exploring the Therapeutic Effects of TV Viewing on Regretted Experi- ences,” Journal of Communication, 56 (4), 689–706. Oliver, Marry B. (1993), “Exploring the Paradox of the Enjoyment of Sad Films,” Human Communication Research, 19 (3), 315–42. ——— (2008), “Tender Affective States as Predictors of Enter- tainment Preference,” Journal of Communication, 58 (1), 40–61. Raghunathan, Rajagopal, and Kim P. Corfman (2004), “Sadness as Pleasure-Seeking Prime and Anxiety as Attentiveness Prime: The ‘Different Affect–Different Effect’ (DADE) Model,” Motivation and Emotion, 28 (1), 23–41. Rime´, Bernard (2009), “Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emo- tion: Theory and Empirical Review,” Emotion Review, 1 (1), 60–85. Schachter, Stanley (1959), The Psychology of Affiliation, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Sheppes, Gal, Susanne Scheibe, Gaurav Suri, and James J. Gross (2011), “Emotion-Regulation Choice,” Psychological Sci- ence, 22 (11), 1391–96. Strizhakova, Yuliya, and Marina Krcmar (2007), “Mood Manage- ment and Video Rental Choices,” Media Psychology, 10 (1), 91–112. Tavris, Carol (1989), Anger: The Misunderstood Emotion, New York: Touchstone Books/Simon & Schuster. Tice, Dianne M., Ellen Bratslavsky, and Roy F. Baumeister (2001), “Emotional Distress Regulation Takes Precedence over Im- pulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do It,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (1), 53–67. Wegener, Duane T., and Richard E. Petty (1994), “Mood Man- agement across Affective States: The Hedonic Contingency Hypothesis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (6), 1034–48. Young, William P. (2007), The Shack, Newbury Park, CA: Wind- blown Media. Zillmann , Dolf (1988), “Mood Management: Using Entertainment Media to Full Advantage,” in Communication, Social Cog- nition and Affect, ed. Lewis Donohew, Howard E. Sypher, and E. Tory Higgins, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 147–71. Zillmann, Dolf, Richard T. Hezel, and Norman J. Medoff (1980), “The Effect of Affective States on Selective Exposure to Tele- vised Entertainment Fare,” Journal of Applied Social Psy- chology, 10 (4), 323–39.
  233. REFERENCES Aarts, Henk, Tanya L. Chartrand (2005), “Social Stereotypes and Automatic Goal Pursuit,” Social Cognition, 23(6), 465-90. ______, Ruud Custers, and Rob W. Holland (2007), “The Nonconscious Cessation of Goal Pursuit: When Goals and Negative Affect are Coactivated,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 165-78. Bardi, Anat, Julie Anne Lee, Nadi Hofmann-Towfigh, and Geoffrey Soutar (2009), “The Structure of Intraindividual Value Change,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 913-29. Bargh, John A. (2002), “Losing Consciousness: Automatic Influences on Consumer Judgment, Behavior, and Motivation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29(2), 280-85. ______, Mark Chen, and Lara Burrows (1996), “Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230-44. Bauer, Raymond A., and Stephen A. Greyser (1968), Advertising in America: The Consumer View, Boston: Harvard University. Brumbaugh, Anne M. (2002), “Source and Nonsource Cues in Advertising and Their Effects on the Activtion of Cultural and Subcultural Knowledge on the Route to Persuasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29(2), 258-69. Durvasula, Srinivas J., Craig Andrews, Steven Lysonski, and Richard G. Netemeyer (1993), “Assessing the Cross-National Applicability of Consumer-Behavior Models: A Model of Attitude Toward Advertising in General,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19(4), 626-36. Feather, Norman T. (1990), “Bridging the Gap Between Values and Action,” in Handbook of motivation and cognition, Vol. 2, E. Tory Higgins and Richard Sorrentino, eds., New York: Guilford Press, 151-92 ______ (1995), “Values, Valences, and Choice: The Influences of Values on the Perceived Attractiveness and Choice of Alternatives,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(6), 1135-51. Fitzsimons, Gráinne M., Tanya L. Chartrand, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2008), “Automatic Effects of Brand Exposure on Motivated Behavior: How Apple Makes You “Think Different”,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 21-35. Forehand, Mark R., and Rohit Deshpandé (2001), “What We See Makes Us Who We Are: Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response,” Journal of Marketing Research, 38(3), 336-48. Gardner, Wendi L., Shira Gabriel, and Angela Y. Lee (1999), ““I” Value Freedom, but “We” Value Relationships: Self-Construal Priming Mirrors Cultural Differences in Judgment,” Psychological Science, 10(4), 321-26. Gerbner, George, Larry Gross, Michael Morgan, Nancy Signorielli, and James Shanahan (2002), “Growing Up with Television: Cultivation Processes,” in Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research, Jennings Bryant and Dolf Zillmann, rds., Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 43-67. Gutman, Jonathan (1982), “A Means-End Model Based on Consumer Categorization Processes,” Journal of Marketing, 46(2), 60-72. Higgins, E. Tory (1996), “Knowledge Activation: Accessibility, Applicability, and Salience,” in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Kruglanski, eds., New York: Guilford Press, 133-68. ______ (2007), “Value,” in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, Second Edition, ArieW. Kruglanski and E.Tory Higgins, eds., New York: Guilford Press, 454- 72. Hitlin, Steven, and Jane A. Piliavin (2004), “Values: Reviving a Dormant Concept,” Annual Review of Sociology, 30, 359-93. Holbrook, Morris B. (1987), “Mirror, Mirror, on the Wall, What’s Unfair in the Reflections on Advertising?” Journal of Marketing, 51(3), 95–103. Karremans, Johan C., Wolfgang Stroebe, and Jasper Claus (2006), “Beyond Vicary’s Fantasies: The Impact of Subliminal Priming and Brand Choice,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(6), 792-8. Kim, Heejung, and Hazel R. Markus (1999), “Deviance or Uniqueness, Harmony or Conformity? A Cultural Analysis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(4), 785-800. Lombardi, Wendy J., E. Tory Higgins, and John A. Bargh (1987), “The Role of Consciousness in Priming Effects on Categorization,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(3), 411-29. Maio, Gregory R., and James M. Olson (1998), “Values as Truisms: Evidence and Implications,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 294-311. ______, Ali Pakizeh, Wing-Yee Cheung, and Kerry J. Rees (2009), “Changing, Priming, and Acting on Values: Effects via Motivational Relations in a Circular Model,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 699-715. Mandel, Naomi (2003), “Shifting Selves and Decision Making: The Effects of Self-Construal Priming on Consumer Risk-Taking,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 30-40. McClelland, David C., Richard Koestner, and Joel Weinberger (1989), “How Do Self- Attributed and Implicit Motives Differ?,” Psychological Review, 96(4), 690-702. Mehta, Abhilasha (2000), “Advertising Attitudes and Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Advertising Research, 40(3), 67-72. Meyers-Levy, Joan, and Prashant Malaviva (1999), “Consumers’ Processing of Persuasive Advertisements: An Integrative Framework of Persuasion Theories,” Journal of Advertising, 16(1), 32-40. Muehling, Darrel D. (1987), “An Investigation of Factors Underlying Attitude-Toward- Advertising-in-General,” Journal of Advertising, 16(1), 32-40. Nairn, Agnes, and Pierre Berthon (2003), “Creating the Customer: The Influence of Advertising on Consumer Market Segments - Evidence and Ethics,” Journal of Business Ethics, 42(1), 83-99. Nelson, Michelle R., Frédéric F. Brunel, Magne Supphellen, and Rajesh V. Manchanda (2006), “Effects of Culture, Gender, and Moral Obligations on Responses to Charity Advertising Across Masculine and Feminine Cultures,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16(1), 45-56. Newman, Leonard S., and James S. Uleman (1990), “Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Spontaneous Trait Inferences,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 224-40. Obermiller, Carl, Eric Spangenberg, and Douglas L. MacLachlan (2005), “Ad Skepticism: The Consequences of Disbelief,” Journal of Advertising, 34(3), 7-17. Pakizeh, Ali, Jochen E. Gebauer, and Gregory R. Maio (2007), “Basic Human Values: Inter- Value Structure in Memory,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43(3), 458-65. Phillips, Barbara J. (1997), “In Defense of Advertising: A Social Perspective,” Journal of Business Ethics, 16(2), 109–18. Pollay, Richard W. (1986), “The Distorted Mirror: Reflections on the Unintended Consequences of Advertising,” Journal of Marketing, 50(2), 18-36. ______ (1987), “On the Value of Reflections on the Values in The Distorted Mirror,” Journal of Marketing, 51(3), 104-10. ______, and Banwari Mittal (1993), “Here’s the Beef: Factors, Determinants and Segments in Consumer Criticism of Advertising,” Journal of Marketing, 57(3), 99-114. ______, and Jonathan Gutman (1984), “Advertising is Image Management,” Journal of Advertising Research, 24(1), 27-37. Richins, Marsha L. (1991), “Social Comparison and the Idealized Images of Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18(1), 71-83. Rokeach, Milton (1973), The nature of human values, New York: Free Press. Sandage, Charles H., and John D. Leckenby (1980), “Student Attitudes Toward Advertising: Institution vs. Instrument,” Journal of Advertising, 9(2), 29-32. Schwartz, Shalom H. (1992), “Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 25, Mark P. Zanna, ed., San Diego, CA: Academic, 1-65. ______, and Wolfgang Bilsky (1987), “Toward a Psychological Structure of Human Values,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550-65. ______ (1990), “Toward a Theory of the Universal Content and Structure of Values: Extensions and Cross Cultural Replications,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 878-91. Seligman, Clive, and Albert N. Katz (1996), “The Dynamics of Value Systems,” in The Ontario Symposium: The psychology of values, Clive Seligman, James M. Olson and Mark P. Zanna, eds., Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 53-75. Shrum, L.J. (1999), “Television and persuasion: Effects of the programs between the ads,” Psychology & Marketing, 16(2), 119-40. Smeesters, Dirk, Thomas Mussweiler, and Naomi Mandel (2010), “The Effects of Thin and Heavy Media Images on Overweight and Underweight Consumers: Social Comparison Processes and Behavioral Implications,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6), 930-49. Strahan, Erin J., Steven J. Spencer, and Mark P. Zanna (2002), “Subliminal Priming and Persuasion: Striking while the Iron is Hot,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 556-58. Triandis, Harry C. (1995), Individualism and Collectivism, Boulder: Westview. Van Baaren, Rick B., and Martin Ruivenkamp (2007), “Self-Construal and Values Expressed in Advertising,” Social Influence, 2(2), 136 – 44. Verplanken, Bas, and Rob W. Holland (2002), “Motivated Decision Making: Effects of Activation and Self-Centrality of Values on Choices and Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(3), 424-47. Waide, John (1987), “The Making of Self and World in Advertising,” Journal of Business Ethics, 6(2), 73-9. Wegener, Duane T., and Richard E. Petty (1995), “Flexible Correction Processes in Social Judgment: The Role of Naïve Theories in Corrections for Perceived Bias,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 36-51. Whittler, Tommy E., and Joan Scattone Spira (2002), “Model’s Race: A Peripheral Cue in Advertising Messages?” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(4), 291-301. Wilson, Timothy D., and Nancy Brekke (1994), “Mental Contamination and Mental Correction: Unwanted Influences on Judgments and Evaluations,” Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 117-42. Zhang, Jing (2009), “The Effect of Advertising Appeals in Activating Self-Construals: A Case of Bicultural Chinese X-Generation Consumers,” Journal of Advertising, 38(1), 63-81.
  234. REFERENCES Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator- Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Re- search: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173–82. Baumeister, Roy F., C. Nathan Dewall, Natalie J. Ciarocco, and Jean M. Twenge (2005), “Social Exclusion Impairs Self-Reg- ulation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (4), 589–604. Baumeister, Roy F., and Mark R. Leary (1995), “The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Funda- mental Human Motivation,” Psychological Bulletin, 117 (3), 497–529. Baumeister, Roy F., and Steven J. Scher (1988), “Self-Defeating Behavior Patterns among Normal Individuals: Review and Analysis of Common Self-Destructive Tendencies,” Psycho- logical Bulletin, 104 (1), 3–22. Carter-Sowell, Adrienne, Zhansheng Chen, and Kipling D. Wil- liams (2008), “Ostracism Increases Social Susceptibility,” So- cial Influence, 3 (3), 143–53. Chen, Zhansheng, Kipling D. Williams, Julie Fitness, and Nicola C. Newton (2008), “When Hurt Will Not Heal: Exploring the Capacity to Relive Social and Physical Pain,” Psychological Science, 19 (8), 789–95. Duclos, Rod, James R. Bettman, Paul N. Bloom, and Gal Zaub- erman (2012), “Charitable Giving: How Ego-Threats Impact Donations of Time and Money,” Working Paper, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology. Furnham, Adrian (1984), “Many Sides of the Coin: The Psychol- ogy of Money Usage,” Personality and Individual Differ- ences, 5 (5), 501–9. He, Xin, J. Jeffrey Inman, and Vikas Mittal (2008), “Gender Jeop- ardy in Financial Risk Taking,” Journal of Marketing Re- search, 45 (August), 414–24. Isen, Alice M., and Robert Patrick (1983), “The Effect of Positive Feelings on Risk Taking: When the Chips Are Down,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 31 (2), 194–202. Johnson, Wendy, and Robert F. Krueger (2006), “How Money Buys Happiness: Genetic and Environmental Processes Link- ing Finances and Life Satisfaction,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (4), 680–91. Lachman, Margie E., and Suzanne L. Weaver (1998), “The Sense of Control as a Moderator of Social Class Differences in Health and Well-Being,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (March), 763–73. Leary, Mark R., Ellen S. Tambor, Sonja K. Terdal, and Deborah L. Downs (1995), “Self-Esteem as an Interpersonal Monitor: The Sociometer Hypothesis,” Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 68 (3), 518–30. Lee, Jaehoon, and L. J. Shrum (2012), “Conspicuous Consumption versus Charitable Behavior in Response to Social Exclusion: A Differential Needs Explanation,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 39 (October), 530–44. Leith, Karen Pezza, and Roy F. Baumeister (1996), “Why Do Bad Moods Increase Self-Defeating Behavior? Emotion, Risk Tak- ing, and Self-Regulation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (6), 1250–67. Mead, Nicole L., Roy F. Baumeister, Tyle F. Stillman, Catherine D. Rawn, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2011), “Social Exclusion Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the Service of Affiliation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (5), 902–19. Mittal, Vikas, and William T. Ross (1998), “The Impact of Positive and Negative Affect and Issue Framing on Issue Interpretation and Risk Taking,” Organizational Behavior and Human De- cision Processes, 76 (3), 298–324. Muller, Dominique, Charles M. Judd, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2005), “When Moderation Is Mediated and Mediation Is Moderated,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (December), 852–63. Olson, Michael A., Russell H. Fazio, and Anthony D. Hermann (2007), “Reporting Tendencies Underlie Discrepancies be- tween Implicit and Explicit Measures of Self-Esteem,” Psy- chological Science, 18 (4), 287–91. Pickett, Cynthia L., Wendi L. Gardner, and Megan Knowles (2004), “Getting a Cue: The Need to Belong and Enhanced Sensitivity to Social Cues,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30 (9), 1095–1107. Preacher, Kristopher J., Derek D. Rucker, and Andrew F. Hayes (2007), “Addressing Moderated Mediation Hypotheses: The- ory, Methods, and Prescriptions,” Multivariate Behavioral Re- search, 42 (1), 185–227. Price, Richard H., Jin Nam Choi, and Amiram D. Vinokur (2002), “Links in the Chain of Adversity Following Job Loss: How Financial Strain and Loss of Personal Control Lead to De- pression, Impaired Functioning and Poor Health,” Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7 (4), 302–12. Putnam, Robert D. (1995), “Bowling Alone: America’s Declining Social Capital,” Journal of Democracy, 6 (1), 65–78. ——— (2000), Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of Amer- ican Community, New York: Simon & Schuster. Raghunathan, Rajagopal, and Michel Tuan Pham (1999), “All Neg- ative Moods Are Not Equal: Motivational Influences of Anx- iety and Sadness on Decision Making,” Organizational Be- havior and Human Decision Processes, 71 (1), 56–77. Shrout, Patrick E., and Niall Bolger (2002), “Mediation in Ex- perimental and Nonexperimental Studies: New Procedures and Recommendations,” Psychological Methods, 7 (4), 422–45. Tang, Thomas L. P. (1995), “The Development of a Short Money Ethic Scale: Attitudes toward Money and Pay Satisfaction Revisited,” Personality and Individual Differences, 19 (6), 809–16. Twenge, Jean M., Kathleen R. Catanese, and Roy F. Baumeister (2002), “Social Exclusion Causes Self-Defeating Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (3), 606– 15. US Bureau of the Census (1998), Statistical Abstract of the United States, Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. Vohs, Kathleen D., Nicole L. Mead, and Miranda R. Goode (2006), “The Psychological Consequences of Money,” Science, 314 (5802), 1154–56. ——— (2008), “Merely Activating the Concept of Money Changes Personal and Interpersonal Behavior,” Current Di- rections in Psychological Science, 17 (3), 208–12. Warburton, Wayne A., Kipling D. Williams, and David R. Cairns (2006), “When Ostracism Leads to Aggression: The Mod- erating Effects of Control Deprivation,” Journal of Experi- mental Social Psychology, 42 (March), 213–20. Watson, David, Lee Anna Clark, and Auke Tellegen (1988), “De- velopment and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (6), 1063–70. Williams, Kipling D. (2007), “Ostracism,” Annual Review of Psy- chology, 58, 425–52. Williams, Kipling D., Christopher K. T. Cheung, and Wilma Choi (2000), “Cyberostracism: Effects of Being Ignored over the Internet,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (5), 748–62. Williams, Kipling D., and Blair Jarvis (2006), “Cyberball: A Pro- gram for Use in Research on Interpersonal Ostracism and Acceptance,” Behavior Research Methods, 38 (1), 174–80. Yamauchi, Kent T., and Donald I. Templer (1982), “The Devel- opment of a Money Attitude Scale,” Journal of Personality Assessment, 46 (5), 522–28. Zhou, Rongrong, and Michel Tuan Pham (2004), “Promotion and Prevention across Mental Accounts: When Financial Products Dictate Consumers’ Investment Goals,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (June), 125–35. Zhou, Xinyue, Kathleen D. Vohs, and Roy F. Baumeister (2009), “The Symbolic Power of Money: Reminders of Money Alter Social Distress and Physical Pain,” Psychological Science, 20 (6), 700–706. Zhu, Rui, Utpal M. Dholakia, Xinlei Chen, and Rene´ Algesheimer (2012), “Does Online Community Participation Foster Risky Financial Behavior?” Journal of Marketing Research, 49 (3), 394–407.
  235. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer L., and Angela Y. Lee (2001), “I Seek Pleasures and We Avoid Pains: The Role of Self-Regulatory Goals in Information Processing and Persuasion,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 28 (June), 33–49. Aaker, Jennifer L., and Patti Williams (1998), “Empathy versus Pride: The Influence of Emotional Appeals across Cultures,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (December), 241–61. Aiken, Leona S., and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bar-Anan, Yoav, Nira Liberman, and Yaacov Trope (2006), “The Association between Psychological Distance and Construal Level: Evidence from an Implicit Association Test,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135 (4), 609–22. Brewer, Marylinn B., and Wendi Gardner (1996), “Who Is This We? Levels of Collective Identity and Self Representations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (July), 83–93. Briley, Donnel A. (2009), “Looking Forward, Looking Back: Cul- tural Differences and Similarities in Time Orientation,” in Understanding Culture: Theory, Research, and Application, ed. R. Wyer Jr., C. Y. Chiu, and Y. Y. Hong, New York: Psychology Press, 311–18. Briley, Donnel A., and Robert S. Wyer Jr. (2002), “The Effect of Group Membership Salience on the Avoidance of Negative Outcomes: Implications for Social and Consumer Decisions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (December), 400–415. Carstensen, Laura L., Derek M. Isaacowitz, and Susan T. Charles (1999), “Taking Time Seriously: A Theory of Socioemotional Selectivity,” American Psychologist, 54 (March), 165–81. Chaiken, Shelly, Roger Giner-Sorolla, and Serena Chen (1996), “Beyond Accuracy: Defence and Impression Motives in Heu- ristic and Systematic Information Processing,” in The Psy- chology of Action: Linking Motivation and Cognition to Be- havior, ed. Peter M. Gollwitzer and John A. Bargh, New York: Guilford, 212–52. Cousins, Steven D. (1989), “Culture and Self-Perception in Japan and the United States,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56 (January), 124–31. English, Tammy, and Serena Chen (2007), “Culture and Self-Con- cept Stability: Consistency across and within Contexts among Asian Americans and European Americans,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 93 (3), 478–90. Eyal, Tal, Nira Liberman, Yaacov Trope, and Eva Walther (2004), “The Pros and Cons of Temporally Near and Distant Action,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (6), 781–95. Fujita, Kentaro, Tal Eyal, Shelly Chaiken, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2008), “Influencing Attitudes toward Near and Dis- tant Objects,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44 (3), 562–72. Gardner, Wendy L., Shira Gabriel, and Angela Y. Lee (1999), “‘I’ Value Freedom but ‘We’ Value Relationships: Self-Construal Priming Mirrors Cultural Differences in Judgment,” Psycho- logical Science, 10 (July), 321–26. Han, Sang-pil, and Sharon Shavitt (1994), “Persuasion and Culture: Advertising Appeals in Individualistic and Collectivistic So- cieties,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30 (July), 326–50. Henderson, Marlone D., Yaacov Trope, and Peter J. Carnevale (2006), “Negotiation from a Near and Distant Time Perspec- tive,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91 (Oc- tober), 712–29. Higgins, E. Tory (1997), “Beyond Pleasure and Pain,” American Psychologist, 52 (December), 1280–1300. Hong, Ying-yi, Michael W. Morris, Chi-yue Chiu, and Veronica Benet-Martinez (2000), “Multicultural Minds: A Dynamic Constructivist Approach to Culture and Cognition,” American Psychologist, 55 (July), 709–20. Kim, Kyeongheui, Meng Zhang, and Xioping Li (2008), “Effects of Temporal and Social Distance on Consumer Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (December), 706–13. Kinagawa, Chie, Susan E. Cross, and Hazel R. Marcus (2001), “‘Who Am I?’ The Cultural Psychology of the Conceptual Self,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (Jan- uary), 90–103. Kitayama, Shinobu, Hyekyung Park, A. Timur Sevincer, Mayumi Karasawa, and Ayse K. Uskul (2009), “A Cultural Task Anal- ysis of Implicit Independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (2), 236–55. Lee, Angela Y., Jennifer L. Aaker, and Wendi L. Gardner (2000), “The Pleasures and Pains of Distinct Self-Construals: The Role of Interdependence in Regulatory Focus,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (June), 1122–34. Lee, Angela Y., and E. Tory Higgins (2009), “The Persuasive Power of Regulatory Fit,” in The Social Psychology of Con- sumer Behavior, ed. Michaela Wa¨nke, New York: Psychology Press, 319–33. Lee, Angela Y., Punam Keller, and Brian Sternthal (2010), “Value from Regulatory Construal Fit,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 36 (5), 735–47. Lee, Fiona, Mark Hallahan, and Thaddeus Herzog (1996), “Ex- plaining Real-Life Events: How Culture and Domain Shape Attributions,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22 (July), 732–41. Lee, Sujin, Angela Y. Lee, and Mary C. Kern (2010), “Viewing Time through the Lens of the Self: The Fit Effect of Self- Construal and Temporal Distance on Task Perception,” Eu- ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 41 (March), 191–200. Liberman, Nira, and Jens Fo¨rster (2009), “Distancing from Ex- perienced Self: How Global-versus-Local Perception Affects Estimation of Psychological Distance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (2), 203–16. Liberman, Nira, Michael Sagristano, and Yaacov Trope (2002), “The Effect of Temporal Distance on Level of Mental Con- strual,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38 (No- vember), 523–34. Liberman, Nira, and Yaacov Trope (1998), “The Role of Feasibility and Desirability Considerations in Near and Distant Future Decisions: A Test of Temporal Construal Theory,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (July), 5–18. Liberman, Nira, Yaacov Trope, Sean M. McCrea, and Steven J. Sherman (2007), “The Effect of Level of Construal on the Temporal Distance of Activity Enactment,” Journal of Ex- perimental Social Psychology, 43 (January), 143–49. Markus, Hazel R., and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), “Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motiva- tion,” Psychological Review, 98 (April), 224–53. Morris, Michael W., and Kaiping Peng (1994), “Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (December), 949–71. Norenzayan, Ara, Incheol Choi, and Richard E. Nisbett (2002), “Cultural Similarities and Differences in Social Inference: Evidence from Behavioral Predictions and Lay Theories of Behavior,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28 (January), 109–20. Nussbaum, Shiri, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2003), “Creeping Disponitionism: The Temporal Dynamics of Be- havioral Prediction,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 84 (3), 485–97. Pennington, Ginger L., and Neal J. Roese (2003), “Regulatory Focus and Temporal Distance,” Journal of Experimental So- cial Psychology, 39 (November), 563–76. Petty, Richard E., and John T. Cacioppo (1986), “The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion,” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 19, ed. Leonard Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press, 123–205. Petty, Richard E., John T. Cacioppo, and David Schumann (1983), “Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effectiveness: The Moderating Role of Involvement,” Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (September), 135–46. Rhee, Eun, James S. Uleman, Hoon K. Lee, and Robert J. Roman (1995), “Spontaneous Self-Descriptions and Ethnic Identities in Individualistic and Collectivistic Cultures,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 69 (July), 142–52. Shweder, Richard A., and Edmund J. Bourne (1984), “Does the Concept of the Person Vary Cross-Culturally?” in Cultural Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy, ed. Anthony J. Marsella and Geoffrey M. White, Dordrecht: Riedel, 97–137. Singelis, Theodore M. (1994), “The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (October), 580–91. Smith, Pamela K., and Yaacov Trope (2006), “You Focus on the Forest When You Are in Charge of the Trees: Power Priming and Abstract Information Processing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (4), 578–96. Triandis, Harry C. (1989), “The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts,” Psychological Review, 96 (3), 506–20. Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman (2000), “Temporal Construal and Time Dependent Changes in Preference,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 79 (December), 876–89. ——— (2010), “Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Dis- tance,” Psychological Review, 117 (2), 440–63. Underwood, Geoffrey, and R. A. Swain (1973), “Selectivity of Attention and the Perception of Duration,” Perception, 2 (1), 101–5. Vallacher, Robin R., and Daniel M. Wegner (1989), “Levels of Personal Agency: Individual Variation in Action Identifica- tion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (Oc- tober), 660–71. Wakslak, Cheryl J., Yaacov Trope, Nira Liberman, and Rotem Alony (2006), “Seeing the Forest When Entry Is Unlikely: Probability and the Mental Representation of Events,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135 (November), 641–53. Weisz, John R., Fred M. Rothbaum, and Thomas C. Blackburn (1984), “Standing Out and Standing In: The Psychology of Control in American and Japan,” American Psychologist, 39 (September), 955–69.
  236. REFERENCES Bartels, Daniel, and Lance J. Rips (2010), “Psychological Con- nectedness and Intertemporal Choice,” Journal of Experi- mental Psychology: General, 139 (February), 49–69. Bartels, Daniel, and Oleg Urminsky (2011), “On Intertemporal Selfishness: How the Perceived Instability of Identity Un- derlies Impatient Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 38 (June), 182–98. Baucells, Manel, and Franz H. Heukamp (2012), “Probability and Time Trade-Off,” Management Science, 58 (April), 831–42. Bazerman, Max, Don A. Moore, Ann E. Tenbrunsel, Kimberly A. Wade-Benzoni, and Sally Blount (1999), “Explaining How Preferences Change across Joint versus Separate Evaluation,” Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 39 (May), 41–58. Bleichrodt, Han, and Jose L. P. Prades (2009), “New Evidence of Preference Reversals in Health Utility Measurement,” Health Economics, 18 (September), 713–26. Bommier, Antoine (2006), “Uncertain Lifetime and Intertemporal Choice: Risk Aversion as a Rationale for Time Discounting,” International Economic Review, 47 (November), 1223–46. Burson, Katherine A., Richard P. Larrick, and John G. Lynch Jr. (2009), “Six of One, Half Dozen of the Other: Expanding and Contracting Numerical Dimensions Produces Preference Re- versals,” Psychological Science, 20 (September), 1074–78. Chatterjee, Subimal, Timothy B. Heath, and Junhond Min (2009), “The Susceptibility of Mental Accounting Principles to Eval- uation Mode Effects,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Mak- ing, 22 (July), 120–37. Chen, Rong, and Jianmin Jia (2005), “Consumer Choices under Small Probabilities: Overweighting or Underweighting,” Marketing Letters, 16 (March), 5–18. Ebert, Jane E., and Drazen Prelec (2007), “The Fragility of Time: Time-Insensitivity and Valuation of the Near and Far Future,” Management Science, 53 (September), 1423–38. Frederick, Shane, George Loewenstein, and Ted O’Donoghue (2002), “Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review,” Journal of Economic Literature, 40 (June), 351– 401. Gneezy, Uri, John List, and George Wu (2006), “The Uncertainty Effect: When a Risky Prospect Is Valued Less than Its Worse Possible Outcome,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121 (4), 1283–1309. Gneezy, Uri, and Aldo Rustichini (2000), “Pay Enough, or Don’t Pay at All,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115 (August), 791–810. Gonzalez, Richard, and George Wu (1999), “On the Shape of the Probability Weighting Function,” Cognitive Psychology, 38 (January), 129–66. Hershfield, Hal E., Daniel G. Goldstein, William F. Sharpe, Jesse Fox, Leo Yeykelvis, Laura L. Carstensen, and Jeremy N. Bai- lenson (2011), “Increasing Saving Behavior through Age-Pro- gressed Renderings of the Future Self,” Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (November), S23–S37. Hsee, Christopher K. (1996), “The Evaluability Hypothesis: An Explanation of Preference Reversals between Joint and Sep- arate Evaluations of Alternatives,” Organizational Behavior and Human Choice Processes, 46 (September), 247–57. Hsee, Christopher K., and France Leclerc (1998), “Will Products Look More Attractive When Presented Separately or Together?” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 25 (September), 175–86. Hsee, Christopher K., George Loewenstein, Sally Blount, and Max H. Bazerman (1999), “Preference Reversals between Joint and Separate Evaluations of Options: A Review and Theoretical Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin, 125 (September), 576–91. Hsee, Christopher K., and Yuval Rottenstreich (2004), “Music, Pandas and Muggers: On the Affective Psychology of Value,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133 (March), 23–30. Hsee, Christopher K., Fang Yu, Jiao Zhang, and Yan Zhang (2003), “Medium Maximization,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (June), 1–14. Hsee, Christopher K., and Jiao Zhang (2004), “Distinction Bias: Misprediction and Mischoice due to Joint Evaluation,” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (May), 680–95. ——— (2010), “General Evaluability Theory: An Analysis of When What We Care about Matters,” Perspectives on Psy- chological Science, 5 (September), 343–55. Kable, Joseph K., and Paul W. Glimcher (2010), “An ‘As Soon as Possible’ Effect in Human Intertemporal Decision Making: Behavioral Evidence and Neural Mechanisms,” Journal of Neurophysiology, 103 (5), 2513–31. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky (1979), “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk,” Econometrica, 47 (March), 263–91. Kilka, Michael, and Martin Weber (2001), “What Determines the Shape of the Probability Weighting Function under Uncer- tainty?” Management Science, 47 (December), 1712–26. Kluger, Avraham N., Elena Stephan, Yoav Ganzach, and Meirav Hershkovitz (2004), “The Effect of Regulatory Focus on the Shape of Probability-Weighting Function: Evidence from a Cross-Modality Matching Method,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95 (1), 20–39. Kogut, Tehila, and Ilana Ritov (2005), “The Singularity Effect of Identified Victims in Separate and Joint Evaluations,” Or- ganizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97 (April), 106–16. Kunreuther, Howard, Nathan Novemsky, and Daniel Kahneman (2001), “Making Low Probabilities Useful,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 23 (September), 103–20. Laibson, David (1997), “Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discount- ing,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112 (May), 443–77. Laran, Juliano, and Keith Wilcox (2011), “Choice, Rejection, and Elaboration on Preference-Inconsistent Alternatives,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (August), 229–41. Lichtenstein, Sarah, and Paul Slovic (1971), “Reversals of Pref- erence between Bids and Choices in Gambling Decisions,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89 (1), 46–55. List, John A. (2002), “Preference Reversals of a Different Kind: The ‘More Is Less’ Phenomenon,” American Economic Re- view, 92 (December), 1636–43. Loewenstein, George (1996), “Out of Control: Visceral Influences on Behavior,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65 (March), 272–92. Malkoc, Selin A., and Gal Zauberman (2006), “Deferring versus Expediting Consumption: The Effect of Outcome Concrete- ness on Sensitivity to Time Horizon,” Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (November), 618–27. Mischel, Walter, Yuichi Shoda, and Monica L. Rodriguez (1989), “Delay of Gratification in Children,” Science, 244 (May), 933–38. Moore, Don A. (1999), “Order Effects in Preference Judgments: Evidence for Context Dependence in the Generation of Pref- erences,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- cesses, 78 (May), 146–65. Nowlis, Stephen M., and Itamar Simonson (1997), “Attribute-Task Compatibility as a Determinant of Consumer Preference Re- versals,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34 (May), 205–18. Okada, Erica Mina (2005), “Justification Effects on Consumer Choice of Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods,” Journal of Mar- keting Research, 42 (February), 43–53. Paharia, Neeru, Karim S. Kassam, Joshua D. Greene, and Max H. Bazerman (2009), “Dirty Work, Clean Hands: The Moral Psy- chology of Indirect Agency,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109 (July), 134–41. Palmeira, Mauricio M. (2011), “The Zero-Comparison Effect,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (June), 16–26. Prelec, Drazen (1998), “The Probability Weighting Function,” Econometrica, 66 (May), 497–527. Prelec, Drazen, and George Loewenstein (1991), “Decision Mak- ing over Time and under Uncertainty: A Common Approach,” Management Science, 37 (July), 770–86. Rachlin, Howard, Jay Brown, and David Cross (2000), “Discount- ing in Judgments of Delay and Probability,” Journal of Be- havioral Decision Making, 13 (2), 145–59. Read, Daniel (2001), “Is Time-Discounting Hyperbolic or Sub- additive?” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 23 (July), 5–32. Read, Daniel, Shane Frederick, Burcu Orsel, and Juwaria Rahman (2005), “Four Score and Seven Years from Now: The Data/ Delay Effect in Temporal Discounting,” Management Science, 51 (September), 1326–35. Read, Daniel, and N. L. Read (2004), “Time Discounting over the Lifespan,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94 (May), 22–32. Schmeltzer, Christopher, Jean-Paul Caverni, and Massimo War- glien (2004), “How Does Preference Reversal Appear and Disappear? Effects of the Evaluation Mode,” Journal of Be- havioral Decision Making, 17 (December), 395–408. Scholten, Marc, and Daniel Read (2006), “Discounting by Inter- vals: A Generalized Model of Intertemporal Choice,” Man- agement Science, 52 (September), 1424–36. Shamosh, Noah A., Colin G. DeYoung, Adam E. Green, Deidre L. Reis, Matthew R. Johnson, Andrew R. A. Conway, Randall W. Engle, Todd S. Braver, and Jeremy R. Gray (2008), “In- dividual Differences in Delay Discounting,” Psychological Science, 19 (September), 904–11. Shampanier, Kristina, Nina Mazar, and Dan Ariely (2007), “Zero as a Special Price: The True Value of Free Products,” Mar- keting Science, 26 (November), 742–57. Shen, Luxi, Christopher K. Hsee, Qingsheng Wu, and Claire I. Tsai (2012), “Overpredicting and Underprofiting in Pricing Decisions,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25 (De- cember), 512–21. Slovic, Paul (1987), “Perception of Risk,” Science, 236 (April), 280–85. Tversky, Amos, Shmuel Sattath, and Paul Slovic (1988), “Contin- gent Weighting in Judgment and Choice,” Psychological Re- view, 95 (July), 371–84. Van den Bergh, Bram, Siegfried Dewitte, and Luk Warlop (2008), “Bikinis Instigate Generalized Impatience in Intertemporal Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (June), 85–97. Weber, Bethany J., and Gretchen B. Chapman (2005), “Playing for Peanuts: Why Is Risk Seeking More Common for Low-Stakes Gambles?” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97 (May), 31–46. Zauberman, Gal, B. K. Kim, Selin A. Malkoc, and James R. Bett- man (2009), “Discounting Time and Time Discounting: Sub- jective Time Perception and Intertemporal Preferences,” Jour- nal of Marketing Research, 46 (August), 543–56. Zauberman, Gal, and John G. Lynch Jr. (2005), “Resource Slack and Propensity to Discount Delayed Investments of Time ver- sus Money,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134 (October), 23–37.
  237. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer L. (1997), "Dimensions of Brand Personality," Journal of Marketing Research, 34 (3), 347- 56. Aron, Arthur, Elaine N. Aron, Michael Tudor, and Greg Nelson (1991), "Close Relationships as Including Other in the Self," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (2), 241-53. Beggan, James K. (1992), "On the Social Nature of Nonsocial Perception - the Mere Ownership Effect," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 (2), 229-37. Belk, Russell W. (1988), "Possessions and the Extended Self," Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 139-68. Bless, Herbert and Norbert Schwarz (2010), "Mental Construal and the Emergence of Assimilation and Contrast Effects: The Inclusion/Exclusion Model," in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 42, San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press Inc, 319-73. Brewer, Marilynn B. (1991), "The Social Self - on Being the Same and Different at the Same Time," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17 (5), 475-82. Brewer, Marilynn B. and W. Gardner (1996), "Who Is This ''We''? Levels of Collective Identity and Self Representations," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (1), 83-93. Bruner, Jerome S. (1957), "On Perceptual Readiness," Psychological Review, 64 (2), 123-52. Cadinu, Maria R. and Myron Rothbart (1996), "Self-Anchoring and Differentiation Processes in the Minimal Group Setting," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (4), 661-77. Cunningham, Shelia J., David J. Turk, Lynda M. Macdonald, and C. Neil Macrae (2008), "Yours or Mine? Ownership and Memory," Consciousness and Cognition, 17 (1), 312-18. Dunning, David and Andrew F. Hayes (1996), "Evidence for Egocentric Comparison in Social Judgment," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (2), 213-29. Duval, T. Shelly and Robert A Wicklund (1972), A Theory of Objective Self-Awareness, New York: Academic Press. Edney, Julian J. (1974), "Human Territoriality," Psychological Bulletin, 81 (12), 959-75. Fazio, R. H., C. J. Williams, and M. C. Powell (2000), "Measuring Associative Strength: Category-Item Associations and Their Activation from Memory," Political Psychology, 21 (1), 7-25. Fenigstein, Allan, Michael F. Scheier, and Arnold H. Buss (1975), "Public and Private Self-Consciousness - Assessment and Theory " Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43 (4), 522-27. Ferraro, Rosellina., Jennifer E. Escalas, and James R. Bettman (2011), "Our Possessions, Our Selves: Domains of Self-Worth and the Possession-Self Link," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21 (2), 169-77. Fitzsimons, Gavan J. (2008), "Death to Dichotomizing," Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 5-8. Foroni, Francesco. and Myron Rothbart (2011), "Category Boundaries and Category Labels: When Does a Category Name Influence the Perceived Similarity of Category Members?," Social Cognition, 29 (5), 547-76. Franke, Nikolaus, Martin Schreier, and Ulrike Kaiser (2010), "The "I Designed It Myself" Effect in Mass Customization," Management Science, 56 (1), 125-40. Furby, Lita (1978), "Possession in Humans - Exploratory-Study of Its Meaning and Motivation," Social Behavior and Personality, 6 (1), 49-65. Furby, Lita (1980), "The Origins and Early Development of Possessive Behavior," Political Psychology, 2 (1), 30-42. Gawronski, bertram, Galen V. Bodenhausen, and Rainer Banse (2005), "We Are, Therefore They Aren't: Ingroup Construal as a Standard of Comparison for Outgroup Judgments," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41 (5), 515-26. Gawronski, bertram, Galen V. Bodenhausen, and Andrew P. Becker (2007), "I Like It, Because I Like Myself: Associative Self-Anchoring and Post-Decisional Change of Implicit Evaluations," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43 (2), 221-32. Gibbons, Frederick X. (1990), "Self-Attention and Behavior: A Review and Theoretical Update," Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 249-303. Goffman, Erving (1972), Rlations in Pubic, New York: Harper and Row. Goldstone, Robert L., Yvonne. Lippa, and Richard M. Shiffrin (2001), "Altering Object Representations through Category Learning," Cognition, 78 (1), 27-43. Grohmann, Bianca (2009), "Gender Dimensions of Brand Personality," Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (1), 105-19. Herr, Paul M., Steven J. Sherman, and Russell H. Fazio (1983), "On the Consequences of Priming: Assimilation and Contrast Effects," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19 (4), 323-40. Higgins, E. T. (1987), "Self-Discrepancy - a Theory Relating Self and Affect," Psychological Review, 94 (3), 319-40. Higgins, E. T., G. A. King, and G. H. Mavin (1982), "Individual Construct Accessibility and Subjective Impressions and Recall," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43 (1), 35-47. Higgins, E. Tory (1996), "Knowledge Activation: Accessibility, Applicability and Salience," in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, ed. E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Kruglanski, New York: Guilford Press. Higgins, E. Tory, William S. Rholes, and Carl R. Jones (1977), "Category Accessibility and Impression- Formation," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13 (2), 141-54. Huang, Yunhui H., Lei Wang, and Junqi Shi (2009), "When Do Objects Become More Attractive? The Individual and Interactive Effects of Choice and Ownership on Object Evaluation," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35 (6), 713-22. James, William (1890), The Principles of Psychology Vol. 1, New York: Henry Holt. Johar, Gita Venkataramani, Jaideep Sengupta, and Jennifer L. Aaker (2005), "Two Roads to Updating Brand Personality Impressions: Trait Versus Evaluative Inferencing," Journal of Marketing Research, XLII (November), 458–69. Johnson, E. J., G. Haubl, and A. Keinan (2007), "Aspects of Endowment: A Query Theory of Value Construction," Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 33 (3), 461- 74. Kahneman, Daniel, Jack L. Knetsch, and Richard H. Thaler (1991), "Anomalies - the Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status-Quo Bias," Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5 (1), 193-206. Kardes, Frank R. , Steven S. Posavac, and Maria L. Cronley (2004), "Consumer Inference: A Review of Processes, Bases, and Judgment Contexts," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (3), 230–56. Kleine, Suzan S., Robert E. Kleine, and Chris T. Allen (1995), "How Is a Possession ''Me'' or ''Not Me''? Characterizing Types and an Antecedent of Material Possession Attachment," Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (3), 327-43. Lingle, John H., Mark W. Altom, and Douglas L. Medin (1984), "Of Cabbages and Kings: Assessing the Extendibility of Natural Object Concept. Models to Social Things," in Handbook of Social Cognition, ed. R.S. Wyer and T.K. Srull Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Muller, Dominique., Charles M. Judd, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2005), "When Moderation Is Mediated and Mediation Is Moderated," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 852-63. Mussweiler, Thomas and Galen V. Bodenhausen (2002), "I Know You Are, but What Am I? Self- Evaluative Consequences of Judging in-Group and out-Group Members," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82 (1), 19-32. Otten, Sabine and Dirk Wentura (2001), "Self-Anchoring and in-Group Favoritism: An Individual Profiles Analysis," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37 (6), 525-32. Parker, Jeffery R. and Rom Y. Schrift (2011), "Rejectable Choice Sets: How Seemingly Irrelevant No- Choice Options Affect Consumer Decision Processes," Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (5), 840- 54. Peck, Joann and Suzanne B. Shu (2009), "The Effect of Mere Touch on Perceived Ownership," Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (3), 434-47. Penton-Voak, I. S. and J. Y. Chen (2004), "High Salivary Testosterone Is Linked to Masculine Male Facial Appearance in Humans," Evolution and Human Behavior, 25 (4), 229-41. Pham, Michel Tuan, Caroline Goukens, Donald R. Lehmann, and Jennifer Ames Stuart (2010), "Shaping Customer Satisfaction through Self-Awareness Cues," Journal of Marketing Research, 47 (5). Pierce, Jon L., Tatiana Kostova, and Kurt T. Dirks (2003), "The State of Psychological Ownership: Integrating and Extending a Century of Research," Review of General Psychology, 7 (1), 84-107. Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew F. Hayes (2004), "Spss and Sas Procedures for Estimating Indirect Effects in Simple Mediation Models," Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36 (4), 717-31. Rogers, Timothy B., Nicholas A. Kuiper, and W. S. Kirker (1977), "Self-Reference and Encoding of Personal Information," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (9), 677-88. Schultheiss, O.C. and S.J. Stanton (2009), "Assessment of Salivary Hormones," in Methods in the Neurobiology of Social and Personality Psychology, ed. E. Harmon-Jones and J. S. Beer, New York, NY: Guilford, 17–44. Schwarz, Norbert, Herbert Bless, Fritz Strack, Gisela Klumpp, Helga Rittenauerschatka, and Annette Simons (1991), " Ease of Retrieval as Information - Another Look at the Availability Heuristic," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (2), 195-202. Seligman, Martin E. P. (1975), Helplessness, San Francisco: Freeman. Sellers, Jennifer G., Matthias R. Mehl, and Robert A. Josephs (2007), "Hormones and Personality: Testosterone as a Marker of Individual Differences," Journal of Research in Personality, 41 (1), 126-38. Singelis, Theodore M. (1994), "The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (5), 580-91. Srull, Thomas K. and Robert S. Wyer, Jr. (1979), "Role of Category Accessibility in the Interpretation of Information About Persons - Some Determinants and Implications," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (10), 1660-72. Sujan, Mita and Christine Dekleva (1987), "Product Categorization and Inference Making: Some Implications for Comparative Advertising," The Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (3), 372-78. Tajfel, Henri, Michael G. Billig, Roberet P. Bundy, and C. Flament (1971), "Social Categorization and Intergroup Behavior," European Journal of Social Psychology, 1 (2), 149-77. Turk, David J., Kim van Bussel, Gordon D. Waiter, and C. Neil Macrae (2011), "Mine and Me: Exploring the Neural Basis of Object Ownership," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (Early Access), 1-12. Watson, David, Lee Anna Clark, and Auke Tellegen (1988), "Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The Panas Scales," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (6), 1063-70. Zhao, Xinshu, John G. Lynch, Jr., and Qimei Chen (2010), "Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths About Mediation Analysis," Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (2), 197-206.
  238. References Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 267–299). New York: Academic Press. Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably irrational. New York: Harper Collins. Ariely, D. (2009). The end of rational economics. Harvard Business Review, 87, 78–84. Baker, W. E., & Jimerson, J. B. (1992). The sociology of money. American Behavioral Scientist, 35, 678–693. Banaji, M., Bazerman, M., & Chugh, D. (2003). How (un)ethical are you? Harvard Business Review, 81(12), 56–64. Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. New York: Guilford Press. Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 147–168. Bradley, J. C., Brief, A. P., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). The good corporation. In D. B. Smith (Ed.), The people make the place. Exploring dynamic linkages between individuals and organizations (pp. 175–223). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Butterfield, K. D., Treviño, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2000). Moral awareness in business organizations: Influences of issue-related and social context factors. Human Relations, 53, 981–1018. Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1996). Life values and adolescent mental health. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Coleman, J. S. (1974). Power and the structure of society. New York: Norton. Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Dante (1985). Inferno (M. Musa, Trans.). In The divine comedy (Vol. 1). New York: Penguin (Original work circa the 14th century). Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Sweitzer, V. L. (2008). Moral disengagement in ethical decision making: A study of antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 374–391. de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. de Waal, F. (2006). Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Doyle, K. O. (1992). Toward a psychology of money. American Behavioral Scientist, 35, 708–724. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of human relations. New York: Free Press. Fiske, A. P. (1992). Four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723. Giddens, A. (1990). The consequences of modernity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Gino, F., Norton, M., & Ariely, D. (2010). The counterfeit self: The deceptive costs of faking it. Psychological Science, 21(5), 712–720. Gino, F., & Pierce, L. (2009). The abundance effect: Unethical behavior in the presence of wealth. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(2), 142–155. Gioia, D. A. (1992). Pinto fires and personal ethics: A script analysis of missed opportunities. Journal of Business Ethics, 11, 379–391. Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: The role of consequences. American Economic Review, 95(1), 384–394. Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517–523. Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127. Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316, 998–1002. Haidt, J. (2008). Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 65–72. Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 797–832). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. Hegarty, W. H., & Sims, H. P. Jr., (1978). Some determinants of unethical decision behavior: An experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(4), 451–457. Heyman, J., & Ariely, D. (2004). Effort for payment: A tale of two markets. Psychological Science, 15, 787–793. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. New York: Guilford. Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S., & Hepp, S. (2009). Proscriptive versus prescriptive morality: Two faces of moral regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 521–537. Kant, I. (1964). Foundation of the metaphysics of morals (Lewis White Beck, Trans.) Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill (Original work published 1785). Kasser, T., Cohn, S., Kanner, A. D., & Ryan, R. M. (2007). Some costs of American corporate capitalism: A psychological exploration of value and goal conflicts. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 1–22. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422. Kay, A., & Ross, L. (2003). The perceptual push: The interplay of implicit cues and explicit situational construals on behavioral intentions in the Prisoner’s Dilemma. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 634–643. Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 83–96. Lea, S. E. G., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 161–209. Levine, D. (1985). The fight from ambiguity: Essays in social and cultural theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Liu, J., Smeesters, D., & Vohs, K. (2012). Reminders of money elicit feelings of threat and reactance in response to social influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 1030–1046. Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., Murnane, T., Vaes, J., Reynolds, C., & Suitner, C. (2010). Objectification leads to depersonalization: The denial of mind and moral concern to objectified others. European Journal of Social Psychology 709–717. MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614. Marx, K. (1964). The economic and philosophic manuscripts of 1844 (M. Milligan, Trans.). New York: International Publishers. McCabe, D. L., & Treviño, L. K. (1995). Cheating among business students: A challenge for business leaders and educators. The Journal of Management Education, 19(2), 205–218. Maurer, B. (2006). The anthropology of money. Annual Review of Anthropology, 35, 15–36. Messick, D. M. (1999). Alternative logics for decision making in social settings. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 39(1), 11–30. Mitchell, T. R., & Mickel, A. E. (1999). The meaning of money: An individual difference perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 568–578. Mogilner, C. (2010). The pursuit of happiness: Time, money, and social connection. Psychological Science, 21, 1348–1354. Mogilner, C., & Aaker, J. (2009). The time vs. money effect: Shifting product attitudes and decisions through personal connection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 277–291. Pillutla, M. M., & Chen, X.-P. (1999). Social norms and cooperation in social dilemmas: The effects of context and feedback. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78(2), 81–103. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, 57–75. Reed, A., II, & Aquino, K. (2003). Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral regard toward out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1270–1286. Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York, NY: Praeger. Reynolds, S. J., Leavitt, K., & DeCelles, K. A. (2010). Automatic ethics: Implicit assumptions and ethical managerial conduct. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 752–760. Simmel, G. (1978). The philosophy of money (T. Bottomore & D. Frisby, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul (Original work published 1907). Singer, P. (1981). The expanding circle: Ethics and sociobiology. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux. Solomon, R. C. (1994). Business and the humanities: An Aristotelian approach to business ethics. In T. Donaldson & R. E. Freeman (Eds.), Business as humanity (pp. 45–75). New York: Oxford University. Tang, T. L. P. (1992). The meaning of money revisited. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 197–202. Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (1999). Sanctioning systems, decision frames, and cooperation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 684–707. Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). Ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. Academy of Management Annals, 2, 545–607. Tenbrunsel, A. E., Smith-Crowe, K., & Umphress, E. E. (2003). Building houses on rocks: The role of the ethical infrastructure in organizations. Social Justice Research, 16(3), 285–307. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314, 1154–1156. Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2008). Merely activating the concept of money changes personal and interpersonal behavior. Current Perspectives in Psychological Science, 17(3), 208–212. Wang, L., Malhotra, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2011). Economics education and greed. The Academy of Management Learning and Education, 10, 643–660. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). The symbolic power of money: Reminders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6), 700–706.
  239. References Amato, P. R., & Rogers, S. J. (1997). A longitudinal study of marital problems and subsequent divorce. Journal of Marriage and Family, 59(3), 612 – 624. Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 670 – 676. Berry, C. J. (1994). The idea of luxury: a conceptual and historical investigation. New York: Cambridge University Press. Briers, B., Pandelaere, M., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2006). Hungry for money: the desire for caloric resources increases the desire for financial resources and vice versa. Psychological Science, 17(11), 939 – 943. Burgoyne, C. B., & Lea, S. E. G. (2006). Money is material. Science, 314(5802), 1091 – 1092. Chatterjee, P., & Rose, R. L. (2012). Do payment mechanisms change the way consumers perceive products? Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 1129 – 1139. Chatterjee, P., Rose, R. L., & Sinha, J. (2012). Why money meanings matter in decisions to donate time and money. Marketing Letters, 21(2), 1 – 10. Chernev, A. (2004). Goal orientation and consumer preference for status quo. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3), 557 – 565. Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2000). Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 37(1), 60 – 71. Feinberg, R. A. (1986). Credit cards as spending facilitating stimuli: a conditioning interpretation. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(3), 348 – 356. Hansen, J., Kutzner, F., & Wänke, M. (2013). Money and thinking: reminders of money trigger abstract construal and shape consumer judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(6). Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280 – 1300. Higgins, E. T., Roney, C. J. R., Crowe, E., & Hymes, C. (1994). Ideal versus ought predilections for approach and avoidance: distinct self-regulatory systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(2), 276 – 286. Hirschman, E. C., & Holbrook, M. B. (1982). Hedonic consumption: emerging concepts, methods, and propositions. Journal of Marketing, 46(3), 92 – 101. Keller, P., Lee, A.Y., & Sternthal.B. (2004). Construing fit to judgment: the effects of regulatory focus and level of Construal. Working paper, Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University. Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2005) A behavioral decision theoretic perspective on hedonic and utilitarian choice. In Ratneshwar, S. & Mick, D. G. (Eds.), Inside consumption: consumer motives, goals, and desires (pp. 144 – 165). London: Routledge. Kivetz, R., & Zheng, Y. (2006). Determinants of justification and self-control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 135(4), 572 – 587. Liu, W., & Aaker, J. (2008). The happiness of giving: the time-ask effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), 543 – 557. Mandel, N., & Johnson, E. J. (2002). When web pages influence choice: effects of visual primes on experts and novices. Journal of Consumer Research, 29(2), 235 – 245. Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: Harper and Row. Mogilner, C. (2010). The pursuit of happiness: time, money, and social connections. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1348 – 1354. Mogilner, C., & Aaker, J. (2009). The time vs. money effect: shifting product attitudes and decisions through personal connection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(2), 277 – 291. Okada, E. M. (2005). Justification effects on consumer choice of hedonic and utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(1), 43 – 53. Pennington, G. L., & Roese, N. J. (2003). Regulatory focus and temporal distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 563 – 576. Pham, M. T., & Avnet, T. (2004). Ideals and thoughts and the reliance on affect versus substance in persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(4), 503 – 518. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879 – 891. Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1998). The red and the black: mental accounting of savings and debt. Marketing Science, 17(1), 4 – 28. Prelec, D., & Simester, D. (2001). Always leave home without it: a further investigation of the credit-card effect on willingness to pay. Marketing Letters, 12(1), 5 – 12. Safer, D. (1998). Preference for luxurious or reliable products: promotion and prevention focus as moderators. Unpublished Dissertation, Columbia University, New York Sengupta, J., & Zhou, R. (2007). Understanding impulsive eaters ’ choice behaviors: the motivational influences of regulatory focus. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(2), 297 – 308. Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: the interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278 – 292. Soman, D. (2001). Effects of payment mechanism on spending behavior: the role of rehearsal and immediacy of payments. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(4), 460 – 474. Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314 (5802), 1154 – 1156. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197 – 206. Zhou, R., & Pham, M. T. (2004). Promotion and prevention across mental accounts: when financial products dictate consumers ’ investment goals. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 125 – 135. Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). The symbolic power of money: reminders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6), 700 – 706.
  240. REFERENCES Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82(4), 463-509. Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics: The interaction of successive motivational states within the individual. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Ainslie, G., & Haendel, V. (1983). The motives of the will. In E. Gottheil, K. A. Druley, T. E. Skoloda, & H. M. Waxman (Eds.), Etiologic aspects of alcohol and drug abuse (pp. 119-140). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Angeletos, G., Laibson, D., Repetto, A., Tobacman, J., & Weinberg, S. (2001). The hyperbolic consumption model: Calibration, simulation, and empirical evaluation. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15(3), 48-68. Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: Self-control by precommitment. Psychological Science, 13(3), 219-224. Ashraf, N., Karlan, D., & Yin, W. (2006). Tying Odysseus to the mast: Evidence from a commitment savings product in the Philippines. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(2), 635-672. Bazerman, M. H., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Wade-Benzoni, K. (1998). Negotiating with yourself and losing: Making decisions with competing internal preferences. Academy of Management Review, 23(2), 225-241. Bartels, D. M., & Rips, L. J. (2010). Psychological connectedness and intertemporal choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 139(1), 49-69. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265. Bell, C. (2013, February 11). Inside the Coalition’s controversial ‘nudge unit’. The Telegraph. Retrieved from http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/politics/9853384/Inside-the-Coalitions- controversial-Nudge-Unit.html Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., Madrian, B., & Sakong, J. (2011). Self control and liquidity: How to design a commitment contract. RAND Corporation Publications Department Working Papers. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. C. (2013). Planning prompts as a means of increasing rates of immunization and preventive screening. Public Policy and Aging Report, 22(4), 16-19. Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Staats, B. R. (2014). A day in the saddle can take its toll: The impact of accumulated time, work intensity, and work breaks on hand hygiene compliance. Working Paper. Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., & Riis, J. (In press). The fresh start effect: Temporal landmarks motivate aspirational behavior. Management Science. Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., & Riis, J. (2014). Put your imperfections behind you: Why and how meaningful temporal landmarks motivate aspirational behavior. Working Paper. Danziger, S., Levav, J., & Avnaim-Pesso, L. (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17), 6889-6892. DellaVigna, S., & Malmendier, U. (2006). Paying not to go to the gym. The American Economic Review, 96(3), 694-719. Festinger, L. (1962). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Finkelstein, E. A., Trogdon, J. G., Cohen, J. W., & Dietz, W. (2009). Annual medical spending attributable to obesity: Payer- and service-specific estimates. Health Affairs, 28(5), 822- 831. Fedorikhin, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2010). Positive mood and resistance to temptation: The interfering influence of elevated arousal. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 698-711. Fudenberg, D., & Levine, D. (2006). A dual self model of impulse control. American Economic Review, 96(5), 1449-1476. Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 351-367. Giné, X., Karlan, D., & Zinman, J. (2010). Put your money where your butt is: A commitment contract for smoking cessation. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 2, 213- 235. Giner-Sorolla, R. (2001). Guilty pleasures and grim necessities: Affective attitudes in dilemmas of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 206-221. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493-503. Hershfield, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W. F., Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., & Bailenson, J. N. (2011). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, S23-S37. Higgins, E. T. (1996). The “self digest”: Self-knowledge serving self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1062-1083. Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R.F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K.D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1318-1335. Houser, D., Schunk, D., Winter, J., & Xiao, E. (2010). Temptation and commitment in the laboratory. Working Paper Series. Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, University of Zurich. Kahneman, D., & Ritov, I. (1994). Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A study in the headline method. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 9(1), 5-37. Kavanagh, D.J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112(2), 446-467. Kaur, S., Kremer, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2010). Self-control and the development of work arrangements. The American Economic Review, 100(2), 624-628. Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2006). Licensing effect in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(2), 259-266. Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2007). Where there is a way, is there a will? The effect of future choices on current preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 136(2), 277-288. King, G. R., & Logue, A. W. (1987). Choice in a self-control paradigm with human subjects: Effects of changeover delay duration. Learning and Motivation, 18(4), 421-438. Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 126(1), 54-70. Kirby, K. N., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed rewards. Psychological Science, 6(2), 83-89. Kivetz, R., & Keinan, A. (2006). Repenting hyperopia: An analysis of self-control regrets. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(2), 273-282. Kivetz, R., & Simonson, I. (2002). Earning the right to indulge: Effort as a determinant of customer preferences toward frequency program rewards. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(2), 155-170. Labroo, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2008). Why happiness helps you see the big picture. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 800-809. Laibson, D. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 444-477. Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1250-1267. Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. U. (2013). The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science, 24(1), 72-79. Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. (1965). Effects of fear and specificity of recommendation upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2(1), 20-29. Liberman, N., Sagristano, M., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 523-535. Loewenstein, L. K. (1996). Streets of San Francisco: The origins of street and place names (3rd Ed.). Berkeley, CA: Wilderness Press. Loewenstein, G. F., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 573-597. McClure, S. M., Laibson, D., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503-507. Milkman, K. L. (2012). Unsure what the future will bring? You may overindulge: Uncertainty increases the appeal of wants over shoulds. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119(2), 163-176. Milkman, K. L., Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. C. (2011). Using implementation intentions prompts to enhance influenza vaccination rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(26), 10415-10420. Milkman, K. L., Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. C. (2013). Planning prompts as a means of increasing preventive screening rates. Preventive Medicine 56(1), 92-93. Milkman, K. L., Chugh, D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). How can decision making be improved? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4), 379-383. Milkman, K., Minson, J., & Volpp, K. (2014). Holding the Hunger Games hostage at the gym: An evaluation of temptation bundling. Management Science, 60(2), 283-299. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Harnessing our inner angels and demons: What we have learned about want/should conflicts and how that knowledge can help us reduce short-sighted decision making. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(4), 324-338. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Highbrow films gather dust: Time- inconsistent preferences and online DVD rentals. Management Science, 55(6), 1047- 1059. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T. & Bazerman, M. H. (2010). I'll have the ice cream soon and the vegetables later: A study of online grocery purchases and order lead time. Marketing Letters, 21(1), 17-36. Muraven, M. (2010). Practicing self-control lowers the risk of smoking lapse. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(3), 446-452. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247-259. O'Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (1999). Doing it now or later. The American Economic Review, 89(1), 103-124. Oster, S., & Scott Morton, F. M. (2005). Behavioral biases meet the market: The case of magazine subscription prices. Berkeley Electronic Journals Economic Analysis and Policy, Advances, 5(1), 1-32. Prelec, D., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1991). Preferences or principles: Alternative guidelines for choice, in Zeckhauser, R. J. (Ed.), The strategy and choice (pp. 319-340), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pyone, J. S., & Isen, A. M. (2011). Positive affect, intertemporal choice, and levels of thinking: Increasing consumers' willingness to wait. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3), 532- 543. Read, D. (2001). Intrapersonal dilemmas. Human Relations, 54(8), 1093-1117. Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Kalyanaraman, S. (1999). Mixing virtue with vice: Combining the immediacy effect and the diversification heuristic. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(4), 257-273. Robinson, J. A. (1986). Temporal reference systems and autobiographical memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 159-188). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Future lock-in: Future implementation increases selection of ‘should’ choices. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 106(1), 1-20. Rogers, T., Milkman, K., John, L., & Norton, M. I. (2013). Making the best laid plans better: How plan-making increases follow-through. Working Paper. Rogers, T., Milkman, K. L., & Volpp, K. G. M. (2014). Harnessing commitment devices for health behavior change. Working Paper. Royer, H., Stehr, M. F., & Sydnor, J. R. (2012). Incentives, commitments and habit formation in exercise: Evidence from a field experiment with workers at a Fortune-500 company (No. w18580). National Bureau of Economic Research. Schelling, T. C. (1984). Choice and consequence: Perspectives of an errant economist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Schroeder, S. A. (2007). Shattuck lecture: We can do better—Improving the health of the American people. New England Journal of Medicine, 357(12), 1221-1228. Schwartz, J., Riis, J., Elbel, B., & Ariely, D. (2012). Inviting consumers to downsize fast-food portions significantly reduces calorie consumption. Health Affairs, 31(2), 399-407. Sen, A. K. (1977). Rational fools: A critique of the behavioral foundations of economic theory. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 6(4), 317-344. Shefrin, H. M., & Thaler, R. H. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry, 26(4), 609-643. Shiv, B. & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278-292. Shum, M. S. (1998). The role of temporal landmarks in autobiographical memory processes. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 423-442. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Advancing the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 701-717. Strotz, R. H. (1956). Myopia and inconsistency in dynamic utility maximization. Review of Economic Studies, 23(3), 165-180. Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8(3), 201-207. Thaler, R. H., & Benartzi, S. (2004). Save more tomorrow™: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112(S1), S164-S187. Thaler, R. H., & Shefrin, H. (1981). An economic theory of self control. Journal of Political Economy, 89(2), 392-406. Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(3), 379-384. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403- 421. Wertenbroch, K. (1998). Consumption self-control by rationing purchase quantities of virtue and vice. Marketing Science, 17(4), 317-337.
  241. References Adkins, Natalie Ross and Julie L. Ozanne (2005), “The Low Liter- ate Consumer,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (June), 93–105. Ajzen, Icek and Martin Fishbein (1980), Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Alexander, Gordon J., Jonathan D. Jones, and Peter J. Nigro (1998), “Mutual Fund Shareholders: Characteristics, Investor Knowledge and Sources of Information,” Financial Services Review, 7 (4), 301–316. American Payroll Association (2006), “APA Pay Media Survey Shows Direct Deposit Use Is on the Increase: 56% of Employ- ees Now Receive Pay Electronically,” (accessed February 2, 2006), [available at http://www.americanpayroll.org/new55. html]. Anderson, Elijah (1999), Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral Life of the Inner City. New York: W.W. Norton. Asch, Solomon E. (1956), “Studies of Independence and Confor- mity: A Minority of One Against a Unanimous Majority,” Psy- chological Monographs, 70 (9), 1–70. Association of Community Organizations for Reform Now (2006), “Predatory Lending Practices,” (accessed February 2, 2006), [available at http://www.acorn.org/index.php?id=754]. Ausubel, Lawrence M. (1991), “The Failure of Competition in the Credit Card Market,” American Economic Review, 81 (1), 50–81. Banks, Sara M., Peter Salovey, Susan Greener, Alexander Roth- man, Anne Moyer, John Beauvais, and Elissa Epel (1995), “The Effects of Message Framing on Mammography Utilization,” Health Psychology, 14 (2), 178–84. Barr, Michael S. (2004a), “Banking the Poor,” Yale Journal on Regulation, 21, 121–237. ——— (2004b), “Banking the Poor: Policies to Bring Low- Income Americans into the Financial Mainstream,” The Brook- ings Institution Research Brief (September). Benartzi, Shlomo and Richard H. Thaler (2004), “Save More Tomorrow: Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Employee Saving,” Journal of Political Economy, 112 (1), 164–87. Bertrand, Marianne, Dean Karlan, Sendhil Mullainathan, Eldar Shafir, and Jonathan Zinman (2005), “What’s Psychology Worth? A Field Experiment in the Consumer Credit Market,” Working Paper No. 11892, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA. ———, Sendhil Mullainathan, and Eldar Shafir, (2004), “A Behavioral-Economics View of Poverty,” American Economic Review, 94 (2), 419–23. Beverly, Sondra, Daniel Schneider, and Peter Tufano, (2005), “Splitting Tax Refunds and Building Savings: An Empirical Test,” Harvard Business School Finance Working Paper No. 06- 018. Botti, Simona and Sheena S. Iyengar (2006), “The Dark Side of Choice: When Choice Impairs Social Welfare,” Journal of Pub- lic Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 24–38. Briley, Donnel and Jennifer Aaker (2006), “Bridging the Culture Chasm: Ensuring That Consumers Are Healthy, Wealthy, and Wise,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 53–66. Buehler, Roger, Dale Griffin, and Michael Ross (1994), “Explor- ing the ‘Planning Fallacy’: Why People Under-Estimate Their Task Completion Times,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (September), 366–81. Caskey, John P. (1996), Fringe Banking: Check-Cashing Outlets, Pawnshops and the Poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Cialdini, Robert B. (2001), Influence: Science and Practice, 4th ed. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. ——— (2003), “Crafting Normative Messages to Protect the Envi- ronment,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12 (4), 105–109. Cohen, Geoffrey L. and Claude M. Steele (2002), “A Barrier of Mistrust: How Negative Stereotypes Affect Cross-Race Mentor- ing,” in Improving Academic Achievement: Impact of Psycho- logical Factors on Education, Joshua Aronson, ed. San Diego: Academic Press, 303–327. Consumer Reports (2004), “Financial Services: Don’t Get Taken by Hidden Fees,” (May), (accessed February 2, 2006), [avail- able at http://cdn.consumerreports.org/cro/personal-finance/ financial-services-hidden-fees-504/overview.htm]. Crocker, Jennifer, Brenda Major, and Claude M. Steele (1998), “Social Stigma,” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 2, Daniel T. Gilbert, Susan T. Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey, eds. New York: McGraw-Hill, 504–553. Croizet, Jean-Claude and Theresa Claire (1998), “Extending the Concept of Stereotype Threat to Social Class: The Intellectual Underperformance of Students from Low Socioeconomic Back- grounds,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 (6), 588–94. Currie, Janet (2004), “The Take Up of Social Benefits,” Working Paper No. 10488, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cam- bridge, MA. ——— and Jeffrey Grogger (2001), “Explaining Recent Declines in Food Stamp Program Participation,” in Brookings Papers on Urban Affairs, William G. Gale and Janet Rothenberg Pack, eds. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution, 203–244. Darley, John M. and C. Daniel Batson (1973), “From Jerusalem to Jericho: A Study of Situational and Dispositional Variables in Helping Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 27 (1), 100–108. DeParle, Jason (2004), American Dream: Three Women, Ten Kids, and a Nation’s Drive to End Welfare. New York: Viking. Duflo, Esther, William G. Gale, Jeffrey B. Liebman, Peter Orszag, and Emmanuel Saez (2005), “Saving Incentives for Low- and Middle-Income Families: Evidence from a Field Experimentwith H&R Block,” Working Paper No. 11680, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA. Eagly, Alice H. and Shelly Chaiken (1993), The Psychology of Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. Families USA (2005), “Only One Out of Nine Low-Income Seniors Approved for New Medicare Drug Subsidies,” press release, (December 5), [available at www.familiesusa.org]. Fazio, Russell H. (1986), “How Do Attitudes Guide Behavior?” in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, Richard M. Sorrentino and E. Tory Higgins, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 204–243. Federal Trade Commission (2000), “Funerals: A Consumer Guide,” (June), (accessed February 2, 2006), [available at http://www.ftc.gov/bcp/conline/pubs/services/funeral.htm]. Ferraro, Fabrizio, Jeffrey Pfeffer, and Robert I. Sutton (2005), “Economics Language and Assumptions: How Theories Can Become Self-Fulfilling,” Academy of Management Review, 30 (1), 8–24. Gans, Joshua S. (2005), “‘Protecting Consumers by Protecting Competition’: Does Behavioral Economics Support this Con- tention?” Competition and Consumer Law Journal, 13 (1), 40–50. Gollwitzer, Peter M. and Veronika Brandstatter (1997), “Imple- mentation Intentions and Effective Goal Pursuit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (1), 186–99. Higgins, E. Tory (2000), “Making a Good Decision: Value from Fit,” American Psychologist, 55 (11), 1217–30. ———, Lorraine Chen Idson, Antonio L. Freitas, Scott Spiegel, and Daniel C. Molden (2003), “Transfer of Value from Fit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (6), 1140–53. Iyengar, Sheena S., Wei Jiang, and Gur Huberman (2004), “How Much Choice Is Too Much: Determinants of Individual Contri- butions in 401K Retirement Plans,” in Pension Design and Structure: New Lessons from Behavioral Finance, Olivia Mitchell and Stephen Utkus, eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 83–97. ——— and Mark R. Lepper (2000), “When Choice Is Demotivat- ing: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 995–1006. Johnson, Eric. J. and Daniel Goldstein (2003), “Do Defaults Save Lives?” Science, (November 21), 1338–39. ———, John Hershey, Jacqueline Meszaros, and Howard Kun- reuther (1993), “Framing, Probability Distortions, and Insurance Decisions,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 7 (1), 35–51. ——— and Amos Tversky (1983), “Affect, Generalization, and the Perception of Risk,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 45 (1), 20–31. Kahn, Barbara E. and Mary Frances Luce (2006), “Repeated- Adherence Protection Model: ‘I’m OK, and It’s a Hassle,’” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 79–89. Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky (1979), “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk,” Econometrica, 47 (2), 263–91. ——— and ——— (2000), Choices, Values and Frame. Cam- bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Keller, Punam Anand, Isaac M. Lipkus, and Barbara K. Rimer (2003), “Affect, Framing, and Persuasion,” Journal of Market- ing Research, 40 (February), 54–64. Knetsch, Jack L. (1989), “The Endowment Effect and Evidence of Nonreversible Indifference Curves,” American Economic Review, 79 (5), 1277–84. Koehler, Derek J. and Connie S.K. Poon (2006), “Self-Predictions Overweight Strength of Current Intentions,” Journal of Experi- mental Social Psychology, forthcoming. Landsberger, Michael (1966), “Windfall Income and Consump- tion: Comment,” American Economic Review, 56 (3), 534–39. LeBlanc, Adrian N. (2004), Random Family: Love, Drugs, Trou- ble, and Coming of Age in the Bronx. New York: Scribner. LeBoeuf, Robyn A. and Eldar Shafir (2005), “The Conflicting Choices of Alternating Selves,” working paper, Warrington Col- lege of Business, University of Florida. Lepper, Mark R., David Greene, and Richard E. Nisbett (1973), “Undermining Children’s Intrinsic Interest with Extrinsic Reward: A Test of the ‘Overjustification’ Hypothesis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 (1), 129–37. Leventhal, Howard, Robert Singer, and Susan Jones (1965), “Effects of Fear and Specificity of Recommendation upon Atti- tudes and Behaviour,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 2 (2), 20–29. Lewin, Kurt (1951), Field Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper. ——— (1952), Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoreti- cal Papers. London: Tavistock Publications. Liebman, Jeffrey B. and Richard J. Zeckhauser (2004), “Schmed- uling,” working paper, John F. Kennedy School of Government, Harvard University. Lynch, John G., Jr., and Gal Zauberman (2006), “When Do You Want It? Time, Decisions, and Public Policy,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 67–78. Madrian, Brigitte C. and Dennis F. Shea (2001), “The Power of Suggestion: Inertia in 401(k) Participation and Savings Behav- ior,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116 (4), 1149–87. Mendel, Dick (2005), Double Jeopardy: Why the Poor Pay More. Baltimore: The Annie E. Casey Foundation. Milgram, Stanley (1974), Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper and Row. O’Brien, Doug, Kimberly Prendergast, Eleanor Thompson, Mar- cus Fruchter, and Halley Torres Aldeen (2001), “The Red Tape Divide: State-by-State Review of Food Stamp Applications,” report of the Public Policy and Research Department, America’s Second Harvest. Redelmeier, Donald A. and Eldar Shafir (1995), “Medical Deci- sion Making in Situations that Offer Multiple Alternatives,” Journal of the American Medical Association, 273 (4), 302–305. Ross, Lee and Richard E. Nisbett (1991), The Person and the Sit- uation: Perspectives of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Samuelson, William and Richard J. Zeckhauser (1988), “Status Quo Bias in Decision Making,” Journal of Risk and Uncer- tainty, 1 (1), 7–59. Schelling, Thomas C. (1984), “Self-Command in Practice, in Pol- icy, and in Theory of Rational Choice,” American Economic Review, 74 (2), 1–11. Schultz, Ellen E. (1995), “Helpful or Confusing? Fund Choices Multiply for Many Retirement Plans,” The Wall Street Journal, (December 22), C1, C25. Schwartz, J., M. Bertrand, S. Mullainathan, and E. Shafir (2006), “Boosting Program Take-Up: An Experiment with Flexible Spending Accounts,” working paper, The Center for Health and Well-Being, Princeton University. Shafir, Eldar, Itamar Simonson, and Amos Tversky (1993), “Reason-Based Choice,” Cognition, 49 (1–2), 11–36. Shih, Margaret, Todd L. Pittinsky, and Nalini Ambady (1999), “Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and Shifts in Quan- titative Performance,” Psychological Science, 10 (1), 80–83. Steele, Claude M. (1997), “A Threat in the Air: How Stereotypes Shape the Intellectual Identities and Performance of Women and African Americans,” American Psychologist, 52 (6), 613–29. ——— and Joshua Aronson (1995), “Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African Americans,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (5), 797–811. Stein, Robin (2004), “The Ascendancy of the Credit Card Indus- try,” from the FRONTLINE/New York Times report Secret His- tory of the Credit Card, (November 23, 2006), (accessed Febru- ary 2, 2006), [available at http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/ frontline/shows/credit/more/rise/html]. Sunstein, Cass R. and Richard H. Thaler (2003), “Libertarian Paternalism Is Not an Oxymoron,” University of Chicago Law Review, 70 (4), 1159–1202. Sylvan, Louise (2004), “Activating Competition: The Consumer- Competition Interface,” Competition and Consumer Law Jour- nal, 12 (2), 191–206. Thaler, Richard H. (1985), “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,” Marketing Science, 4 (3), 199–214. ——— (1990), “Savings, Fungibility, and Mental Accounts,” Journal of Economic Perspectives, 4 (1), 193–205. ——— (1992), The Winner’s Curse: Paradoxes and Anomalies of Economic Life. New York: The Free Press. ——— (1999), “Mental Accounting Matters,” Journal of Behav- ioral Decision Making, 12 (3), 183–206. Turner, John C. (1987), “A Self-Categorization Theory,” in Redis- covering the Social Group, John C. Turner, Michael A. Hogg, Penelope J. Oakes, Steven D. Reicher, and Margaret S. Wetherell, eds. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 42–67. Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman (1991), “Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference Dependent Model,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106 (4), 1039–1061. ——— and Eldar Shafir (1992), “Choice Under Conflict: The Dynamics of Deferred Decision,” Psychological Science, 3 (6), 358–61. Tyler, Tom R. (2000), “Social Justice: Outcome and Procedure,” International Journal of Psychology, 35 (2), 117–25. Van Dort, Bernice E. and Rudolph H. Moos (1976), “Distance and the Utilization of a Student Health Center,” Journal of American College Health Association, 24 (3), 159–62. Verplanken, Bas and Wendy Wood (2006), “Interventions to Break and Create Consumer Habits,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 90–103. Zanna, Mark P. and Russell H. Fazio (1982), “The Attitude- Behavior Relation: Moving Toward a Third Generation of Research,” in Consistency in Social Behavior: The Ontario Sym- posium, Vol. 2, Mark P. Zanna, E. Tory Higgins, and C. Peter Herman, eds. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 283–301.
  242. REFERENCES Ainslie, George and Nick Haslam (1992), “Self-Control,” in Choice Over Time, G. Loewenstein and J. Elster, eds. New York: Sage Publications, 177–209. Amit, Elinor, Daniel Algom, and Yaacov Trope (2009), “Distance- Dependent Processing of Pictures and Words,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138 (August), 400–415. Bandura, Albert and Karen M. Simon (1977), “The Role of Proxi- mal Intentions in Self-Regulation of Refractory Behavior,” Cog- nitive Therapy and Research, 1 (3), 177–93. Baumeister, Roy F. (2002), “Yielding to Temptation: Self-Control Failure, Impulsive Purchasing, and Consumer Behavior,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 28 (March), 670–76. Bem, Daryl J. (1967), “Self-Perception: An Alternative Inter- pretation of Cognitive Dissonance Phenomena,” Psychological Review, 74 (May), 183–200. Benartzi, Shlomo and Richard H. Thaler (2004), “Save More Tomorrow TM : Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Em- ployee Saving,” Journal of Political Economy, 112 (February), 164–87. Bertrand, Marianne, Sendhil Mullainathan, and Eldar Shafir (2006), “Behavioral Economics and Marketing in Aid of Deci- sion Making Among the Poor,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 8–23. Botti, Simona and Sheena S. Iyengar (2006), “The Dark Side of Choice: When Choice Impairs Social Welfare,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 24–38. Briley, Donnel A. and Jennifer L. Aaker (2006), “Bridging the Culture Chasm: Ensuring That Consumers Are Healthy, Wealthy, and Wise,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 53–66. Canova, Luigina, Anna M. Rattazzi, and Paul Webley (2005), “The Hierarchical Structure of Saving Motives,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 26 (February), 21–34. Carter, D. Bruce, Charlotte J. Patterson, and Susan J. Quasebarth (1979), “Development of Children’s Use of Plans for Self- Control,” Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3 (4), 407–413. Carver, Charles S. and Michael Scheier (1981), Attention and Self- Regulation: A Control Theory Approach to Human Behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag. Cheema, Amar and Vanessa M. Patrick (2008), “Anytime ver- sus Only: Mind-Sets Moderate the Effect of Expansive versus Restrictive Frames on Promotion Evaluation,” Journal of Mar- keting Research, 45 (August), 462–72. Diener, Ed, Marissa Diener, and Carol Diener (1995), “Factors Predicting the Subjective Well-Being of Nations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (November), 851–64. Escalas, Jennifer E. and Mary F. Luce (2003), “Process vs. Out- come Thought Focus and Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 246–54. Freitas, Antonio L., Peter M. Gollwitzer, and Yaacov Trope (2004), “The Influence of Abstract and Concrete Mindsets on Anticipating and Guiding Others’ Self-Regulatory Efforts,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40 (November), 739–52. Fujita, Kentaro, Yaacov Trope, Nira Liberman, and Maya Levin- Sagi (2006), “Construal Levels and Self-Control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (March), 351–67. Garland, Howard (1983), “Influence of Ability, Assigned Goals, and Normative Information on Personal Goals and Perfor- mance: A Challenge to the Goal Attainability Assumption,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 68 (February), 20–30. Hall, Robert E. (1978), “Stochastic Implications of the Life Cycle- Permanent Income Hypothesis: Theory and Evidence,” Journal of Political Economy, 86 (December), 971–87. Harkins, Stephen G. and Malia D. Lowe (2000), “The Effects of Self-Set Goals on Task Performance,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30 (January), 1–40. Hinsz, Verlin B., Lynn R. Kalnbach, and Nichole R. Lorentz (1997), “Using Judgmental Anchors to Establish Chal- lenging Self-Set Goals Without Jeopardizing Commitment,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71 (September), 287–308. Hollenbeck, John R. and Howard J. Klein (1987), “Goal Commit- ment and the Goal-Setting Process: Problems, Prospects, and Proposals for Future Research,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 72 (May), 212–20. Iyengar, Sheena S., Wei Jiang, and Gur Huberman (2004), “How Much Choice Is Too Much: Determinants of Individual Contributions in 401K Retirement Plans,” in Pension Design and Structure: New Lessons from Behavioral Finance, Olivia S. Mitchell and Steve Utkus, eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 83–97. Kirschenbaum, Daniel S., Laura L. Humphrey, and Sheldon D. Malett (1981), “Specificity of Planning in Adult Self- Control: An Applied Investigation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 (May), 941–50. Kotlikoff, Laurence J., Avia Spivak, and Lawrence H. Summers (1982), “The Adequacy of Savings,” American Economic Review, 72 (December), 1056–1069. Lee, Angela Y., Punam Anand Keller, and Brian Sternthal (2010), “Value from Regulatory Construal Fit: The Persuasive Impact of Fit Between Consumer Goals and Message Concreteness,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (February), 735–47. Liberman, Nira and Yaacov Trope (1998), “The Role of Feasibil- ity and Desirability Considerations in Near and Distant Future Decisions: A Test of Temporal Construal Theory,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (July), 5–18. Locke, Edwin A., Dong-Ok Chah, Scott Harrison, and Nancy Lustgarten (1989), “Separating the Effects of Goal Specificity from Goal Level,” Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- sion Processes, 43 (April), 270–87. and Gary P. Latham (1990), A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. and (2006), “New Directions in Goal- Setting Theory,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15 (October), 265–68. Lynch, John G., Jr. and Gal Zauberman (2006), “When Do You Want It? Time, Decisions, and Public Policy,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25 (Spring), 67–78. Madrian, Brigitte C. and Dennis F. Shea (2001), “The Power of Suggestion: Inertia in 401(K) Participation and Savings Behavior,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116 (November), 1149–88. Mischel, Walter and C.J. Patterson (1976), “Substantive and Structural Elements of Effective Plans for Self-Control,” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (November), 942–50. Modigliani, Franco (1986), “Life Cycle, Individual Thrift, and the Wealth of Nations,” American Economic Review, 76 (June), 297–313. Muller, Dominique, Charles M. Judd, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2005), “When Moderation Is Mediated and Mediation Is Moderated,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (December), 852–63. Naylor, James C. and Daniel R. Ilgen (1984), “Goal Setting: A Theoretical Analysis of a Motivational Technology,” in Research in Organizational Behavior, Vol. 6, L. Cummings and B. Staw, eds. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 95–140. Pham, Lien and Shelley Taylor (1999), “From Thought to Action: Effects of Process- versus Outcome-Based Mental Simulations on Performance,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 (February), 250–60. Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew F. Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indi- rect Effects in Multiple Mediator Models,” Behavior Research Methods, 40 (August), 879–91. , Derek D. Rucker, and Andrew F. Hayes (2007), “Addressing Moderated Mediation Hypotheses: Theory, Meth- ods, and Prescriptions,” Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42 (1), 185–227. Semin, Gün R. and Eliot R. Smith (1999), “Revisiting the Past and Back to the Future: Memory Systems and the Linguistic Representation of Social Events,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (June), 877–92. Shefrin, Hersh M. and Richard H. Thaler (1988), “The Behav- ioral Life-Cycle Hypothesis,” Economic Inquiry, 26 (October), 609–643. Soman, Dilip and Amar Cheema (2004), “When Goals Are Coun- terproductive: The Effects of Violation of a Behavioral Goal on Subsequent Performance,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (June), 52–62. Thaler, Richard H. and Cass R. Sunstein (2008), Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Thompson, Debora V., Rebecca W. Hamilton, and Petia K. Petrova (2009), “When Mental Simulation Hinders Behavior: The Effects of Process-Oriented Thinking on Decision Difficulty and Performance,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (December), 562–74. Trope, Yaacov and Nira Liberman (2003), “Temporal Construal,” Psychological Review, 110 (July), 403–421. Ülkümen, Gülden, Manoj Thomas, and Vicki G. Morwitz (2008), “Will I Spend More in 12 Months or a Year? The Effect of Ease of Estimation and Confidence on Budget Estimates,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (August), 245–56. Vallacher, Robin R. and Daniel Wegner (1987), “What Do People Think They’re Doing? Action Identification and Human Behav- ior,” Psychological Review, 94 (January), 3–15. and (1989), “Levels of Personal Agency: Individ- ual Variation in Action Identification,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (October), 660–71. Wilson, Timothy D., Douglas J. Lisle, Jonathan W. Schooler, Sara D. Hodges, Kristen J. Klaaren, and Suzanne J. LaFleur (1993), “Introspecting About Reasons Can Reduce Post-Choice Satis- faction,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19 (June), 331–39. Wright, Patrick M. and K. Michele Kacmar (1994), “Goal Specificity as a Determinant of Goal Commitment and Goal Change,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- cesses, 59 (August), 242–60.
  243. REFERENCES Anderson Craig A. (1997), “Effects of Violent Movies and Trait Irritability on Hostile Feelings and Aggressive Thoughts,” Aggressive Behavior, 23,161–78. Anderson Craig A., Kathryn Anderson, and William E. Deuser (1996), “Examining an Affective Aggression Framework: Weapon and Temperature Effects on Aggressive Thoughts, Affect, and Attitudes,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22 (April), 366-76. Anderson, Craig A. and Brad J. Bushman (2002), “Human Aggression,” Annual Review of Psychology, 53 (February), 27-51. Andersson, Staffan, Sarah R. Pryke, Jonas Ornborg, Michael J. Lawes, and Malte Andersson (2002), “Multiple Receivers, Multiple Ornaments, and a Trade-Off Between Agonistic and Epigamic Signaling in a Widowbird,” The American Naturalist, 160, 683–91. Aslam, Mubeen M. (2006), “Are You Selling The Right Color? A Cross-Cultural Review of Color as a Marketing Cue,” Journal of Marketing Communications, 12, 1 (March), 15-30. Babbitt, Edwin D. (1878), The Principles of Light and Color, New York, NY: University Books. Belk, Russell W. (1975), “Situational Variables and Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Research, 2 (December), 157-64. Bellizzi, Joseph A., Ayn E. Crowley, and Ronald W. Hasty (1983), “The Effects of Color in Store Design,” Journal of Retailing, 59 (1), 21-45. Bellizzi, Joseph A. and Robert E. Hite (1992), “Environmental Color, Consumer Feelings, and Purchase Likelihood,” Psychology and Marketing, 9 (5), 347-63. Bryant, Fred B. and Bruce D. Smith (2001), “Refining the Architecture of Aggression: A Measurement Model for the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire,” Journal of Research in Personality, 35 (2), 138-67. Buss, Arnold H. and Mark Perry (1992), “The Aggression Questionnaire,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63 (3), 452-59. Chattopadhyay, Amitava, Gerald J. Gorn, and Peter Darke (2010), “Differences and Similarities in Hue Preference Between Chinese and Caucasians,” in Sensory Marketing: Research on the Sensuality of Products, Ed. Aradhna Krishna. New York, NY: Routledge Academic, 219-40. Chebat, Jean-Charles and Maureen Morrin (2007), “Colors and Cultures: Exploring the Effects of Mall Décor on Consumer Perceptions,” Journal of Business Research, 60, 189–96. Crowley, Ann E. (1993), “The Two-Dimensional Impact of Color on Shopping,” Marketing Letters, 4 (1), 59-69. Cuthill, Innes C., Sarah Hunt, Colette Cleary, and Corinna Clark (1997), “Colour Bands, Dominance, and Body Mass Regulation in Male Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata),” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264, 1093–99. Diamond, Pamela M. and Philip R. Magaletta (2006), “The Short-Form Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire: A Validation Study with Federal Offenders,” Assessment, 13 (3), 227-40. Dijkstra, Karin, Marcel E. Pieterse, and Ad Pruyn (2008), “Individual Differences in Reactions towards Color in Simulated Healthcare Environments: The Role of Stimulus Screening Ability,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28 (3), 268-77. Elliot, Andrew J., Daniela N. Kayser , Tobias Greitemeyer, Stephanie Lichtenfeld, Richard H. Gramzow, Markus A. Maier, and Huijun Liu (2010), “Red, Rank and Romance in Women Viewing Men,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 399-417. Elliot, Andrew J., Markus A. Maier, Martin J. Binser, Ron Friedman, and Reinhard Pekrun (2009), “The Effect of Red on Avoidance Behavior in Achievement Contexts,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 365-75. Evans, Mathew R. and Ken Norris (1996), “The Importance of Carotenoids in Signalling During Aggressive Interactions Between Male Firemouth Cichlids,” Behavioral Ecology, 7, 1e6. Ganesan, Shankar (1993), “Negotiation Strategies and the Nature of Channel Relationships,” Journal of Marketing Research, 30 (May), 183-203. Gerard, Robert M. (1957), “Differential Effects of Colored Lights on Psychophysiological Functions,” unpublished dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles, CA. Gorn, Gerald J., Amitava Chattopadhyay, Jaideep Sengupta, and Shashank Tripathi (2004), “Waiting for the Web: How Screen Color Affects Time Perception,” Journal of Marketing Research, 41 (May), 215–25. Gorn, Gerald J., Amitava Chattopadhyay, Tracey Yi, and Darren W. Dahl (1997), “Effects of Color as an Executional Cue in Advertising: They’re in the Shade,” Management Science, 43 (10), 1387–419. Hatta, Takeshi, Hirotaka Yoshida, Ayako Kawakami, and Masahiko Okamoto (2002), “Color Of Computer Display Frame in Work Performance, Mood, and Physiological Response,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94 (1), 39-46. Healey, Mo, Tobias Uller, and Mats Olsson (2007), “Seeing Red: Morph-Specific Contest Success and Survival Rates in a Colour-Polymorphic Agamid Lizard,” Animal Behavior, 74, 337-41. Hemphill, Michael (1996), “A Note on Adults' Color-emotion Associations,” The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 157 (3), 275-80. Hill, Russell A. and Robert A. Barton (2005), “Red Enhances Human Performance in Contests,” Nature, 435, 293. Jacobs, Keith W. and Frank G. Hustmyer Jr. (1974), “Effects of Four Psychological Primary Colors on GSR, Heart Rate, and Respiration Rate,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 3 (June), 763-66. Kotler Philip (1973–1974), “Atmospherics as a Marketing Tool,” Journal of Retail, 49, 48–61. Labrecque, Lauren I. and George R. Milne (2011), “Exciting Red and Competent Blue: The Importance of Color in Marketing,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, in press. Little, Anthony C. and Russell A. Hill (2007), "Attribution to Red Suggests Special Role in Dominance Signaling," Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5 (1), 161-68. Lorber, Michael F. (2004), “Psychophysiology of Aggression, Psychopathy,and Conduct Problems: A Meta-Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin, 130 (4), 531-52. Macklin, M. Carole (1996), “Preschoolers’ Learning of Brand Names from Visual Cues,” Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 251-61. Madden, Thomas J., Kelly Hewett, and Martin S. Roth (2000), “Managing Images in Different Cultures: A Cross-National Study of Color Meanings and Preferences,” Journal of International Marketing, 8 (4), 90-107. Mandel, Naomi and Eric J. Johnson (2002), “When Web Pages Influence Choice: Effects of Visual Primes on Experts and Novices,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (2), 235-45. Mathews, Gerald, Dylan M. Jones, and A. Graham Chamberlain (1990), “Refining the Measurement of Mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist,” British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17-42. Mehta, Ravi and Juliet Zhu (2009), “Blue or Red? Exploring the Effect of Color on Cognitive Task Performances,” Science, 323, 1226-29. Patrick, Christopher J. (2008), “Psychophysiological Correlates of Aggression and Violence: An Integrative Review,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 2543-55. Pruitt, Dean G. (1981), Negotiation Behavior, New York, NY: Academic Press, Inc. Pryke, Sarah R. (2009), “Is Red an Innate or Learned Signal of Aggression and Intimidation?” Animal Behavior, 78, 393-98. Pryke, Sarah R. and Staffan Andersson (2003), “Carotenoid-Based Status Signalling in Red- Shouldered Widowbirds Euplectes Axillaris: Epaulet Size and Redness Affect Captive and Territorial Competition,” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, 393-401. Pryke, Sarah R. and Simon C. Griffith (2006), “Red Dominates Black: Agonistic Signalling Among Head Morphs in the Colour Polymorphic Gouldian Finch,” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 273, 949-57. Pryke, Sarah R., Michael J. Lawes, and Staffan Andersson (2001), “Agnonistic Carotenoid Signalling in Male Red-Collared Widowbirds: Aggression Related to the Colour Signal of Both Territory Owner and Model Intruder,” Animal Behaviour, 62, 695-704. Sinha, Atanu R. and Eric A. Greenleaf (2000), “The Impact of Discrete Bidding and Bidder Aggressiveness on Sellers' Strategies in Open English Auctions: Reserves and Covert Shilling,” Marketing Science, 19 (3), 244-65. Suls, Jerry and Choi K. Wan (1993), “Relationship between Trait Hostility and Cardiovascular Reactivity: A Quantitative Review and Analysis,” Psychophysiology, 30, 615–26. Wilson, Glenn D. (1966), “Arousal Properties of Red versus Green,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23 (August-December), 947-49.
  244. References Albarracín, D., & Mitchell, A. L. (2004). The role of defensive confidence in preference for proattitudinal information: How believing that one is strong can sometimes be a defensive weakness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 1565 – 1584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271180. Albarracín, D., Wallace, H. M., & Glasman, L. R. (2004). Survival and change in judgments: A model of activation and comparison. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 36. (pp. 251 – 315): Academic Press, 251 – 315. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(04)36005-3. Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219 – 235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341564. Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1362 – 1377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022- 3514.83.6.1362. Barden, J., & Petty, R. E. (2008). The mere perception of elaboration creates attitude certainty: Exploring the thoughtfulness heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 489 – 509. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/a0012559. Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 637 – 653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022- 3514.71.4.637. Bizer, G. Y., Larsen, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Exploring the valence-framing effect: Negative framing enhances attitude strength. Political Psychology, 32(1), 59 – 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00795.x. Bizer, G. Y., & Petty, R. E. (2005). How we conceptualize our attitudes matters: The effects of valence framing on the resistance of political attitudes. Political Psychology, 26(4), 553 – 568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 9221.2005.00431.x. Bizer, G., Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Memory-based versus on-line processing: Implications for attitude strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 646 – 653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.jesp.2005.09.002. Blankenship, K. L., & Wegener, D. T. (2008). Opening the mind to close it: Considering a message in light of important values increases message processing and later resistance to change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 196 – 213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.2. 94.2.196. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Barden, J. (2007). Happiness versus sadness as a determinant of thought confidence in persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 711 – 727. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.711. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Stavraki, M., Wagner, B. C., & Díaz, D. (2013). Anger and surprise can validate or invalidate thoughts: An appraisal perspective: Unpublished Manuscript. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Tormala, Z. L. (2006). The malleable meaning of subjective ease. Psychological Science, 17(3), 200 – 206. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01686.x. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Valle, C., Rucker, D. D., & Becerra, A. (2007). The effects of message recipients' power before and after persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1040 – 1053. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1040. Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3), 401 – 423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.401. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116 – 131. http://dx.doi.org/10. 1037//0022-3514.42.1.116. Campbell, M., & Kirmani, A. (2008). I know what you're doing and why you're doing it: The use of the persuasion knowledge model in consumer research. In C. Haugtvedt, P. Herr, & F. Kardes (Eds.), Handbook of Consumer Psychology (pp. 549 – 574). New York: Psychology Press. Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Regulatory fit and persuasion: Transfer from “ feeling right ” . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 388 – 404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.388. Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator physical attractiveness and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 1387 – 1397. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1387. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212 – 252). New York: Guilford. Clarkson, J. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2008). A new look at the consequences of attitude certainty: The amplification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 810 – 825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ a0013192. Cohen, J. B., & Reed, A. (2006). A multiple pathway anchoring and adjustment (MPAA) model of attitude generation and recruitment. [Review]. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 1 – 15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/504121. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando, FL, US: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers. Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating positive emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 301 – 331. Evans, A. T., & Clark, J. K. (2012). Source characteristics and persuasion: The role of self-monitoring in self-validation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 383 – 386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011. 07.002. Fazio, R. H. (1979). Motives for social comparison: The construction – validation distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1683 – 1698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.37.10.1683. Fazio, R. H., Ledbetter, J. E., & Towles-Schwen, T. (2000). On the costs of accessible attitudes: Detecting that the attitude object has changed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 197 – 210. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.197. Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978a). Attitudinal qualities relating to the strength of the attitude – behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(4), 398 – 408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031 (78)90035-5. Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978b). On the predictive validity of attitudes: The roles of direct experience and confidence. Journal of Personality, 46(2), 228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.ep7376510. Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G., Jr. (1988). Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement on belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(3), 421 – 435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0021- 9010.73.3.421. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117 – 140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. French, J., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150 – 167). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The Persuasion Knowledge Model: How people cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 1 – 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209380. Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1995). Persuasion Knowledge: Lay peoples and researchers beliefs about the psychology of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(1), 62 – 74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209435. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692 – 731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0033-2909.132.5.692. Gill, M. J., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Silvera, D. H. (1998). On the genesis of confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1101 – 1114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1101. Goethals, G. R., & Darley, J. M. (1977). Social comparison theory: An attributional approach. In J. M. Suls, & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 259 – 278). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Gross, S., Holtz, R., & Miller, N. (1995). Attitude certainty. In R. E. Petty, & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences (pp. 215 – 245). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Guinote, A., Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (2002). Effects of power on perceived and objective group variability: Evidence that more powerful groups are more variable. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(5), 708 – 721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.708. Haddock, G., Rothman, A. J., Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Forming judgments of attitude certainty, intensity, and importance: The role of subjective experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(7), 771 – 782. Hass, R. G. (1981). Effects of source characteristics on cognitive responses and persuasion. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Holland, R. W., Verplanken, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). From repetition to conviction: Attitude accessibility as a determinant of attitude certainty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 594 – 601. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/s0022-1031(03)00038-6. Kelley, C. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Remembering mistaken for knowing: Ease of retrieval as a basis for confidence in answers to general knowledge questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(1), 1 – 24. http://dx.doi. org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1001. Koehler, D. (1994). Hypothesis generation and confidence in judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(2), 461 – 469. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.20.2.461. Koriat, A. (2012). The self-consistency model of subjective confdience. Psychological Review, 119(1), 80 – 113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025648. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum Press. Kruglanski, A. W., Raviv, A., Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., Sharvit, K., Ellis, S., et al. (2005). Says who?: Epistemic authority effects in social judgment. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 37. (pp. 345 – 392): Academic Press, 345 – 392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(05)37006-7. Kruglanski, A. W., Webster, D. M., & Klem, A. (1993). Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 861 – 876. http://dx. doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.65.5.861. Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2098 – 2109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.37.11.2098. Lynch, J. G. (2006). Accessibility – diagnosticity and the multiple pathway anchoring and adjustment model. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 25 – 27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/504129. Lynch, J. G., Jr., Marmorstein, H., & Weigold, M. F. (1988). Choices from Sets including Remembered Brands: Use of Recalled Attributes and Prior Overall Evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 169 – 184. Maglio, S. J., & Trope, Y. (2011). Scale and construal: How larger measurement units shrink length estimates and expand mental horizons. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 18(1), 165 – 170. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/ s13423-010-0025-1. Maheswaran, D., & Chaiken, S. (1991). Promoting systematic processing in low-motivation settings — Effect of incongruent information on processing and judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 13 – 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.13. Maheswaran, D., & Sternthal, B. (1990). The effects of knowledge, motivation, and type of message on ad processing and product judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(1), 66 – 73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208537. Maio, G. R., & Esses, V. M. (2001). The need for affect: Individual differences in the motivation to approach or avoid emotions. Journal of Personality, 69(4), 583 – 615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.694156. Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1985). The effect of certainty on consensus judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(2), 165 – 177. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167285112005. Muthukrishnan, A. V., Pham, M. T., & Mungale, A. (2001). Does greater amount of information always bolster attitudinal resistance? Marketing Letters, 12(2), 131 – 144. Norwick, R., & Epley, N. (2003). Experiential determinants of confidence. Poster presented at the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Los Angeles, CA. Petrocelli, J. V., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Unpacking attitude certainty: Attitude clarity and attitude correctness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 30 – 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514. 92.1.30. Petty, R. E. (2006). A metacognitive model of attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 22 – 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/504128. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2006). A metacognitive approach to “ implicit ” and “ explicit ” evaluations: Comment on Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2006). Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 740 – 744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033- 2909.132.5.740. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2008). Persuasion: From single to multiple to metacognitive processes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 137 – 147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00071.x. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Tormala, Z. L., & Wegener, D. T. (2007). The role of meta-cognition in social judgment. In A. W. Kruganski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 254 – 284) (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Publishers. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 69 – 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.1.69. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 19. (pp. 123 – 205)New York: Academic Press. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Involvement and persuasion: Tradition versus integration. Psychological Bulletin, 107(3), 367 – 374. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.367. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2009). Implicit ambivalence: A meta-cognitive approach. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 119 – 164). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5), 847 – 855. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.41.5.847. Petty, R. E., Haugtvedt, C. P., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Elaboration as a determinant of attitude strength: Creating attitudes that are persistent, resistant, and predictive of behavior. In R. E. Petty, & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 93 – 130). Hillsdale, NJ England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.). (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2004). Resistance to persuasion: An attitude strength perspective. In J. T. Jost, M. R. Banaji, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism in social psychology: The yin and yang of scientific progress (pp. 37 – 51). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus mismatching attitude functions: Implications for scrutiny of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(3), 227 – 240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0146167298243001. Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., & Bizer, G. (2000). Matching effects in persuasion: An elaboration likelihood analysis. In G. Maio, & J. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of attitudes (pp. 133 – 162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The meta-cognitive model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition, 25, 657 – 686. Pomerantz, E. M., Chaiken, S., & Tordesillas, R. S. (1995). Attitude strength and resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 408 – 419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.408. Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 431 – 449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.3.431. Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (2003). The influence of spokesperson trustworthiness on message elaboration, attitude strength, and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(4), 408 – 421. http://dx. doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1304_08. Priester, Joseph R., Petty, Richard E., & Park, K. (2007). Whence univalent ambivalence? From the anticipation of conflicting reactions. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 11 – 21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/513042. Rokeach, M. (1968). Beliefs, attitudes, and values: A theory of organization and change. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Rucker, D. D., Galinsky, A. D., & Dubois, D. (2012). Power and consumer behavior: How power shapes who and what consumers value. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 352 – 368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps. 2011.06.001. Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). When resistance is futile: Consequences of failed counterarguing for attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 219 – 235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514. 86.2.219. Rucker, D. D., Petty, R. E., & Brinol, P. (2008). What's in a frame anyway?: A meta-cognitive analysis of the impact of one versus two sided message framing on attitude certainty. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(2), 137 – 149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2008.01.008. Rydell, R. J., Hugenberg, K., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Resistance can be good or bad: How theories of resistance and dissonance affect attitude certainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(6), 740 – 750. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167205286110. Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Kardes, F. R., & Herr, P. M. (1992). The role of prior knowledge and missing information in multiattribute evaluation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 51(1), 76 – 91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(92)90005-r. Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Kardes, F. R., Houghton, D. C., Ho, E. A., & Posavac, S. S. (2003). Overestimating the importance of the given information in multiattribute judgment. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 289 – 300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP130310. Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Kardes, F. R., Posavac, S. S., & Houghton, D. C. (1997). Contextual Influences on Judgments Based on Limited Information. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69(3), 251 – 264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1997.2686. Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Kardes, F. R., & Sansone, C. (1991). Remembering less and inferring more: The effects of time of judgment on inferences about unknown attributes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(4), 546 – 554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.546. Sheppard, B. H., Jon, H., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), 325 – 343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209170. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813 – 838. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813. Smith, S. M., Fabrigar, L. R., MacDougall, B. L., & Wiesenthal, N. L. (2008). The role of amount, cognitive elaboration, and structural consistency of attitude-relevant knowledge in the formation of attitude certainty. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(2), 280 – 295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ ejsp.447. Snyder, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(4), 526 – 537. http://dx.doi.org/10. 1037/h0037039. Snyder, M., & Kendzierski, D. (1982). Acting on one's attitudes: Procedures for linking attitude and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18(2), 165 – 183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(82)90048-8. Thompson, M. M., Naccarato, M. E., Parker, K. C. H., & Moskowitz, G. (2001). The Personal Need for Structure (PNS) and Personal Fear of Invalidity (PFI) scales: Historical perspectives, present applications and future directions. In G. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 19 – 39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973 – 988. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.973. Tormala, Z. L., Clarkson, J. J., & Henderson, M. D. (2011). Does fast or slow evaluation foster greater certainty? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(3), 422 – 434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167210397378. Tormala, Z. L., Clarkson, J. J., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Resisting persuasion by the skin of one's teeth: The hidden success of resisted persuasive messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 423 – 435. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.423. Tormala, Z. L., & DeSensi, V. L. (2009). The effects of minority/majority source status on attitude certainty: A matching perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(1), 114 – 125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0146167208325677. Tormala, Z. L., DeSensi, V. L., Clarkson, J. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2009). Beyond attitude consensus: The social context of persuasion and resistance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 149 – 154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.jesp.2008.07.020. Tormala, Z. L., DeSensi, V. L., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Resisting persuasion by illegitimate means: A metacognitive perspective on minority influence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(3), 354 – 367. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206295004. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn't kill me makes me stronger: The effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1298 – 1313. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037//0022-3514.83.6.1298. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2004a). Resistance to persuasion and attitude certainty: The moderating role of elaboration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1446 – 1457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264251. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2004b). Source credibility and attitude certainty: A metacognitive analysis of resistance to persuasion. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 427 – 442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp140411. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Contextual contrast and perceived knowledge: Exploring the implications for persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 17 – 30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.007. Tormala, Z. L., Petty, R. E., & Brinol, P. (2002). Ease of retrieval effects in persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1700 – 1712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616702237651. Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Attitude certainty: A review of past findings and emerging perspectives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 469 – 492. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007. 00025.x. Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Seger, C. R. (2008). When increased confidence yields increased thought: A confidence-matching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 141 – 147. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.11.002. Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Tsai, C., Klayman, J., & Hastie, R. (2008). Effects of amount of information on judgment accuracy and confidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107(2), 97 – 105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp. 2008.01.005. Tsai, C. I., & McGill, A. L. (2011). No pain, no gain? How fluency and construal level affect consumer confidence. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 807 – 821. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/655855. Visser, P. S., & Mirabile, R. R. (2004). Attitudes in the social context: The impact of social network composition on individual-level attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 779 – 795. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.779. Wan, E. W., & Rucker, D. D. (2013). Confidence and construal framing: When confidence increases versus decreases information processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 977 – 992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/ 666467. Wan, E. W., Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., & Clarkson, J. J. (2010). The effect of regulatory depletion on attitude certainty. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(3), 531 – 541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.47.3.531. Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction processes in social judgment: The role of naive theories in corrections for perceived bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(1), 36 – 51. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.36. Wegener, D. T., Downing, J., Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Strength-related properties of attitudes: Measures, manipulations, and future directions. In R. E. Petty, & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 455 – 487). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821 – 836. http://dx.doi.org/10. 1037//0022-3514.78.5.821. Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 989 – 1000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989. Wood, W., Kallgren, C. A., & Preisler, R. M. (1985). Access to attitude-relevant information in memory as a determinant of persuasion: The role of message attributes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21(1), 73 – 85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(85)90007-1. Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Biek, M. (1995). Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information-processing analysis. Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Wu, C., & Shaffer, D. R. (1987). Susceptibility to persuasive appeals as a function of source credibility and prior experience with the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(4), 677 – 688. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.4.677. Zakay, D. (1985). Post-decisional confidence and conflict experienced in a choice process. Acta Psychologica, 58(1), 75 – 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001- 6918(85)90035-6.
  245. References Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Exam- ining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377. Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 84, 1054–1068. Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition, Vol. 1: Basic processes (2nd ed.; pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., (Ed.), Advances in social cognition, Vol. 10 (pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and per- sonality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Cambridge University Press. Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11, 211–227. Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P., Barbey, A. K., & Ruppert, J. A. (2003). Social embodiment. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 43 (pp. 43–92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Intuition, affect, and personality: Uncon- scious coherence judgments and self-regulation of negative affect. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1213–1223. Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.). (2004). Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press. Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic co- herence. Psychological Science, 14, 416–421. Bowden, E. M., & Beeman, M. J. (1998). Getting the right idea: Semantic activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems. Psychological Science, 9, 435–440. Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72–110. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2001). Fuzzy-trace theory: Dual processesin memory, reasoning, and cognitive neuroscience. Advances in child development and behavior, 28, 41–100. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 164–169. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Forrest, T. J. (2002). Are young children susceptible to the false-memory illusion? Child Development, 73, 1363– 1377. Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K. (2000). Parent–child interaction as a power contest. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 267– 282. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. Cast, A. D. (2003). Power and the ability to define the situation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 185–201. Chen, S., Lee Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 173–187. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22. de Groot, A. M. B. (1980). Mondelinge woordassociatienormen: 100 woordassociaties op 460 Nederlandse zelfstandige naamwoorden. [Oral word association norms: 100 word associations to 460 Dutch nouns]. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Nonverbal communication. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. 2 (4th ed., pp. 3–40). New York: McGraw-Hill. Depret, E., & Fiske, S. T. (1993). Social cognition and power: Some cognitive consequences of social structure as a source of control depri- vation. In G. Weary, F. Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 176–202). New York: Springer. Depret, E., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). Perceiving the powerful: Intriguing individuals versus threatening groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 461–480. Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, D., Koomen, W., van Knippenberg, A., et al. (1998). Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 862–871. Eckstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Derman, D. (1976). Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Emerson, R. M. (1962). Power-dependence relations. American Sociolog- ical Review, 27, 31–41. Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature, 382, 626–628. Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628. Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. 2 (4th ed., pp. 357–411). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Fo¨rster, J., Friedman, R. S., & Liberman, N. (2004). Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: Consequences for insight and creative cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 177–189. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Amer- ican Psychologist, 56, 218–226. Friedman, R. S., & Fo¨rster, J. (2000). The effects of approach and avoid- ance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–492. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466. Gasper, K. (2004). Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 405–421. Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cognition. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychol- ogy: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 361–399). New York: Guilford Press. Goodwin, S. A., Gubin, A., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2000). Power can bias impression processes: Stereotyping subordinates by default and by design. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 3, 227–256. Guinote, A. (2001). The perception of group variability in a non-minority and a minority context: When adaptation leads to outgroup differentia- tion. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 117–132. Guinote, A., Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (2002). Effects of power on perceived and objective group variability: Evidence that more powerful groups are more variable. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 708–721. Guinote, A., & Trope, Y. (2004). Power differences in responsiveness to situational affordances. Manuscript submitted for publication. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. Hogg, M. A., & Reid, S. A. (2001). Social identity, leadership, and power. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The use and abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on the causes of corruption (pp. 159–180). Phil- adelphia: Psychology Press. Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206– 1217. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. Keltner, D., Young, R. C., Heerey, E. A., Oemig, C., & Monarch, N. D. (1998). Teasing in hierarchical and intimate relations. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 75, 1231–1247. Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local par- adigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38. Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The quest for the gist: On challenges of going abstract in social and personality psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 156–163. Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328. Lee, F., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Is it lonely at the top?: The independence and interdependence of power holders. Research in Organizational Behavior, 23, 43–91. Levy, S. R., Freitas, A. L., & Salovey, P. (2002). Construing action abstractly and blurring social distinctions: Implications for perceiving homogeneity among, but also empathizing with and helping, others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1224–1238. Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 523–534. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5–18. Lin, E. L., Murphy, G. L., & Shoben, E. J. (1997). The effects of prior processing episodes on basic-level superiority. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 50(A), 25– 48. Markman, A. B., & Wisniewski, E. J. (1997). Similar and different: The differentiation of basic-level categories. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 54–70. Martin, M. (1979). Hemispheric specialization for local and global pro- cessing. Neuropsychologia, 17, 33–40. Martinez, A., Moses, P., Frank, L., Buxton, R., Wong, E., & Stiles, J. (1997). Hemispheric asymmetries in global and local processing: Evi- dence from fMRI. Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience, 8, 1685–1689. Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating charac- teristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826– 837. McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 212–230. Milner, A. D., Brechmann, M., & Pagliarini, L. (1992). To halve and to halve not: An analysis of line bisection judgments in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 30, 515–526. Moskowitz, D. S. (2004). Does elevated power lead to approach and reduced power to inhibition? Comment on Keltner, Gruenfeld, and Anderson (2003). Psychological Review, 111, 808–811. Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25, 838–848. Nussbaum, S., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Creeping dispositionism: The temporal dynamics of behavior prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 485–497. Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2001). When power does not corrupt: Superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 81, 549–565. Payne, D. G., Elie, C. J., Blackwell, J. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 261–285. Pollack, I. (1970). A nonparametric procedure for evaluation of true and false positives. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation. Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1–75. Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814. Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Jour- nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233. Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439. Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2003). Anybody can be a boss but only certain people make good subordinates: Behavioral impacts of striving for dominance and dominance aversion. Journal of Personality, 71, 871–891. Schooler, J. W., & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 97–134). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Hand- book of basic principles (pp. 433–465). New York: Guilford Press. Seger, C. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in processing unusual semantic relationships. Neuropsy- chology, 14, 361–369. Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology: General, 117, 34–50. Snodgrass, J. G., Smith, B., Feenan, K., & Corwin, J. (1987). Fragmenting pictures on the Apple Macintosh computer for experimental and clinical applications. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 19, 270–274. Spears, R., Gordijn, E., Dijksterhuis, A., & Stapel, D. A. (2004). Reaction in action: Intergroup contrast in automatic behavior. Personality Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 605–616. Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494– 500. Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001a). I, we, and the effects of others on me: How self-construal level moderates social comparison effects. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 766–781. Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001b). Let’s not forget the past when we go to the future: On our knowledge of knowledge accessibility. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton Sympo- sium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition (pp. 229–246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Stevens, L. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2000). Motivated impressions of a power- holder: Accuracy under task dependency and misperception under eval- uation dependency. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 907–922. Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Prefrontal brain electrical asym- metry predicts the evaluation of affective stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1723–1733. Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). Power and dependence. In J. W. Thibaut & H. H. Kelley (Eds.), The social psychology of groups (pp. 100–125). New York: Wiley. Toglia, M. P., Neuschatz, J. S., & Goodwin, K. A. (1999). Recall accuracy and illusory memories: When more is less. Memory, 7, 233–256. Trope, Y., & Fishbach, A. (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcom- ing temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 493– 506. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2000). Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 876–889. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403–421. Tun, P. A., Wingfield, A., Rosen, M. J., & Blanchard, L. (1998). Response latencies for false memories: Gist-based processes in normal aging. Psychology and Aging, 13, 230–241. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 660–671. Van Kleeck, M. H. (1989). Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1165– 1178. Vescio, T. K., Snyder, M., & Butz, D. A. (2003). Power in stereotypically masculine domains: A social influence strategy stereotype match model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1062–1078. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali- dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. A. (1971). Group Embedded Figures Test Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo- gists Press.
  246. References Ajzen, Icek, and Martin Fishbein (1977), “Attitude-Behavior Re- lations: Theoretical-Analysis and Review of Empirical Re- search,” Psychological Bulletin, 84 (5), 888–918. Andreoni, James, and B. Douglas Bernheim (2009), “Social Image and the 50–50 Norm: A Theoretical and Experimental Anal- ysis of Audience Effects,” Econometrica, 77 (5), 1607–36. Ariely, Dan, Anat Bracha, and Stephan Meier (2009), “Doing Good or Doing Well? Image Motivation and Monetary Incentives in Behaving Prosocially,” American Economic Review, 99 (1), 544–55. Ariely, Dan, and Michael I. Norton (2008), “How Actions Create —Not Just Reveal—Preferences,” Trends in Cognitive Sci- ences, 12 (1), 13–16. Bem, Daryl J. (1972), “Self-Perception Theory,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 6, ed. Leonard Ber- kowitz, New York: Academic Press, 1–62. Be´nabou, Roland, and Jean Tirole (2004), “Willpower and Personal Rules,” Journal of Political Economy, 112 (4), 848–86. ——— (2011), “Identity, Morals, and Taboos: Beliefs as Assets,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126 (2), 805–55. Bickman, Leonard (1972), “Environmental Attitudes and Actions,” Journal of Social Psychology, 87 (2), 323–24. Bodner, Ronit, and Drazen Prelec (2003), “The Diagnostic Value of Actions in a Self-Signaling Model,” in The Psychology of Economic Decisions, Vol. 1, ed. Isabelle Brocas and Juan D. Carillo, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 105–23. Carlson, Les, Stephen J. Grove, and Norman Kangun (1993), “A Content Analysis of Environmental Advertising Claims: A Matrix Method Approach,” Journal of Advertising, 22 (3), 27–39. Cialdini, Robert B. (2005), “Don’t Throw in the Towel: Use Social Influence Research,” Observer, http://www.psychologicalscience .org/observer/getArticle.cfm?id p 1762. ——— (2007), Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion, New York: HarperCollins. ——— (2009), “We Have to Break Up,” Perspectives on Psy- chological Science, 4 (1), 5–6. Cialdini, Robert B., and Noah J. Goldstein (2004), “Social Influ- ence: Compliance and Conformity,” Annual Review of Psy- chology, 55 (1), 591–621. Cialdini, Robert B., and Melanie R. Trost (1998), “Social Influence: Social Norms, Conformity, and Compliance,” in The Hand- book of Social Psychology, Vol. 2, ed. Daniel T. Gilbert, Susan T. Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey, New York: McGraw-Hill, 151–92. Cioffi, Delia, and Randy Garner (1996), “On Doing the Decision: Effects of Active versus Passive Choice on Commitment and Self-Perception,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22 (2), 133–47. Costanzo, Mark, Dane Archer, Elliot Aronson, and Thomas Pet- tigrew (1986), “Energy Conservation Behavior: The Difficult Path from Information to Action,” American Psychologist, 41 (5), 521–28. Danziger, Shai, Jonathan Levav, and Liora Avnaim-Pesso (2011), “Extraneous Factors in Judicial Decisions,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108 (17), 6889–92. De Young, Raymond (1988), “Exploring the Difference between Recyclers and Non-recyclers: The Role of Information,” Jour- nal of Environmental Systems, 18 (4), 341–51. Energy Star (2010), “Hospitality Benchmarking Fact Sheet,” http:// www.energystar.gov/ia/business/hospitality/Benchmarking_Fact _Sheet.pdf. Festinger, Leon (1957), A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance, Ev- anston, IL: Row, Peterson. Freedman, Jonathan L., and Scott C. Fraser (1966), “Compliance without Pressure: The Foot-in-the-Door Technique,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 (2), 195–202. Gao, Leilie, S. Christian Wheeler, and Baba Shiv (2009), “The ‘Shaken Self’: Product Choices as a Means of Restoring Self- View Confidence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (1), 29–38. Geller, E. Scott (1992), “It Takes More than Information to Save Energy,” American Psychologist, 47 (6), 814–15. Gneezy, Ayelet, Alex Imas, Leif D. Nelson, Amber Brown, and Michael I. Norton (2012), “Paying to Be Nice: Costly Pro- social Behavior and Consistency,” Management Science, 58 (1), 179–87. Goldstein, Noah J., Robert B. Cialdini, and Vladas Griskevicius (2008), “A Room with a Viewpoint: Using Social Norms to Motivate Environmental Conservation in Hotels,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (3), 472–82. Goldstein, Noah J., Vladas Griskevicius, and Robert B. Cialdini (2007), “Invoking Social Norms: A Social Psychology Per- spective on Improving Hotels’ Linen-Reuse Programs,” Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 48 (2), 145–50. Greenwald, Anthony G. (1980), “The Totalitarian Ego: Fabrication and Revision of Personal History,” American Psychologist, 35 (7), 603–18. Griffin, Patricia (2001), “‘Green’ Hotels Association Says, Hotels Can Save over $6.50 a Day per Occupied Guestroom and Help Protect Our Environment,” press release, “Green” Hotels As- sociation, Houston, http://www.greenhotels.com/pressrel.php#a2. Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, and Bram Van den Bergh (2010), “Going Green to Be Seen: Status, Reputation, and Conspicuous Conservation,” Journal of Personality and So- cial Psychology, 98 (3), 392–404. Gustin, Mary Elizabeth, and Pamela A. Weaver (1996), “Are Ho- tels Prepared for the Environmental Consumer?” Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, 20 (2), 1–14. Han, Heesup, Li-Tzang Jane Hsu, Jin-Soo Lee, and Chwen Sheu (2011), “Are Lodging Customers Ready to Go Green? An Examination of Attitudes, Demographics, and Eco-Friendly Intentions,” International Journal of Hospitality Manage- ment, 30 (2), 345–55. Henriques, Irene, and Perry Sadorsky (1996), “The Determinants of an Environmentally Responsive Firm: An Empirical Ap- proach,” Journal of Environmental Economics and Manage- ment, 30 (3), 381–95. Hollenbeck, John R., and Howard J. Klein (1987), “Goal Com- mitment and the Goal-Setting Process: Problems, Prospects, and Proposals for Future Research,” Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 72 (2), 212–20. Katzev, Richard D., and Theodore R. Johnson (1984), “Comparing the Effects of Monetary Incentives and Foot-in-the-Door Strategies in Promoting Residential Electricity Conservation,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14 (1), 12–27. Langer, Ellen J. (1989), Mindfulness, Reading, MA: Addison-Wes- ley. Langer, Ellen J., Arthur Blank, and Benzion Chanowitz (1978), “The Mindlessness of Ostensibly Thoughtful Action: The Role of ‘Placebic’ Information in Interpersonal Interaction,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 (6), 635– 42. Laroche, Michel, Jasmin Bergeron, and Guido Barbaro-Forleo (2001), “Targeting Consumers Who Are Willing to Pay More for Environmentally Friendly Products,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, 18 (6), 503–20. LeBoeuf, Robyn A., Eldar Shafir, and Julia Belyavsky Bayuk (2010), “The Conflicting Choices of Alternating Selves,” Or- ganizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111 (1), 48–61. Leventhal, Howard, Robert Singer, and Susan Jones (1965), “Ef- fects of Fear and Specificity of Recommendation upon At- titudes and Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 2 (1), 20–29. Levitt, Steven D., and John A. List (2007), “What Do Laboratory Experiments Measuring Social Preferences Reveal about the Real World?” Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21 (2), 153– 74. ——— (2008), “Homo economicus Evolves,” Science, 319 (5865), 909–10. Locke, Edwin A., Dong-Ok Chah, Scott Harrison, and Nancy Lust- garten (1989), “Separating the Effects of Goal Specificity from Goal Level,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43 (2), 270–87. Locke, Edwin A., and Gary P. Latham (1990), A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- tice-Hall. ——— (2002), “Building a Practically Useful Theory of Goal Setting and Task Motivation: A 35-Year Odyssey,” American Psychologist, 57 (9), 705–17. Locke, Edwin A., Karyll N. Shaw, Lise M. Saari, and Gary P. Latham (1981), “Goal Setting and Task Performance, 1969–1980,” Psy- chological Bulletin, 90 (1), 125–52. Menon, Ajay, and Anil Menon (1997), “Enviropreneurial Mar- keting Strategy: The Emergence of Corporate Environmen- talism as Market Strategy,” Journal of Marketing, 61 (1), 51–67. Miles, Morgan P., and Jeffrey G. Covin (2000), “Environmental Marketing: A Source of Reputational, Competitive, and Fi- nancial Advantage,” Journal of Business Ethics, 23 (3), 299–311. National Association of Institutional Linen Management (2001), “Membership Survey, 2000–2001,” Association for Linen Management, Richmond, KY, http://www.almnet.org/. Pallak, Michael S., and William Cummings (1976), “Commitment and Voluntary Energy Conservation,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2 (1), 27–30. Roberts, James A. (1996), “Green Consumers in the 1990s: Profile and Implications for Advertising,” Journal of Business Re- search, 36 (3), 217–31. Ross, Michael (1989), “Relation of Implicit Theories to the Con- struction of Personal Histories,” Psychological Review, 96 (2), 341–57. Russo, Michael V., and Paul A. Fouts (1997), “A Resource-Based Perspective on Corporate Environmental Performance and Prof- itability,” Academy of Management Journal, 40 (3), 534–59. Schultz, Wesley P., Azar M. Khazian, and Adam C. Zaleski (2008), “Using Normative Social Influence to Promote Conservation among Hotel Guests,” Social Influence, 3 (1), 4–23. Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson, and Uri Simonsohn (2011), “False Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Sig- nificant,” Psychological Science, 22 (11), 1359–66. Smith Travel Research (2011), “August US Hotel Performance Strong,” HotelNewsNow.com, Cleveland, http://www.hotelnews now.com/Articles.aspx/6501/STR-August-US-hotel-performance- strong. Solomons, Leon M., and Gertrude Stein (1896), “Normal Motor Automatism,” Psychological Review, 3 (5), 492–512. Thaler, Richard H., and Cass R. Sunstein (2008), Nudge: Improving Decisions on Health, Wealth, and Happiness, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Travelzoo (2010), “Travelzoo Publishes Eco-Certified Hotel Deals for Earth Day’s 40th Anniversary,” press release, Travelzoo, http://ir.travelzoo.com/releases.cfm?Year p 2010&Releases Type p . US Environmental Protection Agency (2011a), “Climate Change: Basic Information—Greenhouse Gas Emissions,” http://www .epa.gov/climatechange/basicinfo.html. ——— (2011b), “WaterSense,” http://www.epa.gov/WaterSense /pubs/questions.html. Vandermerwe, Sandra, and Michael D. Oliff (1990), “Customers Drive Corporations Green,” Long Range Planning, 23 (6), 10–16. Viscusi, Kip, Joel Huber, and Jason Bell (2011), “Promoting Re- cycling: Private Values, Social Norms, and Economic Incen- tives,” American Economic Review: Papers and Proceedings, 101 (3), 65–70. Weissenberg, Adam, Neale Redington, and Diane Kutyla (2008), “The Staying Power of Sustainability: Balancing Opportunity and Risk in the Hospitality Industry,” Deloitte Development, http://www.deloitte.com/us/thl. Werner, Carol M., Jane Turner, Kristen Shipman, F. Shawn Twitch- ell, Becky R. Dickson, Gary V. Bruschke, and Wolfgang B. von Bismarck (1995), “Commitment, Behavior, and Attitude Change: An Analysis of Voluntary Recycling,” Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15 (3), 197–208. Wright, Patrick M., and K. Michele Kacmar (1994), “Goal Spec- ificity as a Determinant of Goal Commitment and Goal Change,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- cesses, 59 (2), 242–60.
  247. References Ainslie, George (1975) “Specious Reward: A Behavioral Theory of Impulsiveness and Impulse Control,” Psychological Bulletin 82 (4), 463-96. Baumeister, Roy, F., Todd F. Heatherton, and Dianne M. Tice (1994), Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Cheema, Ameer and Dilip Soman (2008), “The Effect of Partitions on Controlling Consumption,” Journal of Marketing Research, 45 (6), 665-75. Coombs, Clyde H. and George S. Avrunin (1977), “Single-Peaked Functions and the Theory of Preference.” Psychological Review, 84 (2), 216-30. Epstein, Leonard, H., Anthony R. Caggiula, Joshua S. Rodefer, Lucene Wisniewski, and Shari L. Mitchell (1993), “The Effects of Calories and Taste on Habituation of the Human Salivary Response.” Addictive Behaviors, 18 (2), 179-85. Frederick, Shane and George Loewenstein (1999), “Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology,” in Well-Being: The Foundations of hedonic psychology, ed. Daniel Kahneman, Ed Diener, and Norbert Schwarz, New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation, 302-29. Galak, Jeff, Justin Kruger, and George Loewenstein (2011), “Is Variety The Spice of Life? It All Depends On the Rate of Consumption,” Judgment and Decision Making, 6 (3), 230-38. Galak, Jeff, Joseph Redden, and Justin Kruger (2009), “Variety Amnesia: Recalling Past Variety Can Accelerate Recovery From Satiation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (December), 575-84. Gilbert, Daniel T., Elizabeth C. Pinel, Timothy D. Wilson, Stephen J. Blumberg, and Thalia P. Wheatley (1998), “Immune Neglect: A Source of Durability Bias in Affective Forecasting,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (3), 617-38. Harris, Donald J. (1943), “Habituatory Response Decrement in the Intact Organisms,” Psychological Bulletin, 40 (6), 385-422. Helson, Harry (1964), Adaptation-Level Theory. New York: Harper & Row. Herrnstein, Richard J. and Drazen Prelec (1991), “Melioration: A Theory of Distributed Choice,” The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5 (3), 139-56. Herrnstein, Richard, George Loewenstein, Drazen Prelec, and William Vaughan (1993), “Utility Maximization and Melioration: Internalitites in Individual Choice, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 6 (3), 149-85. Herman, C. Peter and Janet Polivy (2004), “The Self-Regulation of Eating: Theoretical and Practical Problems,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, ed. Roy F. Baumeister and Kathleen D. Vohs, New York, Guilford Press, 492-508. Hoch, Stephen J. and George F. Loewenstein (1991), “Time-Inconsistent Preferences and Consumer Self-Control,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (4), 492-507. Horovitz, Bruce (2006), “100-Calorie Snack Packs Are a Hit,” USA Today, April 13. Kahneman, Daniel and Jackie S. Snell (1992), “Predicting a Changing Taste: Do People Know What They Will Like?” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5 (3), 187-200. Kahneman, Daniel and Amos Tversky (1979), “Intuitive Prediction: Biases and Corrective Procedures,” TIMS Studies in Management Science, 12, 313-27. Langer, Ellen and Judith Rodin (1976), “The Effects of Choice and Enhanced Personal Responsibility for the Aged: A field Experiment in an Institutional Setting,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (2), 191-98. Lichtenstein, Donald R. and Scot Burton (1989), “The Relationship Between Perceived and Objective Price-Quality,” Journal of Marketing Research, 26 (November), 429-43. Loewenstein, George and Erik Angner (2003), “Predicting and Indulging Changing Preferences,” in Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice, ed.George Loewenstein, Daniel Read, and Roy Baumeister, New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation Press, 351-91.. Loewenstein, George, Daniel Read, and Roy Baumeister (eds.) (2003), Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice, New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation Press. Loewenstein, George and Jon Elster (1992), Choice Over Time, New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation Press. Loewenstein, George and Richard Thaler (1989), “Anomalies: Intertemporal Choice,” Journal of Economic Perspectives, 3 (4), 181-193. Luchs, Michael G., Rebecca Walker Naylor, Julie R. Irwin, and Rajagopal Raghunathan (2010), “The Sustainability Liability: Potential Negative Effects of Ethicality on Product Preference” Journal of Marketing, 74 (September), 18-31. Madden, Gregory J. and Warren K. Bickel (2009), Impulsivity: Theory, Science and Neuroscience of Discounting. Washington: American Psychological Association Press. McSweeney, Frances K and Samantha Swindell (1999), “General-Process Theories of Motivation Revisited: The Role of Habituation,” Psychological Bulletin, 125 (4), 437-57. Myers, Christi (2006), “100 Calorie Snack Packs: Do They Help with Weight Loss?”, http://www.ABCNews.com, May 31. Nelson, Leif D. and Tom Meyvis (2008), “Interrupted Consumption: Adaptation and the Disruption of Hedonic Experience,” Journal of Marketing Research, 45 (December), 654- 64. Nelson, Leif D., Tom Meyvis, and Jeff Galak (2009), “Enhancing the Television Viewing Experience through Commercial Interruptions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (August), 160-72. Polivy, Janet and C. Peter Herman (2002), “If at First You Don’t Succeed: False Hopes of Self- Change,” American Psychologist, 57 (9), 677-89. Raghubir, Priya and Joydeep Srivastava (2009), “The Denomination Effect,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (December), 701-13. Raghunathan, Rajagopal, Rebecca Walker Naylor, and Wayne D. Hoyer (2006), “The Unhealthy = Tasty Intuition and Its Effects on Taste Inferences, Enjoyment, and Choice of Food Products,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (October), 170-84. Ratner, Rebecca K, Barbara E Kahn, and Daniel Kahneman (1999), “Choosing Less-Preferred Experiences for the Sake of Variety,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (June), 1-15. Read, Daniel, Gerrit Antonides, Laura van den Ouden, and Harry Trienekens (2001), “Which Is Better: Simultaneous or Sequential Choice?”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84 (1), 54-70. Read, Daniel and George Loewenstein (1995), “Diversification Bias: Explaining the Discrepancy in Variety Seeking Between Combined and Separated Choices,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1 (1), 34-49. Read, Daniel, George Loewenstein, and Shobana, Kalyanaraman (1999), “Mixiing Virtue and Vice: Combining the Immediacy Effect and the Diversification Heuristic,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 257-273. Rolls, Barbara J., P.M. Van Duijvenvoorde, and Edmund T. Rolls (1984), “Pleasantness Changes and Food Intake in a Varied Four-Course Meal,” Appetite, 5 (4), 337–48. Rosnow, Ralph L. and Robert Rosenthal (2007), Beginning Behavioral Research: A Conceptual Primer (6th Edition), Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Scott, Maura L., Stephen M. Nowlis, Naomi Mandel, and Andrea C. Morales (2008), “The Effects of Reduced Food Size and Package Size on the Consumption Behavior of Restrained and Unrestrained Eaters,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 391–405. Sieff, Elaine. M., Robyn. M. Dawes, and George Loewenstein (1999), “Anticipated Versus Actual Reaction to HIV Test Results,” American Journal of Psychology, 112 (2), 297-311. Simonson, Itamar (1990), “The Effect of Purchase Quantity and Timing on Variety-Seeking Behavior,” Journal of Marketing Research, 27 (2), 150-62. Tangney, June P, Roy F Baumeister, and Angie Luzio Boone (2004), “High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success,” Journal of Personality, 72 (2), 271-324. Thaler, Richard H., and H. M. Shefrin (1981), “An Economic Theory of Self-Control,” Journal of Political Economy, 89 (2), 392-406. Thompson, Richard F, and William Alden Spencer (1966), “Habituation: A Model Phenomenon for the Study of Neuronal substrates of Behavior,” Psychological Review 73 (1), 16-43. Thompson, Stephanie (2006), “Consumers Pay More for Less with 100-Calorie Packs,” Advertising Age Online, April 25. Wang, Jing, Nathan Novemsky, and Ravi Dhar (2009), “Anticipating Adaptation to Products,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (August), 149–59. Wertenbroch, Klaus (1998), “Consumption Self-Control by Rationing Purchase Quantities of Virtue and Vice,” Marketing Science, 17 (4), 317-37. Wilcox, Keith, Lauren G. Block, and Eric M. Eisenstein (2011), “Leave Home Without It? The Effects of Credit Card Debt and Available Credit on Spending”, Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (Special issue), S78-S90. Wilson, Timothy D., Thalia P. Wheatley, Jonathan M. Meyers, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Danny Axsom (2000), ”Focalism: A Source of Durability Bias in Affective Forecasting,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (5), 821-36. Zajonc, Robert B. (1968), “Attitudinal effects of mere exposure,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9 (2-2), 1-27.
  248. REFERENCES Aarts, Henk, and Ap Dijksterhuis (2000), “Habits as Knowledge Structures: Automaticity in Goal-Directed Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (1), 53–63. Adriaanse, Marieke A., Johanna M. F. van Oosten, Denise T. D. de Ridder, John B. F. de Wit, and Catherine Evers (2011), “Planning What Not to Eat: Ironic Effects of Implementation Intentions Negating Unhealthy Habits,” Personality and So- cial Psychology Bulletin, 37 (1), 69–81. Aiken, Leona S., and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ajzen, Icek, and Martin Fishbein (1977), “Attitude-Behavior Re- lations: A Theoretical Analysis and Review of Empirical Re- search,” Psychological Bulletin, 84 (5), 888–918. Ajzen, Icek, and Thomas J. Madden (1986), “Prediction of Goal- Directed Behavior: Attitudes, Intentions, and Perceived Be- havioral Control,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 22 (5), 453–74. Ajzen, Icek, Christine Timko, and John B. White (1982), “Self- Monitoring and the Attitude-Behavior Relation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42 (3), 426–35. American Council on Exercise (2009), “ACE Lifestyle and Weight Management Coach Manual,” American Council on Exercise, San Diego, CA, http://www.acefitness.org/calculators/body- fat-calculator.aspx. American Dietetic Association (2009), “Understanding Body Mass Index,” Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, Chicago, http:// www.eatright.org/Public/content.aspx?id p 6844. Bandura, Albert (1977), “Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change,” Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191– 215. ——— (1988), “Self-Regulation of Motivation and Action through Goal Systems,” in Cognitive Perspectives on Emotion and Motivation, ed. V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, and N. H. Fryda, Dordrecht: Nijhoff, 37–61. ——— (1997), Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control, New York: Freeman. Bandura, Albert, and Karen M. Simon (1977), “The Role of Prox- imal Intentions in Self Regulation of Refractory Behavior,” Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1 (3), 177–93. Baron, R. Marjorie, and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator- Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Re- search: Conceptual, Strategic and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173– 82. Baumeister, Roy F., and Kathleen D. Vohs (2004), Handbook of Self Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, New York: Guilford, 211–28. Bayuk, Julia Belyavsky, Chris Janiszewski, and Robyn A. LeBoeuf (2010), “Letting Good Opportunities Pass Us By: Examining the Role of Mind-Set during Goal Pursuit,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 37 (4), 570–83. Bonezzi, Andrea, C. Miguel Brendl, and Matteo De Angelis (2001), “Stuck in the Middle: The Psychophysics of Goal Pursuit,” Psychological Science, 22 (5), 607–12. Brewer, William F., and David A. Dupree (1983), “Use of Plan Schemata in the Recall and Recognition of Goal-Direction Actions,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Recognition, 9 (1), 117–19. Cheema, Amar, and Rajesh Bagchi (2011), “Goal Visualization and Goal Pursuit: Implications for Consumers and Managers,” Journal of Marketing, 75 (March), 109–23. Dalton, Amy N., and Stephen A. Spiller (2012), “Too Much of a Good Thing? The Benefits of Implementation Intentions De- pend on the Number of Goals,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 39 (3), electronically published January 26. Deurenberg, Paul, Jan A. Weststrate, and Jaap C. Seidell (1991), “Body Mass Index as a Measure of Body Fatness: Age- and Sex-Specific Prediction Formulas,” British Journal of Nutri- tion, 65 (2), 105–14. DeWall, C. Nathan, and Roy F. Baumeister (2007), “From Terror to Joy: Automatic Tuning to Positive Affective Information following Mortality Salience,” Psychological Science, 18 (11), 984–90. DeWall, C. Nathan, Jean M. Twenge, Sander L. Koole, Roy F. Baumeister, Alissa Marquez, and Mark W. Reid (2011), “Au- tomatic Emotion Regulation after Social Exclusion: Tuning to Positivity,” Emotion, 11 (3), 623–36. Fishbach, Ayelet, and Ravi Dhar (2005), “Goals as Excuses or Guides: The Liberating Effect of Perceived Goal Progress on Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 370–77. Fitzsimons, Gavan J. (2008), “Death to Dichotomizing,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 5–8. Forster, Jens, E. Tory Higgins, and Lorraine Idson (1998), “Ap- proach and Avoidance Strength during Goal Attainment: Reg- ulatory Focus and the ‘Goal Looms Larger’ Effect,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (5), 1115–31. Gellatly, Ian R., and John P. Meyer (1992), “The Effects of Goal Difficulty on Physiological Arousal, Cognition, and Task Per- formance,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 77 (5), 694–704. Gollwitzer, Peter M. (1993), “Goal Achievement: The Role of Intentions,” in European Review of Social Psychology, Vol. 4, ed. Wolfgang Stroebe and Miles Hewstone, Chichester: Wiley, 141–85. ——— (1996), “The Volitional Benefits of Planning,” in The Psy- chology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Be- havior, ed. Peter M. Gollwitzer and John A. Bargh, New York: Guilford, 287–312. ——— (1999), “Implementation Intentions: Strong Effects of Sim- ple Plans,” American Psychologist, 54 (7), 493–503. Gollwitzer, Peter M., and Veronika Brandstatter (1997), “Imple- mentation Intentions and Effective Goal Pursuit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (1), 186–99. Gollwitzer, Peter M., Heinz Heckhausen, and Birgit Steller (1990), “Deliberative and Implemental Mind-Sets: Cognitive Tuning toward Congruous Thoughts and Information,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (6), 1119–27. Gollwitzer, Peter M., and Gabriele Oettingen (2007), “The Role of Goal Setting and Goal Striving in Medical Adherence,” in Medical Adherence and Aging, Social and Cognitive Per- spectives, ed. Denise C. Park and Linda L. Liu, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 23–47. Greenwald, Anthony G., and Mahzarin R. Banaji (1995), “Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes,” Psychological Review, 102 (1), 4–27. Greiner, Jerry M., and Paul Karoly (1976), “Effects of Self Control Training on Study Activity and Academic Performance: An Analysis of Self-Monitory, Self-Reward, and Systematic- Planning Components,” Journal of Counseling Psychology, 23 (6), 495–502. Hayes-Roth, Barbara, and Frederick Hayes-Roth (1979), “A Cog- nitive Model of Planning,” Cognitive Science, 3 (4), 275–310. Huber, Vandra L. (1985), “Effects of Task Difficulty, Goal Setting, and Strategy on Performance on a Heuristic Task,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 70 (3), 492–504. Hull, Clark L. (1932), “The Goal Gradient Hypothesis and Maze Learning,” Psychological Review, 39 (1), 25–43. Jones, Fiona, Charles Abraham, Peter Harris, Jorg Schulz, and Catherine Chrispin (2001), “From Knowledge to Action Reg- ulation: Modeling the Cognitive Prerequisites of Sun Screen Use in Australian and UK Samples,” Psychology and Health, 16 (2), 191–206. Kahneman, Daniel, and Jackie Snell (1992), “Predicting a Chang- ing Task: Do People Know What They Will Like?” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5 (3), 187–200. Khan, Uzma, and Ravi Dhar (2006), “Licensing Effect in Consumer Choice,” Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (1), 259–66. Kivetz, Ran, Oleg Urminsky, and Yuhuang Zheng (2006), “The Goal-Gradient Hypothesis Resurrected: Purchase Accelera- tion, Illusionary Goal Progress, and Customer Retention,” Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (1), 39–58. Kreitler, Shulamith, and Hans Kreitler (1987), “Plans and Planning: Their Motivational and Cognitive Antecedents,” in Blueprints for Thinking: The Role of Planning in Cognitive Development, ed. Sarah L. Friedman and Ellin Kofsky Scholnick, Mel- bourne: Cambridge University Press, 110–78. Laibson, David (1997), “Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discount- ing,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 62 (2), 443–47. Laibson, David, Andrea Repetto, and Jeremy Tobacman (1998), “Self Control and Savings for Retirement,” Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, 1998 (1), 91–172. Levav, Jonathan, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2006), “When Ques- tions Change Behavior: The Role of Ease of Representation,” Psychological Science, 17 (3), 207–13. Lusardi, Annamaria (2000), “Explaining Why So Many House- holds Do Not Save,” photocopy, Harris School of Public Pol- icy, University of Chicago. Luszczynska, Aleksandra, and Ralf Schwarzer (2003), “Planning and Self-Efficacy in the Adoption and Maintenance of Breast Self-Examinations: A Longitudinal Study on Self-Regulatory Cognitions,” Psychology and Health, 18 (1), 93–108. Mullainathan, Sendhil, and Eldar Shafir (2009), “Savings Policy and Decision-Making in Low-Income Households,” in In- sufficient Funds: Savings, Assets, Credit and Banking among Low-Income Households, ed. Michael Barr and Rebecca Blank, New York: Sage, 121–45. Pham, Michel Tuan (1998), “Representativeness, Relevance, and the Use of Feelings in Decision Making,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 25 (2), 144–59. Polivy, Janet, and C. Peter Herman (1985), “Dieting as a Problem in Behavioral Medicine,” in Advances in Behavioral Medi- cine, Vol. 1, ed. Edward S. Katkin and Stephanie B. Manuck, Greenwich, CT: JAI, 1–37. Richins, Marsha L. (1991), “Social Comparison and the Idealized Images of Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (1), 71–83. Rosenberg, Morris (1965), Society and the Adolescent Self-Image, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Schifter, Deborah E., and Icek Ajzen (1985), “Intention, Perceived Control, and Weight Loss: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 49 (3), 843–51. Sniehotta, Falko F., Urte Scholz, and Ralf Schwarzer (2005), “Bridging the Intention-Behavior Gap: Planning, Self-Effi- cacy, and Action Control in the Adoption and Maintenance of Physical Exercise,” Psychology and Health, 20 (2), 143– 60. Soman, Dilip (2012), “Using Psychology to Enhance Household Savings,” Working Paper, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto. Soman, Dilip, and Amar Cheema (2004), “When Goals Are Coun- terproductive: The Effects of Violation of a Behavioral Goal on Subsequent Performance,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (1), 52–62. Thaler, Richard H., and Shlomo Benartzi (2004), “Save More To- morrow: Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Employee Saving,” Journal of Political Economy, 112 (1), S164–S187. U¨ lku¨men, Gu¨lden, and Amar Cheema (2011), “Framing Goals to Influence Personal Savings: The Role of Specificity and Con- strual Level,” Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (6), 958– 69. Webb, Thomas L., and Paschal Sheeran (2004), “Identifying Good Opportunities to Act: Implementation Intentions and Cue Dis- crimination,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 34 (4), 407–19. Zhang, Ying, Ayelet Fishbach, and Arie W. Kruglanski (2007), “The Dilution Model: How Additional Goals Undermine the Perceived Instrumentality of a Shared Path,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 92 (3), 389–401.
  249. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer (2000), “Accessibility or Diagnosticity? Disentangling the Influence of Culture on Persuasion Processes and Attitudes,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (March), 340–57. Aaker, Jennifer, and Angela Lee (2001), “‘I’ Seek Pleasures and ‘We’ Avoid Pains: The Role of Self-Regulatory Goals in Information Processing of Persuasion,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 28 (June), 33–49. Aaker, Jennifer L., and Bernd Schmitt (2001), “Culture-Dependent Assimilation and Differentiation of the Self: Preferences for Consumption Symbols in the United States and China,” Jour- nal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (September), 561–76. Aaker, Jennifer, and Jaideep Sengupta (2000), “Additivity versus Attenuation: The Role of Culture in the Resolution of Infor- mation Incongruity,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9 (2), 67–82. Agrawal, Nidhi, and Durairaj Maheswaran (2005), “The Effects of Self-Construal and Commitment on Persuasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (March), 841–49. Belk, Russell W. (1988), “Possessions and the Extended Self,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 139–68. Berger, Jonah, and Chip Heath (2007), “Where Consumers Diverge from Others: Identity Signaling and Product Categories,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (2), 121–34. ——— (2008), “Who Drives Divergence? Identity Signaling, Out- Group Dissimilarity, and the Abandonment of Cultural Tastes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (3), 593– 607. Bond, Michael H., and Tak-Sing Cheung (1983), “College Students’ Spontaneous Self-Concept: The Effect of Culture among Re- spondents in Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14 (2), 153–71. Branscombe, Nyla R., and Daniel L. Wann (1994), “Collective Self-Esteem Consequences of Outgroup Derogation When a Valued Social Identity Is on Trial,” European Journal of So- cial Psychology, 24 (6), 641–65. Brewer, Marilynn B. (1991), “The Social Self: On Being the Same and Different at the Same Time,” Personality and Social Psy- chology Bulletin, 17 (5), 475–82. Brewer, Marilynn B., and Wendi Gardner (1996), “Who Is This ‘We’? Levels of Collective Identity and Self-Representa- tions,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (1), 83–93. Brockner, Joel, and Ya-Ru Chen (1996), “The Moderating Roles of Self-Esteem and Self-Construal in Reaction to a Threat to the Self: Evidence from the People’s Republic of China and the United States,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 71 (3), 603–15. Cohen, Geoffrey L., and Julio Garcia (2005), “‘I Am Us’: Negative Stereotypes as Collective Threats,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (4), 566–82. Crocker, Jennifer, and Riia Luhtanen (1990), “Collective Self-Es- teem and In-Group Bias,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (1), 60–67. Crocker, Jennifer, Riia Luhtanen, Bruce Blaine, and Stephanie Broadnax (1994), “Collective Self-Esteem and Psychological Well-Being among White, Black, and Asian College Stu- dents,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (5), 503–13. Dhar, Ravi, and Itamar Simonson (1992), “The Effect of the Focus on Comparison on Consumer Preferences,” Journal of Mar- keting Research, 24 (November), 430–40. Doosje, Betjan, Naomi Ellemers, and Russel Spears (1994), “Per- ceived Intragroup Variability as a Function of Group Status and Identification,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 31 (5), 410–36. Ellemers, Naomi, Russell Spears, and Bertjan Doosje (1997), “Sticking Together or Falling Apart: In-Group Identification as a Psychological Determinant of Group Commitment versus Individual Mobility,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 72 (3), 617–26. Escalas, Jennifer Edison, and James R. Bettman (2005), “Self- Construal, Reference Groups, and Brand Meaning,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (3), 378–89. Forehand, Mark R., and Rohit Deshpande´ (2001), “What We See Makes Us Who We Are: Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response,” Journal of Marketing Research, 38 (3), 336–48. Gao, Leilei, S. Christian Wheeler, and Baba Shiv (2009), “The ‘Shaken Self’: Product Choices as a Means of Restoring Self- View Confidence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (1), 29–38. Gardner, Wendi L., Shira Gabriel, and Angela Y. Lee (1999), “‘I’ Value Freedom but ‘We’ Value Relationships: Self-Construal Priming Mirrors Cultural Differences in Judgment,” Psycho- logical Science, 10 (4), 321–26. Heatherton, Todd F., and Janet Polivy (1991), “Development and Validation of a Scale for Measuring State Self-Esteem,” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (6), 895–910. Heine, Steven J., Shinobu Kitayama, Darrin R. Lehman, Toshitake Takata, Eugene Ide, Cecilia Leung, and Hisaya Matsumoto (2001), “Divergent Consequences of Success and Failure in Japan and North America: An Investigation of Self-Improving Motivations and Malleable Selves,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (4), 599–615. Heine, Steven J., and Darrin R. Lehman (1995), “Cultural Variation in Unrealistic Optimism: Does the West Feel More Invul- nerable than the East?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 (4), 595–607. ——— (1997), “The Cultural Construction of Self-Enhancement: An Investigation of Group-Serving Biases,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 72 (6), 1268–83. Heine, Steven J., Darrin R. Lehman, Hazel R. Markus, and Shinobu Kitayama (1999), “Is There a Universal Need for Positive Self-Regard?” Psychological Review, 106 (4), 766–94. Hoshino-Browne, Etsuko, Adam S. Zanna, Steven J. Spencer, Mark P. Zanna, Shinobu Kitayama, and Sandra Lackenbauer (2005), “On the Cultural Guises of Cognitive Dissonance: The Case of Easterners and Westerners,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (3), 294–310. Kitayama, Shinobu, Hazel R. Markus, Hasaya Matsumoto, and Vinai Norasakkunkit (1997), “Individual and Collective Processes of Self-Esteem Management: Self-Enhancement in the United States and Self-Depreciation in Japan,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 (6), 1245–67. Kleine, Robert E., Susan S. Kleine, and Jerome B. Kernan (1993), “Mundane Consumption and the Self: A Social-Identity Per- spective,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2 (3), 209–35. Knowles, Megan L., and Wendi L. Gardner (2008), “Benefits of Membership: The Activation and Amplification of Group Identities in Response to Social Rejection,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 (9), 1200–1213. Krishna, Aradhna, Rongrong Zhou, and Shi Zhang (2008), “The Effect of Self-Construal on Spatial Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (3), 337–48. Kuhn, Manford H., and Thomas S. McPartland (1954), “An Em- pirical Investigation of Self-Attitudes,” American Sociologi- cal Review, 19 (1), 69–76. Laverie, Debra A., Robert E. Kleine III, and Susan Schultz Kleine (2002), “Reexamination and Extension of Kleine, Kleine, and Kernan’s Social Identity Model of Mundane Consumption: The Mediating Role of the Appraisal Process,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (4), 659–69. Leary, M. R., K. M. Kelly, C. A. Cottrell, and L. S. Schreindorfer (2012), ”Individual Differences in the Need to Belong: Map- ping the Nomological Network,” unpublished manuscript, Duke University. LeBoeuf, Robyn A., Eldar Shafir, and Jullia Belyavsky Bayuk (2010), “The Conflicting Choices of Alternating Selves,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111 (1), 48–61. Luhtanen, Riia, and Jennifer Crocker (1992), “A Collective Self- Esteem Scale: Self-Evaluation of One’s Social Identity,” Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18 (3), 302–18. Mandel, Naomi (2003), “Shifting Selves and Decision Making: The Effects of Self-Construal Priming on Consumer Risk- Taking,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (1), 30–40. Markus, Hazel R., and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), “Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motiva- tion,” Psychological Review, 98 (2), 224–53. Mead, Nicole L., Roy F. Baumeister, Tyler F. Stillman, Catherine D. Rawn, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2011), “Social Exclusion Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the Service of Affiliation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (5), 902–19. Mussweiler, Thomas, Shira Gabriel, and Galen V. Bodenhausen (2000), “Shifting Social Identities as a Strategy for Deflecting Threatening Social Comparisons,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (3), 398–409. Preacher, Kristopher J., and Andrew Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing In- direct Effects in Multiple Mediaton Models,” Behavior Re- search Methods, 40 (3), 879–91. Sedikides, Constantine (1993), “Assessment, Enhancement, and Verification Determinants on the Self-Evaluation Process,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (2), 317– 38. Shrout, Patrick E., and Niall Bolger (2002), “Mediation in Experimental and Nonexperimental Studies: New Proce- dures and Recommendations,” Psychological Methods, 7 (4), 422–45. Singelis, Theodore M. (1994), “The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (5), 580–91. Spears, Russell, Bertjan Doosje, and Naomi Ellemers (1997), “Self- Stereotyping in the Face of Threats to Group Status and Distinctiveness: The Role of Group Identification,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23 (5), 538–53. Steele, Claude M. (1988), “The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self,” in Advances in Exper- imental Social Psychology, Vol. 21, ed. Leonard Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press, 261–302. Steele, Claude M., and Thomas J. Liu (1983), “Dissonance Pro- cesses as Self-Affirmation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 (1), 5–19. Tajfel, Henri, and John C. Turner (1979), “An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict,” in The Social Psychology of Inter- group Relations, ed. William G. Austin and Stephen Worchel, Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole, 33–47. ——— (1986), The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior, Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Tesser, Abraham (2000), “On the Confluence of Self-Esteem Main- tenance Mechanisms,” Personality and Social Psychology Re- view, 4 (4), 290–99. Tesser, Abraham, and David P. Cornell (1991), “On the Confluence of Self Processes,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 27 (6), 501–26. Trafimow, David, Harry C. Triandis, and Sharon G. Goto (1991), “Some Tests of the Distinction between the Private Self and the Collective Self,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 60 (5), 649–55. Triandis, Harry C. (1989), “The Self and Social Behavior in Dif- fering Cultural Contexts,” Psychological Review, 96 (3), 506–20. Turner, John C. (1985), Social Categorization and the Self-Con- cept: A Social Cognitive Theory of Group Behavior, Green- wich, CT: JAI. White, Katherine, and Jennifer J. Argo (2009), “Social Identity Threat and Consumer Preferences,” Journal of Consumer Psy- chology, 19 (3), 313–25. ——— (2011), “When Imitation Doesn’t Flatter: The Role of Consumer Distinctiveness in Responses to Mimicry,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (December), 667–80. White, Katherine, and Darren W. Dahl (2006), “To Be or Not Be: The Influence of Dissociative Reference Groups on Consumer Pref- erences,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16 (4), 404–13. ——— (2007), “Are All Outgroups Created Equal? Consumer Identity and Dissociative Influence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (4), 525–36. White, Katherine, and Darrin R. Lehman (2005a), “Culture and Social Comparison Seeking: The Role of Self-Motives,” Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 (2), 232–42. ——— (2005b), “Looking on the Bright Side: Downward Coun- terfactual Thinking in Response to Negative Life Events,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 (10), 1413– 24. White, Katherine, Darrin R. Lehman, and Dov Cohen (2006), “Cul- ture, Self-Construal, and Affective Reactions to Successful and Unsuccessful Others,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42 (5), 582–92. White, Katherine, and Bonnie Simpson (2012), “The ‘Dos and Don’ts’ of Normative Influence: When Do (and Don’t) Nor- mative Messages Lead to Prosocial Action?” unpublished manuscript, Sauder School of Business, University of British Columbia. Zhang, Yinlong, and L. J. Shrum (2009), “The Influence of Self- Construal on Impulsive Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (February), 838–50.
  250. REFERENCES Adaval, Rashmi (2001), “Sometimes It Just Feels Right: The Dif- ferential Weighting of Affect-Consistent and Affect-Inconsis- tent Information,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (June), 1-17. Albarracin, Dolores, and G. Tarcan Kumkale (2003), “Affect as Information in Persuasion: A Model of Affect Identification and Discounting,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 84, 453–69. Albarracin, Dolores, and Robert S. Wyer (2001), “Elaborative and Nonelaborative Processing of a Behavior-Related Commu- nication,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (6), 691–705. Camerer, Colin F. (2003), Behavioral Game Theory, New York: Russell Sage Foundation; Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Cesario, Joseph, H. Grant, and E. Tory Higgins (2004), “Regu- latory Fit and Persuasion: Transfer from ‘Feeling Right,’” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 388–404. Cohen, Joel B., Michel T. Pham, and Eduardo B. Andrade (2008), “The Nature and Role of Affect in Consumer Behavior,” in Handbook of Consumer Psychology, ed. Curt Haugtvedt, Frank Kardes, and Paul Herr, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 297– 348. Denes-Raj, Veronika, and Seymour Epstein (1994), “Conflict be- tween Intuitive and Rational Processing: When People Behave against Their Better Judgment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (5), 819–29. Feldman, Jack M., and John G. Lynch Jr. (1988), “Self-Generated Validity and Other Effects of Measurement on Belief, Atti- tude, Intention, and Behavior,” Journal of Applied Psychol- ogy, 73 (3), 421–35. Gasper, Karen, and Gerald Clore (2000), “Do You Have to Pay Attention to Your Feelings to Be Influenced by Them?” Per- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26 (6), 698–711. Gilbert, Daniel T. (1991), “How Mental Systems Believe,” Amer- ican Psychologist, 46 (2), 107–19. Gorn, Gerald J., Marvin E. Goldberg, and Kunal Basu (1993), “Mood, Awareness and Product Evaluation,” Journal of Con- sumer Psychology, 2 (3), 237–56. Greifeneder, R., Herbert Bless, and Michel Tuan Pham (2011), “When Do People Rely on Affective and Cognitive Feelings in Judgment? A Review,” Personality and Social Psychology Review 15 (2), 107–41. Guth, Werner, Rolf Schmittberger, and Bernd Schwarze (1982), “An Experimental-Analysis of Ultimatum Bargaining,” Jour- nal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3 (4), 367–88. Isen, Alice M. (2001), “An Influence of Positive Affect on Decision Making in Complex Situations: Theoretical Issues with Prac- tical Implications,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11 (2), 75–85. Kenny, David A. (2008), “Mediation with Dichotomous Out- comes,” Research Note, University of Connecticut. Kim, Hakkyun, Kiwan Park, and Norbert Schwarz (2010), “Will This Trip Really Be Exciting? The Role of Incidental Emo- tions in Product Evaluation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 983–91. Knoch, Daria, Alvaro Pascual-Leone, Kaspar Meyer, Valerie Treyer, and Ernst Fehr (2006), “Diminishing Reciprocal Fair- ness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex,” Science, 314 (5800), 829–32. Lee, Leonard, On Amir, and Dan Ariely (2009), “In Search of Homo Economicus: Cognitive Noise and the Role of Emotion in Preference Consistency,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 173–87. Pham, Michel Tuan (1998), “Representativeness, Relevance, and the Use of Feelings in Decision Making,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 25 (2), 144–59. ——— (2004), “The Logic of Feeling,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (4), 360 69. ——— (2009), “The Lexicon and Grammar of Affect-as-Infor- mation in Consumer Decision Making: The GAIM,” in Social Psychology of Consumer Behavior, ed. Michaela Wa¨nke, Psy- chology Press, 167–200. Pham, Michel Tuan, and Tamar Avnet (2009), “Contingent Reli- ance on the Affect Heuristic as a Function of Regulatory Focus,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- cesses, 108 (2), 267–78. Pham, Michel Tuan, Joel B. Cohen, John W. Pracejus, and G. David Hughes (2001), “Affect Monitoring and the Primacy of Feel- ings in Judgment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (2), 167–88. Pham, Michel Tuan, Maggie Geuens, and Patrick DePelsmaker (2011), “Revisiting the Effects of Ad-Evoked Feelings on Brand Evaluations after 25 Years: An Analysis of Consumer Responses to 1,100 TV Commercials,” Working Paper, Co- lumbia University/University of Ghent. Pham, Michel Tuan, Leonard Lee, and Andrew T. Stephen (2012), “Feeling the Future: The Emotional Oracle Effect,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (October), forthcoming. Pham, Michel Tuan, Tom Meyvis, and Rongrong Zhou (2001), “Beyond the Obvious: Chronic Vividness of Imagery and the Use of Information in Decision-Making,” Organizational Be- havior and Human Decision Processes, 84 (2), 226–53. Raghunathan, Rajagopal, Michel Tuan Pham, and Kim P. Corfman (2006), “Informational Properties of Anxiety and Sadness, and Displaced Coping,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (4), 596–601. Salovey, Peter, Jack D. Mayer, S. L. Goldman, C. Turvey, and T. P. Palfai (1995), “Emotional Attention, Clarity, and Repair: Exploring Emotional Intelligence Using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale,” in Emotion, Disclosure, and Health, ed. J. W. Pen- nebaker, Washington, DC: American Psychological Associ- ation, 125–51. Sanfey, Alan G., James K. Rilling, Jessica A. Aronson, Leigh E. Nystrom, and Jonathan D. Cohen (2003), “The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game,” Sci- ence, 300 (5626), 1755–58. Schwarz, Norbert, and Gerald L. Clore (1983), “Mood, Misattri- bution, and Judgments of Well-Being: Informative and Di- rective Functions of Affective States,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 (3), 513–23. ——— (1988), “How Do I Feel about It? The Information Func- tion of Affective States,” in Affect, Cognition and Social Be- havior: New Evidence and Integrative Attempts, ed. Klaus Fiedler and Joseph Forgas, Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe. ——— (2007), “Feelings and Phenomenal Experiences,” in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, ed. Arie W. Krug- lanski and E. T. Higgins, 2nd ed., Guilford, 385–407. Schwarz, Norbert, Fritz Strack, Detlev Kommer, and Dirk Wagner (1987), “Soccer, Rooms, and the Quality of Your Life: Mood Effects on Judgments of Satisfaction with Life in General and with Specific Domains,” European Journal of Social Psy- chology, 17 (1), 69–79. Shiv, Baba, and Alexander Fedorikhin (1999), “Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Con- sumer Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), 278–92. Siemer, Matthias, and Ranier Reisenzein (1998), “Effects of Mood on Evaluative Judgments: Influence of Reduced Processing Capacity and Mood Salience,” Cognition and Emotion, 12 (6), 783–805. Stephen, Andrew T., and Michel Tuan Pham (2008), “On Feelings as a Heuristic for Making Offers in Ultimatum Negotiations,” Psychological Science, 19 (10), 1051–58. Strack, Fritz (1992), “The Different Routes to Social Judgment: Experiential versus Informational Strategies,” in The Con- struction of Social Judgement, ed. Leonard L. Martin and Abraham Tesser, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. White, Katherine, and Cathy McFarland (2009), “When Do Moods Influence Consumers’ Product Evaluations? The Role of Mood-Acknowledgment and Perceived Appropriateness of Using Moods,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19 (3), 526–36. Winkielman, Piotr, Robert B. Zajonc, and Norbert Schwarz (1997), “Subliminal Affective Priming Resists Attributional Inter- ventions,” Cognition and Emotion, 11, 433–65. Wilson, Timothy D., and Nancy Brekke (1994), “Mental Contam- ination and Mental Correction: Unwanted Influences on Judg- ments and Evaluations,” Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117– 42. Yeung, Catherine W. M., and Robert S. Wyer (2004), “Affect, Appraisal, and Consumer Judgment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 412–24.
  251. REFERENCES Arnold, Stephen J., Tae H. Oum, and Douglas J. Tigert (1983), “Determining Attributes in Retail Patronage: Seasonal, Tem- poral, Regional, and International Comparisons,” Journal of Marketing Research, 20 (May), 149–57. Baumol, William J., and Edward A. Ide (1956), “Variety in Re- tailing,” Management Science, 3 (1), 93–101. Berger, Jonah, Michaela Draganska, and Itamar Simonson (2007), “The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice,” Marketing Science, 26 (July–August), 460–72. Botti, Simona, and Sheena S. Iyengar (2006), “The Dark Side of Choice: When Choice Impairs Social Welfare,” Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 25 (1), 24–38. Boyd, D. Eric, and Kenneth D. Bahn (2009), “When Do Large Product Assortments Benefit Customers? An Information- Processing Perspective,” Journal of Retailing, 85 (Septem- ber), 288–97. Broniarczyk, Susan M. (2008), “Product Assortment,” in Handbook of Consumer Psychology, New York: CRC Press, 755–77. Broniarczyk, Susan M., Wayne D. Hoyer, and Leigh McAlister (1998), “Consumers’ Perceptions of the Assortment Offered in a Grocery Category: The Impact of Item Reduction,” Jour- nal of Marketing Research, 35 (May), 166–76. Chernev, Alexander (2003), “When More Is Less and Less Is More: The Role of Ideal Point Availability and Assortment in Con- sumer Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (Septem- ber), 170–83. ——— (2006), “Decision Focus and Consumer Choice among As- sortments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (June), 50–59. Chernev, Alexander, Ulf Bo¨ckenholt, and Joseph Goodman (2010), “Choice Overload: Is There Anything to It?” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 37 (October), 426–28. Chernev, Alexander, and Ryan Hamilton (2009), “Assortment Size and Option Attractiveness in Consumer Choice among Re- tailers,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (June), 410–20. Day, George S., Allan D. Shocker, and Rajendra K. Srivastava (1979), “Customer-Oriented Approaches to Identifying Prod- uct-Markets,” Journal of Marketing, 43 (Fall), 8–19. Day, Samuel B., and Daniel M. Bartels (2008), “Representation Over Time: The Effects of Temporal Distance on Similarity,” Cognition, 106 (3), 1504–13. Dhar, Ravi, Stephen M. Nowlis, and Steven J. Sherman (1999), “Comparison Effects on Preference Construction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), 293–306. Diehl, Kristin, and Cait Poynor (2010), “Great Expectations?! As- sortment Size, Expectations and Satisfaction,” Journal of Marketing Research, 47 (April), 312–22. Eyal, Tal, Nira Liberman, Yaacov Trope, and Eva Walther (2004), “The Pros and Cons of Temporally Near and Distant Action,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (6), 781– 95. Fo¨rster, Jens, Nira Liberman, and Stephanie Kuschel (2008), “The Effect of Global versus Local Processing on Assimilation versus Contrast in Social Judgment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94 (4), 579–99. Fujita, Kent, Tal Eyal, Shelly Chaiken, Yaakov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2008), “Influencing Attitudes toward Near and Dis- tant Objects,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 227 (21), 9044–62. Fujita, Kent, Yaakov Trope, Nira Liberman, and Maya Levin-Sagi (2006), “Construal Levels and Self-Control,” Journal of Per-Goodman, Joseph K., Susan M. Broniarczyk, Jill G. Griffin, and Leigh McAlister (2012), “Help or Hinder? Recommendation Signage and Choice Expansion from Large Assortments,” un- published manuscript, Washington University in St. Louis, Olin Business School, St. Louis, Missouri. Goodman, Joseph K., Cynthia E. Cryder, and Amar A. Cheema (2012), “Data Collection in a Flat World: Accelerating Be- havioral Research by Using Mechanical Turk,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, forthcoming. Gourville, John T., and Dilip Soman (2005), “Overchoice and As- sortment Type: When and Why Variety Backfires,” Marketing Science, 24 (3), 382 95. Hoch, Stephen J., Eric T. Bradlow, and Brian Wansink (1999), “The Variety of an Assortment,” Marketing Science, 18 (4), 527–46. Huffman, Cynthia, and Barbara E. Kahn (1998), “Variety for Sale: Mass Customization or Mass Confusion,” Journal of Retail- ing, 74 (Winter), 491–513. Inbar, Yoel, Simona Botti, and Karlene Hanko (2010) “Decision Speed and Choice Regret: When Haste Feels Like Waste,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47 (3), 533–40. Iyengar, Sheena S. (2010), The Art of Choosing, New York: Twelve. Iyengar, Sheena S., and Mark Lepper (2000), “When Choice Is De- motivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 995–1006. Kahn, Barbara E., and Donald R. Lehmann (1991), “Modeling Choice among Assortments,” Journal of Retailing, 67 (Fall), 274–99. Kahn, Barbara E., William L. Moore, and Rashi Glazer (1987), “Experiments in Constrained Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (June), 96–113. Khan, Uzma, Meng Zhu, and Ajay Kalra (2011), “When Trade- Offs Matter: The Effect of Choice Construal on Context Ef- fects,” Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (February), 62–71. Kuksov, Dmitri, and J. Miguel Villas-Boas (2010), “When More Alternatives Lead to Less Choice,” Marketing Science, 29 (3), 507–24. Levy, M., and Weitz, B. A. (2001), Retailing Management, 4th ed., Boston: McGraw-Hill. Liberman, Nira, Michael D. Sagristano, and Yaakov Trope (2002), “The Effect of Temporal Distance on Level of Construal,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38 (6), 523–35. Liberman, Nira, and Yaakov Trope (1998), “The Role of Feasibility and Desirability Considerations in Near and Distant Future Decisions: A Test of Temporal Construal Theory,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (1), 5–18. Liviatan, Ido, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2008), “Inter- personal Similarity as a Social Distance Dimension: Impli- cations for Perception of Others’ Actions,” Journal of Ex- perimental Social Psychology, 44 (5), 1256–69. Malkoc, Selin A., and Gal Zauberman (2006), “Deferring versus Expediting Consumption: The Effect of Outcome Concrete- ness on Sensitivity to Time Horizon,” Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (November), 618–27. Malkoc, Selin, Gal Zauberman, and Jim R. Bettman (2010), “Un- stuck from the Concrete: Carryover Effects of Abstract Mind- sets in Intertemporal Preferences,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113 (2), 112–26. Malkoc, Selin A., Gal Zauberman, and Canan Ulu (2005) “Con- suming Now or Later? The Interactive Effect of Timing and Attribute Alignability,” Psychological Science, 16 (5), 411–17. McAlister, Leigh, and Edgar Pessemier (1982), “Variety Seeking Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Review,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (December), 311–22. Mogilner, Cassie, Tamar Rudnick, and Sheena S. Iyengar (2005), “The Mere Categorization Effect: How the Presence of Cat- egories Increases Choosers’ Perceptions of Assortment Va- riety and Outcome Satisfaction,” Journal of Consumer Re- search, 35 (August), 202–15. Morales, Andrea, Barbara E. Kahn, Leigh McAlister, and Susan Bron- iarczyk (2005), “Perceptions of Assortment Variety: The Effects of Congruency between Consumer’s Internal and Retailer’s Ex- ternal Organization,” Journal of Retailing, 81 (2), 159–69. Nowlis, Stephen, Ravi Dhar, and Itamar Simonson (2010), “The Effect of Decision Order on Purchase Quantity Decisions,” Journal of Marketing Research, 47 (August), 725–37. Preacher, Kristopher J., and Andrew F. Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing In- direct Effects in Multiple Mediator Models,” Behavior Re- search Methods, 40 (3), 879–91. Ratner, Rebecca K., Barbara E. Kahn, and Daniel Kahneman (1999), “Choosing Less-Preferred Experiences for the Sake of Variety,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (June), 1–15. Ratneshwar, S., and Allan D. Shocker (1991), “Substitution in Use and the Role of Usage Context in Product Category Struc- tures,” Journal of Marketing Research, 28 (August), 281–95. Redden, Joseph P. (2008), “Reducing Satiation: The Role of Cat- egorization Level,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (Feb- ruary), 624–34. Redden, Joseph P., and Stephen J. Hoch (2009), “The Presence of Variety Reduces Perceived Quantity,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (October), 406–17. Rosch, Eleanor, and Carolyn Mervis (1975), “Family Resem- blances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories,” Cog- nitive Psychology, 7, 573–605. Scheibehenne, Benjamin, Rainer Greifeneder, and Peter M. Todd (2010), “Can There Ever Be Too Many Options? A Meta- Analytic Review of Choice Overload,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (October), 409–25. Schwartz, Barry (2004), The Paradox of Choice, New York: Harper Collins. Schwartz, Barry, Andrew Ward, John Monterosso, Sonja Lyubom- irsky, Katherine White, and Darrin Lehman (2002), “Maxi- mizing versus Satisficing: Happiness Is a Matter of Choice,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (Novem- ber), 1178–97. Sood, Sanjay, Yuval Rottenstreich, and Lyle Brenner (2004), “On Decisions That Lead to Decisions: Direct and Derived Eval- uations of Preference,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (June), 17–25. Srivastava, Rajendra K., Mark I. Alpert, and Allan D. Shocker (1984), “A Customer-Oriented Approach for Determining Mar- ket Structures,” Journal of Marketing, 48 (Spring), 32–45. Todorov, Alexander, Amir Goren, and Yaacov Trope (2007), “Prob- ability as a Psychological Distance: Construal and Preferences,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43 (3), 473–82. Trope, Yaakov, and Nira Liberman (2000), “Temporal Construal and Time-Dependent Changes in Preference,” Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 876–89. ——— (2003), “Temporal Construal,” Psychological Review, 110 (3), 403–21. ——— (2010), “Construal Level Theory and Psychological Dis- tance,” Psychological Review, 117 (2), 440–63. Zauberman, Gal, and John G. Lynch (2005), “Resource Slack and Propensity to Discount Delayed Investments of Time versus Money,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134 (1), 23–37. Zhu, Meng, and Ajay Kalra (2012), ”Limiting Supply of One Re- course Decreases Overacquisition of Unrelated Resources,” working paper, Johns Hopkins University, Carey Business School, Baltimore, Maryland. sonality and Social Psychology, 90 (3), 351–67.
  252. REFERENCES Ariely, Dan (2000), “Controlling the Information Flow: Effects on Consumers’ Decision Making and Preferences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 27 (September), 233–48. Bettman, James, Eric J. Johnson, and John W. Payne (1991), “Con- sumer Decision Making,” in Handbook of Consumer Behav- ior, ed. Thomas S. Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian, En- glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 50–84. Bolton, Lisa E., and Joseph W. Alba (2006), “Price Fairness: Good and Service Differences and the Role of Vendor Costs,” Jour- nal of Consumer Research, 33 (September), 258–65. Chaiken, Shelly, Akiva Liberman, and Alice H. Eagly (1989), “Heuristic and Systematic Information Processing within and beyond the Persuasion Context,” in Unintended Thought, ed. James S. Uleman and John A. Bargh, New York: Guilford, 212–52. Chaiken, Shelly, and Durairaj Maheswaran (1994), “Heuristic Pro- cessing Can Bias Systematic Processing: Effects of Source Credibility, Argument Ambiguity, and Task Importance on Attitude Judgment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 66 (3), 460–73. Chapman, Randall G., and Richard Staelin (1982), “Exploiting Rank Ordered Choice Set Data within the Stochastic Utility Model,” Journal of Marketing Research, 19 (August), 288– 301. Chen, Haipeng, Sharon Ng, and Akshay R. Rao (2005), “Cultural Differences in Consumer Impatience,” Journal of Marketing Research, 42 (August), 291–301. Clarkson, Joshua, Zakary L. Tormala, and Derek Rucker (2008), “A New Look at the Consequences of Attitude Certainty: The Amplification Hypothesis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (4), 810–25. Darby, Michael R., and Edi Karni (1973), “Free Competition and the Optimal Amount of Fraud,” Journal of Law and Eco- nomics, 16 (1), 67–88. Gentner, Dedre, and Arthur B. Markman (1997), “Structural Align- ment in Analogy and Similarity,” American Psychologist, 52 (1), 45–56. Grant, Susan Jung, and Alice M. Tybout (2008), “The Effect of Temporal Frame on Information Considered in New Product Evaluation: The Role of Uncertainty,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (April), 897–913. Hsee, Christopher, and Jiao Zhang (2004), “Distinction Bias: Mis- prediction and Mischoice Due to Joint Evaluation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (5), 680–95. Kahn, Barbara E. (1995), “Consumer Variety-Seeking among Goods and Services: An Integrative Review,” Journal of Re- tailing and Consumer Services, 2 (3), 139–48. Kahn, Barbara E., and Robert J. Meyer (1991), “Consumer Multi- attribute Judgments under Attribute-Weight Uncertainty,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (March), 508–22. Keh, Hean Tat, and Jun Pang (2010), “Customer Reactions to Service Separation,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (2), 55–70. Keh, Hean Tat, and Jin Sun (2008), “The Complexities of Perceived Risk in Cross-Cultural Services Marketing,” Journal of In- ternational Marketing, 16 (1), 120–46. Kivetz, Ran, and Itamar Simonson (2000), “The Effects of Incom- plete Information on Consumer Choice,” Journal of Market- ing Research, 37 (November), 427–48. Laroche, Michel, Zhiyong Yang, Gordon H. G. McDougall, and Jasmin Bergeron (2005), “Internet versus Bricks-and-Mortar Retailers: An Investigation into Intangibility and Its Conse- quences,” Journal of Retailing, 81 (4), 251–67. Lovelock, Christopher H. (1983), “Classifying Services to Gain Strategic Marketing Insights,” Journal of Marketing, 47 (3), 9–20. Malkoc, Selin A., Gal Zauberman, and Canan Ulu (2005), “Con- suming Now or Later? The Interactive Effect of Timing and Attribute Alignability,” Psychological Science, 16 (5), 411– 17. Markman, Arthur B., and Douglas L. Medin (1995), “Similarity and Alignment in Choice,” Organizational Behavior and Hu- man Decision Processes, 63 (2), 117–30. Medin, Douglas L., Robert L. Goldstone, and Arthur B. Markman (1995), “Comparison and Choice: Relations between Simi- larity Processes and Decision Processes,” Psychonomic Bul- letin and Review, 2 (March), 1–19. Mitra, Kaushik, Michelle C. Reiss, and Louis M. Capella (1999), “An Examination of Perceived Risk, Information Search and Behavioral Intentions in Search, Experience and Credence Services,” Journal of Services Marketing, 13 (3), 208–32. Mittal, Banwari (1999), “The Advertising of Services: Meeting the Challenge of Intangibility,” Journal of Service Research, 2 (1), 98–116. Murray, Keith B., and John L. Schlacter (1990), “The Impact of Services versus Goods on Consumers’ Assessment of Per- ceived Risk and Variability,” Journal of the Academy of Mar- keting Science, 18 (1), 51–65. Nam, Myungwoo, Jing Wang, and Angela Y. Lee (2012), “The Difference between Differences: How Expertise Affects Di- agnosticity of Attribute Alignability,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (December), 736–50. Nowlis, Stephen M., and Itamar Simonson (1997), “Attribute-Task Compatibility as a Determinant of Consumer Preference Re- versals,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34 (May), 205–18. Ostrom, Amy, and Dawn Iacobucci (1995), “Consumer Trade-offs and the Evaluation of Services,” Journal of Marketing, 59 (January), 17–28. Preacher, Kristopher J., and Andrew F. Hayes (2004), “SPSS and SAS Procedures for Estimating Indirect Effects in Simple Mediation Models,” in “Web-Based Archive of Norms, Stim- uli, and Data: Part 2,” special issue, Behavior Research Meth- ods, Instruments and Computers, 36 (4), 717–31. Putrevu, Sanjay, Joni Tan, and Kenneth R. Lord (2004), “Consumer Responses to Complex Advertisements: The Moderating Role of Need for Cognition, Knowledge, and Gender,” Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 26 (1), 9–24. Reeves, Carol A., and David A. Bednar (1994), “Defining Quality: Alternatives and Implications,” Academy of Management Re- view, 19 (3), 419–45. Rosenthal, Robert, and Ralph L. Rosnow (1985), Contrast Anal- ysis: Focused Comparisons in the Analysis of Variance, Cam- bridge: Cambridge University Press. Tiedens, Larissa Z., and Susan Linton (2001), “Judgment under Emotional Certainty and Uncertainty: The Effects of Specific Emotion on Information Processing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (December), 973–88. Trope, Yaacov, and Shelly Chaiken (1999), Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology, New York: Guilford. Urbany, Joel E., Peter R. Dickson, and William L. Wilkie (1989), “Buyer Uncertainty and Information Search,” Journal of Con- sumer Research, 16 (September), 208–15. Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1981), “How Consumer Evaluation Processes Differ between Goods and Services,” in Marketing of Ser- vices, ed. James H. Donnelly and William R. George, Chi- cago: American Marketing Association, 186–90. Zeithaml, Valarie A., A. Parasuraman, and Leonard L. Berry (1985), “Problems and Strategies in Services Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 49 (Spring), 33–46. Zhang, Shi, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (1999), “Choice-Process Sat- isfaction: The Influence of Attribute Alignability and Option Limitation,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 77 (3), 192–214. Zhang, Shi, Frank R. Kardes, and Maria L. Cronley (2002), “Com- parative Advertising: Effects of Structural Alignability on Tar- get Brand Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12 (4), 303–11. Zhang, Shi, and Arthur B. Markman (1998), “Overcoming the Early Entrant Advantage: The Role of Alignable and Non- alignable Differences,” Journal of Marketing Research, 35 (November), 413–26. ——— (2001), “Processing Product Unique Features: Alignability and Involvement in Preference Construction,” Journal of Con- sumer Psychology, 11 (1), 13–27. Zhao, Xinshu, John G. Lynch Jr., and Qimei Chen (2010), “Re- considering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Me- diation Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (Au- gust), 197–206.
  253. Excerpt from Chapter 7: 'The Marketing Imagination', of Professor Theodore Levitt's book, The Marketing Imagination , New York/London, Copyright © 1983 by The Free Press
  254. References Blei, D., and Lafferty, J. 2009. Topic models. In Srivastava, A., and Sahami, M., eds., Text Mining: Theory and Applications. Taylor and Francis. Blei, D.; Ng, A.; and Jordan, M. 2003. Latent Dirichlet allocation. Journal of Machine Learning Research 3:993–1022. Cao, N.; Sun, J.; Lin, Y.-R.; Gotz, D.; Liu, S.; and Qu, H. 2010. FacetAtlas: Multifaceted Visualization for Rich Text Cor- pora. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics 16(6):1172 – 1181. Chang, J.; Boyd-Graber, J.; Wang, C.; Gerrish, S.; and Blei, D. 2009. Reading tea leaves: How humans interpret topic models. In Neural Information Processing Systems. Chen, Y.; Wang, L.; Dong, M.; and Hua, J. 2009. Exemplar-based visualization of large document corpus. IEEE Transactions on Vi- sualization and Computer Graphics 15(6):1161–1168. Gardener, M. J.; Lutes, J.; Lund, J.; Hansen, J.; Walker, D.; Ring- ger, E.; and Seppi, K. 2010. The topic browser: An interactive tool for browsing topic models. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Challenges of Data Visualization (in conjunction with NIPS). Gretarsson, B.; O’Donovan, J.; Bostandjiev, S.; Asuncion, A.; Newman, D.; Smyth, P.; and Hllerer, T. 2011. Topicnets: Visual analysis of large text corpora with topic modeling. In ACM Trans- actions on Intelligent Systems and Technology, 1–26. Havre, S.; Hetzler, B.; and Nowell, L. 2000. Themeriver(tm): In search of trends, patterns, and relationships. In Proc. IEEE Sympo- sium on Information Visualization (InfoVis), 115–123. Newman, D.; Asuncion, A.; Chemudugunta, C.; Kumar, V.; Smyth, P.; and Steyvers, M. 2006. Exploring large document collections using statistical topic models. In KDD-2006 Demo Session. Steyvers, M., and Griffiths, T. 2006. Probabilistic topic models. In Landauer, T.; McNamara, D.; Dennis, S.; and Kintsch, W., eds., Latent Semantic Analysis: A Road to Meaning. Laurence Erlbaum.
  255. Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/stu_hon_theses Recommended Citation Heinstein, Raven, "Visual Tension in Graphic Design" (2014). Honors College Capstone Experience/Th esis Projects. Paper 469. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/stu_hon_theses/469 WORKS CITED Samara, Timothy. Design Elements: A Graphic Style Manual. Beverly, MA: Rockport Publishers, Inc., 2007. 11-249. Print. Gatta, Kevin, Gusty Lange, and Marilyn Lyons. Foundations of Graphic Design. Worcester, MA: Davis Publications, Inc., 1991. Print. Berryman, Gregg. Notes On Graphic Design And Visual Communication. Revised. Los Altos, CA: William Kaufmann, Inc., 1984. Print. Rand, Paul. A Designer's Art. New Haven and London: Yale University Press, 1985. Print. Mullet, Kevin, and Darrell Sano. Designing visual interfaces: communication oriented techniques. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: SunSoft Press, 1995. Print. Meggs, Philip. Type & Image: The Language of Graphic Design. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1989. Print. Lauer, David A., and Stephen Pentak. Design basics. Fifth ed. Orlando, FL: Harcourt College Publishers, 2000. Print.
  256. REFERENCES [1] Löfgren M, Witell L, Gustafsson A. “Customer satisfaction in the first and second moments of truth,” Journal of Product and Brand Management, vol. 17, (7) pp. 463-474, 2008. Link [2] Young S. “New and improved indeed: documenting the business value of packaging innovation,” Quirk’s Marketing Research Review, vol. 22, (1) pp. 46-50, 2008. [3] Azzi A, Battini D, Persona A, Sgarbossa F. “Packaging design: general framework and research agenda,” Packaging Technology and Science, vol. 25, (8) pp. 435-456, 2012. Link [4] Ampuero O, Vila N. “Consumer perceptions of product packaging,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, vol. 23, (2) pp. 102-114, 2006. Link [5] Underwood R, Klein N. “Packaging as brand communication: Effects of product pictures on consumer responses to the package and brand,” Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, vol.10, (4) pp. 58-68, 2002. Link [6] Hess J, Story J, Danes J. “A three-stage model of consumer relationship investment,” Journal of Product & Brand Management, vol. 20, (1) pp. 14 – 26, 2011. Link [7] Story J, Hess J. “Ethical brand management: customer relationships and ethical duties,” Journal of Product & Brand Management, vol. 19, (4) pp. 240-249, 2010. Link [8] Underwood R. “The communicative power of product packaging: Creating brand identify via lived and mediated experience,” Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, vol. 11, (1) pp. 62- 76, 2003. Link [9] Abbott D. A. Packaging Perspectives, Dubuque, Kendall/Hunt Publishing Co., 1989. [10] Singh S P, Singh J, Grewal G, Chonhenchob V. “Analyzing color on printed packaging to evaluate brand logo integrity and impact on marketing,” Universal Journal of Marketing and Business Research, vol. 1, (3) pp. 79-88, 2012. Link [11] Singh S P, and Singh J. “Pictorial markings and labels for safe transport and handling of packaged goods,” Journal of Packaging Technology and Science, vol. 18, (3) pp. 133–140, 2005. Link [12] Esch F, Moll T, Schmitt B, Elger C, Neuhaus C, Weber B. “Brands on the brain: Do consumers use declarative information or experienced emotions to evaluate brands?” Journal of Consumer Psychology, vol. 22, (1) pp. 75-85, 2012. Link [13] Connolly A, and Davison L. “How does design affect decisions at point of sale?” Journal of Brand Management, vol. 4, (4) pp. 100-107, 1996. Link [14] Gofman A, Moskowitz H, Mets T. “Accelerating structured consumer- driven package design,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, vol. 27, (2) pp. 157-168, 2012. Link [15] Silayoi P, Speece M. “The importance of packaging attributes: a conjoint analysis approach,” European Journal of Marketing, vol. 41, (11-12) pp. 1495 – 1517, 2007. Link [16] Wells L, Farley H, Armstrong G. “The importance of packaging design for own- label food brands,” International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, vol. 35, (9) pp. 677-690, 2007. Link [17] Bloch P, Brunel F, Arnold T. “Individual differences in the centrality of visual product aesthetics: concept and measurement,” Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 29, (4) pp. 551-565, 2003. Link [18] Karjalainen T, Snelders D. “Designing visual recognition for the brand,” Journal of Product Innovation Management, vol. 27, (1) pp. 6-22, 2009. Link [19] Moskowitz H, Rabino S, Beckley J. “Concept articulation and web research: a case study using quali-quant methods,” Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, vol. 10, (3) pp. 282-299, 2007. Link [20] Metcalf L, Hess J, Danes J, Singh J. “A mixed-methods approach for designing market-driven packaging,” Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, vol. 15, (3) pp. 268-289, 2012. Link [21] Story J, Hess J. “Segmenting consumer- brand relations: beyond the personal relationship metaphor,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, vol. 23, (7) pp. 406-413, 2006. Link [22] Aaker D, Keller K. “Consumer evaluations of brand extensions,” Journal of Marketing, vol. 54, (1) pp. 27-41, 1990. Link [23] Morgan R, Hunt S. “The commitment- trust theory of relationship marketing,” Journal of Marketing, vol. 58, (3) pp. 20-38, 1994. Link [24] Aaker D. Managing Brand Equity, New York: The Free Press, 1990. [25] Thye S, Yoon J, Lawler E. “The theory of relational cohesion: review of a research,” Advances in Group Processes, vol. 19, pp. 139-166, 2002. Link [26] Rusbult C. “Commitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: a test of the investment model,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vol.16, pp. 172-186, 1980. Link [27] Rusbult, C. “A Longitudinal test of the investment model: the development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 45, (1) pp. 101-117, 1983. Link [28] Delgado-Ballester E, Munuera-Aleman L. “Brand trust in the context of consumer loyalty,” European Journal of Marketing, vol. 35, (11-12) pp.1238- 1258, 2001. Link [29] Garbarino E, Johnson S. “The different roles of satisfaction, trust, and commitment in customer relationships,” Journal of Marketing, vol. 63, (2) pp. 70-87, 1999. Link [30] Sirdeshmukh D, Singh J, Sabol B. “Consumer trust, value, and loyalty in relational exchanges,” Journal of Marketing, vol. 66, (1) pp. 15-37, 2002. Link [31] Hess J, Story J. “Trust-based commitment: multidimensional consumer-brand relationships,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, vol. 22, (6) pp. 313-22, 2005. Link [32] Fournier S. “Consumers and their brands: developing relationship theory in consumer research,” Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 24, (2) pp. 343-373, 1998. Link [33] Zeithaml V. “Consumer perceptions of price, quality, and value: a means-end model of synthesis and evidence,” Journal of Marketing, vol. 52, (2) pp. 2-22, 1988. Link [34] Stizhakova Y, Coulter R, Price L. “Branding in a global marketplace: The mediating effects of quality and self-identity brand signals,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, vol. 28, (4) pp. 342-351, 2011. Link [35] Hess J and Story J. “Trust-based commitment: multidimensional consumer-brand relationships,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, vol. 22, (6) pp. 313-22. Link [36] Sheth J, Parvatiyar A. “Relationship marketing in consumer markets: antecedents and consequences,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, vol. 23, (4) pp. 255-271, 1995. Link [37] GfK Mediamark Research & Intelligence. Fall 2011 Product beverage report. GfK MRI Reporter. [38] Jöreskog K, Sörbom D. LISREL 7: A Guide to the Programs and Application, 2d. ed. Chicago: SPSS Inc., 1989. [39] Savalei V, Bentler P M. “Structural equation modeling,” in The Handbook of Marketing Research: Uses, Misuses, and Future Advances, Grover, R. and Vriens, M., Eds. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, 2006, pp. 330-364. Link [40] James L, Mulaik S, Brett J. Causal Analysis: Models, Assumptions and Data, BeverlyHills: Sage Publications, 1982. [41] Hair J, Black W, Babin B, Anderson R, Tatham R. Multivariate Data Analysis, 6d. ed. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall, 2006. [42] Bredahl L. “Cue utilization and quality perception with regard to branded beef,” Food Quality and Science, vol. 15, (1) pp. 65-75, 2004. Link [43] Woodside A. “Consumer evaluations of competing brands: Perceptual versus predictive validity,” Psychology and Marketing, vol. 29, (6) pp. 458-466, 2012. Link
  257. Suggested Citation Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2011). Th e bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas [Electronic version]. Retrieved [insert date], from Cornell University, ILR School site: http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/450/ REFERNCES Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to "The Social Psychology of Creativity." (Vol. 317). Boulder, CO, US: Westview Press. Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 367-403. Audia, P., & Goncalo, J. (2007). Past success and creativity over time: A study of inventors in the hard disk drive industry. Management Science, 53(1), 1. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148. Cai, H., Sriram, N., Greenwald, A. G., & McFarland, S. G. (2004). The Implicit Association Test's "D" Measure can Minimize a Cognitive Skill Confound: Comment on McFarland and Crouch (2002). Social Cognition, 22(6), 673-684. Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5-13. Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391- 404. Dawson, V. L., D'Andrea, T., Affinito, R., & Westby, E. L. (1999). Predicting creative behavior: A reexamination of the divergence between traditional and teacher-defined concepts of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 12(1), 57-66. Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., & Pattershall, J. (2009). The existence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 765-775. Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290-309. Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill Flynn, F. J., & Chatman, J. A. (2001). Strong cultures and innovation: Oxymoron or opportunity? . In T. Holden, N. Sparrow & W. Starbuck (Eds.), International Handbook of Organizational Culture and Climate (pp. 263-287). Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. Ford, C. M., & Gioia, D. A. (2000). Factors Influencing Creativity in the Domain of Managerial Decision Making. Journal of Management, 26(4), 705-732. Gaertner, S., & Dovidio, J. (Eds.). (1986). The aversive form of racism. New York: Academic Press. George, J. M. (2007). Chapter 9: Creativity in Organizations. The Academy of Management Annals, 1(1), 439 - 477. Greenwald, A., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4-27. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197-216. Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17-41. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569- 598. Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M., & Mueller, J. S. (2010). Chapter 46: Consensual Assessment. . In Encyclopedia of Creativity, 4th edition. Metcalfe, J. (1986). Premonitions of insight predict impending error. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(4), 623-634. Moscovici, S. (1976). Social influence and social change. London: UK: Academic Press. Nemeth, C. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence. Psychological Review, 93(1), 23-32. Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. (2006). The philosophical roots of Western and Eastern conceptions of creativity. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. Vol, 26(1-2), 18-38. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers. Special Issue: Web-based archive of norms, stimuli, and data: Part 2, 36(4), 717-731. Rietzschel, E., Nijstad, B., & Stroebe, W. (2009). The selection of creative ideas after individual idea generation: Choosing between creativity and impact. British Journal of Psychology, 0(1-23). Rubenson, D. L., & Runco, M. A. (1995). The psychoeconomic view of creative work in groups and organizations. Creativity and Innovation Management, 4(4), 232-241. Runco, M. A. (1989). Parents' and teachers' ratings of the creativity of children. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 4(1), 73-83. Runco, M. A. (2010). Creativity has no dark side. In D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman & M. A. Runco (Eds.), The Dark Side of Creativity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Runco, M. A., & Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative ideas. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(3), 295-302. Sanchez-Burks, J. (2005). Protestant relational ideology: The cognitive underpinnings and organizational implications of an American anomaly. Research in organizational behavior, 26, 265-305. Simonton, D. K. (1984). Genius, Creativity, and Leadership: Histriometric Inquiries. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press Staw, B. M. (1995). Why no one really wants creativity. In C. Ford & D. A. Gioia (Eds.), Creative Action in Organizations: Ivory Tower Visions and Real World Voices. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 607-627. Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973-988. West, M. A. (2002). Ideas are ten a penny: It's team implementation not idea generation that counts. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51(3), 411-424. Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the classroom? Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 1-10. Whitson, J., & Galinsky, A. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322(5898), 115. Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 224-228.
  258. References Bostrom, N. (2003). Are You Living in a Computer Simulation? Philosophical Quarterly, 53(211), 243-255. Bostrom. N. (2006). What is a Singleton? Linguistic and Philosophical Investigations, 5(2), 48-54. Bostrom, N. (2012). Information Hazards: A Typology of Potential Harms from Knowledge. Review of Contemporary Philosophy, 10, 44-79. [www.nickbostrom.com/information-hazards.pdf] Chalmers, D. (2010): The Singularity: A Philosophical Analysis. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17, 7-65. Chislenko, A. (1997). Technology as Extension of Human Functional Architecture. Extropy Online. [project.cyberpunk.ru/idb/technology_as_extension.html] de Blanc, P. (2011). Ontological Crises in Artificial Agent’s Value Systems. Manuscript. The Singularity Institute for Artificial Intelligence. [arxiv.org/pdf/1105.3821v1.pdf] Dewey, D. (2011). Learning What to Value. In Schmidhuber, J., Thorisson, K. R., Looks, M. (eds.). Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Artificial General Intelligence, AGI 2011 (pp. 309-314), Heidelberg: Springer. Forgas, J. et al. (eds.) (2009). The Psychology of Attitudes and Attitude Change. London: Psychology Press. Lewis, D. (1988). Desire as belief. Mind, 97(387), 323-332. Omohundro, S. (2008a). The Basic AI Drives. In P. Wang, B. Goertzel, and S. Franklin (eds.). Proceedings of the First AGI Conference, 171, Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence and Applications. Amsterdam: IOS Press. Omohundro, S. (2008b). The Nature of Self-Improving Artificial Intelligence. Manuscript. [selfawaresystems.files.wordpress.com/2008/01/nature_of_self_improving_ai.pdf] Omohundro, S. (forthcoming 2012). Rationally-Shaped Artificial Intelligence. In Eden, A. et al. (eds.). The Singularity Hypothesis: A Scientific and Philosophical Assessment (Springer, forthcoming). Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and Persons. (pp. 123-4). Reprinted and corrected edition, 1987. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Parfit, D. (2011). On What Matters. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Russell, S. and Norvig, P. (2010). Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach. (3rd ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Sandberg, A. and Bostrom, N. (2008). Whole Brain Emulation: A Roadmap. Technical Report 2008– 3. Oxford: Future of Humanity Institute, Oxford University. [www.fhi.ox.ac.uk/Reports/2008-3.pdf] Shulman, C. (2010). Omohundro’s “Basic AI Drives” and Catastrophic Risks. Manuscript. [singinst.org/upload/ai-resource-drives.pdf] Sinhababu, N. (2009). The Humean Theory of Motivation Reformulated and Defended. Philosophical Review, 118(4), 465-500. Smith, M. (1987). The Humean Theory of Motivation. Mind, 46 (381): 36-61. Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer Power and Human Reason: From Judgment to Calculation. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Yudkowsky, E. (2008). Artificial Intelligence as a Positive and Negative Factor in Global Risk. In Bostrom, N. and Cirkovic, M. (eds.). Global Catastrophic Risks. (pp. 308-345; quote from p. 310). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Yudkowsky, E. (2011). Complex Value Systems Are Required to Realize Valuable Futures. In Schmidhuber, J., Thorisson, K. R., Looks, M. (eds.). Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Artificial General Intelligence, AGI 2011 (pp. 388-393). Heidelberg: Springer.
  259. REFERENCES Bahrick, H. P., & Phelps, E. (1987). Retention of Spanish vocabulary over 8 years. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 344–349. Bloom, K. C., & Shuell, T. J. (1981). Effects of massed and distributed practice on the learning and retention of second-language vocabulary. Journal of Educational Research, 74, 245–248. Carpenter, S. K., Pashler, H., & Cepeda, N. J. (2009). Using tests to enhance 8th grade students’ retention of U.S. history facts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 760–771. Cepeda, N. J., Coburn, N., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., Mozer, M. C., & Pashler, H. (2009). Optimizing distributed practice: Theoretical analysis and practical implications. Experimental Psychology, 54, 236–246. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: A temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychological Science, 19, 1095–1102. Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions from massed or distributed exposures. Developmental Psychology, 38, 967–978. Cuvo, A. J. (1975). Developmental differences in rehearsal and free recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 265–278. Dempster, F. N. (1987). Effects of variable encoding and spaced presenta- tions on vocabulary learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 162–170. Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist, 43, 627–634. Dempster, F. N. (1990). The spacing effect: Research and practice. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 23, 97–101. DeRemer, P., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1974). Locus of distributes lag effect in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 167–171. Fishman, J., Keller, L., & Atkinson, R. (1968). Massed versus distributed practice in computerized spelling drills. Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 59, 290–296. Glenberg, A. M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetition on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 7, 95–112. Harris, J. (2003). WordCount. Available: http://wordcount.org/ [2 May 2010] Moss, V. D. (1995). The efficacy of massed versus distributed practice as a function of desired learning outcomes and grade level of the student (Doctoral dissertation, Utah State University, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 5204. Pashler, H., Rohrer, D., Cepeda, N. J., & Carpenter, S. K. (2007). Enhancing learning and retarding forgetting: Choices and consequences. Psycho- nomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 187–1193. Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2006). The effects of overlearning and distributed practice on the retention of mathematics knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1209–1224. Seabrook, R., Brown, G. D., & Solity, J. E. (2005). Distributed and massed practice: From laboratory to classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 107–122. Toppino, T. C., & DeMesquita, M. (1984). Effects of spacing repetitions on children’s memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 637– 648. Verkoeijen, P. P. (2005). Explaining the spacing effect: Study-phase retrie- val, contextual-variability and priming accounts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Erasmus University Rotterdam. Vlach, H. A., Sandhofer, C. M., & Kornell, N. (2008). The spacing effect in children’s memory and category induction. Cognition, 109, 163–167. Wilson, W. P. (1976). Developmental changes in the lag effect: An encoding hypothesis for repeated word recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 113–122. Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). When you know that you know and when you think that you know but you don’t. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 41–44.
  260. References Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., & Logan, J. M. (2007). Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval? A critical review of the extant literature. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III (pp. 83–106). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55 Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Application of the testing and spacing effects to name learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 619–636. doi:10.1002/acp.1101 Cull, W. L. (2000). Untangling the benefits of multiple study opportunities and repeated testing for cued recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 215–235. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200005/06)14:3 215::AID- ACP640 3.0.CO;2-1 Cull, W. L., Shaughnessy, J. L., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1996). Expanding understanding of the expanding-pattern-of-retrieval mnemonic: Toward confidence in applicability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Ap- plied, 2, 365–378. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.2.4.365 Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I. M., & Bleasdale, F. A. (1973). Retrieval difficulty and subsequent recall. Memory & Cognition, 1, 213–216. doi:10.3758/BF03198098 Greene, R. L. (2008). Repetition and spacing effects. In H. L. Roediger (Ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference. Vol. 2: Cog- nitive psychology of memory (pp. 65–78). Oxford, England: Elsevier. Jacoby, L. L., Shimizu, Y., Daniels, K. A., & Rhodes, M. G. (2005). Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of re- trieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 852–857. doi:10.3758/ BF03196776 Karpicke, J. D. (2009). Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 469–486. doi:10.1037/a0017341 Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007a). Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 704–719. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.704 Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007b). Repeated retrieval during learning is the key to long-term retention. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 151–162. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.004 Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008, February 15). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319, 966–968. doi: 10.1126/science.1152408 Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2010). Is expanding retrieval a superior method for learning text materials? Memory & Cognition, 38, 116–124. doi:10.3758/MC.38.1.116 Koriat, A., & Ma’ayan, H. (2005). The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 478–492. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.01.001 Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). London, England: Aca- demic Press. Logan, J. M., & Balota, D. A. (2008). Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: Exploring different schedules of spacing and retention interval in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15, 257–280. doi:10.1080/13825580701322171 Madigan, S., Neuse, J., & Roeber, U. (2000). Retrieval latency and “at-risk” memories. Memory & Cognition, 28, 523–528. doi:10.3758/BF03201242 Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1994). Norms of paired-associate recall during multitrial learning learning of Swahili–English translation equiv- alents. Memory, 2, 325–335. doi:10.1080/09658219408258951 Pashler, H., Zarow, G., & Triplett, B. (2003). Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1051–1057. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1051 Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2007). Examining the efficiency of sched- ules of distributed retrieval practice. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1917– 1927. doi:10.3758/BF03192925 Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2009). Testing the retrieval effort hypoth- esis: Does greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 437– 447. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.004 Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210. doi:10.1111/ j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Sci- ence, 17, 249–255. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2010). Intricacies of spaced retrieval: A resolution. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: Essays in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 23–47). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Storm, B. C., Bjork, R. A., & Storm, J. C. (2010). Optimizing retrieval as a learning event: When and why expanding retrieval practice enhances long-term retention. Memory & Cognition, 38, 244–253. doi:10.3758/ MC.38.2.244
  261. References Balota, D. A., Watson, J. M., Duchek, J. M., & Ferraro, F. R. ( 1999 ). Cross-modal semantic and homograph priming in healthy young, healthy old, and Alzheimer’s disease individuals. Jour- nal of International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 626 – 640 . Bar, M., & Aminoff, E. ( 2003 ). Cortical analysis of visual context. Neuron, 38, 347 – 358 . Block, N. ( 1995 ). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227 – 287 . Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Dale, A. M., Rotte, M., & Rosen, B. R. ( 1998 ). Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval: Selective averaging of event-related fMRI trials to test the retrieval success hypoth- esis. NeuroImage, 7, 163 – 175 . Buckner, R. L., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2004 ). Neural correlates of memory’s successes and sins. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences ( 3 rd ed., pp. 739 – 752 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Buckner, R. L., & Wheeler, M. E. ( 2001 ). The cognitive neuroscience of remember- ing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 , 624 – 634 . Budson, A. E., Daffner, K. R., Desikan, R., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2000 ). When false recogni- tion is unopposed by true recognition: Gist- based memory distortion in Alzheimer’s dis- ease. Neuropsychology, 14, 277 – 287 . Budson, A. E., Desikan, R., Daffner, K. R., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2001 ). Perceptual false recog- nition in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 15, 230 – 243 . Budson, A. E., Sullivan, A. L., Daffner, K. R., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2003 ). Semantic versus phonological false recognition in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 51, 251 – 261 . Cabeza, R., Rao, S. M., Wagner, A. D., Mayer, A. R., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2001 ). Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 4805 – 4810 . Cermak, L. S. ( 1982 ). Human memory and amne- sia. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. ( 2002 ). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201 – 215 . Curran, T. ( 2004 ). Effects of attention and con- fidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia, 42 , 1088 – 1106 . Dale, A. M., & Buckner, R. L. ( 1997 ). Selective averaging of rapidly presented individual trials using fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 329 – 340 . Deese, J. ( 1959 ). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17 – 22 . Dobbins, Foley, Schacter, & Wagner, ( 2002 ). Dobbins, I. G., Rice, H. J., Wagner, A. D., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2003 ). Memory orientation and success: Separable neurocognitive com- ponents underlying episodic recognition. Neu- ropsychologia, 41, 318 – 333 . Dobbins, I. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2004 ). Cortical activity reduc- tions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 42 8, 316 – 319 . Duarte, A., Ranganath, C., Winward, L., Hay- ward, D., & Knight, R. T. ( 2004 ). Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollec- tion during the encoding and retrieval of pic- tures. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 255 – 272 . Dunn, J. C. ( 2004 ). Remember-know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review, 111, 524 – 542 . D ¨uzel, E., Yonelinas, A. P., Mangun, G. R., Heinze, H., & Tulving, E. ( 1997 ). Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of con- scious awareness in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94, 5973 – 5978 . Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Engel, S. A. ( 2000 ). Remembering episodes: A selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 1149 – 1152 . Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. ( 1998 ). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392 , 598 – 601 . Fabiani, M., Stadler, M. A., & Wessels, P. M. ( 2000 ). True but not false memories produce a sensory signature in human lateralized brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 , 941 – 949 . Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C., & Richardson- Klavehn, A. ( 2002 ). Recognition memory and decision processes: A meta-analysis of remem- ber, know, and guess responses. Memory, 10, 83 – 98 . Goodale, M. A., & Westwood, D. A. ( 2004 ). An evolving view of duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for percep- tion and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiol- ogy, 14, 203 – 211 . Gooding, P. A., Mayes, A. R., & van Eijk, R. ( 2000 ). A meta-analysis of indirect memory tests for novel material in organic amnesics. Neuropsychologia, 38, 666 – 676 . Gottfried, J. A., Smith, A. P. R., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. ( 2004 ). Remembrance of odors past: Human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron, 42 , 687 – 695 . Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. ( 1985 ). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal subjects and amnesic patients. Journal of Exper- imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog- nition, 11, 501 – 518 . Gratton, G., Corballis, P. M., & Jain, S. ( 1997 ). Hemispheric organization of visual memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 92 – 104 . Guzeldere, G., Flanagan, O., & Hardcastle, V. G. ( 2000 ). The nature and function of consciousness: Lessons from blindsight. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences ( 2 nd ed., pp. 1277 – 1284 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Henson, R. N. ( 2003 ). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology, 70, 53 – 81 . Henson, R. N. A., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., Josephs, O., & Dolan, R. J. ( 1999 ). Recollec- tion and familiarity in recognition memory: An event-related functional magnetic reso- nance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 3962 – 3972 . Henson, R., Shallice, T., & Dolan, R. ( 2000 ). Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. Science, 2 87, 1269 – 1272 . Holdstock, J. S., Mayes, A. R., Gong, Q. Y., Roberts, N., & Kapur, N. ( 2005 ). Item recog- nition is less impaired than recall and asso- ciative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal damage. Hippocampus, 15, 203 – 215 . Holdstock, J. S., Mayes, A. R., Roberts, N., Ceza- yirli, E., Isaac, C. L., O’Reilly, R. C., & Norman, K. A. ( 2002 ). Under what conditions is recog- nition spared relative to recall after selective hippocampal damage in humans? Hippocam- pus, 12 , 341 – 351 . Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. ( 2000 ). The neural mechanisms of top- down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 284 – 291 . James, T. W., Humphrey, G. K., Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., & Goodale, M. A. ( 2000 ). The effects of visual object priming on brain activa- tion before and after recognition. Current Biol- ogy, 10, 1017 – 1024 . Johnson, M. K., Nolde, S. F., Mather, M., Kounios, J., Schacter, D. L., & Curran, T. ( 1997 ). The similarity of brain activity associ- ated with true and false recognition memory depends on test format. Psychological Science, 8, 250 – 257 . Kahn, I., Davachi, L., & Wagner, A. D. ( 2004 ). Functional-neuroanatomic correlates of recollection: Implications for models of recognition memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 2 4, 4172 – 4180 . Kapur, S., Craik, F. I., Jones, C., Brown, G. M., Houle, S., & Tulving, E. ( 1995 ). Functional role of the prefrontal cortex in retrieval of memories: A PET study. NeuroReport, 6, 1880 – 1884 . Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. ( 1995 ). Remem- bering and knowing: Two different expressions of declarative memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 2 1, 699 – 710 . K¨ohler, S., & Moscovitch, M. ( 1997 ). Uncon- scious visual processing in neuropsychological syndromes: A survey of the literature and eval- uation of models of consciousness. In M. D. Rugg (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 305 – 373 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Konishi, S., Wheeler, M. E., Donaldson, D. I., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2000 ). Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. NeuroImage, 12 , 276 – 286 . Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., Brenner, C., & Jackson, E. M. ( 1999 ). Perceptu- ally based false recognition of novel objects in amnesia: Effects of category size and similarity to category prototypes. Cognitive Neuropsychol- ogy, 16, 317 – 341 . Manns, J. R., Hopkins, R. O., Reed, J. M., Kitch- ener, E. G., & Squire, L. R. ( 2003 ). Recognition memory and the human hippocampus. Neu- ron, 37, 171 – 180 . Maril, A., Simons, J. S., Mitchell, J. P., Schwartz, B. L., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2003 ). Feeling-of- knowing in episodic memory: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 18, 827 – 836 . Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. ( 1997 ). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire com- pared. Memory & Cognition, 2 5, 826 – 837 . McDermott, K. B., Jones, T. C., Petersen, S. E., Lageman, S. K., & Roediger, H. L. III. ( 2000 ). Retrieval success is accompanied by enhanced activation in anterior prefrontal cortex during recognition memory: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 , 965 – 976 . Melo, B., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. ( 1999 ). False recall and false recognition: An examination of the effects of selective and combined lesions to the medial tempo- ral lobe/diencephalon and frontal lobe struc- tures. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 343 – 359 . Moscovitch, M. ( 1984 ). The sufficient conditions for demonstrating preserved amnesia: A task analysis. In L. R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (pp. 104 – 114 ). New York: Guilford Press. Moscovitch, M. ( 1994 ). Memory and working with memory: Evaluation of a component pro- cess model and comparisons with other models. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.) Memory systems 1994 (pp. 269 – 331 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Moscovitch, M. ( 1995 ). Recovered con-scious- ness: A hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. Journal of Clinical and Exper- imental Neuropsychology, 17, 276 – 290 . Moscovitch, M. ( 2000 ). Theories of memory and consciousness. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 609 – 625 ). London: Oxford University Press. Moscovitch, M., Kapur, S., K¨ohler, S., & Houle, S. ( 1995 ). Distinct neural correlates of visual long-term memory for spatial location and object identity: A positron emission tomog- raphy study in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92 , 3721 – 3725 . Moscovitch, M., & McAndrews, M. P. ( 2002 ). Material-specific deficits in “remembering” in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1335 – 1342 . Moscovitch, M., Vriezen, E., & Goshen- Gottstein, Y. ( 1993 ). Implicit tests of memory in patients with focal lesions or degenerative brain disorders. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 8 , pp. 133 – 173 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. ( 1997 ). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hip- pocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neuro- biology, 7, 217 – 227 . Nolde, S. F., Johnson, M. K., & D’Esposito, M. ( 1998 ). Left prefrontal activation during episodic remembering: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroReport, 9, 3509 – 3514 . Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. ( 1997 ). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory mem- ories. Memory & Cognition, 2 5, 838 – 848 . Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R., & Tul- ving, E. ( 2000 ). Reactivation of encoding- related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11120 – 11124 . Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A. R., Houle, S., Nils- son, L. G., & Tulving, E. ( 1996 ). Activation of medial temporal structures during episodic memory retrieval. Nature, 380, 715 – 717 . Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L., Sand- blom, J., ´ Åberg, C., & Ingvar, M. ( 2001 ). Reac- tivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. NeuroImage, 14, 521 – 528 . Nyberg, L., Tulving, E., Habib, R., Nilsson, L. G., Kapur, S., Houle, S., Cabeza, R., & McIntosh, A. R. ( 1995 ). Functional brain maps of retrieval mode and recovery of episodic information. NeuroReport, 7, 249 – 252 . Okado, Y., & Stark, C. ( 2003 ). Neural process- ing associated with true and false memory retrieval. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neu- roscience, 3, 323 – 334 . Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. ( 1993 ). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In H. Spinnler & F. Boller (Eds.), Handbook of neu- ropsychology (Vol. 8 , pp. 63 – 131 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Roediger, H. L.III, & McDermott, K. B. ( 1995 ). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2 1, 803 – 814 . Rotello, C. M., Macmillan N. A., & Reeder, J. A. ( 2004 ). Sum-difference theory of remember- ing and knowing: A two-dimensional signal- detection model. Psychological Review, 111, 588 – 616 . Rugg, M. D., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Frack- owiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. ( 1996 ). Differential activation of the prefrontal cortex in successful and unsuccessful memory retrieval. Brain, 119, 2073 – 2083 . Rugg, M. D., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Frack- owiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. ( 1997 ). Brain regions supporting intentional and incidental memory: A PET study. NeuroReport, 2 4, 1283 – 1287 . Rugg, M. D., Henson, R. N. A., & Robb, W. G. K. ( 2003 ). Neural correlates of retrieval process- ing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks. Neuropsychologia, 41, 40 – 52 . Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloer- scheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S., & Allan, K. ( 1998 ). Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature, 392 , 595 – 598 . Schacter, D. L. ( 1987 ). Implicit memory: His- tory and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501 – 518 . Schacter, D. L., Alpert, N. M., Savage, C. R., Rauch, S. L., & Albert, M. S. ( 1996 a). Con- scious recollection and the human hippocam- pal formation: Evidence from positron emis- sion tomography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 93, 321 – 325 . Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. ( 1998 ). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 2 0, 185 – 195 . Schacter, D. L., Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Dale, A. M., & Rosen, B. R. ( 1997 a). Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during true and false recognition: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 6, 259 – 269 . Schacter, D. L., & Curran, T. ( 2000 ). Memory without remembering and remembering with- out memory: Implicit and false memories. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neuro- sciences ( 2 nd ed., pp. 829 – 840 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Schacter, D. L., Dobbins, I. G., & Schnyer, D. M. ( 2004 ). Specificity of priming: A cogni- tive neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 853 – 862 . Schacter, D. L., McAndrews, M. P., & Moscov- itch, M. ( 1988 ). Access to consciousness: Dis- sociations between implicit and explicit knowl- edge. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought with- out language (pp. 242 – 278 ). New York: Oxford University Press. Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A., & Koutstaal, W. ( 1998 a). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289 – 318 . Schacter, D. L., Reiman, E., Curran, T., Yun, L. S., Bandy, D., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. III. ( 1996 b). Neuroanatomical correlates of veridical and illusory recognition memory: Evidence from positron emission tomography. Neuron, 17, 267 – 274 . Schacter, D. L., Reiman, E., Uecker, A., Polster, M. R., Yun, L. S., & Cooper, L. A. ( 1995 ). Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information. Nature, 376, 587 – 590 . Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. ( 2004 ). The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron, 44, 149 – 160 . Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Anes, M. D. ( 1997 b). Illusory memories in amnesic patients: Conceptual and perceptual false recognition. Neuropsychology, 11, 331 – 342 . Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., Anes, M. D., & Racine, C. ( 1998 b). When true recognition suppresses false recognition: Evidence from amnesic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- science, 10, 668 – 679 . Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Pradere, D. ( 1996 c). The neuropsychology of mem- ory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory and Lan- guage, 35, 319 – 334 . Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D., & Loftus, E. F. ( 1986 ). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of Exper- imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog- nition, 12 , 171 – 181 . Slotnick, S. D., & Dodson, C. S. ( 2005 ). Sup- port for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory. Mem- ory & Cognition, 33, 151 – 170 . Slotnick, S. D., Moo, L. R., Segal, J. B., & Hart, J. Jr. ( 2003 ). Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source mem- ory for visual shapes. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 75 – 82 . Slotnick, S. D., & Schacter, D. L. ( 2004 ). A sensory signature that distinguishes true from false memories. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 664 – 672 . Smith, M. E. ( 1993 ). Neurophysiological man- ifestations of recollective experience during recognition memory judgments. Journal of Cog- nitive Neuroscience, 5, 1 – 13 . Squire, L. R. ( 1992 ). Memory and the hippo- campus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195 – 231 . Squire, L. R., Ojemann, J. G., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., Videen, T. O., & Raichle, M. E. ( 1992 ). Activation of the hippocampus in nor- mal humans: A functional anatomical study of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 89, 1837 – 1841 . Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E. L., & Clark, R. E. ( 2004 ). The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2 7, 279 – 306 . Trott, C. T., Friedman, D., Ritter, W., Fabiani, M., & Snodgrass, J. G. ( 1999 ). Episodic prim- ing and memory for temporal source: Event- related potentials reveal age-related differences in prefrontal functioning. Psychology and Aging, 14, 390 – 413 . Tulving, E. ( 1985 a). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40, 385 – 398 . Tulving, E. ( 1985 b). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 2 6, 1 – 12 . Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M., & Houle, S. ( 1994 ). Hemispheric encod- ing/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission tomography findings. Pro- ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 2016 – 2020 . Uecker, A., Reiman, E. M., Schacter, D. L., Pol- ster, M. R., Cooper, L. A., Yun, L. S., & Chen, K. ( 1997 ). Neuroanatomical correlates of implicit and explicit memory for structurally possible and impossible visual objects. Learning & Memory, 4, 337 – 355 . Vaidya, C. J., Zhao, M., Desmond, J. E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. ( 2002 ). Evidence for corti- cal encoding specificity in episodic memory: Memory-induced re-activation of picture pro- cessing areas. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2136 – 2143 . Velanova, K., Jacoby, L. L., Wheeler, M. E., McAvoy, M. P., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2003 ). Functional-anatomic correlates of sustained and transient processing components engaged during controlled retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 2 3, 8460 – 8470 . Wagner, A. D., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. ( 1998 ). Prefrontal cor- tex and recognition memory: Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval pro- cesses. Brain, 12 1, 1985 – 2002 . Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. ( 1974 ). The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 12 , 419 – 428 . Wheeler, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2003 ). Functional dissociation among components of remembering: Control, perceived oldness, and content. Journal of Neuroscience, 2 3, 3869 – 3880 . Wheeler, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2004 ). Functional-anatomic correlates of remember- ing and knowing. NeuroImage, 2 1, 1337 – 1349 . Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. ( 2000 ). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11125 – 11129 . Wiggs, C. L., & Martin, A. ( 1998 ). Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Cur- rent Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 227 – 233 . Wilding, E. L., & Rugg, M. D. ( 1996 ). An event- related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain, 119, 889 – 905 . Wixted, J. T., & Stretch, V. ( 2004 ). In defense of the signal detection interpretation of remem- ber/know judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 616 – 641 . Yonelinas, A. P., Kroll, N. E. A., Dobbins, I., Lazzara, M., & Knight, R. T. ( 1998 ). Recollec- tion and familiarity deficits in amnesia: Con- vergence of remember-know, process dissocia- tion, and receiver operating characteristic data. Neuropsychology 12 , 323 – 339 . Yonelinas, A. P., Kroll, N. E., Quamme, J. R., Laz- zara, M. M., Sauve, M. J., Widarman, K. F., & Knight, R. T. ( 2002 ). Effects of extensive tem- poral lobe damage or mild hypoxia on recol- lection and familiarity. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1236 – 1241 . Young, A. W. ( 1994 ). Conscious and nonconsious recognition of familiar faces. In C. Umilt`a & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information pro- cessing (pp. 153 – 178 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  262. Further Reading Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us about Normal Vision. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MA. Grill-Spector, K., Kourtzi, Z., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res. 41, 1409–1422. Kanwisher, N., Downing, P., Epstein, R., and Kourtzi, Z. (2000). In The Handbook on Functional Neuroimaging (R. Cabeza and A. Kingstone, Eds.). Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK. Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Macko, K. A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414–417. Wandell, B. (1999). In Cognitive Neuroscience (M. Gazzaniga, Ed.,). Zeki, S. (1995), A Vision of the Brain, Blackwell, Oxford, UK.
  263. References Aaker, J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34(3), 347–356. Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O'Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32(3), 537–551. Ariely, D., & Berns, G. S. (2010). Neuromarketing: The hope and hype of neuroimaging in business. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 284–292. Ariely, D., & Norton, M. I. (2008). How actions create—not just reveal - preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 13–16. Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: Self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 670–676. Benhabib, J., & Bisin, A. (2008). Choice and process: theory ahead of measurement. In A. Caplin, & A. Schotter (Eds.), Foundations of positive and normative economics, methodologies of modern economics, Vol. 1, : Oxford University Press. Bernheim, B. D. (2009). On the potential of neuroeconomics: A critical (but hopeful) appraisal. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 1(2), 1–41. Bettman, J. R. (1979). Memory factors in consumer choice—Review. Journal of Marketing, 43(2), 37–53. Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Payne, J. W. (1998). Constructive consumer choice processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 187–217. Bhatt, M. A., Lohrenz, T., Camerer, C. F., & Montague, P. R. (2010). Neural signatures of strategic types in a two-person bargaining game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(46), 19720–19725. Brasel, S. A., & Gips, J. (2011). Red Bull “Gives You Wings” for better or worse: A double-edged impact of brand exposure on consumer performance. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 57–64. Bucklin, R. E., & Gupta, S. (1992). Brand choice, purchase incidence, and segmentation—An Integrated Modeling Approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 29(2), 201–215. Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198. Bushong, B., King, L. M., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2010). Pavlovian processes in consumer choice: The physical presence of a good increases willingness-to-pay. American Economic Review, 100(4), 1556–1571. Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage, 17(3), 1394–1402. Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Bach, D. R., Roepstorff, A., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2010). How the opinion of others affects our valuation of objects. Current Biology, 20(13), 1165–1170. Caplin, A., & Dean, M. (2008). Dopamine, reward prediction error, and economics. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 123(2), 663–701. Chang, L. J., Smith, A., Dufwenberg, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2011). Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. Neuron, 70(3), 560–572. Chartrand, T. L. (2005). The role of conscious awareness in consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 203–210. Chib, V. S., Rangel, A., Shimojo, S., & O'Doherty, J. P. (2009). Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(39), 12315–12320. Churchill, G. A., & Iacobucci, D. (2005). Marketing research: Methodological foundations (9th ed.). Mason, Ohio: Thomson/South-Western. Clithero, J. A., Tankersley, D., & Huettel, S. A. (2008). Foundations of neuroeconomics: From philosophy to practice. PLoS Biology, 6(11), e298. De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684–687. Delgado, M. R., Gillis, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Regulating the expectation of reward via cognitive strategies. Nature Neuroscience, 11(8), 880–881. Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P. K., van Baaren, R. B., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2005). The unconscious consumer: Effects of environment on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 193–202. Ferraro, R., Bettman, J. R., & Chartrand, T. L. (2009). The power of strangers: The effect of incidental consumer brand encounters on brand choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 729–741. Fishbach, A., Ratner, R. K., & Zhang, Y. (2011). Inherently loyal or easily bored?: Nonconscious activation of consistency versus variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 38–48. FitzGerald, T. H., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2009). The role of human orbitofrontal cortex in value comparison for incommensurable objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(26), 8388–8395. Fitzsimons, G. M., Chartrand, T. L., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Automatic effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How Apple makes you "Think Different". Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 21–35. Forehand, M. R., Perkins, A., & Reed, A. (2011). When are automatic social comparisons not automatic? The effect of cognitive systems on user imagery-based self-concept activation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 88–100. Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model—How people cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(1), 1–31. Gabaix, X., Laibson, D., Moloche, G., & Weinberg, S. (2006). Costly information acquisition: Experimental analysis of a boundedly rational model. American Economic Review, 96(4), 1043–1068. Geschwind, D. H., & Konopka, G. (2009). Neuroscience in the era of functional genomics and systems biology. Nature, 461(7266), 908–915. Greenberg, M., & McDonald, S. S. (1989). Successful needs/benefits segmentation: A user's guide. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 6(3), 29–36. Gul, F., & Pesendorfer, W. (2008). The case for mindless economics. In A. Caplin, & A. Schotter (Eds.), The foundations of positive and normative economics. : Oxford University Press. Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 651–659. Hamburg, M. A., & Collins, F. S. (2010). The path to personalized medicine. The New England Journal of Medicine, 363(4), 301–304. Hang, H. M., & Auty, S. (2011). Children playing branded video games: The impact of interactivity on product placement effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(1), 65–72. Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision- making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648. Hare, T. A., Malmaud, J., & Rangel, A. (2011). Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 11077–11087. Hariri, A. R. (2009). The neurobiology of individual differences in complex behavioral traits. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 225–247. Harrison, G. W. (2008). Neuroeconomics: A critical reconsideration. Economics and Philosophy, 24(3), 303–344. Haynes, J. D., & Rees, G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 523–534. Hertwig, R., & Erev, I. (2009). The description-experience gap in risky choice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 517–523. Hubert, M., & Kenning, P. (2008). A current overivew of consumer neuroscience. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7, 272–292. Izuma, K., Matsumoto, M., Murayama, K., Samejima, K., Sadato, N., & Matsumoto, K. (2010). Neural correlates of cognitive dissonance and choice-induced prefer- ence change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(51), 22014–22019. Kable, J. W. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience toolkit for the neuroeconomist: A functional overview. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4(2), 63–84. Kahneman, D., Wakker, P. P., & Sarin, R. (1997). Back to Bentham?—Explorations of experienced utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 375–405. Kahnt, T., Heinzle, J., Park, S. Q., & Haynes, J. D. (2010). The neural code of reward anticipation in human orbitofrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(13), 6010–6015. Kang, M. J., Rangel, A., Camus, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2011). Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(2), 461–468. Kay, K. N., Naselaris, T., Prenger, R. J., & Gallant, J. L. (2008). Identifying nat- ural images from human brain activity. Nature, 452(7185), 352–356. Keller, K. L. (2008). Strategic brand management: building, measuring, and man- aging brand equity (3 rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall. Kenning, P. H., & Plassmann, H. (2008). How neuroscience can inform con- sumer research. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 16(6), 532–538. Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron, 53(1), 147–156. Krajbich, I., Camerer, C., Ledyard, J., & Rangel, A. (2009). Using neural measures of economic value to solve the public goods free-rider problem. Science, 326(5952), 596–599. Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(11), 479–487. Lebreton, M., Jorge, S., Michel, V., Thirion, B., & Pessiglione, M. (2009). An automatic valuation system in the human brain: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuron, 64(3), 431–439. Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self- defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1250–1267. Levy, I., Lazzaro, S. C., Rutledge, R. B., & Glimcher, P. W. (2011). Choice from non-choice: Predicting consumer preferences from blood oxygenation level- dependent signals obtained during passive viewing. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(1), 118–125. Loewenstein, G. F., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & G. H. Hill (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). Oxford: Oxford University Press. McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., & Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44(2), 379–387. Milosavlijevic, M., Navalpakkam, V., Koch, C., & Rangel, A. (in press). Relative visual saliency differences induce sizeable bias in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology. Montague, P. R., King-Casas, B., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Imaging valuation models in human choice. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 417–448. Mur, M., Bandettini, P. A., & Kriegeskorte, N. (2009). Revealing representational content with pattern-information fMRI—an introductory guide. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 101–109. Murphy, E. R., Illes, J., & Reiner, P. B. (2008). Neuroethics of neuromarketing. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7(4–5), 293–302. Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J.D., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cogni- tive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23(2), 483–499. O'Doherty, J. P., Buchanan, T. W., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Predictive neural coding of reward preference involves dissociable responses in human ventral midbrain and ventral striatum. Neuron, 49(1), 157–166. Pessiglione, M., Petrovic, P., Daunizeau, J., Palminteri, S., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2008). Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron, 59(4), 561–567. Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158. Peters, J., & Buchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal–mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66(1), 138–148. Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53. Plassmann, H., O'Doherty, J., & Rangel, A. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(37), 9984–9988. Plassmann, H., O'Doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(3), 1050–1054. Plassmann, H., Yoon, C., Feinberg, F., & Shiv, B. (2011). Consumer Neuroscience. In R. P. Bagozzi, & A. Ruvio (Eds.), Wiley international encyclopedia of marketing, 3, Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley. Plassmann, H., Ramsøy, T. Z., & Milosavlijevic, M. (in press). Branding the brain: A critical review and outlook. Journal of Consumer Psychology. Poldrack, R. A. (2010). Mapping mental function to brain structure: how can cognitive neuroimaging succeed? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(6), 753–761. Poldrack, R. A., Halchenko, Y. O., & Hanson, S. J. (2009). Decoding the large- scale structure of brain function by classifying mental states across individuals. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1364–1372. Pradeep, A. K. (2010). The buying brain: Secrets for selling to the subconscious mind. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley. Renvoisé, P., & Morin, C. (2007). Neuromarketing : understanding the “buy button” in your customer's brain (Rev. and updated. ed.). Nashville, Tenn.: T. Nelson. Reutskaja, E., Nagel, R., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2011). Search dynamics in consumer choice under time pressure: An eye-tracking study. American Economic Review, 101(2), 900–926. Richins, M. L. (1997). Measuring emotions in the consumption experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 127–146. Rolls, E. T., & Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(3), 216–244. Rushworth, M. F., Mars, R. B., & Summerfield, C. (2009). General mechanisms for making decisions? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(1), 75–83. Rustichini, A. (2009). Is there a method of neuroeconomics? American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 1(2), 48–49. Sanfey, A. G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 108–116. Schindler, R. M. (1992). The real lesson of new coke: The value of of focus groups for predicting the effects of social influence. Marketing Research, 4(4), 22–27. Schwarz, N., Bless, H., & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion—Affective states influence the processing of persuasive communications. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 161–199. Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. (2008). Emotion, decision making, and the amygdala. Neuron, 58(5), 662–671. Sharot, T., De Martino, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2009). How choice reveals and shapes expected hedonic outcome. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(12), 3760–3765. Shiv, B. (2007). Emotions, decisions, and the brain. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(3), 174–178. Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278–292. Simonson, I., Carmon, Z., Dhar, R., Drolet, A., & Nowlis, S. M. (2001). Consumer research: In search of identity. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 249–275. Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H. J., & Haynes, J. D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(5), 543–545. Story, L. (2007, January 15). Anywhere the Eye Can See, it's Likely to See an Ad, The New York Times. Retrieved from. http://www.nytimes.com/2007/ 01/15/business/media/15everywhere.html Tse, D. K., & Wilton, P. C. (1988). Models of consumer satisfaction formation— An extension. Journal of Marketing Research, 25(2), 204–212. Tusche, A., Bode, S., & Haynes, J. D. (2010). Neural responses to unattended products predict later consumer choices. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(23), 8024–8031. Venkatraman, V., Chuah, Y. M. L., Huettel, S. A., & Chee, M. W. L. (2007). Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep, 30(5), 603–609. Venkatraman, V., Huettel, S. A., Chuah, L. Y., Payne, J. W., & Chee, M. W. (2011). Sleep deprivation biases the neural mechanisms underlying economic preferences. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 3712–3718. Venkatraman, V., Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Huettel, S. A. (2009). Separate neural mechanisms underlie choices and strategic preferences in risky decision making. Neuron, 62(4), 593–602. Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K.N. (2008). Prefrontal–subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59(6), 1037–1050. Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85. Wheeler, S. C., & Berger, J. (2007). When the same prime leads to different effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(3), 357–368. Yankelovich, D., & Meer, D. (2006). Rediscovering market segmentation. Harvard Business Review, 84(2), 122–131. Yoon, C., Gutchess, A. H., Feinberg, F., & Polk, T. A. (2006). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of neural dissociations between brand and person judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 31–40.
  264. References Amidzic, O., Riehle, H. J., Fehr, T., Wienbruch, C., & Elbert, T. (2001, August 9). Pattern of focal γ-bursts in chess players. Nature, 412, 603. Atherton, M., Zhuang, X., Bart, W., Hu, X., & He, S. (2000, April). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of chess expertise. Paper presented at the 2000 Cognitive Neuroscience Society Meeting, San Francisco. Atherton, M., Zhuang, J., Bart, W., Hu, X. & He, S. (2003). Functional MRI study of high-level cognition. I. The game of chess. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 26- 31. Christiaen, J., & Verholfstadt, D.C. (1978). Chess and cognitive development. Nederlandse Tydschrift voor de Psychologie en haar Grensegebieten, 36, 561-582. Cranberg, L. D., & Albert, M. L. (1988). The chess mind. In L. Kober, & D. Fein (Eds.), The exceptional brain: Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities (pp. 156-190). New York: The Guilford Press. DeFirmian, N., & Korn, W. (1990). Modern chess openings (13th ed.). New York: David McKay. Gershunsky, B., & Gufeld, E. (September, 2000). Can chess aid American schools? Chess Life, 55 (9), 41-43. Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look- ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from Grand-Master- Level Chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52-55. Nichelli, P., Grafman, J., Pietrini, P., Alway, D., Carton, J., & Miletich, R. (1994, May 19). Brain activity in chess playing. Nature, 369, 191. Onofrj, M., Curatola, L., Valentini, G., Antonelli, M., Thomas, A., & Fulgente, T. (1995). Non-dominant dorsal-prefrontal activation during chess problem solution evidenced by single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT). Neuroscience Letters, 198, 169-172. Saariluoma, P. (1995). Chess players’ thinking: A cognitive psychological approach. London: Routledge. Smith, J. P., & Cage, B. N. (2000). The effects of chess instruction on the mathematics achievement of southern, rural, black secondary students. Research in the Schools, 7, 19-26. Soltis, A. (1994). The inner game of chess: How to calculate and win. New York: David McKay.
  265. References [1] Amalric, M. and Koob, G.F., Depletion of dopamine in the caudate nucleus but not in nucleus accumbens impairs reaction-time perfor- mance in rats, J. Neurosci., 7 (1987) 2129-2134. [2] Baddeley, A.D., Time estimation at reduced body temperature, Am. J. Psychol., 79 (1966) 475-479. [3] Bartus, R.T., Dean, III, R.L., Beer, B. and Lippa, A.S., The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. Science, 217 (1982) 408-417. [4] Bartus, R.T., Dean, R.L., Pontecorvo, M.J. and Flicker, C., The cholinergic hypothesis: a historical overview, current perspective, and future directions. In D.S. Olton et al. (Eds.), Memory Dysfunc- tions: An Integration of Animal and Human Research From Pre- clinical and Clinical Perspectives, Ann. N.E Acad. Sci., 444 (1985) 332-358. [5] Berz, S., B~ittig, K. and Welzl, H., The effects of anticholinergic drugs on delayed time discrimination performance in rats, Physiol. Behav., 51 (1992)493-499. [6] Blusztajn, J.K. and Wurtman, R.J., Choline and cholinergic neu- rons, Science, 221 (1983) 614-620. [7] Branch, M.N., Behavioral pharmacology. In I.H. Iversen and K.A. Lattal (Eds.), Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Part 2, Elsevier, New York, 1991, pp. 21-77. [8] Branch, M.N. and GoUub, L.R., A detailed analysis of the effects of d-amphetamine on behavior under fixed interval schedules, J. Exp. Anal Behav., 21 (1974) 519-539. [9] Bruhwyler, J., Chleide, E. and Mercier, M., Effects of low doses of neuroleptics on temporal regulation in a differential reinforcement of response duration (DRRD) schedule in the dog, Pharm. Biochem. Behav., 37 (1990) 607-611. [10] Carlsson, M. and Carlsson, A., The NMDA antagonist MK-801 causesmarked locomotor stimulation in monoamine-depleted mice, Z Neural Trans., 75 (1989) 221-226. [11] Carlsson, M. and Carlsson, A., Interactions between glutamatergic and monoaminergic systems within the basal ganglia - implica- tions for schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease, TINS, 13 (1990) 272-276. [12] Cheramy, A., Leviel, V. and Glowinski, S., Dendritic release of dopamine in the substantia nigra, Nature, 289 (1981) 537-542. [13] Chevalier, G., Vacher, S., Deniau, J.M., and Desban, M., Disinhibi- tion as a basic process in the expression of striatal function. I. The striato-nigral influence on tecto-spinal/tecto-diencephalic neurons, Brain Res., 334 (1985) 215-226. [14] Church, R.M., Properties of the internal clock. In J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423 (1984) 566-582. [15] Church, R.M., Meck, W.H. and Gibbon, J., Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav., 20 (1994) 135-155. [16] Corkin, S., Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: clinical course in experimental findings in H.M., Neu- rology, 4 (1984) 249-259. [17] Daniel, S.A. and Thompson, T., Methadone-induced attenuation of the effects of D9-tetrahydrocannabinol on temporal discrimination in pigeons, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 213 (1980) 247-253. [18] Dawson T.M., Gehlert, D.R., Yamamura, H.I., Barnett, A. and Wamsley, J.K., D1 dopamine receptors in the rat brain: autoradio- graphic localization using [3H]SCH 23390, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 108 (1985) 323-325. [19] Edmonds, D.E., Stellar, J.R. and Gallistel, C.R., Parametric analy- sis of brain stimulation reward in the rat. II. Temporal summation in the reward system, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 87 (1974) 860-869. [20] Edmonds, D.E. and Gallistel, C.R., Parametric analysis of brain stimulation reward in the rat. III. Effect of performance variables on the reward summation function, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 87 (1974) 876-883. [21] Ellenbroek, B., Schwarz, M. and Sontag, K-H., Muscular rigidity and delineation of a dopamine specific neostriatal subregion: tonic EMG activity in rats, Brain Res., 345 (1985) 732-740. [22] Engber, T.M., Susel, Z., Kuo, S., Chase, T.N., Chronic levodopa treatment alters basal and dopamine agonist-stimulated cerebral glucose utilization, J. Neurosci., 10, (1990) 3889-3895. [23] Fraisse, P., Perception and estimation of time, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 35 (1984) 1-36. [24] Fuster, J.M., The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, Raven Press, New York, 1980. [25] Gallistel, C.R., Self-stimulation. In J.A. Deutsch (Ed.), The Physio- logical Basis of Memory, Academic Press, New York, 1983, pp. 269-349. [26] Gale, K. and Casu, M., Dynamic utilization of GABA in substantia nigra: regulation by dopamine and GABA in the striatum, and its clinical and behavioral implications, Mol. Cell Biochem., 39 (1981) 369-405. [27] Gerfen, C.R., Engber, T.M. and Mahan, L.C., D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopalli- dal neurons, Science, 250 (1990) 1429-1432. [28] Gibbon, J., Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing, Psychol. Rev., 84 (1977), 279-325. [29] Gibbon, J., Origins of scalar timing, Learn. Motiv., 22 (1991) 3-38. [30] Gibbon, J. and Church, R.M., Sources of variance in an informa- tion processing theory of timing. In H.L. Roitblat, T.G. Bever, and H.S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal Cognition, Erlbaum, HiUsdale, NJ, 1984, pp. 465-488. [31] Gibbon, J. and Church, R.M., Representation of time, Cognition, 37 (1990) 23-54. [32] Gibbon, J. and Church, R.M., Comparison of variance and covari- ance patterns in parallel and serial theories of timing, J. Expt. Anal. Behav., 57 (1992) 393-406. [33] Gibbon, J., Church, R.M. and Meck, W.H., Scalar timing in memory. In J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423 (1984) 52-77. [34] Goody, W., Disorders of the time sense. In P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 3, John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York, 1969, pp. 229-250. [35] Graybiel, A.M., Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in the basal ganglia, Trends Neurosci., 13 (1990) 244-254. [36] Hashimoto, C.P., Effects of repeated administration of pyrithiamine and oxythiamine on timing behavior in rats, Fol. Pharmacol. Jpn., 78 (1981) 521-528. [37] Hasselmo, M.E., Anderson, B.P. and Bower, J.M., Cholinergic modulation may enhance cortical associative memory function. In R.P. Lippman, J. Moody, and D.S. Touretzky (Eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, Vol. 3, Morgan Kaufman, San Mateo, CA, 1991, pp. 50-57. [38] Hattori, T. and Fibiger, H.C., Brain Res., 238 (1982) 245-250. [39] Hicks, R.E, Gualtieri, C.T., Mayo, J.P. and Perez-Reyes, M., Cannabis, atropine, and temporal information processing, Neu- ropsychobiology, 12 (1984) 229-237. [40] Hoagland, H., The physiological control of judgements of duration: evidence for a chemical clock, J. Gen. Psychol., 9 (1933) 267-287. [41] Hoebel, B.G., Monaco, A.P., Hernandez, L., Aulissi, E., Stanley, B.G. and Lenard, L., Self-injection of amphetamine directly into the brain, Psychopharmacology, 81 (1983) 158-163. [42] Jenkins, J.C., Krogsgaard-Larsen, P. and Honore, T., Structure-ac- tivity relationships in the development of excitatory amino acid receptor agonists and competitive antagonists, TIPS, 11 (1990) 25-33. [43] John, E.R. and Killam, T., Electrophysiological correlates of avoid- ance and conditioning in the cat, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 125 (1959) 252-275. [44] Johnson, S.W., Mercuri, N.B. and North, R.A., 5-Hydroxytryp- taminelB receptors block the GABAB synaptic potential in rat dopamine neurons, J. Neurosci., 12 (1992) 2000-2006. [45] Kebabian, J.W. and Calne, D.B., Multiple receptors for dopamine, Nature, 277 (1979) 197-199. [46] Kelley, A.E. and Delfs, J.M., Dopamine and conditioned reinforce- ment I. Differential effects of amphetamine microinjections into striatal subregions, Psychopharmacology, 103 (1991) 187-196. [47] Kelley, A.E, Stinus, L. and Iversen, S.D., Interactions between D-Ala-Met enkephalin, A10 dopaminergic neurones, and sponta- neous behaviour in the rat, Behav. Brain Res., 1 (1980) 3-24. [48] Kelly, P.H., Iversen, S.D., Selective 6-OHDA induced destruction of medolimbic dopamine neurons: abolition of psychostimulant-in- duced locomotor activity in rats, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 40 (1976) 45-56. [49] Kemel, M-L., Desban, M., Glowinsk, J., Gauchy, C., Distinct presynaptic control of dopamine release in striosomal and matrix areas of the cat caudate nucleus, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 86 9006-9010. [50] Kitai, S.T., Koscis, J.D., Preston, R.J. and Sugimori, M., Monosy- naptic inputs to caudate neurons identified by intracellular injection of horseadish peroxidase, Brain Res., 109 (1976) 601-606. [51] Klockgether, T. and Turski, L., Toward an understanding of the role of glutamate in experimental Parkinsonism: Agonist-sensitive sites in the basal ganglia, Ann. Neurol., 34 (1993) 585-593. [52] Kolb, B., Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative review, Brain Res. Rev., 8 (1984) 65-98. [53] Koob, G.F., Simon, H., Herman, J.P. and Le Moal, M., Neurolep- tic-like disruption of the conditioned avoidance response requires destruction of both the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine systems, Brain Res., 303 (1984) 319-329. [54] LaHoste, G.J. and Marshall, J.F., Nigral D1 and striatal D2 recep- tors mediate the behavioral effects of dopamine agonists, Behav. Brain Res., 38 (1990) 233-242. [55] Landfield, P.W., Baskin, R.K. and Pitier, T.A., Brain aging corre- lates: retardation by hormonal treatments, Science, 214 (1981) 581-584. [56] Lin, R.L., Makowiec, R.L., Hollingsworth, Z.R., Excitatory amino acid binding sites in the basal ganglia of the rat: a quantitative autoradiographic study, Neuroscience, 46 (1992) 35-48. [57] LiSschmann, P.-A., Eblen, F., Wtillner, U., Klockgether, T. and Turski, L., Behavioural role of glutamate in the basal ganglia, Behav. Pharmacol.,5 (1994) 12. [58] Luria, A.R., Frontal lobe syndromes. In P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 2, John Wiley and Sons, New York, NY, 1969, pp. 725-757. [59] Lyness, W.H., Friedle, N.M. and Moore, K.E., Destruction of dopaminergic nerve terminals in nucleus accumbens: effects of d-amphetamine self-stimulation, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 11 (1979) 553-556. [60] Malapani, C., Pillon, B., Lo Russo, G., Dubois, B., Munari, C. and Agid, Y., Damage to striatofrontal loops impairs simultaneous temporal processing (STP) ability, Neurology, 42 (1992) 183. [61] Malapani, C., Deweer, B., Pillon, B., Bubois, B., Agid, Y., Rakitin, B.C., Penney, T.B., Hinton, S.C., Gibbon, J. and Meck, W.H., Impaired time perception in Parkinson's disease is reversed with apomorphine, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 19 (1993) 631. [62] Maricq, A.V., Roberts, S. and Church, R.M., Methamphetamine and time estimation, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav., 7 (1981) 18-30. [63] Maricq, A.V. and Church, R.M., The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat, Psychopharmacology, 79 (1983) 10-15. [64] McAuley, F. and Leslie, J.C., Molecular analyses of the effects of d-amphetamine on fixed-interval schedule performance of rats, J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 45 (1986) 207-219. [65] Meck, W.H., Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav., 9 (1983) 171-201. [66] Meck, W.H., Attentional bias between modalities: effect on the internal clock, memory, and decision stages used in animal time discrimination. In J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423 (1984) 528-541. [67] Meck, W.H., Affinity for the dopamine D 2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 25 (1986) 1185-1189. [68] Meck, W.H., Vasopressin metabolite neuropeptide facilitates simul- taneous temporal processing, Behav. Brain Res., 23 (1987) 147- 157. [69] Meck, W.H., Internal clock and reward pathways share physio- logically similar information-processing stages. In M.L. Commons, R.M. Church, J.R. Stellar and A.R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Biological Determinants of Reinforcement, Vol. 7, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1988, pp. 121-138. [70] Meck, W.H., Hippocampal function is required for feed-back con- trol of an internal clock's criterion, Behav. Neurosci., 102 (1988) 54-60. [71] Meck, W.H., Modality-specific circadian rhythmicities influence mechanisms of attention and memory for interval timing, Learn. Motiv., 22 (1991) 153-179. [72] Meck, W.H., Cholinergic function and the internal clock, Behav. Pharm., 5 (1994) 27. [73] Meck, W.H., [3H]-Choline uptake in the frontal cortex of mature and aged rats is proportional to the absolute error of a temporal memory translation constant, submitted. [74] Meck, W.H., Frontal cortex or nucleus basalis magnocellularis lesions, but not hippocampal or medial septal area lesions, occasion the loss of control of the speed of an internal clock, submitted. [75] Meck, W.H., Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: a functional link between mesolimbic-mesocortical and nigrostriatal dopaminergic systems, submitted. [76] Meck, W.H. and Angell, K.E., Repeated administration of pyrithi- amine leads to a proportional increase in the remembered duration of events, Psychobiology, 20 (1992) 39-46. [77] Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Opioid effects on timing behavior in the rat: possible actions on dopaminergic and GABAergic neurons, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 10 (1984) 1103. [78] Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Simultaneous temporal processing, J. Exp. Psychol. Anita. Behav., 10 (1984) 1-29. [79] Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory, Behav. Neurosci., 101 (1987) 457- 464. [80] Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Nutrients that modify the speed of the internal clock and memory storage processes, Behav. Neurosci., 101 (1987) 465-475 [81] Meck, W.H., Church, R.M. and Olton, D.S., Hippocampus, time, and memory, Behav. Neurosci., 98 (1984) 3-22. [82] Meck, W.H., Church, R.M. and Wenk, G.L., Arginine vasopressin inoculates against age-related discrepancies in the content of tem- poral memory and increases in sodium-dependent high affinity choline uptake, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 130 (1986) 327-331. [83] Meck, W.H., Church, R.M. and Wenk, G.L., Temporal memory in mature and aged rats is sensitive to choline acetyltransferase inhibi- tion, Behav. Brain Res., in press. [84] Meck, W.H., Church, R.M., Wenk, G.L. and Olton, D.S., Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory, J. Neurosci., 7 (1987) 3505-3511. [85] Meck, W.H., Shapiro, A.G. and Williams, C.L., Prenatal choline exposure produces long-term improvement in temporal processing, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 14 (1988) 58. [86] Miller, W., DeLong, M.L., Altered tonic activity of neurons in the globus pallidus and subtthalamic nucleus in the primate MPTP model of parkinsonism. In M.B. Carpenter and E. Jayaraman (Eds.), The Basal Ganglia, Part II, Plenum, New York, 1987, pp. 414-427. [87] Milner, B., Some cognitive effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., 298 (1982) 211-226. [88] Milner, B. and Petrides, M., Behavioural effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man, TINS, 7, (1984) 403-407. [89] Milner, B., Petrides, M. and Smith, M.L., Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of memory, Human Neurobiol., 4 (1985) 137-142. [90] Nichelli, P., Venneri, A., Molinari, M., Tavani, F. and Grafman, J., Precision and accuracy of subjective time estimation in different memory disorders, Cogn. Brain Res., 1 (1993) 87-93. [91] O'Keefe, J. and Nadel, L., The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map, Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1978. [92] Olton, D.S., Frontal cortex, timing and memory, Neuropsycholo- gia, 27 (1989) 121-130. [93] Olton, D.S., Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Separation of hip- pocampal and amygdaloid involvement in temporal memory dys- function, Brain Res., 404 (1987) 180-188. [94] Olton, D.S., Wenk, G.L., Church, R.M. and Meck, W.H., Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing, Neuropsychologia, 26 (1988) 307-318. [95] Ornstein, R.E., On the Experience of Time, Penguin, Har- mondsworth, UK, 1969. [96] Pennartz, C.M.A. and Kitai, S.T., Hippocampal inputs to identified neurons in an in vitro slice preparation of the rat nucleus accum- bens: evidence for feed-forward inhibition, J. Neurosci., 11 (1991) 2838-2847. [97] Penney, T.B., Holder, M.D. and Meck, W.H., Clonidine-induced antagonism of norepinephrine modulates the attentional processes involved in peak-interval timing, Exp. Clin. Psychopharm., 4 (1996) 82-92. [98] Robertson, G.S., Robertson, H.A., D1 and D2 dopamine agonist synergism: separate sites of action? Trends. Pharmacol. Sci.,8 (1987) 295-299. [99] Robertson, G.S. and Robertson, H.A., Evidence that the substantia nigra is a site of action for L-DOPA, Neurosci. Lett., 89 (1988) 204-208 [100] Robbins, T.W., Giardini, V., Jones, G.H., Reading, P. and Sa- hakian, B.J., Effects of dopamine depletion from the caudate-puta- men and nucleus accumbens septi on the acquisition and perfor- mance of a conditional discrimination task, Behav. Brain Res., 38 (1990) 243-261. [101] Richards, W., Time reproductions by H.M., Acta Psychol., 37 (1973) 279-282. [102] Rummsayer, T., Is there a common dopaminergic basis of time perception and reaction time?, Neuropsychobiology, 21 (1989) 37-42. [103] Salmon, P. and Gray, J.A., Comparison between the effects of propranolol and chlordiazepoxide on timing behavior in the rat, Psychopharmacology, 87 (1985) 219-224. [104] Saanishi, S., Molecular diversity of glutamate receptors and ima- tions for brain function, Science, 258, (1992) 597-603. [105] Schultz, W. and Ungerstedt, U., Short-term increase and long-term reversion of striatal cell activity after degeneration of the nigrostrri- atal dopamine system, Exp. Brain Res., 33 (1978) 159-165. [106] Shurtleff, D., Raslear, T.G., Genovese, R.F. and Simmons, L., Perceptual bisection in rats: the effects of physostigmine, scopo- lamine and pirenzepine, Physiol. Behav., 51 (1992) 381-390. [107] Silver, R. and Bittman, E.L., Reproductive mechanisms: interaction of circadian and interval timing. In J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423 (1984) pp. 488-514. [108] Smith, M.L. and Milner, B., Differential effects of frontal-lobe lesions on cognitive estimation and spatial memory, Neuropsy- chologia, 22 (1985) 697-705. [109] Spetch, M.L. and Treit, D., The effect of d-amphetamine on short-term memory for time in pigeons, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 21 (1984) 663-666. [110] Stubbs, D.A. and Thomas, J.R., Discrimination of stimulus dura- tion and d-amphetamine in pigeons: a psychophysical analysis, Psychopharmalogia, 36 (1974) 313-322. [ 111] Taylor, J.R. and Robbins, T.W., 6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens, but not of the caudate nucleus, attenuate enhanced responding with reward-related stimuli produced by in- tra-accumbens d-amphetamine, Psychopharmacology, 90 (1986) 390-397. [112] Terman, M., Gibbon, J., Fairhurst, S. and Waring, A., Daily meal anticipation: Interaction of circadian and interval timing, In J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and Time Perception, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423 (1984) 470-487. [113] Turski, L., Havemann, U., and Kuschinsky, K., GABAergic mech- anisms in mediating mescular rigidity, catalepsy and postural asym- metry in rats: differences between dorsal and ventral striatum, Brain Res., 322 (1984) 49-57. [114] Turski, L., Klockgether, T., Turski, W.A., Schwarz, M. and Son- tag, K.-H., Blockade of excitatory neurotransmission in the globus pallidus induces rigidity and akinesia in the rat: implications for excitatory neurotransmission in pathogenesis of Parkinson's dis- eases, Brain Res., 512 (1990) 125-131. [115] Ungerstedt, U., Stereotaxic mapping of the monoamine pathways in the rat brain, Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl., 367 (1971) 1-48. [116] Ungerstedt U., Butcher, L.L., Butcher, S.G., Anden, N.-E. and Fuxe, K., Direct chemical stimulation of dopaminergic mechanisms in the neostriatum of the rat, Brain Res., 14 (1969) 461-467. [117] Wachtel, H., Kunow, M. and Loschmann, P-A., NBQX and CPP potentiate dopamine induced rotations in substantia nigra lesioned rats, Neurosci. Lett., 142 (1992) 179-182. [118] Waiters J.R., Bergstrom, D.A., Carlson, J.H., Chase, T.N., Braun, A.D., D1 dopamine receptor activation required for postsynaptic expression of D2 agonist effects, Science, 236 (1987) 719-722. [119] Warenycia, M.W., McKenzie, G.M., Murphy, M. and Szerb, J.C., Neuropharmacology, 26 (1987) 1107-1114. [120] Wearden, J.H., Prescriptions for models of biopsychological time. In M. Oaksford and G. Brown (Eds.), Neurodynamics and Psychol- ogy, Adademic Press, New York, 1994, pp. 215-236. [121] Wearden, J.H. and Penton-Voak, I.S., Feeling the heat: body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited, Q.J. Exp. Psychol., in press. [122] Welzl, H., Berz, S. and B~ittig, K., The effects of the noncompeti- tive NMDA receptor antagonist MK 801 on DRL performance in rats, Psychobiology, 19 (1991) 211-216. [123] White, F.J., D1 dopamine receptor stimulation enables the inhibi- tion of nucleus accumbens neurons by a D2 receptor agonist, Eur. J. Pharmacol., 135 (1987) 101-105. [124] White, N.M., A functional hypothesis concerning the striatal matrix and patches: mediation of S-R memory and reward, Life. Sci., 45 (1989) 1943-1957. [125] White, N.M., Reward or reinforcement: what's the difference?, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 13 (1989) 181-186. [126] White, N.M. and Viaud, M., Localized intracaudate dopamine D2 receptor activation during the post-training period improves mem- ory for visual or olfactory conditioned emotional responses in rats, Behav. Neural Biol., 55 (1991) 255-269. [127] Williams, D., Temporal lobe syndromes. In P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 2, John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1969, pp. 700-724. [128] Williams, J.M., Medwedeeff, C.H. and Haban, G., Memory disor- ders and subjective time estimation, J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 11 (1989) 713-723. [129] Zigmond, M.J., Berger, T.W., Grace, A.A. and Stricker, E.M., Compensatory responses to nigrastriatal bundle injury: studies with 6-hydroxydopamine in an animal model of Parkinsonism, Mol. Chem. Neuropathol., 10 (1989) 185-200. [130] Zis, A.P, Fibiger, H.C and Phillips, A.G., Reversal by L-dopa of impaired learning due to destruction of the dopaminergic nigro- neostriatal projection, Science, 185 (1974) 960-962.
  266. References 1. Beckwith NE, Lehmann DR. The importance of halo effects in multi-attribute attitude models. J. Mark. Res 1975;12:265–275. 2. Day GS. The threats to marketing research. J. Mark. Res 1975;12:462–467. 3. Griffin A, Hauser JR. The voice of the customer. Mark. Sci 1993;12:1–27. 4. Green PE, Srinivasan V. Conjoint analysis in marketing: new developments with implications for research and practice. J. Mark 1990;54:3–19. 5. Lindstrom, M. Buyology. Truth and Lies About Why We Buy. New York: Doubleday; 2008. 6. Hauser JR, Shugan SM. Intensity measures of consumer preference. Oper. Res 1980;28:278–320. 7. Buchanan B, Henderson PW. Assessing the bias of preference, detection, and identification measures of discrimination ability in product design. Mark. Sci 1992;11:64–75. 8. Kahneman D, Wakker PP, Sarin R. Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility. Q. J. Econ 1997;112:375–405. 9. Plassmann H. O’Doherty, J. & Rangel, A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J. Neurosci 2007;27:9984–9988. [PubMed: 17855612] 10. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 2009;324:646–648. [PubMed: 19407204] 11. O’Doherty JP, Deichmann R. Critchley, H. D. & Dolan, R. J. Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 2002;33:815–826. [PubMed: 11879657] 12. Aharon I, et al. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 2001;32:537–551. [PubMed: 11709163] 13. Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhume VB. When the brain plays music: auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Rev. Neurosci 2007;8:547–558. [PubMed: 17585307] 14. Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci 2001;21:RC159. [PubMed: 11459880] 15. O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neurosci 2001;4:95–102. [PubMed: 11135651] 16. Izuma K, Saito DN, Sadato N. Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 2008;58:284–294. [PubMed: 18439412] 17. Rilling JK, et al. A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 2002;35:1–20. [PubMed: 12123599] 18. Ploghaus A, Becerra L, Borras C, Borsook D. Neural circuitry underlying pain modulation: expectation, hypnosis, placebo. Trends Cogn. Sci 2003;7:197–200. [PubMed: 12757820] 19. Ploghaus A, et al. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 1999;284:1979– 1981. [PubMed: 10373114] 20. Koyama T, McHaffie JG, Laurienti PJ, Coghill RC. The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 2005;102:12950–12955. [PubMed: 16150703] 21. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Rev. Neurosci 2002;3:655–666. [PubMed: 12154366] 22. Hare TA, O’Doherty J, Camerer CF, Schultz W, Rangel A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci 2008;28:5623–5630. [PubMed: 18509023] 23. Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Shiv B, Rangel A. Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 2008;105:1050–1054. [PubMed: 18195362] 24. Poldrack RA. Can. cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn. Sci 2006;10:59–63. [PubMed: 16406760] 25. Poldrack RA. The role of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience: where do we stand? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 2008;18:223–227. [PubMed: 18678252] 26. Delgado MR. Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Ann. NY Acad. Sci 2007;1104:70– 88. [PubMed: 17344522] 27. Fox PT, Lancaster JL. Mapping context and content: the BrainMap model. Nature Rev. Neurosci 2002;3:319–321. [PubMed: 11967563] 28. Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron 2007;53:147–156. [PubMed: 17196537] 29. Grosenick L, Greer S, Knutson B. Interpretable classifiers for FMRI improve prediction of purchases. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng 2008;16:539–548. 30. Rangel A, Camerer C, Montague PR. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Rev. Neurosci 2008;9:545–556. [PubMed: 18545266] 31. Camerer C, Loewenstein G, Prelec D. Neuroeconomics: how neuroscience can inform economics. J. Econ. Lit 2005;43:9–64. 32. Glimcher PW. Decisions, decisions, decisions: choosing a biological science of choice. Neuron 2002;36:323–332. [PubMed: 12383785] 33. Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Rewardguided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur. J. Neurosci 2008;28:1437–1448. [PubMed: 18793321] 34. Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad JA. Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 2006;441:223–226. [PubMed: 16633341] 35. Schoenbaum G, Roesch M. Orbitofrontal cortex, associative learning, and expectancies. Neuron 2005;47:633–636. [PubMed: 16129393] 36. Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn. Sci 2006;10:424–430. [PubMed: 16899397] 37. Haynes JD, Rees G. Decoding mental states from activity in humans. Nature Rev. Neurosci 2006;7:523–534. [PubMed: 16791142] 38. Kamitani Y, Tong F. Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain. Nature Neurosci 2005;8:679–685. [PubMed: 15852014] 39. Kay KN, Naselaris T, Prenger RJ, Gallant JL. Identifying natural images from human brain activity. Nature 2008;452:352–356. [PubMed: 18322462] 40. Hasson U, Nir Y, Levy I, Fuhrmann G, Malach R. Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 2004;303:1634–1640. [PubMed: 15016991] 41. Mitchell TM, et al. Predicting human brain activity associated with the meanings of nouns. Science 2008;320:1191–1195. [PubMed: 18511683] 42. Speer NK, Zacks JM, Reynolds JR. Human brain activity time-locked to narrative even boundaries. Psychol. Sci 2007;18:449–455. [PubMed: 17576286] 43. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA. Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:1674–1682. [PubMed: 16687157] 44. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA. Decoding human brain activity during real-world experiences. Trends Cogn. Sci 2007;11:356–365. [PubMed: 17618161] 45. McClure SM, et al. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron 2004;44:379–387. [PubMed: 15473974] 46. Ambler T, Ioannides A, Rose S. Brands on the brain: neuro-images of advertising. Bus. Strategy Rev 2000;11:17–30. 47. Rossiter JR, Silberstein RB, Harris PG, Nield G. Brain-imaging detection of visual scene encoding in long-term memory for TV commercials. J. Advert. Res 2001;41:13–22. 48. Astolfi L, et al. Neural basis for brain responses to TV commercials: a high-resolution EEG study. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng 2008;16:522–531. 49. Tverksy A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 1981;211:453–458. [PubMed: 7455683] 50. De Martino B, Kumaran D, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science 2006;313:684–687. [PubMed: 16888142] 51. Waber RL, Shiv B, Carmon Z, Ariely D. Commercial features of placebo and therapeutic efficacy. JAMA 2008;299:1016–1017. [PubMed: 18319411] 52. Montague PR, Berns GS. Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron 2002;36:265–284. [PubMed: 12383781] 53. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 1997;275:1593–1599. [PubMed: 9054347] 54. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter? Nature Rev. Neurosci 2005;6:545–552. [PubMed: 15995725] 55. Wager TD, et al. Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 2004;303:1162–1167. [PubMed: 14976306] 56. Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK. Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect. J. Neurosci 2005;25:10390–10402. [PubMed: 16280578] 57. Kenning PH, Plassmann H. How neuroscience can inform consumer research. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng 2008;16:532–538. 58. Lee N, Broderick AJ, Chamberlain L. What is neuromarketing? A discussion and agenda for future research. Int. J. Psychophysiol 2007;63:199–204. [PubMed: 16769143] 59. Yoon C, Gutchess AH, Feinberg F, Polk TA. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of neural dissociations between brand and person judgments. J. Consum. Res 2006;33:31–40. 60. Small DM, Prescott J. Odor/taste integration and the perception of flavor. Exp. Brain Res 2005;166:345–357. [PubMed: 16028032] 61. Rolls ET. Brain mechanisms underlying flavour and appetite. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci 2006;361:1123–1136. [PubMed: 16815796] 62. De Araujo IET, Rolls ET. The representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. J. Neurosci 2004;24:3086–3093. [PubMed: 15044548] 63. Hasson U, Furman O, Clark D, Dudai Y, Davachi L. Enhanced intersubject correlations during movie viewing correlate with successful episodic encoding. Neuron 2008;57:452–462. [PubMed: 18255037] 64. Eberhard JP. Applying neuroscience to architecture. Neuron 2009;62:753–756. [PubMed: 19555644] 65. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA. Neural substrates of driving behaviour. NeuroImage 2007;36:245–255. [PubMed: 17412611] 66. Calhoun VD, et al. Different activation dynamics in multiple neural systems during simulated driving. Hum. Brain Map 2002;16:158–167. 67. Freedman J. This is your brain on politics. New York Times. 2005 Jan 18; 68. Westen D, Blagov PS, Harenski K, Kilts C, Hamann S. Neural bases of motivated reasoning: an FMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 US Presidential election. J. Cogn. Neurosci 2006;18:1947–1958. [PubMed: 17069484] 69. Kato J, et al. Neural correlates of attitude change following positive and negative advertisements. Front. Behav. Neurosci 2009;3:6. [PubMed: 19503749] 70. Tsao DY, Livingstone MS. Mechanisms of face perception. Ann. Rev. Neurosci 2008;31:411–437. [PubMed: 18558862] 71. Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Transact. Roy. Soc. B Biol. Sci 2006;361:2109–2128. 72. Spezio ML, et al. A neural basis for the effect of candidate appearance on election outcomes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 2008;3:344–352. [PubMed: 19015087] 73. Kaplan JT, Freedman J, Iacoboni M. Us versus them: political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural response to faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:55–64. [PubMed: 16764897] 74. Fehr E, Camerer C. Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn. Sci 2007;11:419–427. [PubMed: 17913566] 75. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 2005;435:673–676. [PubMed: 15931222] 76. King-Casas B, et al. Getting to know you: reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science 2005;308:78–83. [PubMed: 15802598] 77. Baumgartner T, Heinrichs M, Vonlanthen A, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron 2008;58:639–650. [PubMed: 18498743] 78. Davidson RJ, Ekman P, Saron CD, Senulis JA, Friesen WV. Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology I. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1990;58:330–341. [PubMed: 2319445] 79. Harmon-Jones E. Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. Psychophysiology 2003;40:838–848. [PubMed: 14986837] 80. Huster RJ, Stevens S, Gerlach AL, Rist F. A spectroanalytic approach to emotional responses evoked through picture presentation. Psychophysiol 2008;72:212–216. 81. Ohme R, Reykowska D, Wiener D, Choromanska A. Analysis of neurophysiological reactions to advertising stimuli by means of EEG and galvanic skin response measures. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ 2009;2:21–31. 82. Kobayashi M, Pascual-Leone A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in neurology. Lancet Neurol 2003;2:145–156. [PubMed: 12849236] 83. Illes J, et al. Incidental findings in brain imaging research. Science 2006;311:783–784. [PubMed: 16469905] 84. Thirion B, et al. Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. NeuroImage 2007;35:105–120. [PubMed: 17239619]
  267. References [1] Wikipedia list of lists of lists. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List of lists of lists. 3 [2] B. Alexe, T. Deselares, and V. Ferrari. Measuring the objectness of image windows. In PAMI, 2013. 3, 6 [3] D. Batra, P. Yadollahpour, A. Guzman-Rivera, and G. Shakhnarovich. Diverse M-best solutions in markov random fields. In ECCV, 2012. 4 [4] A. Bergamo and L. Torresani. Exploiting weakly-labeled web images to im- prove object classification: a domain adaptation approach. In NIPS, 2010. 3 [5] L. Bourdev and J. Malik. Poselets: Body part detectors trained using 3d human pose annotations. In ICCV, 2009. 1 [6] C.-C. Chang and C.-J. Lin. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. 2001. 4 [7] X. Chen, A. Shrivastava, and A. Gupta. NEIL: Extracting visual knowledge from web data. In ICCV, 2013. 3 [8] X. Chen, A. Shrivastava, and A. Gupta. Enriching Visual Knowledge Bases via Object Discovery and Segmentation. In CVPR, 2014. 8 [9] O. Chum and A. Zisserman. An exemplar model for learning object classes. In CVPR, 2007. 1, 3 [10] T. Dean et al. Fast, accurate detection of 100,000 object classes on a single machine. In CVPR, 2013. 7 [11] J. Deng et al. Imagenet: A large-scale hierarchical image database. In CVPR, 2009. 1, 2 [12] C. Desai and D. Ramanan. Detecting actions, poses, and objects with relational phraselets. In ECCV, 2012. 2, 3, 6, 7 [13] S. K. Divvala, A. A. Efros, and M. Hebert. How important are ‘deformable parts’ in the deformable parts model? In ECCV Workshop on Parts and At- tributes, 2012. arXiv:1206.3714. 1, 3, 5 [14] C. Dubout and F. Fleuret. Exact acceleration of linear object detectors. In ECCV, 2012. 7 [15] M. Everingham, L. V. Gool, C. Williams, J. Winn, and A. Zisserman. The pascal visual object classes (voc) challenge. In IJCV, 2010. 2, 4, 6 [16] A. Farhadi, I. Endres, D. Hoiem, and D. Forsyth. Describing objects by their attributes. In CVPR, 2009. 1, 2, 3 [17] A. Farhadi et al. Every picture tells a story: Generating sentences for images. In ECCV, 2010. 8 [18] P. Felzenszwalb, R. Girshick, D. McAllester, and D. Ramanan. Object detection with discriminatively trained part based models. PAMI, 2010. http://www.cs.berkeley.edu/ rbg/latent. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 [19] R. Fergus, F.-F. L., P. Perona, and A. Zisserman. Learning object categories from internet image searches. In Proc. of IEEE, 2010. 3 [20] A. Khosla, T. Zhou, T. Malisiewicz, A. A. Efros, and A. Torralba. Undoing the damage of dataset bias. In ECCV, 2012. 7 [21] G. Kim and E. P. Xing. On multiple foreground cosegmentation. In CVPR, 2012. 3, 8 [22] A. Kulesza and B. Taskar. k-DPPs: Fixed-size determinantal point processes. In ICML, 2011. 4 [23] C. H. Lampert, H. Nickisch, and S. Harmeling. Learning to detect unseen object classes by between-class attribute transfer. In CVPR, 2009. 1, 2, 3 [24] T. Lan, M. Raptis, L. Sigal, and G. Mori. From subcategories to visual com- posites: A multi-level framework for object detection. In ICCV, 2013. 3 [25] H. Lee et al. Deterministic coreference resolution based on entity-centric, precision-ranked rules. In Computational Linguistics, 2013. 7 [26] Y. J. Lee, A. A. Efros, and M. Hebert. Style-aware mid-level representation for discovering visual connections in space and time. In ICCV, 2013. 7 [27] C. Li, D. Parikh, and T. Chen. Automatic discovery of groups of objects for scene understanding. In CVPR, 2012. 3 [28] L.-J. Li and L. Fei-Fei. Optimol: Automatic online picture collection via incre- mental model learning. In IJCV, 2010. 3 [29] S. Li, G. Kulkarni, T. L. Berg, A. C. Berg, and Y. Choi. Composing simple im- age descriptions using web-scale n-grams. In Computational Natural Language Learning, 2011. 8 [30] Y. Lin et al. Syntactic annotations for the google books ngram corpus. In ACL, 2012. http://books.google.com/ngrams/datasets. 3 [31] T. Malisiewicz and A. A. Efros. Recognition by association via learning per- exemplar distances. In CVPR, 2008. 4 [32] E. Mezuman and Y. Weiss. Learning about canonical views from internet image collections. In NIPS, 2012. 2 [33] J.-B. Michel et al. Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. In Science, 2010. 2, 3 [34] V. Ordonez, J. Deng, Y. Choi, A. C. Berg, and T. L. Berg. From large scale image categorization to entry-level categories. In ICCV, 2013. 8 [35] F. Palermo, J. Hays, and A. Efros. Dating historical color images. In ECCV, 2012. 7 [36] M. Pandey and S. Lazebnik. Scene recognition and weakly supervised object localization with deformable part-based models. In ICCV, 2011. 2, 3, 5 [37] D. Parikh, A. Farhadi, K. Grauman, T. Berg, and A. Gupta. Attributes. In CVPR Tutorial, 2013. https://filebox.ece.vt.edu/ parikh/attributes/. 3 [38] O. Parkhi, A. Vedaldi, and A. Zisserman. On-the-fly specific person retrieval. In Image Analysis for Multimedia Interactive Services, 2012. 3 [39] A. Prest, C. Leistner, J. Civera, C. Schmid, and V. Ferrari. Learning object class detectors from weakly annotated video. In CVPR, 2012. 2, 3, 6, 7 [40] M. Rohrbach, M. Stark, G. Szarvas, I. Gurevych, and B. Schiele. What helps where and why semantic relatedness for knowledge transfer. In CVPR, 2010. 8 [41] M. Rubinstein, A. Joulin, J. Kopf, and C. Liu. Unsupervised joint object dis- covery and segmentation in internet images. In CVPR, 2013. 3, 8 [42] B. C. Russell et al. 3d wikipedia: Using online text to automatically label and navigate reconstructed geometry. In Siggraph Asia, 2013. 8 [43] M. Sadeghi and A. Farhadi. Recognition using visual phrases. In CVPR, 2011. 1, 2, 3, 8 [44] F. Schroff, A. Criminisi, and A. Zisserman. Harvesting image databases from the web. In PAMI, 2011. 3 [45] P. Siva and T. Xiang. Weakly supervised object detector learning with model drift detection. In ICV, 2011. 3, 6, 7 [46] H. O. Song, R. Girshick, and T. Darrell. Discriminatively activated sparselets. In ICML, 2013. 7 [47] A. Torralba and A. Efros. Unbiased look at dataset bias. In CVPR, 2011. 1, 2, 7 [48] D. Tsai et al. Large-scale image annotation using visual synset. In ICCV, 2011. 3 [49] S. Vijayanarasimhan and K. Grauman. Large-scale live active learning: Train- ing object detectors with crawled data and crowds. In CVPR, 2011. 3 [50] L. Wu, R. Jin, and A. K. Jain. Tag completion for image retrieval. In PAMI, 2013. 8 [51] X. Zhu, C. Vondrick, D. Ramanan, and C. Fowlkes. Do we need more training data or better models for object detection? In BMVC, 2012. 3, 5
  268. References and Notes 1. W. A. Press, A. A. Brewer, R. F. Dougherty, A. R. Wade, B. A. Wandell, Vision Res. 41, 1321 (2001). 2. R. B. H. Tootell et al., J. Neurosci. 17, 7060 (1997). 3. K. Grill-Spector, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 159 (2003). 4. U. Hasson, M. Harel, I. Levy, R. Malach, Neuron 37, 1027 (2003). 5. A. Ishai, L. G. Ungerleider, A. Martin, H. L. Schouten, J. V. Haxby, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 9379 (1999). 6. D. L. Ringach, M. J. Hawken, R. Shapley, J. Vis. 2, 12 (2002). 7. E. T. Rolls, N. C. Aggelopoulos, F. S. Zheng, J. Neurosci. 23, 339 (2003). 8. D. L. Sheinberg, N. K. Logothetis, J. Neurosci. 21, 1340 (2001). 9. T. Hartley, E. A. Maguire, H. J. Spiers, N. Burgess, Neuron 37, 877 (2003). 10. E. A. Maguire et al., Science 280, 921 (1998). 11. J. M. Zacks et al., Nat. Neurosci. 4, 651 (2001). 12. A. Bartels, S. Zeki, Hum. Brain Mapp. 21, 75 (2004). 13. L. Vincenzoni, S. Leone, The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly, S. Leone, Director, 161 min, Metro-Goldwyn Mayer (MGM), 1966. 14. Materials and methods are available on Science On- line. BrainShow.exe is available at www.weizmann. ac.il/neurobiology/labs/malach/ReverseCorrelation. 15. In order to search for correlation between two cor- responding regions across subjects (intersubject cor- relation), we first aligned all brains into Talairach coordinate system and used a Gaussian filter of 12 mm full width at half maximum value (FWHM) to the data [similar results were obtained with the use of 8 mm FWHM (17)]. To remove preprocessing artifacts, we excluded the first and last 10 time points of the experiment from the analysis. We then used the time course of each voxel of the source subject as a GLM predictor for modeling the activity in the correspond- ing voxel of the target subject. Only voxels whose P value was no more than 0.05 (corrected) were con- sidered significant. 16. P. Janata et al., Science 298, 2167 (2002). 17. U. Hasson, Y. Nir, I. Levy, G. Fuhrmann, R. Malach, unpublished data. 18. J. P. Jones, L. A. Palmer, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 1187 (1987). 19. For the reverse correlation, each given time course was z-normalized and smoothed with a moving average of three time points. A t test was applied to each time point along the averaged time course, and only time points that were significantly different (P 0.05) from the average mean value (i.e., 0) were included in the analysis (colored in Figs. 3 and 4). With the use of the inhouse “Brain Show” software, we then reconstructed a short “movie” com- posed of the frames that led to the highest BOLD signal in each specific region, assuming our standard hemodynamic lag of 3 s. We then examined those movies in an attempt to evaluate each region’s functional specialization under natural viewing conditions. 20. To remove the nonselective component from the movie data set for the region-specific time course, we z-normalized the common “time course” of each subject (Fig. 2B), smoothed it with a moving average of three time points, and then used it as a GLM predictor, which was fitted to the entire data set of the subject. The residual time course (which was not explained by the predictor) was saved as a new time course. 21. N. Kanwisher, J. McDermott, M. M. Chun, J. Neurosci. 17, 4302 (1997). 22. J. V. Haxby, E. A. Hoffman, M. I. Gobbini, Trends Cognit. Sci. 4, 223 (2000). 23. G. K. Aguirre, E. Zarahn, M. D’Esposito, Neuron 21, 373 (1998). 24. R. Epstein, N. Kanwisher, Nature 392, 598 (1998). 25. To view all frames associated with the peaks of activa- tion in the fusiform face-related, building-related, and PCS regions, use the DVD of (13) and BrainShow.exe (14). 26. P. E. Downing, Y. Jiang, M. Shuman, N. Kanwisher, Science 293, 2470 (2001). 27. M. S. Beauchamp, K. E. Lee, J. V. Haxby, A. Martin, Neuron 34, 149 (2002). 28. A. Puce, T. Allison, S. Bentin, J. C. Gore, G. McCarthy, J. Neurosci. 18, 2188 (1998). 29. E. A. Hoffman, J. V. Haxby, Nat. Neurosci. 3, 80 (2000). 30. A. R. Damasio, The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Har- court Brace, New York, ed. 1, 1999). 31. J. V. Haxby et al., Science 293, 2425 (2001). 32. H. D. Critchley et al., Brain 126, 2139 (2003). 33. J. J. DiCarlo, J. H. Maunsell, Nat. Neurosci. 3, 814 (2000). 34. K. Grill-Spector et al., Neuron 24, 187 (1999). 35. M. Ito, H. Tamura, I. Fujita, K. Tanaka, J. Neurophysiol. 73, 218 (1995). 36. J. A. Brefczynski, E. A. DeYoe, Nat. Neurosci. 2, 370 (1999). 37. K. M. O’Craven, P. E. Downing, N. Kanwisher, Nature 401, 584 (1999). 38. R. B. Tootell et al., Neuron 21, 1409 (1998). 39. G. Rizzolatti, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, Curr. Opin. Neu- robiol. 7, 562 (1997). 40. G. Rizzolatti, L. Fadiga, V. Gallese, L. Fogassi, Brain Res. Cognit. Brain Res. 3, 131 (1996). 41. S. Cochin, C. Barthelemy, S. Roux, J. Martineau, Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1839 (1999). 42. A. Dijksterhuis, J. A. Bargh, in Advances in Experimen- tal Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2001), vol. 33, pp. 1–40. 43. J. M. Kilner, Y. Paulignan, S. J. Blakemore, Curr. Biol. 13, 522 (2003). 44. This study was funded by the Benoziyo Center. We thank the Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging in the Tel-Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; M. Behr- mann, K. Grill-Spector, G. Avidan, M. Peleg, and S. Gilaie-Dotan for fruitful discussions and com- ments; R. Goebel and D. Paserman for helping with the intersubject analysis; M. Harel for three- dimensional brain reconstructions; E. Okon for technical assistance; E. Malach for programming the “brain-show” reverse-correlation analysis tool; and R. Mukamel for software development. We thank C. Eastwood and H. Grimaldi for kindly pro- viding us with permission to use the frames. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/303/5664/1634
  269. References Adkins, N. R., Ozanne, J. L. (2005), "The low literate consumer," Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (1), 93-105. Alfano, K. M., Cimino, C. R. (2008), "Alteration of expected hemispheric asymmetries: Valence and arousal effects in neuropsychological models of emotion," Brain and Cognition, 66 (3), 213-220. Anderson, A. K., Phelps, E. A. (2001), “Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events,” Nature, 411(6835), 305-309. Aquino, K., Reed II, A. (2002), "The self-importance of moral identity," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (6), 1423-1440. Aron, Elaine N. (1998) The highly sensitive person: How to thrive when the world overwhelms you. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press. Arshamian, A., Willander, J., Larsson, M. (2011), "Olfactory awareness is positively associated to odour memory," Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23 (2), 220-226. Babin, Laurie A. and Alvin C. Burns (1997), "Effects of Print Ad Pictures and Copy Containing Instructions to Imagine on Mental Imagery That Mediates Attitudes," Journal of Advertising, 26 (3), 33-44. Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., Pieters, R., Zeelenberg, M. (2000), “The role of emotions in goal-directed behavior,” The why of consumption: Contemporary perspectives on consumer motives, goals, and desires, 36-58. Baker, S. M. (2006), "Consumer normalcy: Understanding the value of shopping through narratives of consumers with visual impairments," Journal of Retailing, 1, 37-50. Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., Engle, R. W. (2004), "Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind," Psychological Bulletin, 130 (4), 553-573. Barsalou, L. W. (2008), “Grounded cognition,” Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645. Bechara, Antoine (2004), "The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage," Brain and Cognition, 55 (1), 30-40. Bechara, A. (2005), “Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective,” Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1458-1463. Bensafi, M., Pouliot, S., Sobel, N. (2005), "Odorant-specific patters of sniffing during imagery distinguish 'bad' and 'good' olfactory imagers," Chemical Senses, 30 (6), 521-529. Bensafi, M., Rouby, C. (2007), “Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception,” Chemical Senses, 32(3), 237-244. Bensafi, M., Sobel, N., Khan, R. M. (2007), "Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception," Journal of Neurophysiology, 98 (6), 3254-3262. Bernhardson, B. M., Tishelman, C., Rutqvist, L. E. (2008), “Self-reported taste and smell changes during cancer chemotherapy,” Supportive Care in Cancer, 16(3), 275-283. Bernhardson, B. M., Tishelman, C., Rutqvist, L. E. (2009), “Taste and smell changes in patients receiving cancer chemotherapy: Distress, impact on daily life, and self-care strategies,” Cancer Nursing, 32(1), 45-54. Bosmans, A. (2006), “Scents and sensibility: when do (in) congruent ambient scents influence product evaluations?” Journal of Marketing, 32-43. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J. (2001), “Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing,” Emotion, 1(3), 276-298. Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J. (2000), “Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and physiology,” Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion, 25, 49-59. Bromley, S. M. (2000), "Smell and taste disorders: A primary care approach," American Family Physican, 61 (2), 427-436. Bulsing, P. J., Smeets, M. A., Gemeinhardt, C., Laverman, M., Schuster, B., Van den Hout, M. A., Hummel, T. (2010), “Irritancy expectancy alters odor perception: evidence from olfactory event-related potential research,” Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(5), 2749-2756. Bulsing, P. J., Smeets, M. A. M., Van den Hout, M. A. (2009), "The implicit association between odors and illness," Chemical Senses, 34 (2), 111-119. Burns, Alvin C., Abhijit Biswas, and Laurie A. Babin (1993), "The Operation of Visual Imagery as a Mediator of Advertising Effects," Journal of Advertising, 22 (2), 71-85. Bushdid C., Magnasco M. O., Vosshall L. B., and Keller A. (2014), “Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli,” Science, 343 (6177), 1370-1372. Cahill, L., Babinsky, R., Markowitsch, H. J., McGaugh, J. L. (1995). “The amygdala and emotional memory,” Nature, 337, 295-296. Cain, W. S., Gent, J. F. (1991), "Olfactory sensitivity: Reliability, generality, and association with aging," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17 (2), 382-391. Cameron, E. L. (2007) “Measures of human olfactory perception during pregnancy,” Chemical Senses, 32(8), 775-782. Cardinal, Rudolf N., John A. Parkinson, Jeremy Hall, and Barry J. Everitt (2002), "Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex," Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26 (3), 321-352. Carrasco, M., Ridout, J. B. (1993), "Olfactory perception and olfactory imagery: A multidimensional analysis," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19 (2), 287-301. Chebat, Jean-Charles and Richard Michon (2003), "Impact of ambient odors on mall shoppers' emotions, cognition, and spending: A test of competitive causal theories," Journal of Business Research, 56 (7), 529-539. Childers, Terry L. and Carol Kaufman-Scarborough (2009), "Expanding opportunities for online shoppers with disabilities," Journal of Business Research, 62 (5), 572-578. Chrea, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le Calve, B., Aymard, L., Velazco, M. I., Sander, D., Scherer, K. R. (2009), "Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to odors," Chemical Senses, 34 (1), 49-62. Clark, James M., and Paivio, Allan (2004), “Extensions of the Paivio, Yuille, and Madigan (1968) norms,” Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36 (3), 371-383. Comeau, T. B., Epstein, J. B., Migas, C. (2001), “Taste and smell dysfunction in patients receiving chemotherapy: a review of current knowledge,” Supportive Care in Cancer, 9(8), 575-580. Conway, M. A., Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000), "The Construction of Autobiographical Memories in the Self-Memory System," Psychological Review, 107 (2), 261-288. Cross, Samantha N.N., Meng-Hsien Lin, Terry Childers, “Stumbling blocks: Individual differences in olfaction and its impact on consumer well-being,” working paper. Crowder, R. G., Schab, F. R. (1995), “Imagery for odors,” Memory for Odors, 93-107. Croy, I., Olgun, S., Joraschky, P. (2011), "Basic emotions elicited by odors and pictures," Emotion, 11 (6), 1331-1335. Dalton, P. (1999), “Cognitive influences on health symptoms from acute chemical exposure,” Health Psychology, 18(6), 579-590. Dalton, P., Hummel, T. (2000), “Chemosensory function and response in idiopathic environmental intolerance,” Occupational Medicine-Philadelphia, 15(3), 539-556. Dalton, P., Wysocki, C. J., Brody, M. J., Lawley, H. J. (1997), “The influence of cognitive bias on the perceived odor, irritation and health symptoms from chemical exposure,” International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 69(6), 407-417. D'Argembeau, A., Van der Linden, M. (2006), "Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies," Consciousness and Cognition, 15 (2), 342-350. Dahl, D. W., Chattopadhyay, A., Gorn, G. J. (1999), "The use of visual mental imagery in new product design," Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (1), 18-28. Dalton, P. (1996a), "Odor Perception and Beliefs about Risk," Chemical Senses, 21 (4), 447- 458. ---- (1996b), "Odor perception and beliefs about risk," Chemical Senses, 21 (4), 447-458. ---- (2002), "Odor, irritation and perception of health risk," International Archives of Occumpational and Environmental Health, 75 (5), 283-390. De Volder, A. G., Toyama, H., Kimura, Y., Kiyosawa, M., Nakano, H., Vanlierde, A., & Senda, M. (2001), “Auditory triggered mental imagery of shape involves visual association areas in early blind humans,” Neuroimage, 14(1), 129-139. Djordjevic, J., R. J. Zatorre, M. Petrides, J. A. Boyle, and M. Jones-Gotman (2005), "Functional neuroimaging of odor imagery," Neuroimage, 24 (3), 791-801. Djordjevic, J., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M., Jones-Gotman, M. (2004), "The mind's nose: Effects of odor and visual imagery on odor detection," Psychological Science, 15 (3), 143- 148. Doty, Richard L., Paul Shaman, and Michael Dann (1984), "Development of the university of pennsylvania smell identification test: A standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function," Physiology & Behavior, 32 (3), 489-502. Ehrlichman, H., Halpern, J. N. (1988), "Affect and memory: Effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories," Journal of Personality and Social Psycholgy, 55 (5), 769-779. Ekman, P. (1992) “An argument for basic emotions,” Cognition & Emotion, 6(3-4), 169-200. Elder, Ryan S., and Aradhna Krishna (2012), “The “Visual depiction effect” in advertising: Facilitating embodied mental simulation through product orientation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (6), 988-1003. Engen, T. (1972), “The effect of expectation on judgments of odor,” Acta Psychologica, 36(6), 450-458. Ferdenzi C., Schirmer A., Roberts S. C., Delplanque S., Porcherot C., Cayeux I., Velazco M. I., Sander D., Scherer K. R., Grandjean D. (2011), "Affective dimensions of odor perception: A comparison between Swiss, British, and Singaporean populations," Emotion, 11 (5), 1168- 1181. Festjens, Anouk, Sabrina Bruyneel and Siegfried Dewitte (2014), “What a feeling! Touching sexually laden stimuli makes women seek rewards,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24 (2), Floch, J. L., M. Paul, G. L. Lievre, R. Peynegre, M. Labroue, and L. Perlemuter (1993), "Smell dysfunction and related factors in diabetic patients," Diabetes Care, 16 (6), 934-937. Frye, R. E., Schwartz, B. S., Doty, R. L. (1990), “Dose-related effects of cigarette smoking on olfactory function,” JAMA: the journal of the American Medical Association, 263(9), 1233-1236. Geisler, M. W., Murphy, C. (2000), “Event-related brain potentials to attended and ignored olfactory and trigeminal stimuli,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37(3), 309-315. Gilbert, A. N., Crouch, M., Kemp, S. E. (1998), "Olfactory and visual mental imagery," Journal of Mental Imagery, 22 (3-4), 137-146. Goldin, Philippe R., Kateri McRae, Wiveka Ramel, and James J. Gross (2008), "The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion," Biological Psychiatry, 63 (6), 577-586. Gollwitzer, P. M., Sheeran, P. (2009), "Self-regulation of consumer decision making and behavior: The role of implementation intentions," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19 (4), 593-607. González, J., Barros-Loscertales, A., Pulvermüller, F., Meseguer, V., Sanjuán, A., Belloch, V., Ávila, C. (2006), “Reading “cinnamon” activates olfactory brain regions,” Neuroimage, 32(2), 906-912. Gottfried, J. A., Dolan, R. J. (2003), "The nose smells what the eye sees: Crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception," Neuron, 39 (2), 375-386. Goukens, C., Dewitte, S., Pandelaere, M., Warlop, L. (2007), "Wanting a bit(e) of everything: Extending the valuation effect to variety seeking," Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (3), 386-394. Gross, J. J. (1998), "The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review," Review of General Psychology, 2 (3), 271-299. Gross, James J. (2002), "Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences," Psychophysiology, 39 (3), 281-291. Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., Etkin, A. (2011), "Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual- process framework," Cognition and Emotion, 25 (3), 400-412. Haidt, J., McCauley, C., Rozin, P. (1994), "Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors," Personality and Individual Differences, 16 (5), 701-713. Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., Olvet, D. M. (2010), "Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: An integrative review," Developmental Neuropsychology, 35 (2), 129- 155. Hajcak, G., Nieuwenhuis, S. (2006), “Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to unpleasant pictures,” Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(4), 291-297. Hajcak, G., Weinberg, A., MacNamara, A., Foti, D. (2012), “ERPs and the study of emotion,” Oxford Handbook of ERP Components, 441-474. Hamann, S. (2003), "Nosing in on the emotional brain," Nature Neuroscience, 6 (2), 106-108. Hart, D., Atkins, R., Ford, D. (1999), "Family influences on the formation of moral identity in adolescence: longitudinal analysis," Journal of Moral Education, 28 (3), 375-386. Heller, Sharon (2002) Too loud, too bright, too fast, too tight. New York, NY: Harper. Henkin, R. I., Levy, L. M. (2002), “Functional MRI of congenital hyposmia: brain activation to odors and imagination of odors and tastes,” Journal of computer assisted tomography, 26(1), 39-61. Herman, C.P., Polivy, J. (1980), “Restrained Eating,” In A.J. Stunkard (Ed.), Obesity (pp. 208-225). Philadelphia: Sauders. Herz, R. S., Engen, T. (1996), "Odor memory: Review and analysis," Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3 (3), 300-313. Herz, R. S., McCall, C., Cahill, L. (1999), "Hemispheric lateralization in the processing of odor pleasantness versus odor names," Chemical Senses, 24 (6), 691-695. Herz, R. S., Schankler, C., Beland, S. (2004), "Olfaction, emotion and associative learning: Effects on motivated behavior," Motivation and Emotion, 28 (4), 363-383. Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., Aarts, H. (2005), "Smells like clean spirit: Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior," Psychological Science, 16 (9), 689-693. Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., Dalgleish, T. (2008), "The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues," Emotion, 3, 395-409. Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., Cohen, A. B. (2009), "Disgust and the moralization of purity," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (6), 963-976. Hummel, T., Nordin, S. (2005), "Olfactory disorders and their consequences for quality of life," Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 125 (2), 116-121. Hung, Iris W., and Aparna A. Labroo, (2011), “From firm muscles to firm willpower: Understanding the role of embodied cognition in self-regulation,” Journal of Consumer Research 37 (6), 1046-1064. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Bloom, P. (2012), "Disgusting smells cause decreased liking of gay men," Emotion, 12 (1), 23-27. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., Bloom, P. (2009), “Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays,” Emotion, 9(3), 435-439. Inman, Jeffrey J. (2001), “The role of sensory specific satiety in attribute-level variety seeking,” Journal of Consumer Research 28 (1), 105-120. Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T. (1998), “Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations,” Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(4), 887-900. Izard, C. E. (1972), “Patterns of emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression,” San Diego, CA: Academic Press. John, O. P., Gross, J. J. (2004), "Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development," Journal of Personality, 72 (6), 1301-1334. Katotomichelakis, M., Balatsouras, D., Tripsianis, G., Davris, S., Maroudias, N., Danielides, V., Simopoulos, C. (2007), “The effect of smoking on the olfactory function,” Rhinology, 45(4), 273-280. Keil, A., Bradley, M. M., Hauk, O., Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Lang, P. J. (2002), “Large ‐ scale neural correlates of affective picture processing,” Psychophysiology, 39(5), 641-649. Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L. (2001), “Neural foundations of imagery,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635-642. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L. (2003), “When is early visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery?” Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 723-746. Krishna, A. (2011), “An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 332-351. Krishna, A., Morrin, M. (2008), “Does touch affect taste? The perceptual transfer of product container haptic cues,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34(6), 807-818. Krishna, A., Lwin, M. O., Morrin, M. (2010), “Product scent and memory,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37(1), 57-67. Krompinger, J. W., Moser, J. S., Simons, R. F. (2008), “Modulations of the electrophysiological response to pleasant stimuli by cognitive reappraisal,” Emotion, 8(1), 132-137. Laeng, B., Teodorescu, D. S. (2002), “Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene,” Cognitive Science, 26(2), 207-231. Lang, P., Bradley, M. M. (2007), “The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention,” Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment, 29-46. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N. (1998), “Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology,” Biological Psychiatry, 44(12), 1248-1263. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N. (1998), “Emotion and motivation: measuring affective perception,” Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15(5), 397-408. Larsson, M., Finkel, D., Pedersen, N. L. (2000), "Odor identification influences of age, gender, cognition, and personality," The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 55 (5), 304-310. Lazarus, R. S. (1991), “Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion,” American Psychologist, 46(8), 819-834. Lee, S. W. S., Schwarz, N. (2010a), "Dirty hands and dirty mouths: Embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression," Psychological Science, 21 (10), 1423-1425. ---- (2010b), "Washing away postdecisional dissonance," Science, 328 (5979), 709. Le Floch, J. P., Le Lièvre, G., Labroue, M., Paul, M., Peynegre, R., Perlemuter, L. (1993), “Smell dysfunction and related factors in diabetic patients,” Diabetes Care, 16(6), 934-937. Lerner, J. S., Keltner, D. (2000), “Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice,” Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473-493. Lerner, J. S., Keltner, D. (2001), “Fear, anger, and risk,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146-159. Lerner, J. S., Tiedens, L. Z. (2006), “Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger's influence on cognition,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115-137. Levy, L. M., Henkin, R. I., Lin, C. S., Hutter, A., Schellinger, D. (1999), “Odor memory induces brain activation as measured by functional MRI,” Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 23(4), 487-498. Lewis, P. A., Critchley, H. D., Rotshtein, P., Dolan, R. J. (2007), "Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words," Cerebral Cortex, 17 (3), 742-748. Lim, J., Urban, L., Green, B. G. (2008), “Measures of individual differences in taste and creaminess perception,” Chemical Senses, 33(6), 493-501. Lundström, J. N., Seven, S., Olsson, M. J., Schaal, B., Hummel, T. (2006), “Olfactory event- related potentials reflect individual differences in odor valence perception,” Chemical Senses, 31(8), 705-711. Lyman, B. J., McDaniel, M. A. (1990), "Memory for odors and odor names: Modalities of elaboration and imagery," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16 (4), 656-664. MacInnis, Deborah J. and Linda L. Price (1987), "The Role of Imagery in Information Processing: Review and Extensions," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (4), 473-491. Marzillier, S., Davey, G. (2005), "Anxiety and disgust: Evidence for an undirectional relationship," Cognition and Emotion, 19 (5), 729-750. Metcalfe, Janet and Walter Mischel (1999), “A hot/cool-system analyses of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower,” Psychological Review, 106 (1), 3-19. Menon Satya and Barbara E. Kahn (1995), “The impact of context on variety seeking in product choices,” Journal of Consumer Behavior, 22 (3), 285-295. Michon, R., Chebat, J. C., & Turley, L. W. (2005), “Mall atmospherics: the interaction effects of the mall environment on shopping behavior,” Journal of Business Research, 58(5), 576-583. Mischel, Walter, Ozlem Ayduk, and Rodolfo Mendoza ‐ Denton (2003), “Sustaining Delay of Gratification over Time: A Hot ‐ Cool Systems Perspective,” in Time and Decision, ed. George Lowenstein, Daniel Read, and Roy Baumeister, New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 175–200. Mitchell, Deborah J., Barbara E. Kahn, and Susan C. Knasko (1995), “There’s something in the air: Effects of congruent or incongruent ambient odor on consumer decision making,” Journal Consumer Research, 22 (2), 229-238. Morales, A. C., Fitzsimons, G. J. (2007), "Product contagion: Changing consumer evaluations with physical contact through "disgusting" products," Journal of Marketing Research, 44 (2), 272-283. Morrin, M., Krishna, A., Lwin, M. O. (2011), “Is scent-enhanced memory immune to retroactive interference?” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(3), 354-361. Morrin, M., & Chebat, J. C. (2005), “Person-place congruency the interactive effects of shopper style and atmospherics on consumer expenditures,” Journal of Service Research, 8(2), 181-191. Morrot, G., Brochet, F., & Dubourdieu, D. (2001)l “The color of odors,” Brain and language, 79(2), 309-320. Murphy, C., C. R. Schubert, K. J. Cruickshanks, B. E. K. Klein, Ronald Klein, and D. M. Nondahl (2002), "Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults," The Journal of the American Medical Association, 288 (18), 2307-2312. Murphy, Claire, Charlie D. Morgan, Mark W. Geisler, Spencer Wetter, James W. Covington, Michael D. Madowitz, Steven Nordin, and John M. Polich (2000), "Olfactory event-related potentials and aging: normative data," International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36 (2), 133-145. Nordin, S., Broman, D. A., Olofsson, J. K., Wulff, M. (2004), "A longitudinal descriptive study of self-reported abnormal smell and taste perception in pregnant women," Chemical Senses, 29 (5), 391-402. Nordin, S., Millqvist, E., Lowhagen, O., Bende, M. (2003), "The chemical sensitivity scale: Psychometric properties and comparison with the noise sensitivity scale," Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23 (4), 359-367. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I. (2009), "Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism," Psychological Bulletin, 135 (2), 303-321. Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., Gabrieli, J. D. (2002), “Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion,” Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 14(8), 1215-1229. Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., Gross, J. J. (2004), “For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down-and up- regulation of negative emotion,” Neuroimage, 23(2), 483-499. Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., Elwood, L. S. (2007), "The disgust scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement," Psychological Assessment, 19 (3), 281-297. Pause, B. M., Krauel, K. (2000), "Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to the psychology of odors," International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36 (2), 105-122. Peck, J., Childers, T. L. (2003), “Individual differences in haptic information processing: The “need for touch” scale,” Journal of Consumer Research, 30(3), 430-442. Peretz, I. (2008), “Musical disorders from behavior to genes,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(5), 329-333. Peretz, I., Brattico, E., Järvenpää, M., Tervaniemi, M. (2009), “The amusic brain: in tune, out of key, and unaware,” Brain, 132(5), 1277-1286. Poldrack, R. A., Wagner, A. D., Ochsner, K. N., Gross, J. J. (2008), "Cognitive emotion regulation insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience," Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17 (2), 153-158. Pollatos, O., Kopietz, R., Linn, J., Albrecht, J., Sakar, V. Anzinger, A., Schandry, R., Wiesmann, M. (2007), "Emotional stimulation alters olfactory sensitivity and odor judgment," Chemical Senses, 32 (6), 583-589. Porcherot, C., Delplanque, S., Raviot-Derrien, S., Le Calve, B., Chrea, c., Gaudreau, N., Cayeux, I. (2010), "How do you feel when you smell this? Optimization of a verbal measurement of odor-elicited emotions," Food Quality and Preference, 21 (8), 938-947. Pylyshyn, Z. (2003), “Return of the mental image: are there really pictures in the brain?” Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(3), 113-118. Ray, R., Ochsner, K. N., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Gross, J. J. (2005), "Individual differences in trait rumination and the neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal," Cognitive, Affect, and & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5 (2), 156-168. Reimann, M., Feye, W., Malter, A. J., Ackerman, J. M., Castaño, R., Garg, N., Kreuzbauer, R., Labroo, A. A., Lee, A. Y., Morrin, M., Nenkov, G.Y., Nielsen, J. H., Perez, M., Pol, G., Rosa, J.A., Yoon, C., Zhong, C., (2012), “Embodiment in judgment and choice,” Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 5(2), 104-123. Rinck, F., Rouby, C., Bensafi, M. (2009), "Which format for odor images?," Chemical Senses, 34 (1), 11-13. Rolls, E. T., Grabenhorst, F., Margot, C., Silva, M. A. A. P., Velazco, M. I. (2008), "Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes olfactory stimuli," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 (10), 1815-1826. Royet, J. P., Delon-Martin, C., Kareken, D. A., Seqebarth, C. (2003), "fMRI of emotional responses to odors: Infuence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender," NeuroImage, 20 (2), 713-728. Royet, J. P., Zald, D., Versace, R., Costes, N., Lavenne, F., Koenig, O., Gervais, R. (2000), "Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant olfactory, visual, and auditory stimuli: A positron emission tomography study," The Journal of Neuroscience, 20 (20), 7752-7759. Russell, J. A. (2003), “Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion,” Psychological Review, 110(1), 145-172. Russell, R., Duchaine, B., Nakayama, K. (2009) “Super-recognizers: People with extraordinary face recognition ability,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 252-257. Schnall, S., Benton, J., Harvey, S. (2008a), "With a clean conscience cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments," Psychological Science, 19 (12), 1219-1222. Schnall, S., Haldt, J., Clore, G. L., Jordan, A. H. (2008b), "Disgust as Embodied Moral Judgment," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 (8), 1096-1109. Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., Junghofer, M. (2006) “Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies,” Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31-51. Schwarz, N. (1990), “Feelings as information: informational and motivational functions of affective states,” Guilford Press. Segal, S. J., Fusella, V. (1970), “Influence of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(31), 458-464. Semal, C., Demany, L. (2006), “Individual differences in the sensitivity to pitch direction,” The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 3907-3915. Seubert, J., Rea, A. F., Loughead, J., Habel, U. (2009), "Mood induction with olfactory stimuli reveals differential affective responses in males and females," Chemical Senses, 34 (1), 77-84. Shimp, T. A., Stuart, E. W. (2004), "The role of disgust as an emotional mediator of advertising effects," Journal of Advertising, 33 (1), 43-53. Shiv, B., Fedorikhin, A. (1999), "Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making," Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (3), 278-292. Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., Barsalou, L. W. (2005) “Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward,” Cerebral Cortex, 15(10), 1602-1608. Sloboda, J. A., Wise, K. J., Peretz, I. (2005), “Quantifying tone deafness in the general population,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060(1), 255-261. Smith, C. A., Ellsworth, P. C. (1985), “Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813-838. Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Hartley, C. A., Desmond, J. E., Zhao, Z., Glover, G. H., Sullivan, E. V. (1998), “Odorant-induced and sniff-induced activation in the cerebellum of the human,” The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(21), 8990-9001. Spangenberg, E. R., Crowley, A. E., Henderson, P. W. (1996). “Improving the store environment: do olfactory cues affect evaluations and behaviors?” The Journal of Marketing, 60(2), 67-80. Spence, C. (2002), "Multisensory attention and tactile information-processing," Behavioral Brain Research, 135 (1), 57-64. Spence, C., Kettenmann, B., Kobal, G., McGlone, F. P. (2001), "Shared attentional resources for processing visual and chemosensory information," The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A (3), 775-783. Spivey, M. J., Geng, J. J. (2001), “Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects,” Psychological research, 65(4), 235-241. Steenkamp, Jan-Benedict E., and Hans Baumgartner (1992), “The role of optimum stimulation level in exploratory consumer behavior,” Journal of Consumer Behavior, 19 (3), 434-448. Steinbach, S., Hummel, T., Böhner, C., Berktold, S., Hundt, W., Kriner, M., Harbeck, N. (2009), “Qualitative and quantitative assessment of taste and smell changes in patients undergoing chemotherapy for breast cancer or gynecologic malignancies,” Journal of Clinical Oncology, 27(11), 1899-1905. Stevenson, R. J. (2010), “An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction,” Chemical Senses, 35(1), 3-20. Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I. (2005), "Olfactory imagery: A review," Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12 (2), 244-264. Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. B., Green, M. C., Lerner, J. S. (2000), "The psychology of the unthinkable: taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (5), 853-870. Tiggemann, M., Kemps, E. (2005), "The phenomenology of food cravings: The role of mental imagery," Appetite, 45 (3), 305-313. Uhl, F., Kretschmer, T., Lindinger, G., Goldenberg, G., Lang, W., Oder, W., Deecke, L. (1994), "Tactile mental imagery in sighted persons and in patients suffering from peripheral blindness early in life," Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 91 (4), 249- 255. Vennemann, M. M., T. Hummel, and K. Berger (2008), "The association between smoking and smell and taste impairment in the general population," Journal of Neurology, 255, 1121- 1126. Verplanken, B., Faes, S. (1999), "Good intentions, bad habits, and effects of forming implementation intentions on healthy eating," European Journal of Social Psychology, 29 (5- 6), 591-604. Wack, J. T., Rodin, J. (1982), “Smoking and its effects on body weight and the systems of caloric regulation,” The American journal of clinical nutrition, 35(2), 366-380. Warrenburg, S. (2005), "Effects of Fragrance on Emotions: Moods and Physiology," Chemical Senses, 30 (1), 248-249. Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., Jenike, M. A. (1998), “Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge,” The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411-418. Weinberg, P., Gottwald, W. (1982), "Impulsive consumer buying as a result of emotions," Journal of Business Research, 10 (1), 43-57. Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., Schwarz, N. (1997), “Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions, “Cognition & Emotion, 11(4), 433-465. Winston, J. S., Gottfried, J. A., Kilner, J. M., Dolan, R. J. (2005), "Integrated neural representations of odor intensity and affective valence in human amygdala," The Journal of Neuroscience, 25 (39), 8903-8907. Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C., Rozin, P. (1999), "Odor and affect: Individual differences in the impact of odor on liking for places, things and people," Chemical Senses, 24 (6), 713- 721. Xu, Alison J., and Aparna A. Labroo (2014), “Incandescent affect: Turning on the hot emotional system with bright light,” 24 (2), 205-216. Yeshurun, Y., Sobel, N. (2010), “An odor is not worth a thousand words: From multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects,” Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 219-241. Yoo, S. S., Freeman, D. K., McCarthy III, J. J., Jolesz, F. A. (2003), “Neural substrates of tactile imagery: a functional MRI study,” Neuroreport, 14(4), 581-585. Zald, David H. and Jose V. Pardo (1997), "Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94 (8), 4119-4124. Zellner, D. A., Kautz, M. A. (1990), “Color affects perceived odor intensity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(2), 391-397. Zhong, Chen-Bo and Katie Liljenquist (2006), "Washing away your sins: threatened morality and physical cleansing," Science, 313 (5792), 1451-1452. Zwebner, Yonat, Leonard Lee, and Jacob Goldenberg (2014), “The temperature premium: Warm temperatures increase product valuation,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24 (2), 251-259.
  270. References 1. Carpenter PA, Just MA, Shell P (1990) What one intelligence test measures: A theoret- ical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychol Rev 97:404–431. 2. Gray JR, Thompson PM (2004) Neurobiology of intelligence: Science and ethics. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:471–482. 3. Neisser U, et al. (1996) Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am Psychol 51:77–101. 4. Rohde TE, Thompson LA (2007) Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability. Intelligence 35:83–92. 5. te Nijenhuis J, van Vianen AEM, van der Flier H (2007) Score gains on g-loaded tests: No g. Intelligence 35:283–300. 6. Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C (2007) Intelligence and educational achieve- ment. Intelligence 35:13–21. 7. Gottfredson LS (1997) Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24:79–132. 8. Cattell RB (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. J Educ Psychol 54:1–22. 9. Baltes PB, Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U (1999) Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annu Rev Psychol 50:471–507. 10. Steele CM, Aronson JA (2004) Stereotype threat does not live by Steele and Aronson (1995) alone. Am Psychol 59:47–48; discussion 48–49. 11. Elliott R, et al. (1997) Effects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology 131:196–206. 12. Kimberg DY, D’Esposito M, Farah MJ (1997) Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. NeuroReport 8:3581–3585. 13. Haier RJ, et al. (1992) Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospa- tial/motor task: A positron emission tomographic study. Brain Res 570:134–143. 14. Smith ME, McEvoy LK, Gevins A (1999) Neurophysiological indices of strategy devel- opment and skill acquisition. Brain Res Cognit Brain Res 7:389–404. 15. Green CS, Bavelier D (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423:534–537. 16. Bors DA, Vigneau F (2003) The effect of practice on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Learn Individ Differ 13:291–312. 17. Ackerman PL (1987) Individual differences in skill learning: An integration of psycho- metric and information processing perspectives. Psychol Bull 102:3–27. 18. Healy AF, Wohldmann EL, Sutton EM, Bourne LE, Jr (2006) Specificity effects in training and transfer of speeded responses. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 32:534–546. 19. Singley MK, Anderson JR (1989) The transfer of cognitive skill (Harvard Univ Press, Cambridge, MA). 20. Ericsson AK, Delaney PF (1998) in Working Memory and Thinking, eds Logie R, Gilhooly KJ (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ), pp 93–114. 21. Chase WG, Ericsson KA (1981) Skilled memory. Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition, ed Anderson, J. R. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ), pp 141–189. 22. Halford GS, Cowan N, Andrews G (2007) Separating cognitive capacity from knowl- edge: A new hypothesis. Trends Cognit Sci 11:236–242. 23. Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS (2003) Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci 6:316–322. 24. KaneMJ,etal.(2004)Thegeneralityofworkingmemorycapacity:Alatent-variableapproach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:189–217. 25. Kane MJ, Engle RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspec- tive. Psychon Bull Rev 9:637–671. 26. Ackerman PL, Beier ME, Boyle MO (2005) Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychol Bull 131:30–60. 27. Conway AR, Kane MJ, Engle RW (2003) Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cognit Sci 7:547–552. 28. Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T (2004) Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci 7:75–79. 29. Westerberg H, Klingberg T (2007) Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory: A single-subject analysis. Physiol Behav 92:186–192. 30. Smith EE, Jonides J (1998) Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:12061–12068. 31. Oberauer K (2005) Binding and inhibition in working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. J Exp Psychol Gen 134:368–387. 32. Kane MJ, Conway AR, Miura TK, Colflesh GJ (2007) Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: A question of construct validity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 33:615–622. 33. Jaeggi SM, et al. (2007) On how high performers keep cool brains in situations of cognitive overload. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci 7:75–89. 34. Jonides J, et al. (1997) Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. J Cognit Neurosci 9:462–475. 35. Raven JC (1990) Advanced Progressive Matrices: Sets I, II (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford). 36. Hossiep R, Turck D, Hasella M (1999) Bochumer Matrizentest: BOMAT–Advanced–Short Version (Hogrefe, Go¨ ttingen). 37. Frearson W, Eysenck HJ (1986) Intelligence, reaction time (RT), and a new odd-man-out RT paradigm. Pers Individ Differ 7:807–818. 38. Tewes U (1991) Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest fu¨ r Erwachsene (HAWIE-R) (Hans Huber, Bern, Switzerland), rev 1991. 39. Daneman M, Carpenter PA (1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 19:450–466.
  271. REFERENCES Babchuk, W. A., and M. Badiee (2010), “Realizing the poten- tial of qualitative methodologies: A conceptual guide for research and practice,” in Proceedings of the 28th Annual Midwest Research Research-to-Practice Conference in Adult, Continuing, and Community Education, J. Dirkx, ed., 25–31. Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 26–28 September 2010. Bernardin, T., P. Kemp-Robertson, D. W. Stewart, C. Yan, H. Wan, J. R. Rossiter, S. Erevelles, R. Roundtree, G. M. Zinkhan, and N. Fukawa (2008), “Envisioning the Future of Advertising Creativity Research,” Journal of Advertising, 37 (4), 131–49. Creswell, J. (2007), Qualitative Inquiry and Research Design: Choosing Among Five Approaches, 2nd ed., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Elkin, Noah (2010), “Mobile Marketing Trends, Insights, and Best Practices” (webinar), http://www.slideshare.net/ eMarketerInc/emarketer-webinar-mobile-marketing- trends-insights-best-practices#text-version Griffin, G., D. K., Morrison, and K. B. Sheehan (2009), “Per - spectives of Digital Creativity,” Journal of Interactive Adver- tising, 9 (2), 63–65. Hubspot.com (2011), “Social Media Marketing: 27 Stats, Soundbites, and Slides,” http://www.hubspot.com/ social-media-slides/?source=socialmedia-slides-ebook Li, H. (2011), “The Interactive Web,” Journal of Advertising Re- search, 51 (1), 13–34. Mallia, K. L., and K. Windels (2011), “Will Changing Media Change the World? An Exploratory Investigation of the Im- pact of Digital Advertising on Opportunities for Creative Women,” Journal of Interactive Advertising, 11 (2), 30–44. Martin, K., and I. Todorov (2010), “How Will Digital Platforms Be Harnessed in 2010 and How Will They Change the Way People Interact with Brands?,” Journal of Interactive Advertis- ing, 10 (2), 61–66. McLuhan, M. (1995), “The Medium Is the Message,” in Essen- tial McLuhan, Eric McLuhan and Frank Zingrone, eds., New York, NY: Basic Books, 151–61. (Reprinted from Marshall McLuhan, Understanding Media: The Extensions of Man, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964, 7–21) Moustakas, C. (1994), Phenomenological Research Methods, Thou- sand Oaks, CA: Sage. Muniz, A., and H. Schau (2007), “Vigilante Marketing and Consumer-Created Communications,” Journal of Advertis- ing, 36 (3), 187–202. Roberts, M., and H. Ko (2001), “Global Interactive Advertis- ing: Defining What We Mean and Using What We Have Learned,” Journal of Interactive Advertising, 1 (2), 18–27. Robinson, L., and D. Halle (2002), “Digitization, the Internet, and the Arts: eBay, Napster, SAG, and e-Books,” Qualitative Sociology, 25 (3), 359–83. Saldana, J. (2011), Fundamentals of Qualitative Research, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Sasser, S. L., S. Koslow, and E. A. Riordan (2007), “Creative and Interactive Media Use by Agencies: Engaging an IMC Media Palette for Implementing Advertising Campaigns,” Journal of Advertising Research, 47 (3), 237–56. Sharma, C., J. Herzog, and V. Melfi (2008), Mobile Advertis- ing: Supercharge Your Brand in the Exploding Wireless Market, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Sheehan, K. B., and D. K. Morrison (2009), “The Creativity Challenge: Media Confluence and Its Effects on the Evolv - ing Advertising Industry,” Journal of Interactive Advertising, 9 (2), 84–88. Smillie, Dirk (2010), “Digital Lift-Off,” Forbes.com, March 8, http://www.forbes.com/2010/03/07/advertising-web- ads-digital-business-media-outsell.html Stuhlfaut, M. W. (2011), “The Creative Code: An Organiza- tional Influence on the Creative Process in Advertising,” In- ternational Journal of Advertising, 30 (2), 283–304. Taylor, C.R. (2009), “The Six Principles of Digital Advertis- ing,” International Journal of Advertising, 28 (3), 411–18. Xue, D., and H. Li (2008), Creative Use of QR Codes in Con- sumer Communication,” International Journal of Mobile Mar- keting, 3 (2), 61–67. VanBoskirk, S. (2011), US Interactive Marketing Forecast, 2011 to 2016, Cambridge, MA: Forrester Research. Vega, T. (2011), “Advertising Companies Fret Over a Digital Talent Gap,” New York Times, October 30.
  272. References Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: Questions about mem- ory we forgot to ask. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 296–308. Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., & Logan, J. M. (2007). Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval? A critical review of the extant literature. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III (pp. 83–106). New York: Psychology Press. Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., & Paullin, R. (1989). Age-related differences in the impact of spacing, lag, and retention interval. Psychology and Aging, 4, 3–9. Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal interval spacing? Explorations of spacing effects in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 21, 19–31. Benjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Retrieval fluency as a metacog- nitive index. In L. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 309–338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68. Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier: An interpretation of negative recency and related phenomena. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Informa- tion processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bjork, R. A. (1988). Retrieval practice and the maintenance of knowledge. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1, pp. 396–401). New York: Wiley. Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bjork, R. A. (1999). Assessing our own competence: Heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII. Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 435–459). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bjork, R. A., & Allen, T. W. (1970). The spacing effect: Consolidation or differential encoding? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 567–572. Bower, G. H. (1972). Stimulus sampling theory of encoding variability. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 85–123). New York: Wiley. Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (in press). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulated classroom setting. European Journal of Cogni- tive Psychology. Camp, C. J. (2006). Spaced retrieval: A model for dissemination of a cognitive intervention for persons with dementia. In D. K. Attix & K. A. Welsh-Bohmer (Eds.), Geriatric neuropsychology: Assessment and in- tervention (pp. 275–292). New York: Guilford Press. Camp, C. J., Bird, M. J., & Cherry, K. E. (2000). Retrieval strategies as a rehabilitation aid for cognitive loss in pathological aging. In R. D. Hill, L. Backman, & A. S. Neely (Eds.), Cognitive rehabilitation in old age (pp. 224–248). New York: Oxford University Press. Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Application of the testing and spacing effects to name learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 619–636. Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: Support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory & Cognition, 34, 268–276. Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention. Memory & Cognition, 20, 633–642. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. Chan, J. C. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). Retrieval induced facilitation: Initially nontested material can benefit from prior testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 553–571. Craik, F. I. M. (1970). The fate of primary memory items in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 143–148. Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294. Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1973). The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 599– 607. Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cull, W. L. (2000). Untangling the benefits of multiple study opportunities and repeated testing for cued recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 215–235. Cull, W. L., Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1996). Expanding understanding of the expanding-pattern-of-retrieval mnemonic: Toward confidence in applicability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Ap- plied, 2, 365–378. Deutsch, J. A., & Terrace, H. S. (1967, May 19). Discrimination learning and inhibition. Science, 156, 988–989. Estes, W. K. (1955). Statistical theory of distributional phenomena in learning. Psychological Review, 62, 369–377. Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I. M., & Bleasdale, F. A. (1973). Retrieval difficulty and subsequent recall. Memory & Cognition, 1, 213–216. Glenberg, A. M. (1976). Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 1–16. Glover, J. A. (1989). The “testing” phenomenon: Not gone but nearly forgotten. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 392–399. Hochhalter, A. K., Overmier, J. B., Gasper, S. M., Bakke, B. L., & Holub, R. J. (2005). A comparison of spaced retrieval to other schedules of practice for people with dementia. Experimental Aging Research, 31, 101–118. Hogan, R. M., & Kintsch, W. (1971). Differential effects of study and test trials on long-term recognition and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 562–567. Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 649–667. Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (in press). Test format and corrective feedback modify the effect of testing on long-term retention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. Karpicke, J., & Hearst, E. (1975). Inhibitory control and errorless discrim- ination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 159–166. Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). Does expanding retrieval work with prose materials? Unpublished manuscript, Washington University in St. Louis. Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (in press). Repeated retrieval during learning is the key to long-term retention. Journal of Memory and Language. Kelley, C. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Remembering mistaken for knowing: Ease of retrieval as a basis for confidence in answers to general knowledge questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 1–24. Killeen, P. R. (2005). An alternative to null-hypothesis significance tests. Psychological Science, 16, 345–353. Koriat, A., & Ma’ayan, H. (2005). The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 478–492. Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). London: Academic Press. Logan, J. M., & Balota, D. A. (in press). Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: Exploration of schedule of spacing and reten- tion interval in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. Madigan, S. A. (1969). Intraserial repetition and coding processes in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 828–835. Madigan, S. A., & McCabe, L. (1971). Perfect recall and total forgetting: A problem for models of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learn- ing and Verbal Behavior, 10, 101–106. Martin, E. (1968). Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer: An encoding variability hypothesis. Psychological Review, 75, 421–441. Matvey, G., Dunlosky, J., & Guttentag, R. (2001). Fluency of retrieval at study affects judgments of learning (JOLs): An analytic or nonanalytic basis for JOLs? Memory & Cognition, 29, 222–232. McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (in press). Testing the testing effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2005). Material appropriate difficulty: A framework for determining when difficulty is desirable for improving learning. In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Experimental cognitive psychology and its applications (pp. 73–85). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McDaniel, M. A., & Masson, M. E. J. (1985). Altering memory represen- tations through retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- ing, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 371–385. Melton, A. W. (1970). The situation with respect to the spacing of repe- titions and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 596–606. Modigliani, V. (1976). Effects on a later recall by delaying initial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 609–622. Pashler, H., Zarow, G., & Triplett, B. (2003). Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1051–1057. Peterson, L. R., Wampler, R., Kirkpatrick, M., & Saltzman, D. (1963). Effect of spacing of presentations on retention of paired-associates over short intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 206–209. Rea, C. P., & Modigliani, V. (1985). The effect of expanded versus massed practice on the retention of multiplication facts and spelling lists. Human Learning, 4, 11–18. Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210. Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Sci- ence, 17, 249–255. Schacter, D. L., Rich, S. A., & Stampp, M. S. (1985). Remediation of memory disorders: Experimental evaluation of the spaced retrieval tech- nique. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 79– 96. Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for train- ing. Psychological Science, 3, 207–217. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools. Serra, M. J., & Dunlosky, J. (2005). Does retrieval fluency contribute to the underconfidence-with-practice effect? Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1258–1266. Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1992). Memory-monitoring accuracy as influenced by the distribution of retrieval practice. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 125–128. Spitzer, H. F. (1939). Studies in retention. Journal of Educational Psy- chology, 30, 641–656. Starch, D. (1927). Educational psychology. Oxford, England: Macmillan. Terrace, H. S. (1963). Discrimination learning with and without “errors.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 1–27. Terrace, H. S. (1966, December 30). Discrimination learning and inhibi- tion. Science, 154, 1677–1680. Thompson, C. P., Wenger, S. K., & Bartling, C. A. (1978). How recall facilitates subsequent recall: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 210–221. Wheeler, M. A., Ewers, M., & Buonanno, J. F. (2003). Different rates of forgetting following study versus test trials. Memory, 11, 571–580. Wheeler, M. A., & Roediger, H. L. (1992). Disparate effects of repeated testing: Reconciling Ballard’s (1913) and Bartlett’s (1932) results. Psy- chological Science, 3, 240–245. Whitten, W. B., & Bjork, R. A. (1977). Learning from tests: Effects of spacing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 465–478. Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A. D., Evans, J., & Sheil, A. (1994). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsycho- logical Rehabilitation, 4, 307–326.
  273. REFERENCES (2010) From The Editors. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:71. doi: 10.1038/nrn2802 Ashby, W. R. (1962). “Principles of the self-organizing system,” in Principles of Self- Organization: Transactions of the University of Illinois Symposium, eds H. Von Foerster and G. W. Zopf, Jr. (London, UK: Pergamon Press), 255–278. Barreto, E., So, P., Gluckman, B. J., and Schiff, S. J. (2000). From generalized synchrony to topological decoherence: emergent sets in coupled chaotic systems. Phys. Rev. Lett. 84, 1689–1692. doi: 10.1103/physrevlett.84.1689 Bhowmik, D., and Shanahan, M. (2013). Metastability and inter-band frequency modulation in networks of oscillating spiking neuron populations. PLoS One 8:e62234. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062234 Bressler, S. L., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2001). Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01564-3 Bressler, S. L., Richter, C. G., Chen, Y., and Ding, M. (2007). Cortical functional network organization from autoregressive modeling of local field potential oscillations. Stat. Med. 26, 3875–3885. doi: 10.1002/sim.2935 Bressler, S. L., and Seth, A. K. (2011). Wiener–granger causality: a well established methodology. Neuroimage 58, 323–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.059 Bressler, S. L., and Tognoli, E. (2006). Operational principles of neurocognitive networks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 60, 139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.008 Buzsáki, G. (2007). The structure of consciousness. Nature 446:267. doi: 10. 1038/446267a Catani, M., and ffytche, D. H. (2005). The rises and falls of disconnection syn- dromes. Brain 128, 2224–2239. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh622 Charcot, J. M. (1878). Leçons sur les Localisations dans les Maladies du Cerveau. Paris: Delahaye et Co. Chialvo, D. R. (2010). Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat. Phys. 6, 744–750. doi: 10.1038/nphys1803 Cruikshank, S. J., and Connors, B. W. (2008). State-sanctioned synchrony. Nature 454, 839–840. doi: 10.1038/454839a Eccles, J. C. (1966). The ionic mechanisms of excitatory and inhibitory synap- tic action. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 137, 473–494. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1966. tb50176.x Ecker, A. S., Berens, P., Keliris, G. A., Bethge, M., Logothetis, N. K., and Tolias, A. S. (2010). Decorrelated neuronal firing in cortical microcircuits. Science 327, 584–587. doi: 10.1126/science.1179867 Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Jordan, W., Brosch, M., Kruse, W., Munk, M., et al. (1988). Coherent oscillations: a mechanism of feature linking in the visual cortex? Multiple electrode and correlation analyses in the cat. Biol. Cybern. 60, 121–130. doi: 10.1007/bf00202899 Edelman, G. M. (1993). Neural Darwinism: selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function. Neuron 10, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90304-a Eigen, M., and Schuster, P. (1979). The Hypercycle: A Principle of Natural Self- organisation. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. Farmer, S. (2011). Review of dynamic coordination in the brain: from neurons to mind. J. Psychophysiol. 25, 201–202. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000055 Freeman, W. J. (1975). Mass Action in the Nervous System. New York: Academic Press. Freeman, W. J., and Holmes, M. D. (2005). Metastability, instability, and state transition in neocortex. Neural Netw. 18, 497–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2005. 06.014 Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics. 2005.08.011 Friston, K. J. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107 Friston, K. J. (1997). Transients, metastability and neuronal dynamics. Neuroimage 5, 164–171. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0259 Gallos, L. K., Sigman, M., and Makse, H. A. (2012). The conundrum of functional brain networks: small-world efficiency or fractal modularity. Front. Physiol. 3:123. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00123 Gray, C. M., and Singer, W. (1989). Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 86, 1698– 1702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.5.1698 Grossberg, S. (1976). Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: I. Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors. Biol. Cybern. 23, 121–134. doi: 10.1007/bf00344744 Haken, H. (1983). Synergetics, an Introduction: Nonequilibrium Phase Transitions and Self-Organization in Physics, Chemistry and Biology. New York: Springer- Verlag. Hammond, C., Bergman, H., and Brown, P. (2007). Pathological synchronization in Parkinson’s disease: networks, models and treatments. Trends Neurosci. 30, 357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.004 Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New-York: John Wiley. Hodgkin, A. L., and Huxley, A. F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J. Physiol. 117, 500–544. Honey, C. J., Kötter, R., Breakspear, M., and Sporns, O. (2007). Network struc- ture of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.07015 19104 Honey, C. J., Thivierge, J. P., and Sporns, O. (2010). Can structure predict function in the human brain? Neuroimage 52, 766–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010. 01.071 Hopfield, J. J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collec- tive computational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 79, 2554–2558. doi: 10. 1073/pnas.79.8.2554 Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. Kelso, J. A. S. (2008). An essay on understanding the mind: the A.S. Iberall Lecture. Ecol. Psychol. 20, 180–208. doi: 10.1080/10407410801949297 Kelso, J. A. S. (2012). Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 906–918. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351 Kelso, J. A. S., and Engstrøm, D. A. (2006). The Complementary Nature. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Kelso, J. A. S., Dumas, G., and Tognoli, E. (2013). Outline of a general theory of behavior and brain coordination. Neural Netw. 37, 120–131. doi: 10.1016/j. neunet.2012.09.003 Kelso, J. A. S., and Haken, H. (1995). “New laws to be expected in the organism: synergetics of brain and behavior,” in What is Life? The Next Fifty Years: Speculations on the Future of Biology, eds M. P. Murphy and L. A. J. O’Neill (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press), 137–160. Kelso, J. A. S., and Tognoli, E. (2007). “Toward a complementary neuroscience: metastable coordination dynamics of the brain,” in Neurodynamics of Higher- level Cognition and Consciousness, eds R. Kozma and L. Perlovsky (Heidelberg: Springer), 39–59. Kelso, J. A. S., and Tuller, B. (1981). Toward a theory of apractic syndromes. Brain Lang. 12, 224–245. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(81)90016-x Kostrubiec, V., Zanone, P.-G., Fuchs, A., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2012). Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner —experimental evidence and theoretical model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:222. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00222 Lehnertz, K., Bialonski, S., Horstmann, M. T., Krug, D., Rothkegel, A., Staniek, M., et al. (2009). Synchronization phenomena in human epileptic brain networks. J. Neurosci. Methods 183, 42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009. 05.015 McLeod, P., Plunkett, K., and Rolls, E. T. (1998). Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes. Oxford, UK, New-York, NY: Oxford University Press. Meunier, D., Lambiotte, R., and Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Modular and hierarchi- cally modular organization of brain networks. Front. Neurosci. 4:200. doi: 10. 3389/fnins.2010.00200 Poulet, J. F. A., and Petersen, C. C. H. (2008). Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature 454, 881–885. doi: 10.1038/nature07150 Rabinovich, M. I., Huerta, R., Varona, P., and Afraimovich, V. S. (2008). Transient cognitive dynamics, metastability and decision making. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000072 Renart, A., de la Rocha, J., Bartho, P., Hollender, L., Parga, N., Reyes, A., et al. (2010). The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science 327, 587–590. doi: 10. 1126/science.1179850 Robert, A. (1999). Lamination and Within-Area Integration in the Neocortex. San Diego: University of California, Doctoral dissertation. Rulkov, N. F., Sushchik, M. M., Tsimring, L. S., and Abarbanel, H. D. (1995). Generalized synchronization of chaos in directionally coupled chaotic systems. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Phys. Plasmas Fluids Relat. Interdiscip. Topics 51, 980–994. doi: 10.1103/physreve.51.980 Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., and The PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Volume 1: Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27, 623–656. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb00917.x Sperry, R. W. (1969). A modified concept of consciousness. Psychol. Rev. 76, 532– 536. doi: 10.1037/h0028156 Sporns, O., Chialvo, D. R., Kaiser, M., and Hilgetag, C. C. (2004). Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 418– 425. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008 Sporns, O., Tononi, G., and Edelman, G. M. (2000). Theoretical neuroanatomy: relating anatomical and functional connectivity in graphs and cortical connec- tion matrices. Cereb. Cortex 10, 127–141. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.2.127 Tognoli, E., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2009). Brain coordination dynamics: true and false faces of phase synchrony and metastability. Prog. Neurobiol. 87, 31–40. doi: 10. 1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.014 Tognoli, E., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2013). “On the brain’s dynamical complexity: coupling and causal influences across spatiotemporal scales,” in Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (III), ed Y. Yamaguchi (Netherlands: Springer), 259–265. Tognoli, E., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron 81, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022 Tononi, G., Sporns, O., and Edelman, G. M. (1994). A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 91, 5033–5037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91. 11.5033 Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2006). Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52, 155–168. doi: 10. 1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020 Varela, F., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229– 239. doi: 10.1038/35067550 von der Malsburg, C., Phillips, W. A., and Singer, W. (Eds). (2010). Dynamic Coordination in the Brain: From Neurons to Mind, Strüngmann Forum Report, Vol. 5. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wade, J. J., McDaid, L. J., Harkin, J., Crunelli, V., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2013). Biophysically-based computational models of astrocyte ∼ neuron coupling and their functional significance. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:44. doi: 10.3389/fncom. 2013.00044 Werner, G. (2007). Metastability, criticality and phase transitions in brain and its models. Biosystems 90, 496–508. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2006.12.001 Yates, E. (2008). Homeokinetics/Homeodynamics: a physical heuristic for life and complexity. Ecol. Psychol. 20, 148–179. doi: 10.1080/10407410801977546 Zilles, K., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Grefkes, C., Scheperjans, F., Boy, C., Amunts, K., et al. (2002). Architectonics of the human cerebral cortex and transmitter recep- tor fingerprints: reconciling functional neuroanatomy and neurochemistry. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 587–599. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(02)00108-6
  274. References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis. Anderson, R. J. (1994). Representations and requirements: The value of ethnography in system design. Human–Computer Interaction, 9, 151– 182. Ansburg, P. I., & Hill, K. (2003). Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differ- ences, 34, 1141–1152. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. J. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bul- letin, 133, 1–24. Beck, J. (1979). The effect of variations in incubation activity and incubation time on creative response production. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University. Bennett, S. (1975). Effect of incubation on the associative process of creativity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Notre Dame. Both, L., Needham, D., & Wood, E. (2004). Examining tasks that facil- itate the experience of incubation while problem solving. Alberta Jour- nal of Educational Research, 50(1), 57–67. Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003a). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 730–737. Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003b). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(5), 634–639. Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C. G., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72–110. Brockett, C. (1985). Neuropsychological and cognitive components of creativity and incubation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Virginia Commonwealth University. Browne, B. A., & Cruse, D. F. (1988). The incubation effect: Illusion or illumination. Human Performance, 1(3), 177–185. Christensen, B. T., & Schunn, C. D. (2005). Spontaneous access and analogical incubation effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17(2–3), 207–220. Chronicle, E. P., Ormerod, T. C., & MacGregor, J. N. (2001). When insight just won’t come: The failure of visual cues in the nine-dot problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(A), 903–919. Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (1994). The handbook of research synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environ- mental clues during incubation. Creativity Research Journal, 14(3–4), 287–305. Dodds, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (in press). Incubation in problem solving and creativity. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity re- search handbook (Vol. 3). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Dominowski, R. L., & Jenrick, R. (1972). Effect of hints and interpolated activity on solution of an insight problem. Psychonomic Science, 26(6), 335–338. Dorfman, J. (1990). Metacognitions and incubation effects in insight problem solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cali- fornia, San Diego. Dreistadt, R. (1969). The use of analogies and incubation in obtaining insights in creative problem solving. The Journal of Psychology, 71, 159–175. Durlak, J. A., & Lipsey, M. W. (1991). A practitioner’s guide to meta- analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 291–332. Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. P. (1968). Short-term incubation in divergent production. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 241–246. Fulgosi, A., & Guilford, J. (1972). A further investigation of short-term incubation. Acta Instituti Psychologici, 64–73, 67–70. Gall, M., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1967). Effects of facilitating techniques and subject–experimenter interaction on creative problem solving. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 211–216. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., & DeSoto, M. C. (2004). Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathemat- ical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 121–151. Ghiselin, B. (1985). The creative process: A symposium. Berkeley: Uni- versity of California Press. Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non- insight problems. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 279–302. Goldman, W., Wolters, N., & Winograd, E. (1992). A demonstration of incubation in anagram problem solving. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 36–38. Hansberry, M. T. (1998). Fixation and incubation effects in problem solving. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hampshire, United Kingdom. Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Henley, R. J. (1999). Priming incubated problems. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Sussex, United Kingdom. Houtz, J. C., & Frankel, A. D. (1992). Effects of incubation and imagery training on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 5(2), 183–189. Jamieson, B. A. (1999). Incubation and aging: The nature of processing underlying insight. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia. Jung-Beeman, M., & Bowden, E. (2000). The right hemisphere maintains solution-related activation for yet-to-be solved insight problems. Mem- ory and Cognition, 28, 1231–1241. Kaplan, C. (1990). Hatching a theory of incubation: Does putting a problem aside really help? If so, why? Unpublished doctoral disserta- tion, Carnegie Mellon University. Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 3–13. Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1534–1555. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lynch, M. D., & Swink, E. (1967). Some effects of priming, incubation and creative aptitude on journalism performance. Journal of Communi- cation, 17(4), 372–382. Macaskill, P., Walter, S. D., & Irwing, L. (2001). A comparison of methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 20, 641–654. MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (2001). Information- processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 176–201. Martindale, C. (1995). Creativity and connectionism. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 249–268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Medd, E., & Houtz, J. C. (2002). The effects of facilitated incubation on fourth graders’ creative writing. Educational Research Quarterly, 26(2), 13–16. Mednick, M., Mednick, S., & Mednick, E. (1964). Incubation of creative performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 84–88. Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232. Metcalfe, J., & Weibe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and non-insight problem-solving. Memory and Cognition, 15, 238–246. Moss, S. A. (2002). The impact of environmental clues in problem solving and incubation: The moderating effect of ability. Creativity Research Journal, 14(2), 207–211. Murray, H. G., & Denny, J. P. (1969). Interaction of ability level and interpolated activity (opportunity for incubation) in human problem solving. Psychological Reports, 24(1), 271–276. Newton, E. J., & Roberts, M. J. (2005). The window of opportunity: A model for strategy discovery. In M. J. Roberts & E. J. Newton (Eds.), Methods of thought: Individual differences in reasoning strategies (pp. 129–158). Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press. Olton, R. M. (1979). Experimental studies of incubation: Searching for the elusive. Journal of Creative Behavior, 13, 9–22. Olton, R. M., & Johnson, D. M. (1976). Mechanism of incubation in creative problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 89(4), 617– 630. Patalano, A. L., & Seifert, C. M. (1994). Memory for impasses during problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 22, 234–242. Patrick, A. S. (1986). The role of ability in creative “incubation.” Per- sonality and Individual Differences, 7(2), 169–174. Penney, C. G., Godsell, A., Scott, A., & Balsom, R. (2004). Problem variables that promote incubation effects. Journal of Creative Behavior, 38, 35–55. Perkins, D. N. (1995). Insight in minds and genes. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 495–533). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Peterson, C. (1974). Incubation effects in anagram solution. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 3, 29–30. Posner, M. I. (1973). Cognition: An introduction. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Rae, C. M. (1997). The creative power of doing nothing. Writer, 110(7), 13–15. Reder, L. M., & Schunn, C. D. (1996). Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and mem- ory. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 45–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rickards, T. (1991). Innovation and creativity: Woods, trees and pathways. R&D Management, 21(2), 97–108. Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bul- letin, 118, 183–192. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cam- bridge University Press. Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic memory revisited. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. Scheerer, M. (1963). Problem solving. Scientific American, 208(4), 118– 128. Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 166–183. Seabrook, R., & Dienes, Z. (2003, July). Incubation in problem solving as a context effect. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1065–1069). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Available from http:// www.cogsci.rpi.edu/CSJarchive/Proceedings/2003/pdfs/203.pdf Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity Re- search Journal, 16(1), 141–149. Seifert, M. C., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65–124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: A microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy: General, 127, 377–397. Silveira, J. (1972). Incubation: The effect of interruption timing and length on problem solution and quality of problem processing. Unpub- lished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon. Simon, H. A. (1966). Scientific discovery and the psychology of problem solving. In R. G. Colodny (Ed.), Mind and cosmos: Essays in contem- porary science and philosophy (pp. 22–40). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press. Sio, U. N., & Rudowicz, E. (2007). The role of an incubation period in creative problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 307– 318. Smilek, D., Enns, J. T., Eastwood, J. D., & Merikle, P. A. (2006). Relax! Cognitive style influences visual search. Visual Cognition, 14, 543–564. Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and creative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 135–146). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27(4), 311–314. Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 61–87. Snyder, A., Mitchell, D. J., Ellwood, S., Yates, A., & Pallier, G. (2004). Nonconscious idea generation. Psychological Reports, 94, 1325–1330. Thornton, A., & Lee, P. (2000). Publication bias in meta-analysis—Its causes and consequences. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53(2), 207–216. Torrance-Perks, J. (1997). The incubation effect: Implication for under- lying mechanisms. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wa- terloo, Ontario, Canada. VanLehn, K. (1988). Towards a theory of impasse-driven learning. In H. Mandl & A. Lesgold (Eds.), Learning: Issues for intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 19–41). New York: Springer-Verlag. Vul, E., & Pashler, H. (2007). Incubation is helpful only when people are misled. Memory and Cognition, 35(4), 701–710. Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. London: Cape. Webster, A., Campbell, C., & Jane, B. (2006). Enhancing the creative process for learning in primary technology education. International Journal of Technology & Design Education, 16(3), 221–235. Wertheimer, M. (1985). A Gestalt perspective on computer simulations of cognitive processes. Computers in Human Behavior, 1, 19–33. Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology. New Delhi, India: Oxford & IBH. Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and meta-cognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem- ory, and Cognition, 13(2), 187–205. Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Untersuchungen zur Handlungs- und Affektpsy- chologie: III. Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter andlungen [In- vestigations on the psychology of action and affection: III. The memory of completed and uncompleted actions]. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85. Zeigarnik, B. (1938). On finished and unfinished tasks. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A sourcebook of Gestalt psychology (pp. 300–314). New York: Humanities Press.
  275. References Ask.com. (n.d.). What Is Traditional Marketing?. What Is Traditional Marketing. Retrieved March 21, 2014, from http://www.ask.com/question/what-is-traditional-marketing Basanyat, D. I. (n.d.). Theories of Public Relations. Theories of Public Relations. Retrieved March 19, 2014, from http://linda.perry.net/nm2219/theories8.htm Brafton introduces strategic video content marketing. (2012, Dec 13). PR Newswire. http://ezproxy.lib.calpoly.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1237496776 ?accountid=10362 Cole, J.T., & Greer, J.D. (2013). Audience Response to Brand Journalism: The Effect of Frame, Source, and Involvement. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 90(4), 673-690. Doi: 10.1177/1077699013503160 Content Marketing vs Traditional Marketing | Newzsocial. (2013, July 6).Newzsocial. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.newzsocial.com/content-marketing-vs-traditional- marketing/ Content Marketing vs. Traditional Advertising [Infographic]. (n.d.).Marketo.com. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.marketo.com/infographics/content-marketing-vs- traditional-advertising/ Greer, A. (n.d.). Becoming a Brand Journalist by Finding Stories from Within Your SMB. Scoopit Blog. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://blog.scoop.it/2013/10/31/becoming-a-brand-journalist-by-finding-stories-from- within-your-smb/ Handley, A. (2012, March 8). Real-World Education for Modern Marketers.MarketingProfs. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.marketingprofs.com/opinions/2012/23504/seven-reasons-your-content- marketing-needs-a-brand-journalist Hosford, C. (2013). BtoB study: Content marketing goes mainstream. B to B, 98(6), 3. LaChance, E. (2013, September 1). A Blueprint for the Video Content Marketing Revolution. RSS. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://socialmediatoday.com/erikalachance/1707916/blueprint-video-content-marketing- revolution Marketing Institute. (n.d.). What is Content Marketing?. Content Marketing Institute. Retrieved March 21, 2014, from http://contentmarketinginstitute.com/what-is-content-marketing/ New research shows increasing reliance on content marketing to drive brand awareness, inbound traffic and sales. (2013, Oct 01).Business Wire. http://ezproxy.lib.calpoly.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1437739586 ?accountid=10362 Park, J. (n.d.). Tap into the Thriving Video Content Scene: 5 Types of Brand Videos. Content Marketing Institute. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from Pergolino, M., Miller, J., Rothman, D., & Miller, J. (n.d.). The Definitive Guide to Social Marketing. Marketo.com. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.marketo.com/_assets/uploads/Definitive-Guide-to-Social-Marketing.pdf Pulizzi, J. (2012, February 16). The Future of Content Marketing – 6 Differences Between Good and Great [Part 2].Content Marketing Institute. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://contentmarketinginstitute.com/2012/02/future-content-marketing-good-great- differences/ Pulizzi, J. (2012). The Rise of Storytelling as the New Marketing. Publishing Research Quarterly. 28(2). 116-123. doi 10.1007/s12109-012-9264-5 Pulizzi, J. (2012). The Transformation of Content Marketing. Econtent, 35(10), 20-21. Reid, C. (2013, February 18). Content Strategy: A Guide to Getting Started.EContent Magazine. Retrieved March 21, 2014, from http://www.econtentmag.com/Articles/Editorial/Feature/Content-Strategy-A-Guide-to- Getting-Started-87086.htm Royse, M. (2012, November 28). The A to Z guide to content marketing. PR Daily News: Public Relations news and marketing in the age of social media. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://prdaily.com/Main/Articles/13239.aspx# Qu, A. (2013, April 3). Custom Content Council. At Nearly $44 Billion, New Survey Shows Rise in Content Marketing Budget. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.customcontentcouncil.com/news/nearly-44-billion-new-survey-shows-rise- content-marketing-budget Sluis, S. (2014). Social Media Lead Content Marketing. CRM Magazine. 18(1), 7 Video features insurance marketing & advertising summit speaker laura packard on content marketing. (2013, Sep 13).Business Wire. http://ezproxy.lib.calpoly.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1432194867 ?accountid=10362 Why brand journalism is the new PR. (2013, December 9). Ragan.com. Retrieved March 20, 2014, from http://www.ragan.com/Main/Articles/Why_brand_journalism_is_the_new_pr47733.aspx
  276. References 1. Spelke, E.: The Baby Lab by Margaret Talbot. New Yorker, Sept. 4 (2006) 2. Pauli, W.: Writings on Physics and Philosophy. In: Enz, C.P., von Meyenn, K. (Eds.) Springer-Verlag, Berlin (1994) 3. Finger, S.: Origins of Neuroscience. Oxford, New York (1994) 4. Ji, S.: Complementarism: A Biology-Based Philosophical Framework to Integrate Western Science and Eastern Tao. Proc. of 16 th International Congress of Psychotherapy (1995) 518-548 5. Grossberg, S.: The Complementary Brain: a Unifying View of Brain Specialization and Modularity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (2000) 233-246 6. Frayn, M.: The Human Touch: Our Part in the Creation of the Universe. Faber & Faber (2006) 7. Kelso, J.A.S., Engstrøm, D.: The Complementary Nature. MIT Press, Cambridge (2006) 8. Bressler, S.L., Kelso, J.A.S.: Cortical Coordination Dynamics and Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (2001) 26-36 9. Jirsa, V.K., Kelso, J.A.S. (Eds.) Coordination Dynamics: Issues and Trends. Springer, Berlin (2004) 10. Kelso, J.A.S.: Behavioral and Neural Pattern Generation: the Concept of Neurobehavioral Dynamical System (NBDS) In: Koepchen, H.P., Huopaniemi, T. (Eds.) Cardiorespiratory and Motor Coordination, Springer-Verlag, Berlin (1991) 11. Kelso, J.A.S.: Coordination Dynamics of Human Brain and Behavior. Springer Proc. in Physics 69 (1992) 223-234 12. Kelso, J.A.S.: Dynamic Patterns: the Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (1995) 13. Tschacher, W., Dauwalder, J.P.: The Dynamical Systems Approach to Cognition: Concepts and Empirical Paradigms Based on Self-Organization, Embodiment and Coordination Dynamics. World Scientific, Singapore (2003) 14. Velazquez, J.L.: Brain, Behaviour and Mathematics: Are we Using the Right Approaches? Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena 212(3-4) (2005) 161-182 15. Schöner, G., Kelso, J.A.S.: Dynamic Pattern Generation in Behavioral and Neural Systems. Science 239 (1988) 1513-1520 16. Başar, E.: Memory and Brain Dynamics: Oscillations Integrating Attention, Perception, Learning, and Memory. Conceptual Advances in Brain Research 7. CRC Press, Boca Raton (2004) 17. Buzsáki, G.: Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2006) 18. Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Jordan, W., Borsch, M., Kruse, W., Munk M., Reitboeck, HJ.: Coherent Oscillations : a Mechanism of Feature Linking in the Visual Cortex. Multiple Electrode Correlation Analyses in the Cat. Biological Cybernetics 60(2) (1988) 121-130 19. Gray, C.M., König, P., Engel, A.K., Singer, W.: Oscillatory Responses in Cat Visual Cortex Exhibit Inter-Columnar Synchronization which Reflects Global Stimulus Properties. Nature 338 (6213) (1989) 334-337 20. Munk, M.H.J., Roelfsema, P.R., König, P., Engel, A.K., Singer, W.: Role of Reticular Activation in the Modulation of Intracortical Synchronization. Science 272 (1996) 271-274 21. Bressler, S.L.: Interareal Synchronization in the Visual Cortex. Behavioral and Brain Research 76 (1996) 37-49 22. Steinmetz, P.N., Roy, A., Fitzgerald, P., Hsiao, S.S., Niebur, E., Johnson, K.O.: Attention Modulates Synchronized Neuronal Firing in Primate Somatosensory Cortex. Nature 404 (2000) 187-190 23. Mima, T., Matsuoka, T., Hallett, M.: Functional Coupling of Human Right and Left Cortical Motor Areas Demonstrated with Partial Coherence Analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 287 (2000) 93-96 24. Fries, P., Reynolds, J.H., Rorie, A.E., Desimone, R.: Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention. Science 291 (2001) 1560- 1563 25. Varela, F.J., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E. Martinerie, J.: The Brainweb: Phase Synchronization and Large-Scale Integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2 (2001) 229-239 26. Brown, P., Marsden, J.F.: Cortical Network Resonance and Motor Activity in Humans. Neuroscientist 7 (2001) 518-527 27. König, P., Engel, A.K., Roelfsema, P.R., Singer, W.: Coincidence Detection or Temporal Integration. The Role of the Cortical Neuron Revisited. Trends Neurosci 19 (1996) 130-137 28. Kelso, J.A.S.: The Informational Character of Self-Organized Coordination Dynamics. Human Movement Science 13 (1994) 393-413 29. Fingelkurts, An.A., Fingelkurts, Al.A.: Making Complexity Simpler: Multivariability and Metastability in the Brain. International Journal of Neuroscience 114(7) (2004) 843-862 30. Bressler, S.L. Tognoli, E.: Operational Principles of Neurocognitive Networks. International Journal of Psychophysiology 60 (2006) 139-148 31. Edelman, G.M.: Naturalizing Consciousness: a Theoretical Framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 100(9) (2004) 520-524 32. Edelman, G.: Second Nature: Brain Science and Human Knowledge. Yale University Press (2006) 33. Edelman, G., Tononi, G.: A Universe of Consciousness, New York, Basic Books (2000) 34. Freeman, W.J., Holmes, M.D.: Metastability, Instability, and State Transition in Neocortex. Neural Networks 18(5-6) (2005) 497-504 35. Friston, K.J.: Transients, Metastability, and Neuronal Dynamics. Neuroimage 5 (1997) 164-171 36. Koch, C.: Thinking about the Conscious Mind. A Review of Mind - An Introduction by John Searle. Science 306 (2004) 979-980 37. Sporns, O.: Complex Neural Dynamics. In: Jirsa, V.K., Kelso, J.A.S., (Eds.) Coordination Dynamics: Issues and trends. Springer-Verlag, Berlin (2004) 197-215 38. Haken, H.: Principles of Brain Functioning. Springer, Berlin (1996) 39. Kelso, J.A.S., Bressler, S.L., Buchanan, S., DeGuzman, G.C., Ding, M., Fuchs, A. Holroyd, T.: A Phase Transition in Human Brain and Behavior. Physics Letters A 169 (1992) 134-144 40. Kelso, J.A.S., DelColle, J., Schöner, G.: Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process. In: Jeannerod, M. (Ed.) Attention and Performance XIII. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1990) 139-169 41. Haken, H., Kelso, J.A.S., Bunz, H.: A Theoretical Model of Phase Transitions in Human Hand Movements. Biological Cybernetics 51 (1985) 347-356 42. Jirsa, V.K., Fuchs, A., Kelso, J.A.S.: Connecting Cortical and Behavioral Dynamics: Bimanual Coordination. Neural Computation 10 (1998) 2019-2045 43. Perez Velazquez, J.L., Wennberg, R.: Metastability of Brain States and the many Routes to Seizures: numerous Causes, same Result. In: Recent Research Developments in Biophysics, 3: Transworld Research Network (2004) 25-59 44. Werner, G.: Metastability, Criticality and Phase Transitions in Brain and its Models. Biosystems (in press) 45. Bressler, S.L.: Interareal Synchronization in the Visual Cortex. Behavioural Brain Research 76 (1996) 37-49 46. Ebbesson, S.O.E.: Evolution and Ontogeny of Neural Circuits. Behavioral and Brain Science 7 (1984) 321-366 47. Deacon, T.W.: Rethinking Mammalian Brain Evolution. American Zoologist 30 (1990) 629-705 48. Jacobs, R.A., Jordan, M.I.: Computational Consequences of a Bias towards Short connections. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4 (1992) 323-336 49. Chklovskii, D.B., Schikorski, T., Stevens, C.F.: Wiring optimization in cortical Circuits. Neuron 34(3) (2002) 341-347 50. Andreasen, N.C., Nopoulos, P., O'Leary, D.S., Miller, D.D., Wassink, T., Flaum, L.: Defining the Phenotype of Schizophrenia: Cognitive Dysmetria and its Neural Mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry 46 (1999) 908-920 51. Tononi, G., Edelman, G.M.: Schizophrenia and the Mechanisms of Conscious Integration. Brain Research Reviews 31 (2000) 391-400 52. Brock, J., Brown, C.C., Boucher, J., Rippon, G.: The Temporal Binding Deficit Hypothesis of Autism. Development and Psychopathology 14 (2002) 209-224 53. Niznikiewicz, M.A., Kubicki, M., Shenton, M.E.: Recent Structural and Functional Imaging Findings in Schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 16 (2003) 123-147 54. Welsh, J.P., Ahn, E.S., Placantonakis, D.G.: Is Autism Due to Brain Desynchronization? Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23 (2005) 253-263 55. Liang, M., Zhou, Y., Jiang, T., Liu, Z., Tian, L., Liu, H.; Hao, Y.: Widespread Functional Disconnectivity in Schizophrenia with Resting-State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Neuroreport 17(2) (2006) 209-213 56. Berger, H.: Ueber das Elektroenkephalogramm des Menschen. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 87 (1929) 527-570 57. Chatrian, G.E., Petersen, M.C., Lazarte, J.A.: The Blocking of the Central Wicket Rhythm and some Central Changes Related to Movement. Electroencephalogr Clinical Neurophysiology 11 (1959) 497-510 58. Chase, M.H., Harper, R.M.: Somatomotor and Visceromotor Correlates of Operantly Conditioned 12-14 Cycles Per Second Sensorimotor Cortical Activity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 31 (1971) 85-92 59. Kuhlman, W.N.: Functional Topography of the Human Mu Rhythm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 44 (1978) 83-93 60. Hughes, S.W., Crunelli, V.: Thalamic Mechanisms of EEG Alpha Rhythms and their Pathological Implications. The Neuroscientist 11 (2005) 357-372 61. Schiff, N.D., Plum, F.: The Role of Arousal and "Gating" Systems in the Neurology of Impaired Consciousness. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 17 (2000) 438-452 62. Glass, L.: Synchronization and Rhythmic Processes in Physiology. Nature 410 (2001) 277-284 63. Kostopoulos, G.K.: Involvement of the Thalamocortical System in Epileptic Loss of Consciousness. Epilepsia 42(30) (2001) 13-19 64. Blumenfeld, H., Taylor, J.: Why Do Seizures Cause Loss of Consciousness? The Neuroscientist 9(5) (2003) 301-310 65. Dominguez, L.G., Wennberg, R., Gaetz, W., Cheyne, D., Snead III, O.C., Perez Velazquez, J.L.: Enhanced Synchrony in Epileptiform Activity? Local Versus Distant Synchronization in Generalized Seizures. Journal of Neuroscience 25(35) (2005) 8077-8084 66. Atlan, H.: Entre le Cristal et la Fumée. Paris, Seuil (1979) 67. Chialvo, D.R.: Critical Brain Networks. Physica A 340(4) (2004) 756-765 68. Tononi, G., Sporns, O., Edelman, G.M.: A Measure for Brain Complexity: Relating Functional Segregation and Integration in the Nervous System. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 91 (1994) 5033-5037 69. Tononi, G., Sporns, O., Edelman, G.M.: Complexity and Coherency: Integrating Information in the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (1998) 474-484 70. Almonte, F., Jirsa, V.K., Large, E.W., Tuller, B.: Integration and Segregation in Auditory Streaming. Physica D 212 (2005) 137-159 71. Başar-Eroglu, C., Struber, D., Kruse, P., Başar, E., Stadler M.: Frontal Gamma-Band Enhancement during Multistable Visual Perception. International Journal of Psychophysiology 24 (1996) 113-125 72. Hock, H.S., Kelso, J.A.S., Schöner, G.: Bistability and Hysteresis in the Organization of Apparent Motion Patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Performance 19 (1993) 63-80 73. Keil, A., Muller, M.M., Ray, W.J., Gruber, T., Elbert, T.: Human Gamma Band Activity and Perception of a Gestalt. Journal of Neuroscience 19 (1999) 7152-7161 74. Kelso, J.A.S.: On the Oscillatory Basis of Movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 18 (1981) 63 75. Kelso, J.A.S.: Phase Transitions and Critical Behavior in Human Bimanual Coordination. American Journal of Physiology 246(6 Pt 2) (1984) R1000-1004 76. Kelso, J.A.S., DeGuzman, G.C. Holroyd, T.: Synergetic Dynamics of Biological Coordination with Special Reference to Phase Attraction and Intermittency. In: Haken, H., Koepchen, H.P. (Eds.) Rhythms in Physiological Systems. Springer Series in Synergetics, Vol. 55. Springer, Berlin (1991), 195-213 77. Tuller, B., Case, P., Ding, M., Kelso, J.A.S.: The Nonlinear Dynamics of Speech Categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20 (1994) 1-14 78. Kelso, J.A.S., Case, P., Holroyd, T., Horvath, E., Raczaszek, J., Tuller, B., Ding, M.: Multistability and Metastability in Perceptual and Brain Dynamics. In: Kruse, P., Stadler, M. (Eds.) Ambiguity in Mind and Nature. Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg (1995) 159-185 79. Thorpe, S., Fize, D., Marlot, C.: Speed of Processing in the Human Visual System. Nature 381 (1996) 520-522 80. Jensen, A.R.: Why Is Reaction Time Correlated with Psychometric g? Current Directions in Psychological Science 2 (1993) 53-56 81. Ditzinger, T., Haken, H.: Oscillations in the Perception of Ambiguous Patterns. Biological Cybernetics 61 (1989) 279-287 82. Ditzinger, T., Haken, H.: The Impact of Fluctuations on the Recognition of Ambiguous Patterns. Biological Cybernetics 63 (1990) 453-456 83. Chen, Y., Ding, M., Kelso, J.A.S.: Task-Related Power and Coherence Changes in Neuromagnetic Activity during Visuomotor Coordination. Experimental Brain Research 148 (2003) 105-116 84. Chen, Y., Ding, M., Kelso, J.A.S.: Long Range Dependence in Human Sensorimotor Coordination. In: Rangarajan, G., Ding, M. (Eds.) Processes with Long-Range Correlations. Springer, Berlin (2003) 309-323 85. Freeman, W.J.: Neurodynamics: an Exploration in Mesoscopic Brain Dynamics. Springer, Berlin (2000) 86. Wright, J.J., Robinson, P.A., Rennie, C.J., Gordon, E., Bourke, P.D., Chapman, C.L., Hawthorn, N., Lees, G.J., Alexander, D.: Toward an Integrated Continuum Model of Cerebral Dynamics: the Cerebral Rhythms, Synchronous Oscillation and Cortical Stability. Journal of Biological and Information Processing Systems 63(1-3) (2001) 71-88 87. Buzsáki, G., Draguhn, A.: Neuronal Oscillations in Cortical Networks. Science 304(5679) (2004) 1926-1929 88. Braitenberg, V., Schuz, A.: Anatomy of the Cortex. Springer-Verlag (1991) 89. Douglas, R.J., Martin, K.A.: A Functional Microcircuit for Cat Visual Cortex. Journal of Physiology 440 (1991) 735-769 90. Buxhoeveden, D.P., Casanova, M.F.: The Minicolumnar Hypothesis in Neurosciences. Brain 125(5) (2002) 935-951 91. Bressler, S.L.: Large-Scale Cortical Networks and Cognition. Brain Research Reviews 20 (1995) 288-304 92. Sporns, O., Kötter, R.: Motifs in Brain Networks. PLoS Biology 2 (2004) 1910-1918 93. Freeman, W.J.: Making Sense of Brain Waves: The Most Baffling Frontier in Neuroscience. In: Parelus, P. Principe, J. Rajasekaran, S. (Eds) Biocomputing. Kluwer, New York (2001) 33-55 94. Jirsa, V.K., Kelso, J.A.S.: Spatiotemporal Pattern Formation in Continuous Systems with Heterogeneous Connection Topologies. Physical Review E 62(6) (2000) 8462- 8465 95. Lehmann, D., Strik, W.K., Henggeler, B., Koenig, T., Koukkou, M.: Brain Electric Microstates and Momentary Conscious Mind States as Building Blocks of Spontaneous Thinking: I. Visual Imagery and Abstract Thoughts. International Journal of Psychophysiology 29(1) (1998) 1-11 96. Kaplan, A.Ya., Shishkin, S.L.: Nonparametric Method for the Segmentation of the EEG. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomed. 60(2) (1999) 93-106 97. Kelso, J.A.S.: The Complementary Nature of Coordination Dynamics: Self- Organization and the Origins of Agency. Journal of Nonlinear Phenomena in Complex Systems 5 (2002) 364-371 98. Pritchard, W.S.: The Brain in Fractal Time: 1/f-like Power Spectrum Scaling of the Human Electroencephalogram. Int. J. Neurosci. 66(1-2) (1992) 119-129 99. Engel, AK., König, P., Singer, W.: Direct Physiological Evidence for Scene Segmentation by Temporal Coding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 88 (1991) 9136-9140 100. Castelo-Branco, M., Goebel, R., Neuenschwander, S.: Neural Synchrony Correlates with Surface Segregation Rules. Nature 405 (2000) 685-689 101. Eckhorn, R., Obermüller, A.: Single Neurons Are Differently Involved in Stimulus- Specific Oscillations in Cat Visual Cortex. Experimental Brain Research 95(1) (1993) 177-182 102. Bressler, S.L., Coppola, R., Nakamura, R.: Episodic Multiregional Cortical Coherence at Multiple Frequencies during Visual Task Performance. Nature 366 (1993) 153-156 103. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Fischer, C.: Oscillatory Synchrony between Human Extrastriate Areas during Visual Short-Term Memory Maintenance. J Neurosci 21 (2001) RC177(1-5) 104. Müller, M.M., Bosch, J., Elbert, T., Kreiter, A., Sosa, M., Valdes-Sosa, P., Rockstroh, B.: Visually Induced Gamma-Band Responses in Human Electroencephalographic Activity: a Link to Animal Studies. Experimental Brain Research 112(1) (1996) 96-102 105. Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J.P., Martinerie, J., Renault, B., Varela, F.: Perception’s Shadow: Long-Distance Synchronization in the Human Brain Activity. Nature 397 (1999) 430-433 106. Haken, H.: Synergetics: an Introduction. Springer Series in Synergetics. Springer, Berlin (1983) 107. Kelso, J.A.S., Haken, H.: New Laws to Be Expected in the Organism: Synergetics of Brain and Behavior. In: Murphy, M. O'Neill, L. (Eds.) What Is Life? The Next 50 Years. University Press, Cambridge (1995) 108. Assisi, C.G., Jirsa, V.K., Kelso, J.A.S.: Synchrony and Clustering in Heterogeneous Networks with Global Coupling and Parameter Dispersion, Physical Review Letters 94 (2005) 018106 109. Jirsa, V.K., Haken, H.: A Derivation of a Macroscopic Field Theory of the Brain from the Quasi-Microscopic Neural Dynamics. Physica D 99 (1997) 503-526 110. Kelso, J.A.S. Jeka, J.J.: Symmetry Breaking Dynamics of Human Multilimb Coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18 (1992) 645-668 111. Gregory, R. L.: Editorial. Perception 29 (2000) 1139-1142 112. Heisenberg, W. Physics and Philosophy: The Revolution in Modern Physics. New York, Harper & Row (1958)
  277. REFERENCES Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2011). The hippocampus and imagining the future: where do we stand? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:173. doi:10.3389/ fnhum.2011.00173 Anagnostaras, S. G., Maren, S., and Fanselow, M. S. (1999). Tem- porally graded retrograde amne- sia of contextual fear after hip- pocampal damage in rats: within- subjects examination. J. Neurosci. 19, 1106–1114. Bakker, A., Kirwan, C. B., Miller, M., and Stark, C. E. L. (2008). Pattern separation in the human hippocam- pal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Sci- ence 319, 1640–1642. doi:10.1126/ science.1152882 Bartlett, F. F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bast, T., Zhang, W.-N., and Feldon, J. (2001). The ventral hippocampus and fear conditioning in rats. Exp. Brain Res. 139, 39–52. doi:10.1007/ s002210100746 Bayley, P. J., Hopkins, R. O., and Squire, L. R. (2003). Successful rec- ollection of remote autobiographical memories by amnesic patients with medial temporal lobe lesions. Neu- ron 38,135–144. doi:10.1016/S0896- 6273(03)00156-9 Besnard,A., Caboche, J., and Laroche, S. (2012). Reconsolidation of memory: a decade of debate. Prog. Neurobiol. 99, 61–80. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio. 2012.07.002 Brown, R., and Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5, 73–99. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(77) 90018-X Buckner, R. L., Wheeler, M. E., and Sheridan, M. A. (2001). Encoding processes during retrieval tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 406–415. doi:10. 1162/08989290151137430 Cho, Y. H., and Kesner, R. P. (1996). Involvement of entorhinal cortex or parietal cortex in long-term spa- tial discrimination memory in rats: retrograde amnesia. Behav. Neu- rosci. 110, 436–442. doi:10.1037/ 0735-7044.110.3.436 Cipolotti, L., Shallice, T., Chan, D., Fox, N., Scahill, R., Harrison, G., et al. (2001). Long-term retro- grade amnesia ... the crucial role of the hippocampus. Neuropsycholo- gia 39, 151–172. doi:10.1016/S0028- 3932(00)00103-2 Clark,R. E.,Broadbent,N. J.,and Squire, L. R. (2005a). Hippocampus and remote spatial memory in rats. Hip- pocampus 15, 260–272. doi:10.1002/ hipo.20056 Clark, R. E., Broadbent, N. J., and Squire, L. R. (2005b). Impaired remote spatial memory after hip- pocampal lesions despite extensive training beginning early in life. Hip- pocampus 15, 340–346. doi:10.1002/ hipo.20076 Corkin, S. (2002). What’s new with the amnesic patient H.M.? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 153–160. doi:10.1038/ nrn726 Daselaar,S. M.,Fleck,M. S.,and Cabeza, R. (2006). Triple dissociation in the medial temporal lobes: recollection, familiarity, and novelty. J. Neuro- physiol. 96, 1902–1911. doi:10.1152/ jn.01029.2005 Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 17–22. doi:10.1037/ h0046671 Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222. doi:10.1146/annurev. ne.18.030195.001205 Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ran- ganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollec- tion and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 11, 379–386. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007. 08.001 Driscoll, I., Howard, S. R., Prusky, G. T., Rudy, J. W., and Sutherland, R. J. (2005). Seahorse wins all races: hippocampus participates in both linear and non-linear visual discrim- ination learning. Behav. Brain Res. 164, 29–35. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2005. 05.006 Ebbinghaus,H. (1885). Memory. A Con- tribution to Experimental Psychol- ogy (Translated to English in 1913). New York: Teachers College, Colum- bia University. Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Fur- manski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Engel, S. A. (2000). Remember- ing episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152. doi:10.1038/ 80671 Fanselow, M. S. (1986). Associative vs. topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: implications for the response selection rules governing species- specific defensive reactions. Learn. Motiv. 17, 16–39. doi:10.1016/0023- 9690(86)90018-4 Fanselow, M. S. (1990). Factors governing one-trial contex- tual conditioning. Anim. Learn. Behav. 18, 264–270. doi:10.3758/ BF03205285 Frankland, P. W., and Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 119–130. doi:10.1038/ nrn1607 Frankland, P. W., Cestari, V., Filip- kowski, R. K., McDonald, R. J., and Silva, A. J. (1998). The dorsal hippocampus is essential for con- text discrimination but not for con- textual conditioning. Behav. Neu- rosci. 112, 863–874. doi:10.1037/ 0735-7044.112.4.863 Gallo,D.A.,Roediger,H. L.,and McDer- mott, K. B. (2001). Associative false recognition occurs without strategic criterion shifts. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 579–586. Gold, A. E., and Kesner, R. P. (2005). The role of the CA3 subregion of the dorsal hippocampus in spatial pattern completion in the rat. Hip- pocampus 15, 808–814. doi:10.1002/ hipo.20103 Goshen, I., Brodsky, M., Prakash, R., Wallace, J., Gradinaru, V., Ramakr- ishnan, C., et al. (2011). Dynamics of Retrieval Strategies for Remote Memories. Cell 147, 678–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033 Guzowski,J. F.,Knierim,J. J.,and Moser, E. I. (2004). Ensemble dynamics of hippocampal regions CA3 and CA1. Neuron 44, 581–584. doi:10.1016/j. neuron.2004.11.003 Haist, F., Bowden Gore, J., and Mao, H. (2001). Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic res- onance imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1139–1145. doi:10.1038/nn739 Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 1726–1731. doi:10.1073/pnas.0610561104 Hirano, M., and Noguchi, K. (1998). Dissociation between specific per- sonal episodes and other aspects of remote memory in a patient with hippocampal amnesia. Percept. Mot. Skills 87, 99–107. doi:10.2466/pms. 1998.87.1.99 Holdstock, J. S., Mayes, A. R., Gong, Q. Y.,Roberts,N.,and Kapur,N. (2004). Item recognition is less impaired than recall and associative recogni- tion in a patient with selective hip- pocampal damage. Hippocampus 15, 203–215. doi:10.1002/hipo.20046 Hunsaker, M. R., and Kesner, R. P. (2013). The operation of pat- tern separation and pattern com- pletion processes associated with different attributes or domains of memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 36–58. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev. 2012.09.014 Kemp, A., and Manahan-Vaughan, D. (2004). Hippocampal long-term depression and long-term potentia- tion encode different aspects of nov- elty acquisition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 8192–8197. doi:10.1073/ pnas.0402650101 Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the spe- cific visual details of presented objects: neuroimaging evidence for effects of emotion. Neuropsycholo- gia 45, 2951–2962. doi:10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2007.05.024 Kesner, R. P. (2007). Behavioral func- tions of the CA3 subregion of the hippocampus. Learn. Mem. 14, 771–781. Kim, J., and Yassa, M. A. (2013). Assess- ing recollection and familiarity of similar lures in a behavioral pattern separation task. Hippocampus 23, 287–294. doi:10.1002/hipo.22087 Kim, J. J., and Fanselow, M. S. (1992). Modality-specific retrograde amne- sia of fear. Science 256, 675–677. doi:10.1126/science.1585183 Kirwan, C. B., Bayley, P. J., Galván, V. V., and Squire, L. R. (2008). Detailed recollection of remote autobio- graphical memory after damage to the medial temporal lobe. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 2676–2680. doi:10.1073/pnas.0712155105 Kriegeskorte, N., Mur, M., and Ban- dettini, P. (2008). Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2:4. doi:10. 3389/neuro.06.004.2008 Lacy, J. W., Yassa, M. A., Stark, S. M., Muftuler, L. T., and Stark, C. E. L. (2011). Distinct pattern separation related transfer functions in human CA3/dentate and CA1 revealed using high-resolution fMRI and variable mnemonic similarity. Learn. Mem. 18, 15–18. doi:10.1101/lm.1971111 Lee, I., Yoganarasimha, D., Rao, G., and Knierim, J. J. (2004). Comparison of population coherence of place cells in hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3. Nature 430, 456–459. doi:10. 1038/nature02739 Lehmann, H., Lacanilao, S., and Sutherland, R. J. (2007). Complete or partial hippocampal damage produces equivalent retrograde amnesia for remote contextual fear memories. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1278–1286. doi:10.1111/j.1460- 9568.2007.05374.x Lehmann, H., Lecluse, V., Houle, A., and Mumby, D. G. (2006). Retro- grade amnesia following hippocam- pal lesions in the shock-probe con- ditioning test. Hippocampus 16, 379–387. doi:10.1002/hipo.20159 Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Moser, M.- B., and Moser, E. I. (2007). Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315, 961–966. doi:10.1126/science. 1135801 Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Treves, A., Meyer, R., Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L., et al. (2005). Progressive transformation of hip- pocampal neuronal representations in“morphed”environments. Neuron 48, 345–358. doi:10.1016/j.neuron. 2005.09.007 Leutgeb, S., Leutgeb, J. K., Treves, A., Moser, M.-B., and Moser, E. I. (2004). Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Science 305, 1295–1298. doi: 10.1126/science.1100265 Lewis, D. J. (1979). Psychobiology of active and inactive memory. Psy- chol. Bull. 86, 1054–1083. doi:10. 1037/0033-2909.86.5.1054 Liu, X., Ramirez, S., Pang, P. T., Puryear, C. B., Govindarajan, A., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2012). Opto- genetic stimulation of a hippocam- pal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 484, 381–385. doi:10. 1038/nature11028 Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinfor- mation in the human mind: a 30- year investigation of the malleabil- ity of memory. Learn. Mem. 12, 361–366. Maren, S., Aharonov, G., and Fanselow, M. S. (1997). Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlov- ian fear conditioning in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 88, 261–274. doi:10.1016/ S0166-4328(97)00088-0 Marr, D. (1971). Simple memory: a the- ory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 262, 23–81. doi:10.1098/rstb.1971.0078 Martin, S. J., De Hoz, L., and Morris, R. G. M. (2005). Ret- rograde amnesia: neither partial nor complete hippocampal lesions in rats result in preferential sparing of remote spatial memory, even after reminding. Neuropsycholo- gia 43, 609–624. doi:10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2004.07.007 Mather, M., and Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6,114–133. doi:10.1177/ 1745691611400234 McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., and O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocam- pus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457. doi:10.1037/0033-295X. 102.3.419 McHugh, T. J., Jones, M. W., Quinn, J. J., Balthasar, N., Coppari, R., Elmquist, J. K., et al. (2007). Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pat- tern separation in the hippocampal network. Science 317, 94–99. doi:10. 1126/science.1140263 Milner, B., Squire, L. R., and Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neu- ron 20,445–468. doi:10.1016/S0896- 6273(00)80987-3 Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R., and Lewis, D. J. (1968). Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160,554–555. doi:10.1126/science.160.3827.554 Monfils, M.-H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., and LeDoux, J. E. (2009). Extinction-reconsolidation bound- aries: key to persistent attenu- ation of fear memories. Science 324, 951–955. doi:10.1126/science. 1167975 Moscovitch, M., Yaschyshyn, T., Ziegler, M., and Nadel, L. (2000). “Remote episodic memory and retrograde amnesia: was Endel Tulving right all along?,” in Memory, Conscious- ness and the Brain: The Tallinn Con- ference, ed. E. Tulving (Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press/Taylor & Fran- cis), 331–345. Nadel, L., and Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 217–227. doi:10.1016/S0959- 4388(97)80010-4 Nadel, L., and Moscovitch, M. (1998). Hippocampal contributions to cor- tical plasticity. Neuropharmacology 37, 431–439. doi:10.1016/S0028- 3908(98)00057-4 Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., and Le Doux, J. E. (2000a). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406, 722–726. doi:10.1038/ 35021052 Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., and LeDoux, J. E. (2000b). The labile nature of con- solidation theory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 216–219. doi:10.1038/35044580 Nakazawa, K., Sun, L. D., Quirk, M. C., Rondi-Reig, L., Wilson, M. A., and Tonegawa, S. (2003). Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neuron 38,305–315. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00165-X Neisser,U. (1982).“Snapshots or bench- marks?,” in Memory Observed, ed. U. Neisser (New York: W. H. Freeman), 45. Norman, K. A. (2010). How hippocam- pus and cortex contribute to recog- nition memory: revisiting the com- plementary learning systems model. Hippocampus 20,1217–1227. doi:10. 1002/hipo.20855 Norman, K. A., and O’Reilly, R. C. (2003). Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recog- nition memory: a complementary- learning-systems approach. Psychol. Rev. 110, 611–646. doi:10.1037/ 0033-295X.110.4.611 O’Reilly, R. C., and Norman, K. A. (2002). Hippocampal and neo- cortical contributions to mem- ory: advances in the comple- mentary learning systems frame- work. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 6, 505–510. doi:10.1016/S1364- 6613(02)02005-3 Power, A. E., Berlau, D. J., McGaugh, J. L., and Steward, O. (2006). Ani- somycin infused into the hippocam- pus fails to block “reconsolidation” but impairs extinction: the role of re- exposure duration. Learn. Mem. 13, 27–34. Roediger, H. L., and McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memo- ries: remembering words not pre- sented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 24, 803–814. doi:10. 1037/0278-7393.21.4.803 Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., Pisoni, D. B., and Gallo, D. A. (2004). Illusory recollection of voices. Memory 12, 586–602. doi:10. 1080/09658210344000125 Rolls, E. T. (2007). An attractor net- work in the hippocampus: theory and neurophysiology. Learn. Mem. 14, 714–731. Rudy, J. W., Biedenkapp, J. C., Moineau, J., and Bolding, K. (2006). Ani- somycin and the reconsolidation hypothesis. Learn. Mem. 13, 1–3. Ryan, L., Nadel, L., Keil, K., Put- nam, K., Schnyer, D., Trouard, T., et al. (2001). Hippocampal com- plex and retrieval of recent and very remote autobiographical memories: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging in neurologi- cally intact people. Hippocampus 11, 707–714. doi:10.1002/hipo.1086 Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory. Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience.Am. Psy- chol. 54,182–203. doi:10.1037/0003- 066X.54.3.182 Schiller, D., Monfils, M.-H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D. C., Ledoux, J. E., and Phelps, E. A. (2010). Prevent- ing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mech- anisms. Nature 463, 49–53. doi:10. 1038/nature08637 Schmolck, H., Buffalo, E. A., and Squire, L. R. (2000). Memory distortions develop over time: recollections of the O.J. Simpson trial verdict after 15 and 32 months. Psychol. Sci. 11, 39–45. doi:10.1111/1467-9280. 00212 Scoville, W. B., and Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilat- eral hippocampal lesions. J. Neu- rol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 20, 11–21. doi:10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11 Sherry, D. F., and Schacter, D. L. (1987). The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psychol. Rev. 94, 439–454. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.439 Sparks, F. T., Lehmann, H., and Sutherland, R. J. (2011). Between- systems memory interference dur- ing retrieval. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 780–786. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568. 2011.07796.x Squire, L. R. (2009). The legacy of patient H.M. for neuroscience. Neu- ron 61, 6–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuron. 2008.12.023 Squire, L. R., and Alvarez, P. (1995). Retrograde amnesia and mem- ory consolidation: a neurobiologi- cal perspective. Curr. Opin. Neuro- biol. 5, 169–177. doi:10.1016/0959- 4388(95)80023-9 Squire, L. R., Cohen, N. J., and Zouzou- nis, J. A. (1984). Preserved mem- ory in retrograde amnesia:sparing of a recently acquired skill. Neuropsy- chologia 22, 145–152. doi:10.1016/ 0028-3932(84)90057-5 Stahl, C., and Klauer, K. C. (2008). A simplified conjoint recognition par- adigm for the measurement of gist and verbatim memory. J. Exp. Psy- chol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34,570–586. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.570 Stark, C. E., and Okado, Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: medial temporal lobe activity associ- ated with incidental memory forma- tion during recognition. J. Neurosci. 23, 6748–6753. Stefanacci, L., Buffalo, E. A., Schmolck, H., and Squire, L. R. (2000). Pro- found amnesia after damage to the medial temporal lobe: a neu- roanatomical and neuropsychologi- cal profile of patient E. P. J. Neurosci. 20, 7024–7036. Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 123, 133–167. Sutherland, R. J., Lehmann, H., Span- swick, S. C., Sparks, F. T., and Melvin, N. R. (2006). Growth Points in Research on Memory and Hip- pocampus. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 166–174. doi:10.1037/cjep20060016 Sutherland, R. J., O’Brien, J., and Lehmann,H. (2008). Absence of sys- tems consolidation of fear memo- ries after dorsal, ventral, or com- plete hippocampal damage. Hip- pocampus 18, 710–718. doi:10.1002/ hipo.20431 Sutherland, R. J., Sparks, F. T., and Lehmann, H. (2010). Hippocam- pus and retrograde amnesia in the rat model: a modest pro- posal for the situation of sys- tems consolidation. Neuropsycholo- gia 48, 2357–2369. doi:10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2010.04.015 Sutherland, R. J., Weisend, M. P., Mumby, D., Astur, R. S., Han- lon, F. M., Koerner, A., et al. (2001). Retrograde amnesia after hippocampal damage: recent vs. remote memories in two tasks. Hippocampus 11, 27–42. doi:10.1002/1098-1063(2001)11: 1<27::AID-HIPO1017>3.0.CO;2-4 Talarico, J. M., and Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, charac- terizes flashbulb memories. Psychol. Sci. 14, 455–461. doi:10.1111/1467- 9280.02453 Teyler,T. J.,and DiScenna,P. (1985). The role of hippocampus in memory: a hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 9, 377–389. Teyler, T. J., and DiScenna, P. (1986). The hippocampal memory index- ing theory. Behav. Neurosci. 100, 147–154. doi:10.1037/0735-7044. 100.2.147 Teyler, T. J., and Rudy, J. W. (2007). The hippocampal indexing the- ory and episodic memory: updat- ing the index. Hippocampus 1169, 1158–1169. doi:10.1002/hipo.20350 Treves, A., and Rolls, E. T. (1994). Com- putational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Hip- pocampus 4, 374–391. doi:10.1002/ hipo.450040319 Tulving, E., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204. doi:10. 1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8: 3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.3.CO;2-J Vazdarjanova, A., and Guzowski, J. F. (2004). Differences in hippocam- pal neuronal population responses to modifications of an environ- mental context: evidence for dis- tinct, yet complementary, functions of CA3 and CA1 ensembles. J. Neu- rosci. 24, 6489–6496. doi:10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.0350-04.2004 Wiltgen, B. J., Sanders, M. J., Anag- nostaras, S. G., Sage, J. R., and Fanselow, M. S. (2006). Context fear learning in the absence of the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 26, 5484–5491. doi:10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.2685-05.2006 Wiltgen, B. J., and Silva, A. J. (2007). Memory for context becomes less specific with time. Learn. Mem. 14, 313–317. Wiltgen, B. J., Zhou, M., Cai, Y., Balaji, J., Karlsson, M. G., Parivash, S. N., et al. (2010). The hippocampus plays a selective role in the retrieval of detailed contextual memories. Curr. Biol. 20, 1336–1344. doi:10.1016/j. cub.2010.06.068 Xue, Y.-X., Luo, Y.-X., Wu, P., Shi, H.- S., Xue, L.-F., Chen, C., et al. (2012). A memory retrieval-extinction pro- cedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science 336, 241–245. doi: 10.1126/science.1215070 Yassa, M. A., Lacy, J. W., Stark, S. M., Albert, M. S., Gallagher, M., and Stark, C. E. L. (2011). Pat- tern separation deficits associated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus activity in non- demented older adults. Hippocam- pus 21, 968–979. doi:10.1002/hipo. 20808 Yassa, M. A., and Stark, C. E. L. (2011). Pattern separation in the hippocam- pus. Trends Neurosci. 34, 515–525. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006 Yonelinas, A. P., Kroll, N. E. A., Quamme, J. R., Lazzara, M. M., Sauvé, M.-J., Widaman, K. F., et al. (2002). Effects of extensive tempo- ral lobe damage or mild hypoxia on recollection and familiarity. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1236–1241. doi:10.1038/ nn961 Yonelinas, A. P., Otten, L. J., Shaw, K. N., and Rugg, M. D. (2005). Sep- arating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. J. Neu- rosci. 25, 3002–3008. doi:10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.5295-04.2005 Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any com- mercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential con- flict of interest. Received: 14 June 2013; accepted: 30 July 2013; published online: 12 August 2013. Citation: Yassa MA and Reagh ZM (2013) Competitive trace theory: a role for the hippocampus in con- textual interference during retrieval. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:107. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00107
  278. References Banks, S.J., Eddy, K.T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P.J., Phan, K.L., 2007. Amygdala frontal con- nectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2 (4), 303–312. Bonanno, G.A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., Coifman, K., 2004. The importance of being flexible: the ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression pre- dicts long-term adjustment. Psychol. Sci. 15 (7), 482–487. Bradley, M.M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., Lang, P.J., 2001. Emotion and motivation II: sex differences in picture processing. Emotion 1 (3), 300–319. Braver, T.S., Cole, M.W., Yarkoni, T., 2010. Vive les differences! Individual variation in neural mechanisms of executive control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20 (2), 242–250. Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention [CSEA-NIMH], 1999. The international affective picture system: digitized photographs. The Center for Research in Psycho- physiology. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P., 1987. The measurement of handedness. Brain Cogn. 6 (2), 175–183. Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. Int. J. 29 (3), 162–173. Davidson, R.J., 1992. Prolegomenon to the structure of emotion: gleanings from neuro- psychology. Cogn. Emotion 6 (3), 245–268. Davidson, R.J., 2003. Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: toward a synthesis. Psychophysiology 40 (5), 655–665. Davidson, R.J., 2004. Well-being and affective style: neural substrates and bio- behavioural correlates. Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci. 359 (1449), 1395–1411. Delgado, M.R., Nearing, K.I., LeDoux, J.E., Phelps, E.A., 2008. Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron 59 (5), 829–838. Dichter, G.S., Tomarken, A.J., Baucom, B.R., 2002. Startle modulation before, during and after exposure to emotional stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 43, 191–196. Dolan, R.J., 2007. The human amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex in behavioural reg- ulation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 362 (1481), 787–799. Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., Hirsch, J., 2008. Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cereb. Cortex 18 (6), 1475–1484. Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., Erb, M., Birbaumer, N., Anders, S., 2007. Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat-related stimuli. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28 (5), 409–423. Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D.M., Kandel, E.R., Hirsch, J., 2006. Resolving emotional con- flict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 51 (6), 871–882. Frewen, P.A., Dozois, D.J.A., Lanius, R.A., 2008. Neuroimaging studies of psychological interventions for mood and anxiety disorders: empirical and methodological re- view. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28 (2), 228–246. Friston, K.J., Buechel, C., Fink, G.R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., Dolan, R.J., 1997. Psychophysio- logical and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage 6 (3), 218–229. Ghashghaei, H.T., Barbas, H., 2002. Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neurosci- ence 115 (4), 1261–1279. Gross, J.J., 1998. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent conse- quences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 224–237. Gross, J.J., 2002. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psy- chophysiology 39 (3), 281–291. Gross, J.J., John, O.P., 2003. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85 (2), 348–362. Hamann, S., Canli, T., 2004. Individual differences in emotion processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14 (2), 233–238. Jackson, D.C., Malmstadt, J.R., Larson, C.L., Davidson, R.J., 2000. Suppression and en- hancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology 37 (4), 515–522. Johnstone, T., Walsh, K.S.O., Greischar, L.L., Alexander, A.L., Fox, A.S., Davidson, R.J., Oakes, T.R., 2006. Motion correction and the use of motion covariates in multiple-subject fMRI analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27 (10), 779–788. Johnstone, T., van Reekum, C.M., Urry, H.L., Kalin, N.H., Davidson, R.J., 2007. Failure to regulate: counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal–subcortical cir- cuitry in major depression. J. Neurosci. 27 (33), 8877–8884. Kalisch, R., 2009. The functional neuroanatomy of reappraisal: time matters. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33 (8), 1215–1226. Kessler, R.C., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K.R., Wang, P.S., 2004. The epidemiology of adult mental disorders, In: Levin, B.L., Petrilia, J., Hennessy, K.D. (Eds.), Mental Health Ser- vices: A Public Health Perspective, Second edition. Oxford University Press, New York. Kosslyn, S.M., Cacioppo, J.T., Davidson, R.J., Hugdahl, K., Lovallo, W.R., Spiegel, D., Rose, R., 2002. Bridging psychology and biology: the analysis of individuals in groups. Am. Psychol. 57 (5), 341. Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N., 1999. International affective picture system (IAPS): technical manual and affective ratings. The Center for Research in Psycho- physiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. Lanteaume, L., Khalfa, S., Regis, J., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P., Bartolomei, F., 2007. Emotion induction after direct intracerebral stimulations of human amygdala. Cereb. Cortex 17 (6), 1307–1313. Lapate, R.C., Lee, H., Salomons, T.V., van Reekum, C.M., Greischar, L.L., Davidson, R.J., 2012. Amygdalar function reflects common individual differences in emotion and pain regulation success. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 148–158. Larson, C.L., Schaefer, H.S., Siegle, G.J., Jackson, C.A.B., Anderle, M.J., Davidson, R.J., 2006. Fear is fast in phobic individuals: amygdala activation in response to fear-relevant stimuli. Biol. Psychiatry 60 (4), 410–417. Lee, H., Shackman, A.J., Jackson, D.C., Davidson, R.J., 2009. Test–retest reliability of vol- untary emotion regulation. Psychophysiology 46, 874–879. Mauss, I.B., Levenson, R.W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F.H., Gross, J.J., 2005. The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion 5 (2), 175–190. McRae, K., Ochsner, K.N., Mauss, I.B., Gross, J.J., Gabrieli, J.J.D., 2008. Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process. Inter- group Relations 11 (2), 143–162. Miller, E.K., Cohen, J.D., 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167. Nacewicz, B.M., Dalton, K.M., Johnstone, T., Long, M.T., McAuliff, E.M., Oakes, T.R., Alexander, A.L., Davidson, R.J., 2006. Amygdala volume and nonverbal social im- pairment in adolescent and adult males with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63 (12), 1417–1428. Ochsner, K.N., Gross, J.J., 2005. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9 (5), 242–249. Ochsner, K.N., Bunge, S.A., Gross, J.J., Gabrieli, J.D.E., 2002. Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14 (8), 1215–1229. Ochsner, K.N., Ray, R.D., Cooper, J.C., Robertson, E.R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Gross, J.J., 2004. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage 23 (2), 483–499. Ongur, D., Price, J.L., 2000. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10 (3), 206–219. Phelps, E.A., LeDoux, J.E., 2005. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48 (2), 175–187. Phillips, M.L., Ladouceur, C.D., Drevets, W.C., 2008. A neural model of voluntary and au- tomatic emotion regulation: Implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 13 (9), 833–857. Ray, R.D., McRae, K., Ochsner, K.N., Gross, J.J., 2010. Cognitive reappraisal of negative af- fect: converging evidence from EMG and self-report. Emotion 10 (4), 587. Siegle, G.J., Ichikawa, N., Steinhauer, S., 2008. Blink before and after you think: blinks occur prior to and following cognitive load indexed by pupillary responses. Psy- chophysiology 45 (5), 679–687. Tassinary, L.G., Cacioppo, J.T., Green, T.R., 1989. A psychometric study of surface elec- trode placements for facial electromyographic recording: I. The brow and cheek muscle regions. Psychophysiology 26, 1–6. Taylor, S.F., Liberzon, I., 2007. Neural correlates of emotion regulation in psychopathol- ogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 (10), 413–418. Thompson, R.A., 1994. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59 (2–3), 25–52 250–283.. Urry, H.L., van Reekum, C.M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N.H., Thurow, M.E., Schaefer, H.S., Jackson, C.A., Frye, C.J., Greischar, L.L., Alexander, A.L., et al., 2006. Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. J. Neurosci. 26 (16), 4415–4425. Urry, H.L., van Reekum, C.M., Johnstone, T., Davidson, R.J., 2009. Individual differences in some (but not all) medial prefrontal regions reflect cognitive demand while reg- ulating unpleasant emotion. NeuroImage 47 (3), 852–863. Van Boxtel, A., 2001. Optimal signal bandwidth for the recording of surface EMG activ- ity of facial, jaw, oral, and neck muscles. Psychophysiology 38 (1), 22–34. van Reekum, C.M., Johnstone, T., Urry, H.L., Thurow, M.E., Schaefer, H.S., Alexander, A.L., Davidson, R.J., 2007. Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. NeuroImage 36 (3), 1041–1055. Wager, T.D., Davidson, M.L., Hughes, B.L., Lindquist, M.A., Ochsner, K.N., 2008. Prefrontal– subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron 59 (6), 1037–1050.
  279. References AUBRY, M., MATURANA, D., EFROS, A., RUSSELL, B., AND SIVIC, J. 2014. Seeing 3D chairs: exemplar part-based 2D-3D alignment using a large dataset of CAD models. In Proc. CVPR. AVIDAN, S., AND SHAMIR, A. 2007. Seam carving for content- aware image resizing. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH. BARNES, C., SHECHTMAN, E., FINKELSTEIN, A., AND GOLD- MAN, D. B. 2009. Patchmatch: a randomized correspondence algorithm for structural image editing. In Proc. ACM SIG- GRAPH, 24:1–24:11. BARRETT, W. A., AND CHENEY, A. S. 2002. Object-based image editing. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 777–784. BARRON, J. T. 2012. Shape, albedo, and illumination from a single image of an unknown object. In CVPR, 334–341. BLANZ, V., AND VETTER, T. 1999. A morphable model for the synthesis of 3d faces. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 187–194. BOKELOH, M., WAND, M., KOLTUN, V., AND SEIDEL, H.-P. 2011. Pattern-aware shape deformation using sliding dockers. ACM Trans. Graph. 30, 6 (Dec.), 123:1–123:10. CHEN, J., PARIS, S., WANG, J., MATUSIK, W., COHEN, M., AND DURAND, F. 2011. The video mesh: A data structure for image-based three-dimensional video editing. In ICCP, 1–8. CHEN, T., ZHU, Z., SHAMIR, A., HU, S.-M., AND COHEN-OR, D. 2013. 3-sweep: Extracting editable objects from a single photo. ACM Trans. Graph. 32, 6, to appear. DEBEVEC, P. E., TAYLOR, C. J., AND MALIK, J. 1996. Mod- eling and rendering architecture from photographs: a hybrid geometry- and image-based approach. In Proc. ACM SIG- GRAPH, 11–20. DEBEVEC, P. 1998. Rendering synthetic objects into real scenes: bridging traditional and image-based graphics with global illu- mination and high dynamic range photography. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 189–198. DURAND, F. 2002. An invitation to discuss computer depiction. In Proc. ACM NPAR, 111–124. FANG, H., AND HART, J. C. 2004. Textureshop: texture synthesis as a photograph editing tool. Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 354–359. FISHER, R. 1953. Dispersion on a sphere. In Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series A, Mathematical and Physical Sciences, vol. 217, 295–305. GAL, R., AND COHEN-OR, D. 2006. Salient geometric features for partial shape matching and similarity. ACM Trans. Graph. 25, 1 (Jan.), 130–150. GAL, R., WEXLER, Y., OFEK, E., HOPPE, H., AND COHEN-OR, D. 2010. Seamless montage for texturing models. Comput. Graph. Forum 29, 2, 479–486. GOLDBERG, C., CHEN, T., ZHANG, F.-L., SHAMIR, A., AND HU, S.-M. 2012. Data-driven object manipulation in images. Computer Graphics Forum 31, 2pt1, 265–274. HABER, T., FUCHS, C., BEKAERT, P., SEIDEL, H.-P., GOESELE, M., AND LENSCH, H. P. A. 2009. Relighting objects from image collections. In CVPR, IEEE, 627–634. HARA, K., NISHINO, K., AND IKEUCHI, K. 2008. Mixture of spherical distributions for single-view relighting. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 30, 1, 25–35. HONG, W., YANG, A. Y., HUANG, K., AND MA, Y. 2004. On symmetry and multiple-view geometry: Structure, pose, and cal- ibration from a single image. IJCV 60, 3 (Dec.), 241–265. KARSCH, K., HEDAU, V., FORSYTH, D., AND HOIEM, D. 2011. Rendering synthetic objects into legacy photographs. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH Asia, 157:1–157:12. KHAN, E. A., REINHARD, E., FLEMING, R. W., AND B ¨ULTHOFF, H. H. 2006. Image-based material editing. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 654–663. KIM, V. G., LIPMAN, Y., AND FUNKHOUSER, T. 2012. Symmetry-guided texture synthesis and manipulation. ACM Trans. Graph. 31, 3 (June), 22:1–22:14. KOLMOGOROV, V. 2006. Convergent tree-reweighted message passing for energy minimization. IEEE TPAMI 28, 10 (Oct.), 1568–1583. KOPF, J., NEUBERT, B., CHEN, B., COHEN, M., COHEN-OR, D., DEUSSEN, O., UYTTENDAELE, M., AND LISCHINSKI, D. 2008. Deep photo: model-based photograph enhancement and viewing. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH Asia, 116:1–116:10. KRAEVOY, V., SHEFFER, A., AND GOTSMAN, C. 2003. Match- maker: constructing constrained texture maps. ACM Trans. Graph. 22, 3 (July), 326–333. KRAEVOY, V., SHEFFER, A., AND VAN DE PANNE, M. 2009. Modeling from contour drawings. In Proceedings of the 6th Eu- rographics Symposium on Sketch-Based Interfaces and Model- ing, ACM, New York, NY, USA, SBIM ’09, 37–44. KWATRA, V., SCHODL, A., ESSA, I., TURK, G., AND BOBICK, A. 2003. Graphcut textures: Image and video synthesis using graph cuts. ACM Trans. Graph. 22, 3 (July), 277–286. LALONDE, J.-F., HOIEM, D., EFROS, A. A., ROTHER, C., WINN, J., AND CRIMINISI, A. 2007. Photo clip art. Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH 26, 3 (August), 3. LAND, E. H., JOHN, AND MCCANN, J. 1971. Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1–11. LEPETIT, V., MORENO-NOGUER, F., AND FUA, P. 2009. Epnp: An accurate o(n) solution to the pnp problem. Int. J. Comput. Vision 81, 155–166. LIM, J., PIRIAVASH, H., AND TORRALBA, A. 2013. Parsing ikea objects: Fine pose estimation. In ICCV. MEI, X., LING, H., AND JACOBS, D. 2009. Sparse representation of cast shadows via l1-regularized least squares. In ICCV. MITRA, N. J., AND PAULY, M. 2008. Symmetry for architectural design. In Advances in Architectural Geometry, 13–16. MITRA, N. J., GUIBAS, L. J., AND PAULY, M. 2006. Partial and approximate symmetry detection for 3d geometry. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 560–568. NEALEN, A., SORKINE, O., ALEXA, M., AND COHEN-OR, D. 2005. A sketch-based interface for detail-preserving mesh edit- ing. ACM Trans. Graph. 24, 3 (July), 1142–1147. NG, R., RAMAMOORTHI, R., AND HANRAHAN, P. 2003. All- frequency shadows using non-linear wavelet lighting approxima- tion. In Proc. ACM SIGGRAPH, 376–381. OH, B. M., CHEN, M., DORSEY, J., AND DURAND, F. 2001. Image-based modeling and photo editing. In Proc. ACM SIG- GRAPH, 433–442. OKABE, T., SATO, I., AND SATO, Y. 2004. Spherical harmonics vs. haar wavelets: Basis for recovering illumination from cast shadows. In CVPR, 50–57. PANAGOPOULOS, A., SAMARAS, D., AND PARAGIOS, N. 2009. Robust shadow and illumination estimation using a mixture model. In CVPR, 651–658. PAULY, M., MITRA, N. J., GIESEN, J., GROSS, M., AND GUIBAS, L. J. 2005. Example-based 3d scan completion. In Proc. SGP. PRASAD, M., ZISSERMAN, A., AND FITZGIBBON, A. W. 2006. Single view reconstruction of curved surfaces. In CVPR. RAMAMOORTHI, R., AND HANRAHAN, P. 2001. On the relation- ship between radiance and irradiance: determining the illumina- tion from images of a convex lambertian object. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 10, 2448–2459. ROMEIRO, F., AND ZICKLER, T. 2010. Blind reflectometry. In ECCV, 45–58. SIMPSON, J., 2003. Oxford English Dictionary Online, 2nd edi- tion. http://www.oed.com/, July. SORKINE, O., AND ALEXA, M. 2007. As-rigid-as-possible sur- face modeling. In Proc. SGP, 109–116. TERZOPOULOS, D., WITKIN, A., AND KASS, M. 1987. Symmetry-seeking models and 3d object reconstruction. Inter- national Journal of Computer Vision 1, 211–221. TZUR, Y., AND TAL, A. 2009. Flexistickers: photogrammetric texture mapping using casual images. ACM Trans. Graph. 28, 3 (July), 45:1–45:10. XU, K., ZHENG, H., ZHANG, H., COHEN-OR, D., , LIU, L., AND XIONG, Y. 2011. Photo-inspired model-driven 3d object modeling. ACM Transactions on Graphics 30, 4. ZHENG, Y., CHEN, X., CHENG, M.-M., ZHOU, K., HU, S.-M., AND MITRA, N. J. 2012. Interactive images: cuboid proxies for smart image manipulation. ACM Trans. Graph. 31, 4 (July), 99:1–99:11. ZOU, H., AND HASTIE, T. 2005. Regularization and variable se- lection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B 67, 301–320.
  280. For reviews on the literature on musical persuasion, see Allan (2008), Bruner (1990), Hahn & Hwang (1999), and North & Hargreaves (2008).
  281. 1. Green, Andrew (2006) What is the impact of the commercial break environment on receptivity to my advertising? Warc Media FAQs, January 2006.
  282. 1. Bower, G. H., (1981). Mood and Memory. American Psychologist, 36 (2), 129-148.
  283. 1. Bower, G. H., (1981). Mood and Memory. American Psychologist, 36 (2), 129-148.
  284. 1. Owolabi, A. B., (2009). Effect of Consumers Mood on Advertising Effectiveness. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 4, 118-127 2. De Pelsmacker, P.Guens ,M., & Ankaert, P., (2002). Media Context And Advertising Effectiveness: The Role of Context Appreciation and Context/Ad Similarity. Journal of Advertising, 31 (2), 49-61
  285. [1]Shapiro, S., MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W., (2001). Understanding Program Induced Mood Effects: Decoupling Arousal from Valence. Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26.
  286. 1. Sharot, T; Phelps, E. (2004). "How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 4 (3): 294–306. 2. Mickley Steinmetz, K; Schmidt, K.; Zucker, H.; Kensinger, E. (2012). "The effect of emotional arousal and retention delay on subsequent-memory effects".Cognitive Neuroscience 3 (4): 150–159. 3. Jeong, E; Biocca, F. (2012). "Corrigendum to "Are there optimal levels of arousal to memory? effects of arousal, centrality, and familiarity on brand memory in video games"". Computers in Human Behavior 28 (4): 1533
  287. 1. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242–276). New York: Oxford University Press. 2. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Klein, D. J., & Poehlmann, K. M. (1997). The psychophysiology of emotion across the lifespan. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 17, 27–74 3. Bachorowski, J. A. (1999). Vocal expression and perception of emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- ence, 8, 53–57. 4. Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214. 5. Barrett, L. F. (in press, a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science.
  288. 1. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying Positive and negative a V ect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1253–1265. 2. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1986). A V ect intensity and reactions to daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 803–814.
  289. 1. Axelrod, J. N., (1963). Induced Moods and Attitude toward Products. Journal of Advertising Research, 3 (2), 19-24.
  290. 1. Soldow, G. F., & Principe, V., (1981). Response to Commercials as a Function of Program Context. Journal of Advertising Research, 21 (2), 59-65. 2. Owolabi, A. B., (2009). Effect of Consumers Mood on Advertising Effectiveness. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 4, 118-127. 3. Moorman, M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2012). Program-Involvement Effects on Commercial Attention and Recall of Successive and Embedded Advertising. Journal of Advertising, 41(2), 25-38. 4. Soldow, G. F., & Principe, V., (1981). Response to Commercials as a Function of Program Context. Journal of Advertising Research, 21 (2), 59-65. 5. Krugman, H. E., (1983). Television Program Interest and Commercial Interruption. Journal of Advertising Research, 23 (1), 21-23.
  291. 1. Marjolein Moorman, Lotte M. Willemsen, Peter C. Neijens and Edith G. Smit Journal of Advertising Vol. 41, No. 2 (Summer 2012), pp. 25-37
  292. 1. Zillmann, Dolf (1996), “The Psychology of Suspense in Dramatic Exposition,” in Suspense: Conceptualizations, Theoretical Analyses, and Empirical Explanations, ed. Peter Vorderer, Hans J. Wulff, and Mike Friedrichsen, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 199-231
  293. 1. Soldow, Gary F. and Victor Principe (1981), “Response to Commercials as a Function of Program Context,” Journal of Advertising Research, 21(2), 59-65
  294. 1. Zillmann, Dolf (1996), “The Psychology of Suspense in Dramatic Exposition,” in Suspense: Conceptualizations, Theoretical Analyses, and Empirical Explanations, ed. Peter Vorderer, Hans J. Wulff, and Mike Friedrichsen, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 199-231
  295. 1. Gorn, Gerald, Michel Tuan Pham, and Leo Yatming Sin (2001), “When Arousal Influences Ad Evaluation and Valence Does Not (and Vice Versa),” Journal of Consumer Psychology,11(1), 43-55 2. Mattes, John and Joanne Cantor (1982), “Enhancing Responses to Television Advertisements via the Transfer of Residual Arousal from Prior Programming,” Journal of Broadcasting, 26(Spring), 553-66 3. Moorman, Marjolein, Peter C. Neijens, and Edith G. Smit - (2005), “The Effects of Program Responses on the Processing of Commercials Placed at Various Positions in the Program and the Block,” Journal of Advertising Research, 45(1), 49-59 4. De Pelsmacker, Patrick, Maggie Geuens, and Pascal Anckaert (2002), “Media Context and Advertising Effectiveness: The Role of Context Appreciation and Context/Ad Similarity,” Journal of Advertising, 31(2), 49-61 5. Singh, Surendra N. and Jacqueline C. Hitchon (1989), “The Intensifying Effects of Exciting Television Programs on the Reception of Subsequent Commercials,” Psychology &Marketing, 6(1), 1-31 6. Shapiro, Stewart, Deborah J. MacInnis, and C. Whan Park (2002), “Understanding Program Induced Mood Effects: Decoupling Arousal from Valence,” Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26 7. Colleen C. Bee and Robert Madrigal “IT'S NOT WHETHER YOU WIN OR LOSE; IT'S HOW THE GAME IS PLAYED: The Influence of Suspenseful Sports Programming on Advertising” Journal of Advertising Vol. 41, No. 1 (Spring 2012), pp. 47-58
  296. Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman (2000), “Temporal Construal and Time-Dependent Changes in Preferences,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 876–89. Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman (2003), “Temporal Construal,” Psychological Review, 110 (July), 403–21. Fo¨rster, Jens (2009), “Relations between Perceptual and Conceptual Scope: How Global versus Local Processing Fits a Focus on Similarity versus Dissimilarity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138 (1), 383–99.
  297. Jordan Etkin and Rebecca K. Ratner, (2013), “Goal Pursuit, Now and Later: Temporal Compatibility of Different versus Similar Means” Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 39, No. 5 (February 2013), pp. 1085-1099 Kruglanski, Arie W., James Y. Shah, Ayelet Fishbach, Ron Friedman, Woo Young Chun, and David Sleeth-Keppler (2002), “A Theory of Goal Systems,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 34, ed. Mark P. Zanna, San Diego: Academic Press, 331–78 Kruglanski, Arie W., Antonio Pierro, and Anna Sheveland (2010), “How Many Roads Lead to Rome? Equifinality Set-Size and Commitment to Goals and Means,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 40 (April), 1–9. Etkin, Jordan, and Rebecca K. Ratner (2012), “The Dynamic Impact of Variety among Means on Motivation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (April), 1076–92.
  298. Fo¨rster, Jens, E. Tory Higgins, and Lorraine Chen Idson (1998), “Approach and Avoidance Strength during Goal Attainment: Regulatory Focus and the ‘Goal Looms Larger’ Effect,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (November),1115–31. Higgins, E. Tory, Joseph Cesario, Nao Hagiwara, Scott Spiegel, and Thane Pittman (2010), “Increasing or Decreasing Interest in Activities: The Role of Regulatory Fit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (4), 559–72. Fujita, Kentaro, Marlone D. Henderson, Juliana Eng, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman (2006), “Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events,” Psychological Science, 17 (April), 278–82. Brooks, Lee R., Geoffrey R. Norman, and Scott W. Allen (1991), “Role of Specific Similarity in a Medical Diagnostic Task,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120 (September), 278–87. Dhar, Ravi, Stephen M. Nowlis, and Steven J. Sherman (1999), “Comparison Effects on Preference Construction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), 293–306. Fo¨rster, Jens (2009), “Relations between Perceptual and Conceptual Scope: How Global versus Local Processing Fits a Focus on Similarity versus Dissimilarity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138 (1), 383–99.
  299. 1. Goodman, Joseph K., and Selin A. Malkoc (2012), “Choosing for Here and Now versus There and Later: The Moderating Role of Psychological Distance on Assortment Size Preferences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (December), electronicallypublished March 26.
  300. Cesario, Joseph, Heidi Grant, and E. Tory Higgins (2004), “Regulatory Fit and Persuasion: Transfer from ‘Feeling Right,’” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86 (3), 388–404. Fishbach, Ayelet, James Y. Shah, and Arie W. Kruglanski (2004), “Emotional Transfer in Goal Systems,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40 (November), 723–38 Fo¨rster, Jens, E. Tory Higgins, and Lorraine Chen Idson (1998), “Approach and Avoidance Strength during Goal Attainment: Regulatory Focus and the ‘Goal Looms Larger’ Effect,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (November),1115–31. Higgins, E. Tory (2005), “Value from Regulatory Fit,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 (August), 209–13. Higgins, E. Tory, Joseph Cesario, Nao Hagiwara, Scott Spiegel, and Thane Pittman (2010), “Increasing or Decreasing Interest in Activities: The Role of Regulatory Fit,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (4), 559–72. Merton, Robert K. (1957), Social Theory and Social Structure, Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Jordan Etkin and Rebecca K. Ratner, (2013), “Goal Pursuit, Now and Later: Temporal Compatibility of Different versus Similar Means” Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 39, No. 5 (February 2013), pp. 1085-1099
  301. 1. Jordan Etkin and Rebecca K. Ratner, (2013), “Goal Pursuit, Now and Later: Temporal Compatibility of Different versus Similar Means” Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 39, No. 5 (February 2013), pp. 1085-1099
  302. Holt, D., Ippolito, P.M., Desrochers, D.M., & Kelly, C.R. (2007). Children’s exposure to TV advertising in 1977 and 2004: Information for the obesity debate. Washington, DC: Federal Trade Commission, Bureau of Economics Staff Report. Allan, D. (2008). A content analysis of music placement in prime-time television advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 48(3), 404-417. Petty, R.E., Brinol, P., & Priester, J.R. (2009). Mass media attitude change: Implications of the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In J. Bryant & M.B. Oliver (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 125-164). New York, NY: Routledge.
  303. August 21st,2012 http://bgr.com/2012/08/22/apple-poised-to-enrage-cable-companies-with-new-ad-blocking-tech/ http://appleinsider.com/articles/12/08/21/apple_patent_lets_users_switch_away_from_broadcast_commercials_automatically.html http://patft.uspto.gov/netacgi/nph-Parser?Sect1=PTO2&Sect2=HITOFF&u=%2Fnetahtml%2FPTO%2Fsearch-adv.htm&r=13&p=1&f=G&l=50&d=PTXT&S1=(apple.ASNM.+AND+20120821.PD.)&OS=an/apple+and+isd/8/21/2012&RS=(AN/apple+AND+ISD/20120821)
  304. August 8th 2012 http://singularityhub.com/2012/08/08/billboards-and-tvs-detect-your-face-advertisers-salivate/ http://www.cnet.com/news/microsoft-readies-nuads-they-watch-you-watching-them/ http://singularityhub.com/2012/02/18/free-facial-recognition-klik-point-your-phone-at-a-friend-and-it-will-tell-you-who-they-are/ https://imrsv.com/
  305. Sept. 7th, 2012 http://www.extremetech.com/extreme/135665-fbi-launches-1-billion-nationwide-facial-recognition-system https://www.eff.org/document/fbi-facial-recognition-initiatives-presentation-2010-biometrics-conference http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg21528804.200-fbi-launches-1-billion-face-recognition-project.html#.U7jgRfldWVM http://www.nist.gov/customcf/get_pdf.cfm?pub_id=905968 http://www.fbi.gov/about-us/cjis/fingerprints_biometrics/ngi https://www.eff.org/files/filenode/vorder_bruegge-Facial-Recognition-and-Identification-Initiatives.pdf
  306. http://singularityhub.com/2014/01/19/with-emotion-recognition-algorithms-computers-know-what-youre-thinking/
  307. http://singularityhub.com/2014/01/19/with-emotion-recognition-algorithms-computers-know-what-youre-thinking/ http://singularityhub.com/2013/07/06/realeyes-emotion-detection-software-knows-how-youre-feeling-about-their-clients-ads/
  308. http://news.discovery.com/autos/drive/in-car-facial-recognition-could-deter-road-rage-140317.htm
  309. http://www.techradar.com/news/gaming/microsoft-interested-in-using-eye-tracking-vr-headset-for-xbox-one-1259657?src=rss&attr=all
  310. https://gigaom.com/2014/10/17/researchers-are-using-deep-learning-to-predict-how-we-pose-its-more-important-than-it-sounds/ http://arxiv-web3.library.cornell.edu/pdf/1409.7963v1.pdf http://arxiv.org/pdf/1312.7302v6.pdf http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=2629500 http://arxiv.org/pdf/1312.4659v3.pdf https://gigaom.com/2014/03/18/facebook-shows-off-its-deep-learning-skills-with-deepface/
  311. http://io9.com/the-human-face-is-far-more-expressive-than-we-thought-1556098821 http://www.pnas.org/content/111/15/E1454.abstract
  312. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/algorithm-recognizes-faces-better-people-can https://medium.com/the-physics-arxiv-blog/the-face-recognition-algorithm-that-finally-outperforms-humans-2c567adbf7fc
  313. http://www.popsci.com/article/gadgets/how-facebooks-new-machine-brain-will-learn-all-about-you-your-photos
  314. http://www.popsci.com/article/gadgets/how-facebooks-new-machine-brain-will-learn-all-about-you-your-photos
  315. http://www.engadget.com/2014/06/25/ford-and-intel-project-mobii-connected-car-cameras/
  316. Sept. 18th, 2012 http://gizmodo.com/5944282/russia-has-more-diamonds-hiding-in-an-asteroid-crater-than-the-rest-of-the-world-does-combined
  317. Sept. 25th, 2012 http://gizmodo.com/5946298/nasa-publishes-the-most-detailed-image-of-the-universe-ever-captured http://gizmodo.com/5421703/new-hubble-ultra-deep-field-image-will-inspire-you-today
  318. November 15th, 2012 http://www.themarysue.com/hubble-finds-new-most-distant-object/
  319. Sept. 29th, 2012 http://news.discovery.com/space/beam-me-to-the-stars-scotty-120928.htm#mkcpgn=rssnws1
  320. http://io9.com/heres-nasas-new-design-for-a-warp-drive-ship-1588948192 http://io9.com/5963263/how-nasa-will-build-its-very-first-warp-drive http://www.youtube.com/watch?feature=player_embedded&v=9M8yht_ofHc http://iopscience.iop.org/0264-9381/11/5/001
  321. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/10/staggering-promise-of-exponential-technologies-in-a-succinct-5-minute-video/
  322. http://news.discovery.com/tech/gear-and-gadgets/inflatable-concrete-forms-a-sustainable-shell-140612.htm
  323. http://news.discovery.com/autos/fuel-and-alternative-fuel-technologies/tesla-gives-up-patents-to-spur-innovaiton-140613.htm http://www.teslamotors.com/blog/all-our-patent-are-belong-you
  324. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/aerodynamics-world-cup-soccer-ball-gifs
  325. http://io9.com/stephen-hawking-interviewed-by-john-oliver-is-absolutel-1591430711
  326. http://io9.com/artist-proves-that-sound-waves-are-everywhere-1591836577 http://io9.com/use-sound-waves-to-make-personalized-jewelry-1499620512
  327. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/removing-salt-relieve-world%E2%80%99s-thirst
  328. http://news.discovery.com/tech/gear-and-gadgets/send-your-sounds-through-a-real-echo-chamber-140617.htm http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn25733-babys-haunting-cry-created-by-online-reverb-room.html
  329. http://earthtechling.com/2014/06/d-link-extends-connected-home-offerings/
  330. December 8th, 2012 http://www.pcworld.com/article/2018961/were-all-clueless-about-privacy-ftc-is-told-at-hearing.html http://www.ftc.gov/news-events/press-releases/2012/12/ftc-host-comprehensive-collection-web-data-workshop-tomorrow http://www.pcworld.com/article/2019241/privacy-in-america-continues-to-erode-online-report-says.html?tk=rel_news http://online.wsj.com/news/articles/SB10001424127887324784404578143144132736214?mg=reno64-wsj&url=http%3A%2F%2Fonline.wsj.com%2Farticle%2FSB10001424127887324784404578143144132736214.html http://online.wsj.com/news/interactive/ANONYMITY1208?ref=SB10001424127887324784404578143144132736214 http://online.wsj.com/news/interactive/ANONYMITY1212?ref=SB10001424127887324784404578143144132736214 http://blogs.wsj.com/digits/2012/12/07/sites-sharing-personal-details-the-journal-methodology/
  331. January 17th, 2013 1. http://www.theverge.com/2013/1/17/3885898/doj-wont-talk-about-gps-tracking-aclu-lawsuit
  332. February 6th, 2013 http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2013-02/your-brain-music-infographic http://visual.ly/psychology-music?utm_source=visually_embed
  333. April 12th, 2013
  334. August 28th, 2013 http://gizmodo.com/we-could-build-entire-cities-out-of-greenhouse-gas-some-1216713649 http://phys.org/news/2013-08-trial-carbon-capture-technology-co2.html http://www.theguardian.com/environment/2012/may/10/carbon-capture-storage http://www.smartplanet.com/blog/bulletin/skyonics-twist-on-carbon-capture-turns-co2-into-baking-soda/ http://content.time.com/time/specials/2007/article/0,28804,1730759_1731383_1731989,00.html
  335. August 20th, 2013 http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2013-08/psychedelics-dont-give-you-mental-health-problems-study-says http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0063972 http://www.popsci.com/science/article/2013-04/new-science-lsd-therapy
  336. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/01/google-acquires-artificial-intelligence.html http://online.wsj.com/news/article_email/SB10001424052702304691904579345641537039368-lMyQjAxMTA0MDIwNjEyNDYyWj
  337. http://www.fastcoexist.com/3025683/ex-nsa-computer-scientist-creates-tool-to-let-you-take-back-emails https://virtru.com/
  338. http://news.discovery.com/tech/gear-and-gadgets/wish-granted-heres-your-portable-holodeck-140210.htm http://www.northropgrumman.com/Capabilities/VIPE/Pages/default.aspx http://www.dvice.com/2014-1-28/northrop-grumman-has-portable-holodeck
  339. http://www.fastcoexist.com/3026132/a-creepy-new-startup-wants-to-create-living-avatars-for-dead-people http://eterni.me/
  340. http://www.linkedin.com/today/post/article/20140218180649-156187-why-movement-is-the-killer-app-for-nonprofits
  341. http://singularityhub.com/2014/03/16/robots-entering-the-workforce-but-are-labor-laws-keeping-up/ http://singularityhub.com/2013/09/24/nearly-half-of-u-s-jobs-could-be-done-by-computers-study-says/ http://singularityhub.com/wp-content/uploads/2013/09/The_Future_of_Employment_OMS_Working_Paper.pdf
  342. http://www.darpa.mil/NewsEvents/Releases/2014/03/18.aspx http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/03/darpa-vtol-x-plane-program-plans-to.html
  343. http://www.extremetech.com/computing/179066-what-is-mesh-networking-and-why-apples-adoption-in-ios-7-could-change-the-world
  344. http://techcrunch.com/2014/04/01/youtube-claims-it-has-been-behind-every-viral-video-trend-ever/
  345. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/400-tons-of-mini-carbon-nanotube-solar.html http://crowlspace.com/?p=1882 https://www.academia.edu/4134421/Maximum_Mass-Beam_Efficiency
  346. http://singularityhub.com/2014/04/02/new-smart-gel-tags-aim-to-prevent-leftovers-from-becoming-science-experiments/
  347. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/terrestrial-energy-successfully-closed.html http://www.menafn.com/f5e0182b-800d-46b5-8d20-9024b5eaa730/Terrestrial-Energy-Inc-Announces-Appointment-of-Hugh-MacDiarmid-asChairman-of-the-Board-Financing-Oversubscribed?src=main http://nextbigfuture.com/2013/04/terrestrial-energy-will-make-integral.html
  348. http://www.engadget.com/2014/04/01/darpa-launches-biotechology-division/
  349. http://www.theverge.com/2014/4/4/5581884/how-advertising-cookies-let-observers-follow-you-across-the-web https://freedom-to-tinker.com/blog/dreisman/cookies-that-give-you-away-the-surveillance-implications-of-web-tracking/ http://randomwalker.info/publications/cookie-surveillance.pdf
  350. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/the-usa-has-poorest-people-in-world.html http://blogs.reuters.com/felix-salmon/2014/04/04/stop-adding-up-the-wealth-of-the-poor/
  351. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/lighter-faster-cheaper-components-and.html http://www.technologyreview.com/news/525796/cheaper-joints-and-digits-bring-the-robot-revolution-closer/
  352. http://techcrunch.com/2014/04/06/ebay-is-betting-on-bringing-commerce-to-wearables-and-other-connected-devices/
  353. http://mashable.com/2014/03/26/aol-ad-buying-one/
  354. http://www.creativebloq.com/app-design/pepsi-augmented-reality-41411281
  355. http://www.theguardian.com/science/blog/2014/apr/11/resveratrol-wonder-chemical-red-wine-cancer-ageing
  356. http://www.creativebloq.com/netmag/aral-balkan-cyborgs-41411329
  357. http://io9.com/all-the-scientific-reasons-caffeine-is-the-best-drug-ev-1563165385 http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YuJOhpNS0IY http://laughingsquid.com/the-science-of-caffeine-the-worlds-most-popular-drug/
  358. http://mashable.com/2014/04/14/google-buys-titan-aerospace-drones/
  359. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/capitalism-is-not-working-analysis-of.html http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2014/03/31/forces-of-divergence?currentPage=all http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/04/future-capitalism-would-be-saved-if.html
  360. http://news.discovery.com/tech/alternative-power-sources/mach-7-railgun-to-be-tested-at-sea-140424.htm
  361. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/05/nanostructured-ceramic-is-50-harder.html http://www.nrl.navy.mil/media/news-releases/2014/nrl-researchers-develop-harder-ceramic-for-armor-windows http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1359645414000470
  362. http://news.discovery.com/tech/gear-and-gadgets/see-sound-play-out-on-waters-surface-140507.htm https://cymascope.com/cymascope.html http://www.learning-mind.com/this-is-what-the-music-would-look-like-if-we-could-see-it-video/
  363. http://io9.com/why-its-so-hard-to-listen-to-a-song-all-the-way-through-1574047471 http://musicmachinery.com/2014/05/02/the-skip/
  364. http://io9.com/why-its-so-hard-to-listen-to-a-song-all-the-way-through-1574047471 http://musicmachinery.com/2014/05/02/the-skip/
  365. http://news.discovery.com/autos/future-of-transportation/woo-hoo-hoverbike-goes-on-sale-2017-140513.htm http://aerofex.com/ http://spectrum.ieee.org/tag/quadrotors
  366. http://www.theguardian.com/science/2014/may/15/sun-lightning-strikes-earth-solar-wind
  367. http://io9.com/venture-capital-firm-appoints-machine-intelligence-as-b-1576797618 http://betabeat.com/2014/05/v-c-firm-names-robot-to-board-of-directors/ http://www.prweb.com/releases/2014/05/prweb11847458.htm
  368. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/05/darpa-warrior-web-supersuit-projects.html http://www.army.mil/article/125315/DARPA_s_Warrior_Web_project_may_provide_super_human_enhancements/ http://navyseal.s5.com/requirements.html http://nextbigfuture.com/2013/06/a-better-look-at-darpa-warrior-web.html http://www.businessweek.com/news/2014-05-09/novel-prosthetic-arm-from-segway-inventor-approved-by-u-dot-s-dot-fda
  369. http://www.popularmechanics.com/technology/gadgets/news/one-of-the-most-important-materials-in-tech-is-now-recyclable-16795168 http://www.sciencemag.org/content/344/6185/732
  370. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/life-cycle-ideas
  371. http://www.popularmechanics.com/science/health/nueroscience/the-science-of-bruce-lees-one-inch-punch-16814527
  372. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/what-does-it-take-make-meat-stem-cells http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167779914000869 http://www.popsci.com/technology/article/2013-08/first-lab-grown-hamburger-served http://pubs.acs.org/doi/abs/10.1021/es200130u
  373. http://gizmodo.com/new-thermal-technology-turns-the-entire-world-into-a-to-1580098110 http://gizmodo.com/5986008/new-dedicated-ar-chips-could-keep-your-reality-augmented-for-ever-and-ever http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=t1HmYNqp8NM
  374. http://www.cnet.com/news/samsung-could-go-head-to-head-with-oculus-rift-vr-headset http://www.cnet.com/uk/news/sonys-project-morpheus-makes-big-bet-on-body-tracking/ http://www.cnet.com/uk/products/oculus-rift/
  375. http://lifehacker.com/don-t-buy-anything-for-the-first-half-hour-at-a-farmer-1580737339 http://blog.foodnetwork.com/fn-dish/2014/05/geoffrey-zakarians-brunch-and-farmers-market-tips/
  376. http://www.cnet.com/news/google-reportedly-in-talks-to-buy-satellite-startup-skybox/ http://www.skyboximaging.com/
  377. http://io9.com/a-visualization-of-popular-opening-chess-moves-over-tim-1582060419 http://mathworld.wolfram.com/Chess.html http://www.randalolson.com/2014/05/26/a-data-driven-exploration-of-the-evolution-of-chess-popularity-of-openings/
  378. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/06/nasa-niac-solar-electric-space-sail.html http://www.nasa.gov/content/heliopause-electrostatic-rapid-transit-system-herts/#.VEtXvvnF-Sp http://nextbigfuture.com/search/label/electric%20sail
  379. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/06/nasa-niac-solar-electric-space-sail.html http://www.nasa.gov/content/heliopause-electrostatic-rapid-transit-system-herts/#.VEtXvvnF-Sp http://nextbigfuture.com/search/label/electric%20sail
  380. http://www.wired.com/2014/06/the-future-of-computer-intelligence-is-everything-but-artificial/ https://www.pharmgkb.org/
  381. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/technology-behind-2014-world-cup-infographic http://standards.ieee.org/worldcup/
  382. http://io9.com/the-marines-are-building-an-anti-drone-weapon-and-yep-1590595005 https://medium.com/war-is-boring/marines-plan-drone-zapping-laser-weapon-141e7fc66c53 http://www.defenseinnovationmarketplace.mil/resources/16_2013ATIP_GBAD.pdf
  383. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/ http://www.technologyreview.com/summit/14/digital/
  384. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  385. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  386. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  387. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  388. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  389. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  390. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  391. http://singularityhub.com/2014/06/15/our-hyperconnected-future-roundup-of-the-2014-mit-tech-review-digital-summit/
  392. http://www.popsci.com/article/technology/robot-learns-asking-strangers-internet http://homes.cs.washington.edu/~rao/chung2014crowd.pdf
  393. http://io9.com/proof-that-some-infinities-are-bigger-than-others-1597467002
  394. http://nextbigfuture.com/2014/07/4d-movie-theater-showing-of.html http://www.thewrap.com/first-u-s-4d-theater-opens-in-los-angeles-with-transformers-4/ https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/4D_film
  395. http://www.engadget.com/2014/06/25/futuristic-hover-monorail-to-be-tested-in-tel-aviv/
  396. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/science-lightning
  397. http://www.popularmechanics.com/cars/motorcycles/news/we-try-on-the-augmented-reality-motorcycle-helmet-17039038
  398. http://www.forbes.com/sites/peterhigh/2014/08/11/mit-team-turns-6-9-million-clicks-into-insights-to-improve-online-education/ http://www.metisstrategy.com/education-technology-innovation-series-summary-in-forbes/
  399. http://www.techradar.com/us/news/future-tech/world-of-tech/mobile-phones/phone-and-communications/siri-s-inventors-plan-to-one-up-apple-with-a-smarter-self-learning-ai-1261271?src=rss&attr=all http://viv.ai/ http://www.wired.com/2014/08/viv/
  400. http://www.popsci.com/article/science/how-world-wastes-food-infographic
  401. http://www.fastcoexist.com/3033762/the-future-of-food-mapped-out-for-the-next-10-years