SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 11
Baixar para ler offline
Beyond the ‘affect heuristic’: the
emotion-risk assemblage

Deborah Lupton
Department of Sociology and Social Policy, University
of Sydney
BEYOND THE ‘AFFECT HEURISTIC’: THE EMOTION-RISK ASSEMBLAGE




                                   Deborah Lupton



                                         2012




 Conference paper presented at the ‘Risk and Uncertainty Plenary Session’, Australian
         Sociological Association Conference, Brisbane, 28 November 2012




Email: deborah.lupton@gmail.com

Twitter: @DALupton

Blog: ‘This Sociological Life’ http://simplysociology.wordpress.com

University webpage:

http://sydney.edu.au/arts/sociology_social_policy/staff/profiles/deborah_lupton.shtml




                                           1
Abstract
Little sociological research, with the notable exception of that on edgework, has focused
directly on the emotional dimensions of risk rationalities. This space has largely been
occupied by cognitive psychological approaches, particularly the ‘affect heuristic’ model.
In this model, emotion is singled out as separate from and often in opposition to
cognition. Emotional responses to risk are positioned as irrational and potentially
misleading because they are viewed as emerging from the body and not from the mind.
In this paper I argue that the theorising of the emotional dimensions of risk must
recognise their fluid, dynamic and often contradictory and ambivalent nature. I take a
relativist approach to both risk and emotion, and contend that emotion configures risk
and risk configures emotion, and that aspects such as embodiment and location in space
and place are important in these configurations. I propose the concept of the ‘emotion-
risk assemblage’ as a way of acknowledging the contingent, constantly changing and
inextricable aspects of the emotion and risk relationship. This concept avoids the
attempt to position emotion as either rational or irrational, contending instead that it
may better be viewed as one form of thinking.

Introduction
My starting point for this paper is that people’s responses to and assessment of risk are
inevitably imbued with emotion, whether this is experienced at the pre-conscious,
unconscious or conscious levels. To construct an event, individual or object as a ‘risk’ to
oneself and to label it as such is to engage in a process in which these phenomena are
deemed threatening, frightening or dangerous. Such emotions as fear, apprehension,
terror, anger, anxiety, guilt, sadness and disgust, as well as the more positive emotions
of excitement and elation, are associated with ‘risk’. The term ‘risk’ is itself is therefore
culturally coded with emotional meaning, which gives it its resonance and power.
Indeed it is asserted by some social theorists that the concept of ‘risk’ has gained
particular resonance and dominance in late modernity because of a widespread
generalised and low-level anxiety and fear, a sense that we are living in uncertain and
disorienting times, that imminent disaster awaits (Beck 1992, Massumi 1993, Lash et al.
1994, Beck 2009, 2011). These feelings have been exacerbated by the environmental
disasters, global financial crises and terrorist attacks of the early years of this century
that have challenged the efficacy of neoliberal forms of governance in dealing with such
events (Giddens 2009, Dean 2010, Beck 2011).

It is surprising, therefore, that the academic literature, with the notable exception of
psychological research, has rarely sought to engage directly with emotional dimensions
of risk. There are vibrant literatures in both the sociology of emotion and the sociology
of risk, for example, but thus far little engagement between the two. A computer search
of the literature using the keyword ‘risk’ combined with ‘affect’ or ‘emotion’ will
generate a long list of journal articles published in psychology journals and in articles

                                             2
appearing in risk analysis and risk management journals written by psychologists, but
very few in sociology, anthropology or cultural studies.

In the sociological literature is evident that in some ways the pleasurable dimensions of
voluntary risk-taking, the thrills and excitement it may offer as well as the satisfactions
achieved by mastering fear, have been explored in greater detail than have the negative
emotions involved in risk rationalities. A now extensive literature on the sociology of
voluntary risk-taking as part of dangerous activities, or what is alternatively termed
‘edgework’ (Lyng 1990), has uncovered the heightened embodied sensations and
emotions that such activities produce. This research has demonstrated the complexities
of the production and management of emotion in edgework. Edgework involves a
balance between wanting to experience the intense thrill engendered as part of
engaging in dangerous activities but also acknowledging the presence of fear and
seeking to exert mastery over this fear, which in turn heightens feelings of being in
control. The combination of intense emotional arousal and focused attention leads
edgeworkers to experience alterations in perception of time and space and feelings of
hyper-reality which generate their sense of the experience as deeply authentic (see, for
example, Lyng 1990, Lois 2001, Lyng 2005, 2012).

Apart from this literature, the vast bulk of research on risk judgements and assessments
has tended to focus on the intricacies of how people develop and express their
understandings of risk while failing to engage with the emotions, often extremely
intense, produced as part of these rationalities. As Wilkinson (2006) has argued, expert
writing on risk tends to adopt a dispassionate tone, failing to convey the full affective
meaning of the pain, fear, loss and suffering that underpin individuals’ responses to risk.
He asserts that researchers need to examine the cultural meanings related to the threat
of suffering and previous experiences of suffering that influence people’s risk
rationalities: poverty, unemployment, ill-health and disease, disability, pain, crime,
violence, sexual abuse and so on. Hallowell (2006) similarly contends that the
‘abstraction of risk’ from the reality of living with a threat often avoids recognition of
the suffering that is generated from being nominated as ‘at risk’.

The ‘affect heuristic’
Much of the psychological literature on the topic of risk and emotion relates to what has
been termed the ‘affect heuristic’, developed in particular by Paul Slovic and colleagues
(Slovic et al. 2002, Slovic et al. 2004, 2007). The affect heuristic is a model of human
behaviour that has been lauded as one of the most important accomplishments in risk
analysis research in the past thirty years (Greenberg et al. 2012). Proponents of this
model essentially propose that emotion is important in guiding judgements or
decisions, acting as a kind of ‘mental shortcut’ by which these judgements or decisions
can be made quickly. In relation specifically to risk, emotion is positioned as
contributing to a linear process of individuals’ thought processes as part of their
responses to and identification of risks. What is variously described as ‘affect’, ‘feeling’,

                                             3
‘intuition’ or ‘emotion’ is typically viewed as preceding and separate from reason (Slovic
et al. 2007, Slovic and Västfjäll 2010).

This approach therefore continues the distinction between embodied emotional
sensations and cognitive reasoning processes that is typical of other psychological
approaches. Emotion is described as belonging to the ‘experiential system’, which is
characterised as intuitive, fast and as often operating at an unconscious level. It is
contended that what is called the ‘analytic system’, in contrast, uses rational, logical and
mathematical reasoning and normative rules. It is argued that each system operates
parallel to each other and informs each other. This formulation has led to a distinction
between ‘risk as feelings’, ‘risk as analysis’ and ‘risk as politics’. ‘Risk as feelings’
involves the experiential system, ‘risk as analysis’ draws upon the analytic system, while
‘risk as politics’ involves a ‘clash’ between the two systems of response (Slovic et al.
2004) (typically portrayed in the risk analysis literature as differences between the
‘overly emotional’ lay public and the ‘analytic’ expert risk assessors).

This concept of emotion as an inherent instinctive response that distorts rational
judgement stems from a long history in human thought that positions the emotions as
‘primitive’ rather than ‘reasoned’ responses because of their physiological components
(Lupton 1998, Kirman et al. 2010). The approach also assumes that expert judgement is
free of emotional involvement, produced through the rational workings of the mind and
therefore typical of ‘modern scientific thought’ unencumbered by bodily responses such
as emotion. Lay people are emotional, and by implication, influenced by their bodies:
experts are rational, their bodily sensations absent from their judgements of and
responses to risk.

The affect heuristic, in its project to identify the discrete cognitive processes leading to
risk judgements and decisions, does not incorporate the complexities of risk
rationalities. Like other cognitive psychological and psychometric models of risk
assessment and decision-making it attempts to discipline emotion by placing it into a
model or a relationship of cause and effect, rendering it into a variable that may be
measured and its effects calculated. There is little if any space here for recognising the
ambivalences, contradictions and ambiguities of risk rationalities, the movement back
and forward between feeling ‘at risk’ and feeling ‘safe’, the dynamic and heterogeneous
contexts in which risk rationalities are constantly configured and reconfigured.

Sociocultural perspectives on emotion
For writers within sociology, anthropology, philosophy, cultural geography and cultural
studies, emotional responses to risk are conceptualised very differently compared with
psychological models of behaviour. Rather than emotions being understood
predominantly as inherent, involuntary responses to stimuli such as threats or dangers,
they are viewed as themselves social constructions, produced through shared
understandings and past experiences (Lupton 1998, Davidson and Milligan 2004, Thrift

                                             4
2004, Davidson et al. 2008). Emotions are viewed as intensities that flow through and
between individuals and things. They are fluid, relational and highly contextual. They
have histories, building on previous experiences and discussions with others or
collective memories. They have cultures and are located within specific spaces. They are
collective: they are not simply or only personal, individualised experiences but may also
be shared between people, circulating between bodies (Ahmed 2003, 2004). As cultural
geographers writing about emotion have argued, features of space and place are
important in the production and expression of emotional states (Davidson and Milligan
2004, Thrift 2004, Davidson et al. 2008). They claim that emotions are only
understandable, made sense of, in the contexts of particular spaces, in what they term
an ‘emotio-spatial hermeneutic’ (Davidson and Milligan 2004, 524).

So too, a relativist position on risk views it as constructed via sociocultural processes
rather than as a pre-existing entity (Lupton 1999a, 1999b, Fox 2002, Tulloch and
Lupton 2002, van Loon 2002, 2005). Risks are always virtual, in the process of
becoming: they are potentialities, both ‘constructed realities’ and ‘real constructions’
that are comprised of complex networks of materialities, procedures, regulations,
discourses and strategies – and emotions (van Loon 2005, 40). Risk, because it involves
an incipient rather than a realised threat or danger, is about projecting ideas into the
future, about imagining the consequences of an action or event. Risk judgements allow
one to place oneself into a future scenario, when something might go wrong and
threaten one’s health, finances, possessions or wellbeing or that of one’s intimate
others. Phenomena – events, people, social groups, objects – are defined as ‘risks’ when
they are considered to be threatening in some way to an individual or a community. To
call something a ‘risk’ is to draw attention to it and recognise its importance to our
subjectivity and wellbeing (Lupton 1999a, 13).

Judgements about what phenomena should be called ‘risks’ are influenced by the social
and cultural context and by personal experience, including the embodied sensations
that are defined as ‘emotions’. Risk rationalities are not static; they are not necessarily
predictable; they do not necessarily follow predictive models of behaviour; they may
contradict each other. When individuals weigh up risks or decide what a risk is, they are
making assessments of the social meaning of phenomena and their place within cultural
norms. They are deciding how these phenomena cohere with their values about what is
acceptable and harmless against what is dangerous or threatening. They are making
judgements based on affective and aesthetic sensibilities that incorporate such aspects
as personal taste and sense of style, bodily dispositions, awareness of concepts of time
and space, membership of subcultures, unarticulated assumptions, imagination,
intuition and pre-conscious affects and the interpretation of signs and symbols (Lash et
al. 1994, Lupton 1999a, Lash 2000, Lupton and Tulloch 2003, Binkley 2009).

Both ‘emotion’ and ‘risk’ are inevitably embodied because, as Merleau-Ponty reminds
us, we are embodied subjects and experience the world through our bodies and our

                                            5
senses. Our experiences and our judgements are always part of our ‘being-in-the-world’
(Merleau-Ponty 1962). Further, we are constantly relating to and responding to other
bodies, so our embodiment is never individual. Indeed the increased interest in emotion
in cultural geography has been inspired by recent writings on the body. It is argued that
the most intimately felt geography is the body, which is also the site of emotional
sensation and expression. Emotions take place within and around the body as it moves
through space and interacts with other bodies and with objects. Emotions are therefore
located in bodies and in spaces (Davidson and Milligan 2004). Emotions are ‘a crucial
element of the body’s apprehension of the world’ and ‘a vital part of the body’s
anticipation of the moment’ (Thrift 2004, 67).

Part of what the focus on space, place and the production of emotion is able to offer is
recognition of the shifting dynamics that are inherent in the embodied nature of risk
rationalities. Our bodies are constantly in movement and entering and leaving different
spaces and places throughout the day. This approach is also able to incorporate the
material world into understandings of embodiment and subjectivity, including objects
as well as place and space. It emphasises that we interact with different others, whether
they are human or non-human, living or non-living, and our senses are engaged
differently depending on the places we inhabit and the things with which we interact.
Just as emotion is interembodied, so too is risk. Just as emotion is a process, so too is
risk. Both concepts of risk and emotion express moral judgements within a specific
historical, cultural, social and political context. Both emotion and risk are
intersubjective, produced through social relations. They are ways of making sense of
situations, naming responses, part of the diverse cultural meaning systems that we use
to try and understand the world.

This approach does not deny that there are embodied features of emotional experience
or that material dangers do exist in the world that it would be wise to avoid. Rather it
claims that these embodied/material dimensions are always interpreted via social and
cultural lens, predicated on individual past experiences as well as social and spatial
location. We name certain embodied sensations as ‘emotions’ based on our
interpretation of them. So too, we name certain phenomena as ‘risks’ based on our
interpretation. Just as ‘nothing is a risk in itself’ (Ewald 1991) until we label it as such, a
physical sensation is not an emotion until we give it that name.

I suggest, therefore, that rather than referring to an affect heuristic related to risk
perceptions, judgements and decisions, the terminology of the ‘emotion-risk
assemblage’ might be better employed. Drawing on the work of Deleuze and Guattari
and writers in science and technology studies on actor network theory, the concept of
the assemblage incorporates affect as well as a constellation of many other elements:
ideational and material, human and non-human, living and non-living (Marcus 2006).
From this anti-essentialist perspective, both ‘emotion’ (Mulcahy 2012) and ‘risk’ (van
Loon 2002) are viewed as configurations or assemblages of diverse phenomena,

                                              6
including each other. Both emotion and risk interact with each other and in the process,
configure each other. Emotions create risks and risks create emotions. They are each
produced through other material and non-material phenomena: individual and
collective memories and experiences, discourses, practices, objects, space and place,
flesh.

Beyond the rational/irrational distinction
In my final minutes, I want to critique the distinction that is so commonly drawn in
psychological and lay discourses between cognition/reason as rational and
disembodied and emotion as embodied, contaminating or distorting reasoned thought.
In response, some writers have sought to demonstrate that emotional responses may be
considered eminently rational. As some philosophers of emotion have pointed out,
emotional responses may serve as important ways of evaluating situations and of
providing important ethical insights. From this perspective, emotions are a form of
cognition, wisdom and knowledge, frequently involving moral assessments of what is
important to us (Nussbaum 2004), including those related to the acceptability of risk
(Kahan 2008, Roeser 2011). Just as emotions may be viewed as an integral dimensions
of moral evaluations, so too risk judgements can be seen as normative statements of
morality (Rigakos and Law 2009).

These arguments have resonances with those put forward by some proponents of the
affect heuristic model, who claim that what they view as the ‘instinctive’ emotional
response of the ‘experiential system’ may at least sometimes lead to accurate
judgements of risk. These writers do display rather an ambivalent attitude to emotion,
however, particularly when they contend that emotional responses can also be
dangerously ‘misleading’. It seems that in this argument that as long as emotional
responses conform to expert assessments of risk then they are ‘accurate’ or
‘appropriate’: if not, they are ‘manipulative’ or ‘deceptive’ (Slovic et al. 2007), producing
the ‘clashes’ that are described as eventuating in the ‘risk as politics’ formulation. So too,
some of the philosophers of emotion referred to above who argue that emotion may
provide important ethical insights also claim that some emotional responses (such as
disgust) are unjustly stigmatising to certain social groups and therefore should be
challenged as to their appropriateness (Nussbaum 2004).

I would question why it is important to define the rationality or otherwise of emotional
responses or, by association, risk judgements that are deemed to be influenced by
emotion. I would go even further by suggesting that such a distinction is difficult, if not
impossible, if we acknowledge the socially contextualised, dynamic, shared,
heterogeneous and often contradictory and ambivalent nature of any kind of human
response to the world. If we accept that our responses are always subjective and
constructed through culture, is it even worthwhile or productive to attempt to identify
emotion as separate from rationality or cognition, given, as I have argued, that these are
intertwined. Can a risk judgement ever be purely dispassionate, with no ‘contaminating’

                                              7
emotions affecting it? Particularly if it is accepted that our responses to the world are at
least partly shaped by unconscious or pre-conscious affect, then are we ever able to
access or be fully aware of all the emotional elements of our responses? Just as all
thought and reason is inevitably emotional (Ahmed 2003), just as science itself is
essentially and inevitably an emotional enterprise (despite dominant counter-claims to
the contrary) (Alderman et al. 2010, Pickersgill 2012), so too is everyday experience.
From the perspective I want to take on emotion and risk, risk cannot separately be
categorised as ‘feelings’ or ‘analysis’, as claimed by Slovic and colleagues (Slovic et al.
2004). Risk is both of these simultaneously.

I do not wish to engage in the argument that we should view emotional responses as
somehow just as rational or ‘truthful’ (or even more so) than cognition and hence
attempt to give them a sense of legitimacy that is so often denied them in risk analysis
discourses. This argument simply preserves the separation of embodiment and
cognition and the privileging of rationality. Because I want to avoid the
rational/irrational distinction, I do preserve the arguments reviewed above that
highlight emotion as relating to moral judgement and evaluation, a form of ethical
reflection.

Finally, I agree with Thrift’s suggestion that emotional response is ‘a form of thinking,
often indirect and non-reflective, it is true, but thinking all the same’ (2004, 60). This is
partly what the exponents of the affect heuristic are trying to get at in their argument
for the value of emotion (at least sometimes) in making risk judgements and decisions.
Unlike them, however, Thrift is very insistent on the point that emotions are neither
irrational nor sublime, but instead simply ‘a different type of intelligence about the
world’ (2004, 60). This concept of emotion, I would suggest, offers a productive way
forward in thinking about the emotion-risk assemblage.




                                             8
Ahmed, S., 2003. The politics of fear in the making of worlds. International journal of qualitative
          studies in education, 16 (3), 377-398.
Ahmed, S., 2004. Affective economies. Social text, 22 (2), 117-139.
Alderman, J., Dollar, K.M. and Kozlowski, L.T., 2010. Understanding the origins of anger, contempt,
          and disgust in public health policy disputes: Applying moral psychology to harm reduction
          debates. Journal of public health policy, 31 (1), 1--16.
Beck, U., 1992. Risk society: Towards a new modernity. London: Sage.
Beck, U., 2009. World at risk. Cambridge: Polity.
Beck, U., 2011. Cosmopolitanism as imagined communities of global risk. American behavioral
          scientist, 55 (10), 1346-1361.
Binkley, S., 2009. Governmentality, temporality and practice: From the individualization of risk to the
          'contradictory movements of the soul'. Time & Society, 18 (1), 86-105.
Davidson, J. and Milligan, C., 2004. Embodying emotion sensing space: Introducing emotional
          geographies. Social & cultural geography, 5 (4), 523-532.
Davidson, J., et al., 2008. Emotion, space and society: Editorial introduction. Emotion, space and
          society, 1 (1), 1-3.
Dean, M., 2010. Power at the heart of the present: Exception, risk and sovereignty. European journal
          of cultural studies, 13 (4), 459-475.
Ewald, F., 1991. Insurance and risks. In G. Burchell, C. Gordon & P. Miller (eds.) The foucault effect:
          Studies in governmentality. London: Harvester/Wheatsheaf, 197-210.
Fox, N.J., 2002. What a 'risky' body can do: Why people's health choices are not all based in
          evidence. Health education journal, 61 (2), 166-179.
Giddens, A., 2009. The politics of climate change. Cambridge: Polity.
Greenberg, M., et al., 2012. Ten most important accomplishments in risk analysis, 1980-2010. Risk
          analysis, 32 (5), 771-781.
Hallowell, N., 2006. Varieties of suffering: Living with the risk of ovarian cancer. Health, risk &
          society, 8 (1), 9-26.
Kahan, D.M., 2008. Two conceptions of emotion in risk regulation. University of Pennsylvania law
          review, 156 (3), 741-766.
Kirman, A., Livet, P. and Teschl, M., 2010. Rationality and emotions. Philosophical transactions of the
          Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 365 (1538), 215-219.
Lash, S., 2000. Risk culture. In B. Adam, U. Beck & J. Van Loon (eds.) The risk society and beyond:
          Critical issues for social theory. London: Sage, 47-62.
Lash, S., Giddens, A. and Beck, U., 1994. Reflexive modernization: Politics, tradition and aesthetics in
          the modern social order. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Lois, J., 2001. Peaks and valleys: The gendered emotional culture of edgework. Gender and society,
          15 (3), 381-406.
Lupton, D., 1998. The emotional self: A sociocultural exploration. London: Sage.
Lupton, D., 1999a. Risk. London: Routledge.
Lupton, D. (ed.) (1999b) Risk and sociocultural theory: New directions and perspectives, Cambridge:
          Cambridge University Press.
Lupton, D. and Tulloch, J., 2003. Risk and everyday life. London: Sage.
Lyng, S., 1990. Edgework: A social psychological analysis of voluntary risk taking. American journal of
          sociology, 95 (4), 851-886.
Lyng, S., 2005. Edgework: The sociology of risk taking. New York: Routledge.
Lyng, S., 2012. Existential transcendence in late modernity: Edgework and hermeneutic reflexivity.
          Human studies, 35 (3), 401-414.
Marcus, G., 2006. Assemblage. Theory, culture & society, 23 (2-3), 101-106.



                                                   9
Massumi, B., 1993. Everywhere you want to be: Introduction to fear. In B. Massumi (ed.) The politics
         of everyday fear. Minneapolis, Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press, 3-38.
Merleau-Ponty, M., 1962. Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Mulcahy, D., 2012. Affective assemblages: Body matters in the pedagogic practices of contemporary
         school classrooms. Pedagogy, culture and society, 20 (1), 9-27.
Nussbaum, M., 2004. Hiding from humanity: Disgust, shame, and the law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
         University Press.
Pickersgill, M., 2012. The co-production of science, ethics, and emotion. Science, technology &
         human values, 37 (6), 579-603.
Rigakos, G.S. and Law, A., 2009. Risk, realism and the politics of resistance. Critical sociology, 35 (1),
         79-103.
Roeser, S., 2011. Nuclear energy, risk, and emotions. Philosophy & technology, 24 (2), 197-201.
Slovic, P., et al., 2002. Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for
         behavioral economics. Journal of socio-economics, 31 (4), 329-342.
Slovic, P., et al., 2004. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk,
         and rationality. Risk analysis, 24 (2), 311-322.
Slovic, P., et al., 2007. The affect heuristic. European journal of operational research, 177 (3), 1333-
         1352.
Slovic, P. and Västfjäll, D., 2010. Affect, moral intuition, and risk. Psychological inquiry, 21 (4), 387-
         398.
Thrift, N., 2004. Intensities of feeling: Towards a spatial politics of affect. Geografiska annaler, 86 B
         (1), 57-78.
Tulloch, J. and Lupton, D., 2002. Risk is part of your life': Risk epistemologies among a group of
         australians. Sociology, 36 (2), 317-334.
Van Loon, J., 2002. Risk and technological culture: Towards a sociology of virulence. London:
         Routledge.
Van Loon, J., 2005. Epidemic space. Critical public health, 15 (1), 39-52.
Wilkinson, I., 2006. Health, risk and ‘social suffering. Health, risk & society, 8 (1), 1-8.




                                                    10

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Care and social work
Care and social workCare and social work
Care and social workStephen Webb
 
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature Review
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature ReviewSelf-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature Review
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature ReviewKeshav Sharma
 
On the Transference and the Counter-Transference
On the Transference and the Counter-TransferenceOn the Transference and the Counter-Transference
On the Transference and the Counter-TransferenceShiva Kumar Srinivasan
 
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'Shiva Kumar Srinivasan
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016Daniel Horsley
 
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up point
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up pointCrimes against humanity attempted cover up point
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up pointMichael Sorrentino
 
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes Series
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes SeriesOn Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes Series
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes SeriesShiva Kumar Srinivasan
 
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 Conference
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 ConferenceHarrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 Conference
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 ConferenceShelly Harrell
 
CFT paper final
CFT paper finalCFT paper final
CFT paper finalKate Lucre
 
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...KJSNA
 
Bipolar disorder and social cognition
Bipolar disorder and social cognitionBipolar disorder and social cognition
Bipolar disorder and social cognitionrockmahadev
 
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...UniversitasGadjahMada
 

Mais procurados (18)

Care and social work
Care and social workCare and social work
Care and social work
 
Visdumb
VisdumbVisdumb
Visdumb
 
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature Review
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature ReviewSelf-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature Review
Self-fulfilling Prophecy-A Literature Review
 
journal.pone.0128451
journal.pone.0128451journal.pone.0128451
journal.pone.0128451
 
On the Transference and the Counter-Transference
On the Transference and the Counter-TransferenceOn the Transference and the Counter-Transference
On the Transference and the Counter-Transference
 
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'
Lacanians on 'Identity and Identification'
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
 
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up point
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up pointCrimes against humanity attempted cover up point
Crimes against humanity attempted cover up point
 
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes Series
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes SeriesOn Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes Series
On Clinical Techniques in Freud and Lacan, Clinical Notes Series
 
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 Conference
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 ConferenceHarrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 Conference
Harrell - Practicing PEaCE- UCLA IPNB 2016 Conference
 
CFT paper final
CFT paper finalCFT paper final
CFT paper final
 
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...
Queens university panel celebrating jaspers general psychopathology prof greg...
 
Health and foreign policy in question: the case of humanitarian action
Health and foreign policy in question: the case of  humanitarian actionHealth and foreign policy in question: the case of  humanitarian action
Health and foreign policy in question: the case of humanitarian action
 
Communication theory
Communication theoryCommunication theory
Communication theory
 
J0371063076
J0371063076J0371063076
J0371063076
 
Bipolar disorder and social cognition
Bipolar disorder and social cognitionBipolar disorder and social cognition
Bipolar disorder and social cognition
 
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...
Affective Reactions Among Students Belonging to Ethnic Groups Engaged in Prio...
 
Mpq SCALE
Mpq SCALEMpq SCALE
Mpq SCALE
 

Semelhante a Beyond the 'affect heuristic': the emotion-risk assemblage

reaction paper ppt on risk perception.ppt
reaction paper ppt on risk perception.pptreaction paper ppt on risk perception.ppt
reaction paper ppt on risk perception.pptDaniWondimeDers
 
Motivation and Emotions
Motivation and EmotionsMotivation and Emotions
Motivation and EmotionsShiva Kakkar
 
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesAutocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesCátia Rodrigues
 
PICI Research Paper
PICI Research PaperPICI Research Paper
PICI Research PaperDiana Rusnov
 
Theories of Personality
Theories of PersonalityTheories of Personality
Theories of PersonalityShantanu Basu
 
Interpretevism vs positivism
Interpretevism vs positivismInterpretevism vs positivism
Interpretevism vs positivismProfessorkhurram
 
Social Cognitive Model
Social Cognitive ModelSocial Cognitive Model
Social Cognitive ModelJenny Smith
 
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...Maciej Behnke
 
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional Perspectives
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional PerspectivesExploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional Perspectives
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional PerspectivesDaniel Faierman
 
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docx
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docxClassics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docx
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docxbartholomeocoombs
 
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityThe Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityAngie Lee
 
Anthropology Of Mental Illness
Anthropology Of Mental IllnessAnthropology Of Mental Illness
Anthropology Of Mental IllnessLisa Brewer
 
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...Rielo Institute for Integral Development
 
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...JohnJulie1
 
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...suppubs1pubs1
 

Semelhante a Beyond the 'affect heuristic': the emotion-risk assemblage (20)

reaction paper ppt on risk perception.ppt
reaction paper ppt on risk perception.pptreaction paper ppt on risk perception.ppt
reaction paper ppt on risk perception.ppt
 
Social Psychology
Social PsychologySocial Psychology
Social Psychology
 
Social Psychology And Social Influence
Social Psychology And Social InfluenceSocial Psychology And Social Influence
Social Psychology And Social Influence
 
Motivation and Emotions
Motivation and EmotionsMotivation and Emotions
Motivation and Emotions
 
Psychopathy and Empathy
Psychopathy and EmpathyPsychopathy and Empathy
Psychopathy and Empathy
 
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesAutocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
 
PICI Research Paper
PICI Research PaperPICI Research Paper
PICI Research Paper
 
Theories of Personality
Theories of PersonalityTheories of Personality
Theories of Personality
 
Interpretevism vs positivism
Interpretevism vs positivismInterpretevism vs positivism
Interpretevism vs positivism
 
Social Cognitive Model
Social Cognitive ModelSocial Cognitive Model
Social Cognitive Model
 
philosopy power point.pptx
philosopy power point.pptxphilosopy power point.pptx
philosopy power point.pptx
 
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...
Splitting the affective atom: Divergence of valence and approach-avoidance mo...
 
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional Perspectives
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional PerspectivesExploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional Perspectives
Exploring Social Contagion from Behavioral and Emotional Perspectives
 
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docx
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docxClassics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docx
Classics in the History of Psychology Aninternet resource .docx
 
PTSD GD 7122016
PTSD GD 7122016PTSD GD 7122016
PTSD GD 7122016
 
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityThe Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
 
Anthropology Of Mental Illness
Anthropology Of Mental IllnessAnthropology Of Mental Illness
Anthropology Of Mental Illness
 
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...
Integral assistance to refugees: an anthropological approach - Dr. M. Lafuent...
 
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
 
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
Cognitive Psychology: Investigation of Interpersonal Relationships with Self-...
 

Mais de Deborah Lupton

Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital Data
Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital DataData Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital Data
Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital DataDeborah Lupton
 
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?Deborah Lupton
 
Critical digital health studies: a research agenda
Critical digital health studies: a research agendaCritical digital health studies: a research agenda
Critical digital health studies: a research agendaDeborah Lupton
 
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...Deborah Lupton
 
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagement
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagementSociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagement
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagementDeborah Lupton
 
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online world
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online worldDigital natives or digital victims: children and the online world
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online worldDeborah Lupton
 
Critical digital health: promises and limitations
Critical digital health: promises and limitationsCritical digital health: promises and limitations
Critical digital health: promises and limitationsDeborah Lupton
 
Open access for academics
Open access for academicsOpen access for academics
Open access for academicsDeborah Lupton
 
Critical studies of digital health
Critical studies of digital healthCritical studies of digital health
Critical studies of digital healthDeborah Lupton
 
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for Academics
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for AcademicsThe Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for Academics
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for AcademicsDeborah Lupton
 
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?Deborah Lupton
 
Thinking about Food and Embodiment
Thinking about Food and EmbodimentThinking about Food and Embodiment
Thinking about Food and EmbodimentDeborah Lupton
 
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopolitics
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopoliticsConfiguring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopolitics
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopoliticsDeborah Lupton
 
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance society
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance societyM-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance society
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance societyDeborah Lupton
 

Mais de Deborah Lupton (16)

Digital risk society
Digital risk societyDigital risk society
Digital risk society
 
Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital Data
Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital DataData Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital Data
Data Sense: People's Engagement with Their Personal Digital Data
 
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?
Live, punk, hacker and digital sociology: shaking up the discipline?
 
Critical digital health studies: a research agenda
Critical digital health studies: a research agendaCritical digital health studies: a research agenda
Critical digital health studies: a research agenda
 
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...
Data gone wild? The quantified self assemblage, technologies of the self and ...
 
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagement
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagementSociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagement
Sociological sensibility and the politics of digital engagement
 
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online world
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online worldDigital natives or digital victims: children and the online world
Digital natives or digital victims: children and the online world
 
Critical digital health: promises and limitations
Critical digital health: promises and limitationsCritical digital health: promises and limitations
Critical digital health: promises and limitations
 
Open access for academics
Open access for academicsOpen access for academics
Open access for academics
 
Critical studies of digital health
Critical studies of digital healthCritical studies of digital health
Critical studies of digital health
 
The Sociology of Fat
The Sociology of FatThe Sociology of Fat
The Sociology of Fat
 
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for Academics
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for AcademicsThe Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for Academics
The Digital Academic: Social and Other Digital Media for Academics
 
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?
Why are fat bodies so stigmatised?
 
Thinking about Food and Embodiment
Thinking about Food and EmbodimentThinking about Food and Embodiment
Thinking about Food and Embodiment
 
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopolitics
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopoliticsConfiguring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopolitics
Configuring maternal, foetal and infant embodiment in the context of biopolitics
 
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance society
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance societyM-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance society
M-health technologies: configuring bodies and health in surveillance society
 

Último

Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Pooja Nehwal
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 

Último (20)

Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 

Beyond the 'affect heuristic': the emotion-risk assemblage

  • 1. Beyond the ‘affect heuristic’: the emotion-risk assemblage Deborah Lupton Department of Sociology and Social Policy, University of Sydney
  • 2. BEYOND THE ‘AFFECT HEURISTIC’: THE EMOTION-RISK ASSEMBLAGE Deborah Lupton 2012 Conference paper presented at the ‘Risk and Uncertainty Plenary Session’, Australian Sociological Association Conference, Brisbane, 28 November 2012 Email: deborah.lupton@gmail.com Twitter: @DALupton Blog: ‘This Sociological Life’ http://simplysociology.wordpress.com University webpage: http://sydney.edu.au/arts/sociology_social_policy/staff/profiles/deborah_lupton.shtml 1
  • 3. Abstract Little sociological research, with the notable exception of that on edgework, has focused directly on the emotional dimensions of risk rationalities. This space has largely been occupied by cognitive psychological approaches, particularly the ‘affect heuristic’ model. In this model, emotion is singled out as separate from and often in opposition to cognition. Emotional responses to risk are positioned as irrational and potentially misleading because they are viewed as emerging from the body and not from the mind. In this paper I argue that the theorising of the emotional dimensions of risk must recognise their fluid, dynamic and often contradictory and ambivalent nature. I take a relativist approach to both risk and emotion, and contend that emotion configures risk and risk configures emotion, and that aspects such as embodiment and location in space and place are important in these configurations. I propose the concept of the ‘emotion- risk assemblage’ as a way of acknowledging the contingent, constantly changing and inextricable aspects of the emotion and risk relationship. This concept avoids the attempt to position emotion as either rational or irrational, contending instead that it may better be viewed as one form of thinking. Introduction My starting point for this paper is that people’s responses to and assessment of risk are inevitably imbued with emotion, whether this is experienced at the pre-conscious, unconscious or conscious levels. To construct an event, individual or object as a ‘risk’ to oneself and to label it as such is to engage in a process in which these phenomena are deemed threatening, frightening or dangerous. Such emotions as fear, apprehension, terror, anger, anxiety, guilt, sadness and disgust, as well as the more positive emotions of excitement and elation, are associated with ‘risk’. The term ‘risk’ is itself is therefore culturally coded with emotional meaning, which gives it its resonance and power. Indeed it is asserted by some social theorists that the concept of ‘risk’ has gained particular resonance and dominance in late modernity because of a widespread generalised and low-level anxiety and fear, a sense that we are living in uncertain and disorienting times, that imminent disaster awaits (Beck 1992, Massumi 1993, Lash et al. 1994, Beck 2009, 2011). These feelings have been exacerbated by the environmental disasters, global financial crises and terrorist attacks of the early years of this century that have challenged the efficacy of neoliberal forms of governance in dealing with such events (Giddens 2009, Dean 2010, Beck 2011). It is surprising, therefore, that the academic literature, with the notable exception of psychological research, has rarely sought to engage directly with emotional dimensions of risk. There are vibrant literatures in both the sociology of emotion and the sociology of risk, for example, but thus far little engagement between the two. A computer search of the literature using the keyword ‘risk’ combined with ‘affect’ or ‘emotion’ will generate a long list of journal articles published in psychology journals and in articles 2
  • 4. appearing in risk analysis and risk management journals written by psychologists, but very few in sociology, anthropology or cultural studies. In the sociological literature is evident that in some ways the pleasurable dimensions of voluntary risk-taking, the thrills and excitement it may offer as well as the satisfactions achieved by mastering fear, have been explored in greater detail than have the negative emotions involved in risk rationalities. A now extensive literature on the sociology of voluntary risk-taking as part of dangerous activities, or what is alternatively termed ‘edgework’ (Lyng 1990), has uncovered the heightened embodied sensations and emotions that such activities produce. This research has demonstrated the complexities of the production and management of emotion in edgework. Edgework involves a balance between wanting to experience the intense thrill engendered as part of engaging in dangerous activities but also acknowledging the presence of fear and seeking to exert mastery over this fear, which in turn heightens feelings of being in control. The combination of intense emotional arousal and focused attention leads edgeworkers to experience alterations in perception of time and space and feelings of hyper-reality which generate their sense of the experience as deeply authentic (see, for example, Lyng 1990, Lois 2001, Lyng 2005, 2012). Apart from this literature, the vast bulk of research on risk judgements and assessments has tended to focus on the intricacies of how people develop and express their understandings of risk while failing to engage with the emotions, often extremely intense, produced as part of these rationalities. As Wilkinson (2006) has argued, expert writing on risk tends to adopt a dispassionate tone, failing to convey the full affective meaning of the pain, fear, loss and suffering that underpin individuals’ responses to risk. He asserts that researchers need to examine the cultural meanings related to the threat of suffering and previous experiences of suffering that influence people’s risk rationalities: poverty, unemployment, ill-health and disease, disability, pain, crime, violence, sexual abuse and so on. Hallowell (2006) similarly contends that the ‘abstraction of risk’ from the reality of living with a threat often avoids recognition of the suffering that is generated from being nominated as ‘at risk’. The ‘affect heuristic’ Much of the psychological literature on the topic of risk and emotion relates to what has been termed the ‘affect heuristic’, developed in particular by Paul Slovic and colleagues (Slovic et al. 2002, Slovic et al. 2004, 2007). The affect heuristic is a model of human behaviour that has been lauded as one of the most important accomplishments in risk analysis research in the past thirty years (Greenberg et al. 2012). Proponents of this model essentially propose that emotion is important in guiding judgements or decisions, acting as a kind of ‘mental shortcut’ by which these judgements or decisions can be made quickly. In relation specifically to risk, emotion is positioned as contributing to a linear process of individuals’ thought processes as part of their responses to and identification of risks. What is variously described as ‘affect’, ‘feeling’, 3
  • 5. ‘intuition’ or ‘emotion’ is typically viewed as preceding and separate from reason (Slovic et al. 2007, Slovic and Västfjäll 2010). This approach therefore continues the distinction between embodied emotional sensations and cognitive reasoning processes that is typical of other psychological approaches. Emotion is described as belonging to the ‘experiential system’, which is characterised as intuitive, fast and as often operating at an unconscious level. It is contended that what is called the ‘analytic system’, in contrast, uses rational, logical and mathematical reasoning and normative rules. It is argued that each system operates parallel to each other and informs each other. This formulation has led to a distinction between ‘risk as feelings’, ‘risk as analysis’ and ‘risk as politics’. ‘Risk as feelings’ involves the experiential system, ‘risk as analysis’ draws upon the analytic system, while ‘risk as politics’ involves a ‘clash’ between the two systems of response (Slovic et al. 2004) (typically portrayed in the risk analysis literature as differences between the ‘overly emotional’ lay public and the ‘analytic’ expert risk assessors). This concept of emotion as an inherent instinctive response that distorts rational judgement stems from a long history in human thought that positions the emotions as ‘primitive’ rather than ‘reasoned’ responses because of their physiological components (Lupton 1998, Kirman et al. 2010). The approach also assumes that expert judgement is free of emotional involvement, produced through the rational workings of the mind and therefore typical of ‘modern scientific thought’ unencumbered by bodily responses such as emotion. Lay people are emotional, and by implication, influenced by their bodies: experts are rational, their bodily sensations absent from their judgements of and responses to risk. The affect heuristic, in its project to identify the discrete cognitive processes leading to risk judgements and decisions, does not incorporate the complexities of risk rationalities. Like other cognitive psychological and psychometric models of risk assessment and decision-making it attempts to discipline emotion by placing it into a model or a relationship of cause and effect, rendering it into a variable that may be measured and its effects calculated. There is little if any space here for recognising the ambivalences, contradictions and ambiguities of risk rationalities, the movement back and forward between feeling ‘at risk’ and feeling ‘safe’, the dynamic and heterogeneous contexts in which risk rationalities are constantly configured and reconfigured. Sociocultural perspectives on emotion For writers within sociology, anthropology, philosophy, cultural geography and cultural studies, emotional responses to risk are conceptualised very differently compared with psychological models of behaviour. Rather than emotions being understood predominantly as inherent, involuntary responses to stimuli such as threats or dangers, they are viewed as themselves social constructions, produced through shared understandings and past experiences (Lupton 1998, Davidson and Milligan 2004, Thrift 4
  • 6. 2004, Davidson et al. 2008). Emotions are viewed as intensities that flow through and between individuals and things. They are fluid, relational and highly contextual. They have histories, building on previous experiences and discussions with others or collective memories. They have cultures and are located within specific spaces. They are collective: they are not simply or only personal, individualised experiences but may also be shared between people, circulating between bodies (Ahmed 2003, 2004). As cultural geographers writing about emotion have argued, features of space and place are important in the production and expression of emotional states (Davidson and Milligan 2004, Thrift 2004, Davidson et al. 2008). They claim that emotions are only understandable, made sense of, in the contexts of particular spaces, in what they term an ‘emotio-spatial hermeneutic’ (Davidson and Milligan 2004, 524). So too, a relativist position on risk views it as constructed via sociocultural processes rather than as a pre-existing entity (Lupton 1999a, 1999b, Fox 2002, Tulloch and Lupton 2002, van Loon 2002, 2005). Risks are always virtual, in the process of becoming: they are potentialities, both ‘constructed realities’ and ‘real constructions’ that are comprised of complex networks of materialities, procedures, regulations, discourses and strategies – and emotions (van Loon 2005, 40). Risk, because it involves an incipient rather than a realised threat or danger, is about projecting ideas into the future, about imagining the consequences of an action or event. Risk judgements allow one to place oneself into a future scenario, when something might go wrong and threaten one’s health, finances, possessions or wellbeing or that of one’s intimate others. Phenomena – events, people, social groups, objects – are defined as ‘risks’ when they are considered to be threatening in some way to an individual or a community. To call something a ‘risk’ is to draw attention to it and recognise its importance to our subjectivity and wellbeing (Lupton 1999a, 13). Judgements about what phenomena should be called ‘risks’ are influenced by the social and cultural context and by personal experience, including the embodied sensations that are defined as ‘emotions’. Risk rationalities are not static; they are not necessarily predictable; they do not necessarily follow predictive models of behaviour; they may contradict each other. When individuals weigh up risks or decide what a risk is, they are making assessments of the social meaning of phenomena and their place within cultural norms. They are deciding how these phenomena cohere with their values about what is acceptable and harmless against what is dangerous or threatening. They are making judgements based on affective and aesthetic sensibilities that incorporate such aspects as personal taste and sense of style, bodily dispositions, awareness of concepts of time and space, membership of subcultures, unarticulated assumptions, imagination, intuition and pre-conscious affects and the interpretation of signs and symbols (Lash et al. 1994, Lupton 1999a, Lash 2000, Lupton and Tulloch 2003, Binkley 2009). Both ‘emotion’ and ‘risk’ are inevitably embodied because, as Merleau-Ponty reminds us, we are embodied subjects and experience the world through our bodies and our 5
  • 7. senses. Our experiences and our judgements are always part of our ‘being-in-the-world’ (Merleau-Ponty 1962). Further, we are constantly relating to and responding to other bodies, so our embodiment is never individual. Indeed the increased interest in emotion in cultural geography has been inspired by recent writings on the body. It is argued that the most intimately felt geography is the body, which is also the site of emotional sensation and expression. Emotions take place within and around the body as it moves through space and interacts with other bodies and with objects. Emotions are therefore located in bodies and in spaces (Davidson and Milligan 2004). Emotions are ‘a crucial element of the body’s apprehension of the world’ and ‘a vital part of the body’s anticipation of the moment’ (Thrift 2004, 67). Part of what the focus on space, place and the production of emotion is able to offer is recognition of the shifting dynamics that are inherent in the embodied nature of risk rationalities. Our bodies are constantly in movement and entering and leaving different spaces and places throughout the day. This approach is also able to incorporate the material world into understandings of embodiment and subjectivity, including objects as well as place and space. It emphasises that we interact with different others, whether they are human or non-human, living or non-living, and our senses are engaged differently depending on the places we inhabit and the things with which we interact. Just as emotion is interembodied, so too is risk. Just as emotion is a process, so too is risk. Both concepts of risk and emotion express moral judgements within a specific historical, cultural, social and political context. Both emotion and risk are intersubjective, produced through social relations. They are ways of making sense of situations, naming responses, part of the diverse cultural meaning systems that we use to try and understand the world. This approach does not deny that there are embodied features of emotional experience or that material dangers do exist in the world that it would be wise to avoid. Rather it claims that these embodied/material dimensions are always interpreted via social and cultural lens, predicated on individual past experiences as well as social and spatial location. We name certain embodied sensations as ‘emotions’ based on our interpretation of them. So too, we name certain phenomena as ‘risks’ based on our interpretation. Just as ‘nothing is a risk in itself’ (Ewald 1991) until we label it as such, a physical sensation is not an emotion until we give it that name. I suggest, therefore, that rather than referring to an affect heuristic related to risk perceptions, judgements and decisions, the terminology of the ‘emotion-risk assemblage’ might be better employed. Drawing on the work of Deleuze and Guattari and writers in science and technology studies on actor network theory, the concept of the assemblage incorporates affect as well as a constellation of many other elements: ideational and material, human and non-human, living and non-living (Marcus 2006). From this anti-essentialist perspective, both ‘emotion’ (Mulcahy 2012) and ‘risk’ (van Loon 2002) are viewed as configurations or assemblages of diverse phenomena, 6
  • 8. including each other. Both emotion and risk interact with each other and in the process, configure each other. Emotions create risks and risks create emotions. They are each produced through other material and non-material phenomena: individual and collective memories and experiences, discourses, practices, objects, space and place, flesh. Beyond the rational/irrational distinction In my final minutes, I want to critique the distinction that is so commonly drawn in psychological and lay discourses between cognition/reason as rational and disembodied and emotion as embodied, contaminating or distorting reasoned thought. In response, some writers have sought to demonstrate that emotional responses may be considered eminently rational. As some philosophers of emotion have pointed out, emotional responses may serve as important ways of evaluating situations and of providing important ethical insights. From this perspective, emotions are a form of cognition, wisdom and knowledge, frequently involving moral assessments of what is important to us (Nussbaum 2004), including those related to the acceptability of risk (Kahan 2008, Roeser 2011). Just as emotions may be viewed as an integral dimensions of moral evaluations, so too risk judgements can be seen as normative statements of morality (Rigakos and Law 2009). These arguments have resonances with those put forward by some proponents of the affect heuristic model, who claim that what they view as the ‘instinctive’ emotional response of the ‘experiential system’ may at least sometimes lead to accurate judgements of risk. These writers do display rather an ambivalent attitude to emotion, however, particularly when they contend that emotional responses can also be dangerously ‘misleading’. It seems that in this argument that as long as emotional responses conform to expert assessments of risk then they are ‘accurate’ or ‘appropriate’: if not, they are ‘manipulative’ or ‘deceptive’ (Slovic et al. 2007), producing the ‘clashes’ that are described as eventuating in the ‘risk as politics’ formulation. So too, some of the philosophers of emotion referred to above who argue that emotion may provide important ethical insights also claim that some emotional responses (such as disgust) are unjustly stigmatising to certain social groups and therefore should be challenged as to their appropriateness (Nussbaum 2004). I would question why it is important to define the rationality or otherwise of emotional responses or, by association, risk judgements that are deemed to be influenced by emotion. I would go even further by suggesting that such a distinction is difficult, if not impossible, if we acknowledge the socially contextualised, dynamic, shared, heterogeneous and often contradictory and ambivalent nature of any kind of human response to the world. If we accept that our responses are always subjective and constructed through culture, is it even worthwhile or productive to attempt to identify emotion as separate from rationality or cognition, given, as I have argued, that these are intertwined. Can a risk judgement ever be purely dispassionate, with no ‘contaminating’ 7
  • 9. emotions affecting it? Particularly if it is accepted that our responses to the world are at least partly shaped by unconscious or pre-conscious affect, then are we ever able to access or be fully aware of all the emotional elements of our responses? Just as all thought and reason is inevitably emotional (Ahmed 2003), just as science itself is essentially and inevitably an emotional enterprise (despite dominant counter-claims to the contrary) (Alderman et al. 2010, Pickersgill 2012), so too is everyday experience. From the perspective I want to take on emotion and risk, risk cannot separately be categorised as ‘feelings’ or ‘analysis’, as claimed by Slovic and colleagues (Slovic et al. 2004). Risk is both of these simultaneously. I do not wish to engage in the argument that we should view emotional responses as somehow just as rational or ‘truthful’ (or even more so) than cognition and hence attempt to give them a sense of legitimacy that is so often denied them in risk analysis discourses. This argument simply preserves the separation of embodiment and cognition and the privileging of rationality. Because I want to avoid the rational/irrational distinction, I do preserve the arguments reviewed above that highlight emotion as relating to moral judgement and evaluation, a form of ethical reflection. Finally, I agree with Thrift’s suggestion that emotional response is ‘a form of thinking, often indirect and non-reflective, it is true, but thinking all the same’ (2004, 60). This is partly what the exponents of the affect heuristic are trying to get at in their argument for the value of emotion (at least sometimes) in making risk judgements and decisions. Unlike them, however, Thrift is very insistent on the point that emotions are neither irrational nor sublime, but instead simply ‘a different type of intelligence about the world’ (2004, 60). This concept of emotion, I would suggest, offers a productive way forward in thinking about the emotion-risk assemblage. 8
  • 10. Ahmed, S., 2003. The politics of fear in the making of worlds. International journal of qualitative studies in education, 16 (3), 377-398. Ahmed, S., 2004. Affective economies. Social text, 22 (2), 117-139. Alderman, J., Dollar, K.M. and Kozlowski, L.T., 2010. Understanding the origins of anger, contempt, and disgust in public health policy disputes: Applying moral psychology to harm reduction debates. Journal of public health policy, 31 (1), 1--16. Beck, U., 1992. Risk society: Towards a new modernity. London: Sage. Beck, U., 2009. World at risk. Cambridge: Polity. Beck, U., 2011. Cosmopolitanism as imagined communities of global risk. American behavioral scientist, 55 (10), 1346-1361. Binkley, S., 2009. Governmentality, temporality and practice: From the individualization of risk to the 'contradictory movements of the soul'. Time & Society, 18 (1), 86-105. Davidson, J. and Milligan, C., 2004. Embodying emotion sensing space: Introducing emotional geographies. Social & cultural geography, 5 (4), 523-532. Davidson, J., et al., 2008. Emotion, space and society: Editorial introduction. Emotion, space and society, 1 (1), 1-3. Dean, M., 2010. Power at the heart of the present: Exception, risk and sovereignty. European journal of cultural studies, 13 (4), 459-475. Ewald, F., 1991. Insurance and risks. In G. Burchell, C. Gordon & P. Miller (eds.) The foucault effect: Studies in governmentality. London: Harvester/Wheatsheaf, 197-210. Fox, N.J., 2002. What a 'risky' body can do: Why people's health choices are not all based in evidence. Health education journal, 61 (2), 166-179. Giddens, A., 2009. The politics of climate change. Cambridge: Polity. Greenberg, M., et al., 2012. Ten most important accomplishments in risk analysis, 1980-2010. Risk analysis, 32 (5), 771-781. Hallowell, N., 2006. Varieties of suffering: Living with the risk of ovarian cancer. Health, risk & society, 8 (1), 9-26. Kahan, D.M., 2008. Two conceptions of emotion in risk regulation. University of Pennsylvania law review, 156 (3), 741-766. Kirman, A., Livet, P. and Teschl, M., 2010. Rationality and emotions. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 365 (1538), 215-219. Lash, S., 2000. Risk culture. In B. Adam, U. Beck & J. Van Loon (eds.) The risk society and beyond: Critical issues for social theory. London: Sage, 47-62. Lash, S., Giddens, A. and Beck, U., 1994. Reflexive modernization: Politics, tradition and aesthetics in the modern social order. Cambridge: Polity Press. Lois, J., 2001. Peaks and valleys: The gendered emotional culture of edgework. Gender and society, 15 (3), 381-406. Lupton, D., 1998. The emotional self: A sociocultural exploration. London: Sage. Lupton, D., 1999a. Risk. London: Routledge. Lupton, D. (ed.) (1999b) Risk and sociocultural theory: New directions and perspectives, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lupton, D. and Tulloch, J., 2003. Risk and everyday life. London: Sage. Lyng, S., 1990. Edgework: A social psychological analysis of voluntary risk taking. American journal of sociology, 95 (4), 851-886. Lyng, S., 2005. Edgework: The sociology of risk taking. New York: Routledge. Lyng, S., 2012. Existential transcendence in late modernity: Edgework and hermeneutic reflexivity. Human studies, 35 (3), 401-414. Marcus, G., 2006. Assemblage. Theory, culture & society, 23 (2-3), 101-106. 9
  • 11. Massumi, B., 1993. Everywhere you want to be: Introduction to fear. In B. Massumi (ed.) The politics of everyday fear. Minneapolis, Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press, 3-38. Merleau-Ponty, M., 1962. Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Mulcahy, D., 2012. Affective assemblages: Body matters in the pedagogic practices of contemporary school classrooms. Pedagogy, culture and society, 20 (1), 9-27. Nussbaum, M., 2004. Hiding from humanity: Disgust, shame, and the law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Pickersgill, M., 2012. The co-production of science, ethics, and emotion. Science, technology & human values, 37 (6), 579-603. Rigakos, G.S. and Law, A., 2009. Risk, realism and the politics of resistance. Critical sociology, 35 (1), 79-103. Roeser, S., 2011. Nuclear energy, risk, and emotions. Philosophy & technology, 24 (2), 197-201. Slovic, P., et al., 2002. Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. Journal of socio-economics, 31 (4), 329-342. Slovic, P., et al., 2004. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk analysis, 24 (2), 311-322. Slovic, P., et al., 2007. The affect heuristic. European journal of operational research, 177 (3), 1333- 1352. Slovic, P. and Västfjäll, D., 2010. Affect, moral intuition, and risk. Psychological inquiry, 21 (4), 387- 398. Thrift, N., 2004. Intensities of feeling: Towards a spatial politics of affect. Geografiska annaler, 86 B (1), 57-78. Tulloch, J. and Lupton, D., 2002. Risk is part of your life': Risk epistemologies among a group of australians. Sociology, 36 (2), 317-334. Van Loon, J., 2002. Risk and technological culture: Towards a sociology of virulence. London: Routledge. Van Loon, J., 2005. Epidemic space. Critical public health, 15 (1), 39-52. Wilkinson, I., 2006. Health, risk and ‘social suffering. Health, risk & society, 8 (1), 1-8. 10