SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                1
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

                                      Prof. ILKKA PYYSIAINEN
                                    Academy Research Fellow
                     Deputy Director, Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies
                                  University of Helsinki, Finland
                                  ilkka.pyysiainen @ helsinki.fi

    AFTER RELIGION: COGNITIVE SCIENCE AND THE STUDY OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR©

                    In this paper, I present a multilevel mechanistic model of
            explanation, arguing that the cognitive science of religion can operate at
            many differing levels of mechanisms and is open for many kinds of cross-
            disciplinary cooperation. Getting rid of the old idea of an essence of religion
            helps understand the so-called “religious” phenomena as a natural part of
            human psychology, culture, and behavioral repertoire.

In the beginning …
        In what follows, I try to explain what kind of a research program the cognitive science of
religion is, as I understand it. In the end of the 19th century, the study of religion came to be called
“comparative religion” because it was first inspired by an attempt to understand why certain types
of beliefs and practices were recurrent in virtually all cultures [see Kippenberg 2002]. The early
phenomenology of religion was by and large based on the view that such recurrence was to be
explained as a human response to a holy, supernatural, or a “wholly other” reality [see Otto
1958/1917; Wiebe 1999; Jensen 2003; Masuzawa 2005, 107–120]. Mircea Eliade famously
reworked this idea into a research program in which religious phenomena were interpreted as
“hierophanies,” that is, profane manifestations of the “sacred” which was regarded as an “authentic”
and unconditional mode of existence [see Rennie 2001, 2006].

        All these attempts have had to sail between the Scylla of unfounded dogmatism and the
Kharybdis of vague speculation. On the one hand, the notion of a “wholly other” reality is
completely meaningless as an explanation in a scientific context, while, on the other hand, the more
liberal views about the sacred as authentic existence are too vague to provide a basis for empirical
research. Thus, William Paden [1996], for example, has attempted to develop a “new
comparativism” which would be more in line with the spirit of science. Yet also his attempt suffers
from lack of theory and thus an operationalizable measuring stick; what exactly is compared when
religions or religious phenomena are compared [Lawson 1996]? There will, of course, always be
room for good ethnography. But when one wants to go beyond description, an explicit research
strategy is needed. Gathering data is one thing, explaining it another thing. In the latter one needs a
theory that says which information is relevant and in what sense.

        In explaining human behavior, one should be able to keep apart what is being explained [the
explanandum] and what is the explanation [the explanans]. In other words, data and theory should
be independent from each other. One problem is that religion is all too easily assumed to be a
coherent category with certain singly necessary and jointly sufficient criteria that make something
an instance of religion [see Fitzgerald 1999]. Yet religion does not have an essence; it is rather a
heterogeneous category and recognizing a phenomenon as religious is based on a comparison with
prototypically religious phenomena [Saler 2000; Day 2005]. Defining religion is a notorious
pseudoproblem of religionists and has received much attention with little being thereby gained [see
Geertz 2004]. Religion is not easily disentangled from non-religion, and in practice the Judeo-
Christian tradition has served as the prototype of religion for Western scholars [Saler 2000]. The
notion of “world religions” also is a pseudocategory with varying and vague criteria on which

     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                2
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

religions supposedly count as a “world religions” [Masuzawa 2005]. Usually the best strategy is
simply to forget about “religion” as a general category and merely to analyze human behavior and
patterns of thought.

Religion and reductionism
        Forming a general definition of religion is neither a precondition, nor the ultimate goal of
comparative religion [see Pyysiäinen in press c]. The scholar never studies religion as a whole; he
or she rather focuses on some specific recurrent pattern in human thought, experience, and
behavior; this pattern may be studied as an instance of the more general category of “religious
phenomena,” although it is not necessary to make such explicit generalization in each and every
individual study. The scholar of religion is free to study all forms of thought and behavior that can
be profitably taken as “religious” in the everyday sense of the term. “Profitably” here means that the
scholar is able to show some connection between his or her interpretation of the data and that of
other scholars of religion.

       The cognitive science of religion took its first steps in 1975 and 1980, when Dan Sperber
[1975] first published his book Rethinking Symbolism and Stewart Guthrie [1980] the paper “A
cognitive theory of religion.” Sperber first expressed the idea that the human mental architecture
seems to channel the cultural spread of religious ideas, and Guthrie argued that the nature of human
perception and cognition leads us to postulate supernatural agents. The new field then truly emerged
in the 1990’s with attempts at explaining how the structure of religious rituals is mentally
represented and how religious concepts are culturally transmitted [Lawson and McCauley 1990;
Boyer 1994; see Pyysiäinen 2008]. Later, also such issues as human evolution and the nature of
emotions have been discussed [Boyer 2001; Pyysiäinen 2001; Atran 2002; Tremlin 2006].
Currently, we can find research on such areas as the mental representation of non-natural agent
concepts [Barrett and Keil 1996; Barrett 2004; Bering 2006], the evolution of the neuro-cognitive
systems that support the acquisition of cultural knowledge [Boyer 1994, 1998, 2001, 2003a],
anthropomorphism [Guthrie 1993], and rituals [Lawson and McCauley 1990; McCauley and
Lawson 2002; Whitehouse 2000, 2004; Whitehouse and McCauley 2004].

       “Cognitive science of religion” is now an established term but the kinds of research it covers
no longer form a homogeneous whole. There are such varying research programs as Boyer’s
“standard model” [Boyer 2005], the views of religion as an adaptation [see Sanderson 2008;
Bulbulia et al. 2008], Whitehouse’s [2004] “modes theory,” and also approaches that emphasize the
constructive role of culture [Geertz 2008]. Three things that yet unite these approaches are their
multidisciplinary nature, emphasis on explanation, and the view that human behavior must be
understood in the light of the cognitive processes that support and direct it.

        Common objections to the cognitive science of religion are that it is “reductionist” and
“explains religion away.” I have dealt with these claims elsewhere and shall here only summarize
the main points [see Pyysiäinen in press a, b, c]. All scientific research is reductionist in some sense
because it is always made from some specific point of view; therefore, it is always possible to say
that all other perspectives have been reduced to the one perspective chosen. Thus, to argue that
religion should not be reduced to anything non-religious, implies that religion only exists at some
specific level and has an irreducible essence.

       This is a highly problematic view, as we have seen; the word “religion” names a very
heterogeneous category and one person’s religion often is another person’s non-religion
[superstition, culture, etc.]. There is no homogenous “religion;” ideological variation can in some
instances be greater within the category of religion than between religion and non-religion. What I
mean is that it is possible to find two religious discourses that contradict each other more sharply
     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
              III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                    NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                 3
                       COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                        COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                        COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

than do some third religious discourse and its supposedly non-religious alternative. Buddhist and
Islamic doctrines about God, for example, are in sharp contrast, whereas it may at times be difficult
to say in what sense the claims presented by liberal-minded Lutheran ministers differ from those of
atheists [Nielsen 1985, 37]. Therefore, there cannot be any homogenous “religious language,”
notwithstanding the philosophical claims about “religious language” differing radically from all
factual language.

        We cannot study religion in itself, pure and simple; instead, we study written texts, art,
social groups and institutions, practices, the behavior of persons, and so forth. Thus, Eliade’s [1969,
132–33] claim that we do not have the right to reduce the religious creations of the human mind “to
something other than what they are, namely spiritual creations,” is dubious. When we peel of
history, culture, sociality, psychology, and so forth, nothing is left in the end. Thus, it is the attempt
to be a true anti-reductionist that ironically leads one to explain [or understand] religion away in the
sense that one no longer knows what it is that one is studying. Eliade, of course, could reply that
religion is in the essence an attempt at finding authentic existence or something like that, and that it
should be studied as such. This, however, is only his view and is shared neither by the millions of
people who practice religion, nor by most scholars of religion.

        The claim that religion should not be “explained away” is based on a confusion between the
explanans [that which explains something] and the explanandum [that which is to be explained]: an
explanation of a religious fact is presupposed to take the place of the religious fact that is explained.
Kelly Bulkeley [2003], for example, writes that “Boyer finds cause to dismiss religious
understandings and replace them with cognitive scientific ones” [emphasis added]. Yet it should be
clear that explaining religion is something we do as scholars; practicing religion is another matter. It
might still be argued that studying religion is dangerous business because it may make religious
truth claims dubious in the eyes of believers [see Dennett 2006]. However, such straightforward
defense of religion is a purely political matter and should not be masked as valid methodological
criticism [see Wiebe 1999]. Whether a given explanation is valid should be judged on the basis of
evidence and logical coherence of the argument, not on the basis of a religious [or anti-religious]
agenda.

Mind matters
        Sometimes anti-reductionist arguments are based on the claim that religious phenomena
should be understood or explained as part of “society” or “culture” and that this immediately
renders all psychological arguments dubious [Durkheim 1925; Geertz 1973; cf. Pyysiäinen 2001].
However, this argument was developed before modern cognitive and developmental psychology
and cognitive neuroscience. Dan Sperber [1975; 1985; 2006] seems to have been the first to have
realized that culture and psychology cannot be in this way opposed, once we understand cultures as
sets of ideas that are passed on to others relying on certain mechanisms of memory. This passing on
involves such sharing of ideas where everyone understands that others understand them to
understand what others understand [and so on …; Dennett 1993, 243–46; see Pyysiäinen in press].
Sperber’s “epidemiology of representations” means the study of the differential spread of concepts
and beliefs in populations. Some concepts and beliefs win and some lose in cultural selection
because not all concepts fit the human mind equally well.

        Pascal Boyer has studied the evolution of the neuro-cognitive systems that support the
acquisition of cultural knowledge along these lines. He has emphasized the role of the so-called
intuitive ontology, that is, a set of ontological categories into which we intuitively assign all objects.
The basic intuitive ontological categories are: PERSONS, ANIMALS, PLANTS, ARTIFACTS and NATURAL
OBJECTS [Boyer 1994, 101; 1998, 878; 2000, 280]. Counterintuitive representations are formed by
adding or deleting a feature that then violates our intuitive expectations. It is possible to add mental
     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
              III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                    NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                 4
                       COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                        COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                        COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

characteristics to an artifact [e.g. a statue that hears prayers] or to deny a biological body to a person
[e.g. gods], for example. Such minimally counterintuitive representations seem to be an important
defining characteristic of religion [Pyysiäinen et al. 2003; see Boyer 2001; Barrett 2004].

        Scott Atran [2002, 10–11] uses the image of evolutionary history as a landscape formed by
different mountain ridges: just as rain converges toward a limited set of rivers and lakes, so also
human experience falls into certain basic types determined by the cognitive architecture of our
species. Atran refers to Conrad Waddington’s [1959] idea of developmental processes as a complex
landscape of hills and branching valleys, descending from a high plateau. This plateau represents
the state of the fertilized egg. The valleys are developmental pathways leading to particular end
states such as a functioning eye, heart, and so on [see Jablonka and Lamb 2006, 63–65, 261–62]. A
set of genes and their mutual interactions forms a developmental system producing a phenotype [as
distinguished from a genotype]. Many features of the phenotype are explained by the dynamical
properties of the system as a whole, not by individual alleles. Developmental canalization here
means a “buffering” against both environmental and genetic parameters: single genes or features of
environment cannot bias development that is bound to go a certain way because of the
developmental canalization. Canalization thus not only blocks effects of environmental variation
but also the effects of variation in certain alleles [Griffiths and Machery 2008, 397–99].

        Atran, however, uses the idea of cognitive architecture as canalizing cultural transmission to
emphasize that certain ways of thinking are a natural part of human nature. Their independence
from cultural construction does not mean that they could develop without a cultural environment; it
is rather that their development is not dependent on any particular culture [as Boyer 2003b, 238–
39] puts it. The crossculturally recurrent patterns of religion studied by the early phenomenologists
could be explained with reference to the nature of the human mind [especially Boyer 1994]. The
epidemiology of representations thus offers an interesting research program for the study of
religion. We might finally be able to explain why certain patterns of belief and behavior are so
contagious.

       Underlying the epidemiology of representations is the idea of selectionism: scholars ask why
some concepts and beliefs are selected for cultural transmission. Scholars thus are asking
population-level questions and their answers are statements about trends in populations, not
explanations of the deepest motives of individuals [see Boyer 2001, 319; Pyysiäinen in press d].

       As Justin Barrett [2008, 298] recently put it, cognitive theories have not been applied to
particular problems, scholars rather studying “why religious rituals appear the way they do
generally, why people believe in gods generally,” and so forth. This is also often accompanied by
attempts at solving only theoretical problems and to do this by conceptual analysis alone. This is
understandable given the different kinds of background of cognitive scientists of religion and the
nature of methodological training at many departments of comparative religion.

        However, it is important to be clear about whether one tries to explain certain recurrent types
of beliefs and practices, or something that characterizes the beliefs, experiences, and acts of
individuals. Evolutionary explanations do not reveal the deepest unconscious motives of
individuals; they only apply to population-level questions. Thus, mere epidemiology is not enough
in the study of religion, because then individual-level questions would be left out of the picture. Yet
we also need to be able to explain why an individual does or believes something.

        There is a curious tension in the program of the cognitive science of religion in the sense
that, on the one hand, emphasis is shifted from culture to the individual, and yet, on the other hand,
it is not so much actual individuals that are studied but individual-level cognitive mechanisms
     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                5
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

[Boyer 1994]. Especially when these are studied within an evolutionary framework, as in Boyer
[2001], the objects of explanation are always population-level phenomena; an evolutionary
explanation does not account for the unconscious motives of an individual in a unique situation [see
Ariew 2003; Pyysiäinen in press d]. Yet we must also bear in mind that mental and cultural
representations are not two different types of representations. It is rather a question of two different
points of view: either we view a concept or a belief as such, or explore its spread in human
populations [Boyer 2003b].

Explanation
       When Dilthey [1924, 144] once argued in the Neokantian spirit: “Die Natur erklären wir,
das Seelenleben verstehen wir,” he could not know anything about things such as cognitive science,
neuropsychology, or modern philosophy of science. We no longer think that explanation is always
causal and based on formulating general laws [see Pyysiäinen in press a, d]. Also the very idea of
“understanding” as well as its use have been critically evaluated [Martin 2000; Ylikoski in press].
Yet some still argue that religion should be “understood” rather than “explained” [see Gothóni
2005]. I shall here not discuss this controversy; instead, I want briefly to outline a view of
explanation that might help understand how religion can be explained.

        Unlike in the old general law model, in modern philosophy of science, explanation is often
understood as based on specifying a mechanism that is responsible for the production of a
phenomenon, either in an etiological or in a constitutive sense. A mechanism thus produces or
supports something. It is something that exists in the real world and thus explanation is not just a
deductive argument [as it was in the covering law model]. It is enough that an explanatory
generalization is stable in the sense that the specified relation between cause and effect holds under
a range of conditions [generally not universal]. Mechanisms are not deterministic; they rather
produce a probability distribution over possible outcomes and show that the explanandum is an
instance of one of those possible outcomes [Craver 2007, 40; see Railton 1978]. Mechanistic
explanation consists in describing the parts, operations, and organization of a mechanism, and
showing how the mechanism realizes the phenomenon to be explained. A mechanism is a “set of
entities and activities organized such that they exhibit the phenomenon to be explained” [Bechtel
2008, 49; Craver 2007, 5, 99].

        Carl Craver’s model of mechanistic explanation is based on the idea of causal relevance. In
this view, X is causally relevant with regard to Y if an “ideal intervention I on X with respect to Y
is a change in the value of X that changes Y, if at all, only via the change in X” [Craver 2007, 95–
96]. Causation is here understood in a manipulationist sense [Woodward 2003]. As explanations
specify counterfactual relationships that are somehow invariant [Craver’s “stable conditions”], it is
possible to say what would have happened if the cause of the event had been manipulated by an
ideal intervention [Woodward 2003]. An intervention means that we manipulate a variable A in
order to see if changes in A have a causal relationship with changes in the variable B. The variables
must have measureable values but it is enough that the intervention or manipulation is logically or
conceptually possible [Woodward 2003, 94, 114, 127-133]. If a manipulation of A introduces a
change in B, A is causally relevant with regard to B.

       Such causal relevance is best explained using counterfactuals [that is, contrastively]. Take
the question: “Did Socrates’ sipping the pint of hemlock cause his death?” If this means: “Did
Socrates’ sipping of the pint of hemlock [rather than wine] cause his death?” the answer is “Yes.”
But if the question is: “Did Socrates’ sipping [rather than guzzling or in some other way
consuming] the pint of hemlock cause his death?” the answer is “No” [Craver 2007, 202–203].
Experimental research relies precisely on such contrastive causal claims. An experiment is arranged
in order to find an answer to the question: “Did A [rather than B] cause C?” The experimental group
     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                 6
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

and the control group differ only with respect to the value of the putative causal variable [Craver
2007, 203]. Here the contrast serves to specify the explanandum as clearly as possible. Objective
relations of causal relevance then explain the effect [Craver 2007, 204].

        For the study of religion, such multilevel mechanistic explanation means that it is possible to
explain different aspect of religion at differing levels by specifying mechanisms that produce and
sustain various religious phenomena. There is no one level at which religion should only be
explained. At which level of mechanisms we look for an explanans depends on what we have
chosen as the explanandum. If we, for example, want to know why praying has diminished as a
practice in this or that society, and then offer a sociological answer to this question, it might be that
somebody objects, saying that the real answers are to be found in the brain. This, however, does not
lead to a better answer to the original question; it is rather a strategy of changing the explanandum
from a sociological phenomenon into a neural one. Sometimes we need to do this, but it is
necessary to understand that when we change the explanans, also explanandum is changes [see
Craver and Bechtel 2007]. As we want to know different kinds of things about saying prayers, there
is room for different kinds of questions. But any specific answer given at some specific level is not
necessarily an answer to another question which exists at another level.

        To take an example, when Pascal Boyer and Pierre Liénard [2006] presented their theory of
action ritualization, some of the commentators understood them to be claiming that religious rituals
are mere obsessive-compulsive disorders. As Joan Hageman [2006, 619] puts it, she wants to argue
“against the theory that cultural ritual behavior is meaningless or that ritual action is solely a by-
product of fearbased precautionary and action-parsing systems.” But Boyer and Liénard were not
saying that all cultural ritual behavior is blindly produced by the hazard precaution system. Their
argument is that actions of an individual are ritualized under specific conditions and that such action
ritualization is one component making cultural ceremonies salient and memorable. Not all ritual
action is ritualized action. Thus, the mechanisms supporting cultural ritual behavior exist at
differing levels, from culture to individuals, cognitive systems, neural systems, and molecular-level
events [see Craver 2007; Pyysiäinen in press d]. To say, for example, that “genes have nothing to do
with God” is true but only in the sense that there is a component of the mechanism missing at the
intermediate level between genes and the idea of God. Explanatory cognitive science of religion can
operate at many differing levels of mechanisms and is open for many kinds of cross-disciplinary
cooperation. Getting rid of the old idea of an essence of religion helps understand the so-called
“religious” phenomena as a natural part of human psychology and behavioral repertoire.

References
Ariew, André. [2003]. Ernst Mayr’s ‘ultimate/proximate’ distinction reconsidered and
reconstructed. Biology and Philosophy 18, 553–65.
Atran, Scott. [2002]. In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Barrett, Justin L. [2004]. Why would anyone believe in God? Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Barrett, Justin L., & Frank Keil. [1996]. Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in
God concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31, 219–47.
Bechtel, William. [2008]. Mental mechanisms: Philosophical perspectives on cognitive
neuroscience. New York: Routledge.
Boyer, Pascal. [1994]. The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley:
University of California Press.


     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                            7
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

Boyer, Pascal. [1998]. Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology
governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist 100[4], 876–89.
Boyer, Pascal. [2000]. Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics? Developmental evidence for
early-developed, intuitive, category-specific, incomplete, and stubborn metaphysical presumptions.
Philosophical Psychology 13[3], 277–97.
Boyer, Pascal. [2001]. Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New
York: Basic Books.
Boyer, Pascal. [2003a]. Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences 7[3], 119–24.
Boyer, Pascal. [2003b]. Are ghost concepts “intuitive,” “endemic” and “innate”? Journal of
Cognition and Culture 3[3], 233–43.
Boyer, Pascal. [2005]. A reductionistic model of distinct modes of religious transmission. In Harvey
Whitehouse & Robert N. McCauley [Eds.], Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive
foundations of religiosity, 3–29. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Boyer, Pascal, & Pierre Liénard. [2006]. Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action-
parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29[6],
595–650 [with commentaries].
Bulbulia, Joseph, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, & Karen Wyman [eds.].
[2008]. The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, & critiques. Santa Margarita, CA: Collins
Family Foundation.
Bulkeley, Kelly. [2003]. Review of Religion Explained by Pascal Boyer; How Religion Works by
Ilkka Pyysiäinen. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 71, 671–74.
Craver, Carl. [2007]. Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Craver, Carl, & William Bechtel. [2007]. Top-down causation without top-down causes. Biology
and Philosophy 22, 547–63.
Day, Matthew. [2005]. The undiscovered and undiscoverable essence: Species and religion after
Darwin. Journal of Religion 85[1], 58–82.
Dennett, Daniel C. [2006]. Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. New York:
Viking.
Dilthey, Wilhelm. [1924]. Die Geistige Welt. [Gesammelte Schriften; 5.] Leipzig: Teubner.
Durkheim, Émile. [1925/1912]. Les formes élémentaires de la vie religieuse. Paris: Alcan.
Fitzgerald, Timothy. [1999]. The ideology of religious studies. New York: Oxford University Press.
Geertz, Armin. [2004]. Definition, categorization, and indecision: Or, how to get on with the study
of religion. In Unterwegs. Neue Pfade in der Religionswissenschaft / New Paths in the Study of
Religions: Festschrift in Honour of Michael Pye on his 65th birthday, edited by Christoph Kleine,
Monika Schrimpf, & Katja Triplett, 109–118. München: Biblion.
Geertz, Armin. [2008]. From apes to devils and angels: Comparing scenarios on the evolution of
religion. In Bulbulia, Joseph, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, & Karen
Wyman [eds.], The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, & critiques, 43–49. Santa Margarita,
CA: Collins Family Foundation.
Geertz, Clifford. [1973]. The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.

    III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
          МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                                8
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

Gothóni, René [Ed.]. [2005]. How to do comparative religion: Three ways, many goals. [Religion
and Reason, 44.] Berlin: De Gruyter.
Griffiths, Paul E., & Edouard Machery. [2008]. Innateness, canalization, and ‘biologicizing the
mind.’ Philosophical Psychology 21[3], 397–414.
Guthrie, Stewart. [1980]. A cognitive theory of religion. Current Anthropology 21, 181–203.
Guthrie, Stewart [E.]. [1993]. Faces in the clouds. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hagen, Joan M: [2006]. Multicultural religious and spiritual rituals: Meaning and praxis.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29[6], 619–20.
Jablonka, Eva, & Marion J. Lamb. [2006/2005]. Evolution in four dimensions. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Jensen, Jeppe Sinding. [2003]. The study of religion in a new key. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.
Kippenberg, Hans G. [2992]. Discovering religious history in the modern age. Tr. From the
German by Barbara Harshaw. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Lawson, E. Thomas. [1996]. Theory and the new comparativism, old and new. Method & Theory in
the Study of Religion 8, ????.
Lawson, E. Thomas, & Robert N. McCauley. [1990]. Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition
and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McCauley, Robert N., & E. Thomas Lawson. [2002]. Bringing ritual to mind: Psychological
foundations of cultural forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Martin, Michael. [2000]. Verstehen: The uses of understanding in social science. New Brunswick:
Transaction Publishers.
Masuzawa, Tomoko. [2005]. The invention of world religions: Or, how European universalism was
preserved in the language of pluralism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Nielsen, Kai. [1985]. Philosophy & atheism: In defense of atheism. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus
Books.
Otto, Rudolf. [1958/1917]. The idea of the Holy. Tr. From the German by John W. Harvey. Lodon:
Oxford University Press.
Paden, William E. [1996]. Elements of a new comparativism. Method & Theory in the Study of
Religion 8, 5–14.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [2001]. How religion works: Towards a new cognitive science of religion.
Leiden: Brill.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [2008]. Introduction: Religion, cognition, and culture. Religion 38[2], 101–108.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press a]. Reduction and explanatory pluralism in the cognitive science of
religion. In Changing minds: Religion and cognition through the ages, edited by István Czachesz &
Tamás Bíró. [Groningen Studies in Cultural Change.] Leuven: Peeters.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press b]. Supernatural agents: Why we believe in souls, gods, and buddhas.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press c]. Religion and the brain – Cognitive science as a basis for theories of
religion. In Theory/Religion/Critique, edited by Richard King. New York: Columbia University
Press.


     III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
     III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
           МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
                СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES
             III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S
                   NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES                            9
                      COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM     
                       COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM  
                       COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM

Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press d]. Mechanistic explanation of ritualized behavior. In Armin Geertz &
Jesper Sørensen [Ed.], Religious ritual, cognition and culture. London: Equinox.
Pyysiäinen, Ilkka, Marjaana Lindeman, & Timo Honkela. [2003]. Counterintuitiveness as the
hallmark of religiosity. Religion 33[4], 341–55.
Railton, Peter. [1978]. A deductive-nomological model of probabilistic explanation. Philosophy of
Science 45, 206–226.
Rennie, Bryan. [2001]. Changing religious worlds: The meaning and end of Mircea Eliade. Albany;
NY: State University of New York Press.
Rennie, Bryan [Ed.]. [2006]. Mircea Eliade: A critical reader. London: Equinox.
Saler, Benson. [2000/1993]. Conceptualizing religion: Immanent anthropologists, transcendent
natives, and unbound categories. With a new preface. New York: Berghahn Books.
Sanderson, Stephen K. [2008]. Adaptation, evolution, and religion. Religion 38[2], 141–56.
Sperber, Dan. [1995/1975]. Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Sperber, Dan. [1985]. Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations.
Man [N.S.] 20, 73–89.
Sperber, Dan. [2006]. Conceptual tools for a naturalistic approach to cultural evolution. In Stephen
C. Levinson and Pierre Jaisson [Eds.], Evolution of culture: A Fyssen Foundation symposium, 147–
65. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tremlin, Todd. [2006]. Minds and gods: The cognitive foundations of religion. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Waddington, Conrad H. [1959]. Canalization of development and genetic assimilation of acquired
characters. Nature 183, 1654–55.
Whitehouse, Harvey. [2000]. Arguments and icons: Divergent modes of religiosity, Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Whitehouse, Harvey. [2004]. Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission.
Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Whitehouse, Harvey, & Robert N. McCauley [Eds]. [2005]. Mind and religion: Psychological and
cognitive foundations of religion. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Wiebe, Donald. [1999]. The politics of religious studies. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Woodward, James. [2003]. Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Ylikoski, Petri. [In press]. The illusion of depth of understanding in science. In Henk De Regt,
Sabinelli, & Eigner [Eds.], Scientific understanding: Philosophical perspectives. Pittsburgh:
Pittsburgh University Press.




    III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ   
    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
          МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ   
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ
               СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ

More Related Content

What's hot

Our Long Journey with Mystery
Our  Long Journey with MysteryOur  Long Journey with Mystery
Our Long Journey with MysteryPeter Creagh
 
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cg
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cgHumss introduction to world religions & belief systems cg
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cgJonathan Labsan
 
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEM
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEMModule 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEM
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEMAngelita Montilla
 
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)Muhammad Sheth
 
Religion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsReligion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsVampire Secret
 
Religion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsReligion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsXyreneKayeYap
 
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDP
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDPCatechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDP
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDPAlvin Jimena
 
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Study
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual StudyThe Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Study
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Studymohd abbas abdul razak
 
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?Maya Bohnhoff
 
Concept of Religion
Concept of ReligionConcept of Religion
Concept of ReligionMiss Ivy
 
World Religion Lesson1
World Religion Lesson1World Religion Lesson1
World Religion Lesson1Shine Abcede
 
Pneumatological consensus by sylvest
Pneumatological consensus by sylvestPneumatological consensus by sylvest
Pneumatological consensus by sylvestjohnboy_philothea_net
 
1 definition of_theology[1]
1 definition of_theology[1]1 definition of_theology[1]
1 definition of_theology[1]kion86
 
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS ppt
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS pptRELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS ppt
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS pptCityRivas
 
Catechetical methods
Catechetical methodsCatechetical methods
Catechetical methodsAlvin Jimena
 

What's hot (20)

Philosophy and religion
Philosophy and religionPhilosophy and religion
Philosophy and religion
 
Our Long Journey with Mystery
Our  Long Journey with MysteryOur  Long Journey with Mystery
Our Long Journey with Mystery
 
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cg
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cgHumss introduction to world religions & belief systems cg
Humss introduction to world religions & belief systems cg
 
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEM
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEMModule 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEM
Module 2. lesson 6: RELIGION AND BELIEF SYSTEM
 
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)
Tauhid (By Prof Omar Hasan Kasule)
 
Religion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsReligion and belief systems
Religion and belief systems
 
Religion and belief systems
Religion and belief systemsReligion and belief systems
Religion and belief systems
 
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDP
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDPCatechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDP
Catechetical Program Basic content Chapter V NCDP
 
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Study
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual StudyThe Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Study
The Spiritual Dimension of Man: An Islamic Psycho Spiritual Study
 
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?
Mythbusting: Are Science and Religion Really at War?
 
Religion ppt
Religion pptReligion ppt
Religion ppt
 
Concept of Religion
Concept of ReligionConcept of Religion
Concept of Religion
 
World Religion Lesson1
World Religion Lesson1World Religion Lesson1
World Religion Lesson1
 
Pneumatological consensus by sylvest
Pneumatological consensus by sylvestPneumatological consensus by sylvest
Pneumatological consensus by sylvest
 
1 definition of_theology[1]
1 definition of_theology[1]1 definition of_theology[1]
1 definition of_theology[1]
 
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS ppt
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS pptRELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS ppt
RELIGION & BELIEF SYSTEMS ppt
 
Medieval philosophy
Medieval philosophyMedieval philosophy
Medieval philosophy
 
Catechetical methods
Catechetical methodsCatechetical methods
Catechetical methods
 
Lecture: P. Chenchiah (1886-1959)
Lecture: P. Chenchiah (1886-1959)Lecture: P. Chenchiah (1886-1959)
Lecture: P. Chenchiah (1886-1959)
 
Spiritual Self
Spiritual SelfSpiritual Self
Spiritual Self
 

Viewers also liked

How to search without Google
How to search without GoogleHow to search without Google
How to search without GoogleSara Batts
 
James Allen - As A Man Thinketh
James Allen - As A Man ThinkethJames Allen - As A Man Thinketh
James Allen - As A Man ThinkethMichael Stewart
 
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth Part 2 of 3
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth  Part 2 of 3Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth  Part 2 of 3
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth Part 2 of 3Satpanth Dharm
 
Death Of Synchronization
Death Of SynchronizationDeath Of Synchronization
Death Of Synchronizationguestaff4da
 
Palmer m.(ed) world religions
Palmer m.(ed) world religionsPalmer m.(ed) world religions
Palmer m.(ed) world religionstudor11111
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
Why would anyone believe in god (barrett 2004) [selected chapters]
 
Shamanism as the original neurotheology (winkelman 2004)
Shamanism as the original neurotheology (winkelman 2004)Shamanism as the original neurotheology (winkelman 2004)
Shamanism as the original neurotheology (winkelman 2004)
 
Three cultures
Three culturesThree cultures
Three cultures
 
How to search without Google
How to search without GoogleHow to search without Google
How to search without Google
 
God do i have your attention (colzato et al. 2010)
God   do i have your attention (colzato et al. 2010)God   do i have your attention (colzato et al. 2010)
God do i have your attention (colzato et al. 2010)
 
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the selfThe sense of agency and the illusion of the self
The sense of agency and the illusion of the self
 
James Allen - As A Man Thinketh
James Allen - As A Man ThinkethJames Allen - As A Man Thinketh
James Allen - As A Man Thinketh
 
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth Part 2 of 3
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth  Part 2 of 3Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth  Part 2 of 3
Series 33 - E - History of Pirana Satpanth Part 2 of 3
 
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics — developmental evidence for earl...
 
Biology and religion (murphy & schloss 2008)
Biology and religion (murphy & schloss 2008)Biology and religion (murphy & schloss 2008)
Biology and religion (murphy & schloss 2008)
 
Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity (kapogiannis et al. 2009)
Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity (kapogiannis et al. 2009)Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity (kapogiannis et al. 2009)
Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity (kapogiannis et al. 2009)
 
Iraqi oil
Iraqi oilIraqi oil
Iraqi oil
 
How science and religion are more like theology and commonsense explanations ...
How science and religion are more like theology and commonsense explanations ...How science and religion are more like theology and commonsense explanations ...
How science and religion are more like theology and commonsense explanations ...
 
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
Why does religiosity persist (sedikides 2010)
 
Zeus problem why representational content biases cannot explain faith in go...
Zeus problem   why representational content biases cannot explain faith in go...Zeus problem   why representational content biases cannot explain faith in go...
Zeus problem why representational content biases cannot explain faith in go...
 
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information   reply to ...
Supernatural agents may have provided adaptive social information reply to ...
 
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...The power of charisma   perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
The power of charisma perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive net...
 
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
Neurobiology and manuscript cultures — the evolution of premodern religious a...
 
Death Of Synchronization
Death Of SynchronizationDeath Of Synchronization
Death Of Synchronization
 
Palmer m.(ed) world religions
Palmer m.(ed) world religionsPalmer m.(ed) world religions
Palmer m.(ed) world religions
 

Similar to After religion cognitive science and the study of human behavior (pyysiäinen 2008)

2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY Research Pap.docx
2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY  Research Pap.docx2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY  Research Pap.docx
2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY Research Pap.docxrhetttrevannion
 
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docx
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docxEssay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docx
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docxYASHU40
 
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1ProfessorWatson
 
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural Change
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural ChangeChristology, Evolution, and Cultural Change
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural Changeliasuprapti
 
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual Review
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual ReviewSpirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual Review
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual ReviewJonathan Dunnemann
 
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docx
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docxReview the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docx
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docxronak56
 
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of Religions
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of ReligionsA Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of Religions
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of ReligionsYolanda Ivey
 
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES 1C H A P T E R 1RELIGIOUS RESP.docx
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES  1C H A P T E R  1RELIGIOUS RESP.docxRELIGIOUS RESPONSES  1C H A P T E R  1RELIGIOUS RESP.docx
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES 1C H A P T E R 1RELIGIOUS RESP.docxaudeleypearl
 
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturata
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature NaturataA Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturata
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturataijtsrd
 
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptx
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptxPhilosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptx
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptxNurulKhusna13
 
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docx
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docxDIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docx
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docxlynettearnold46882
 
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...Alexander Decker
 
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Alexander Decker
 
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Alexander Decker
 

Similar to After religion cognitive science and the study of human behavior (pyysiäinen 2008) (20)

2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY Research Pap.docx
2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY  Research Pap.docx2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY  Research Pap.docx
2LIBERTY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF DIVINITY Research Pap.docx
 
What Is Theology Essay
What Is Theology EssayWhat Is Theology Essay
What Is Theology Essay
 
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docx
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docxEssay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docx
Essay 1 generally good content; but some issues with content as n.docx
 
phil.rel
phil.relphil.rel
phil.rel
 
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1
An Invitation to the Study of World Religions Chapter 1
 
Liberation Theology Essay
Liberation Theology EssayLiberation Theology Essay
Liberation Theology Essay
 
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural Change
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural ChangeChristology, Evolution, and Cultural Change
Christology, Evolution, and Cultural Change
 
Lesson1.ppt
Lesson1.pptLesson1.ppt
Lesson1.ppt
 
Theology Essays
Theology EssaysTheology Essays
Theology Essays
 
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual Review
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual ReviewSpirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual Review
Spirituality as a Positive Youth Development Construct: A Conceptual Review
 
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docx
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docxReview the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docx
Review the articles, Women and Financialization Microcredit, Inst.docx
 
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of Religions
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of ReligionsA Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of Religions
A Muslim Response To The Christian Theology Of Religions
 
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science (mc cauley 2000)
 
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES 1C H A P T E R 1RELIGIOUS RESP.docx
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES  1C H A P T E R  1RELIGIOUS RESP.docxRELIGIOUS RESPONSES  1C H A P T E R  1RELIGIOUS RESP.docx
RELIGIOUS RESPONSES 1C H A P T E R 1RELIGIOUS RESP.docx
 
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturata
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature NaturataA Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturata
A Comparative Study on Philosophy of Spinoza’s Nature and Nature Naturata
 
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptx
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptxPhilosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptx
Philosophical foundations and The Philosophy of Education.pptx
 
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docx
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docxDIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docx
DIRECTIONS What do the four parts of the Christian Biblical Na.docx
 
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...
Religion as a repository of nigeria national communication policy israel udom...
 
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
 
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
Religion in nigeria from 1900 2013
 

More from SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A.

More from SPK División Gráfica Digital de Surpack S.A. (20)

Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete bookReligion explained (boyer 2001)  ----- complete book
Religion explained (boyer 2001) ----- complete book
 
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...In gods we trust   the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
In gods we trust the evolutionary landscape of religion (evolution and cogn...
 
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete bookFaces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995)  ----- complete book
Faces in the clouds (guthrie 1993, 1995) ----- complete book
 
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
Bottles are men, glasses are women (guthrie 2007)
 
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
Neurobiology, stratified texts, and the evolution of thought — from myths to ...
 
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
 
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
El dogma de cristo (erich fromm)
 
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
Atheism explained (lamnan 2011)
 
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...Theistic percepts in other species   can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
Theistic percepts in other species can chimpanzees represent the minds of n...
 
The relative unnaturalness of atheism on why geertz and markússon are both ...
The relative unnaturalness of atheism   on why geertz and markússon are both ...The relative unnaturalness of atheism   on why geertz and markússon are both ...
The relative unnaturalness of atheism on why geertz and markússon are both ...
 
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
The folk psychology of souls (bering 2006)
 
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
The existencial theory of mind (bering 2002)
 
The evolution of religion how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
The evolution of religion   how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...The evolution of religion   how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
The evolution of religion how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heur...
 
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...Supernaturalizing social life   religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
Supernaturalizing social life religion and the evolution of human cooperati...
 
Supernatural agents why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
Supernatural agents   why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)Supernatural agents   why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
Supernatural agents why we believe in souls gods and buddhas (pyysiäinen 2009)
 
Supernatural agents citas
Supernatural agents   citasSupernatural agents   citas
Supernatural agents citas
 
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
Spsp 2010 program (bering sobre ateos mágicos pp. 32 33)
 
Science and god an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
Science and god   an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...Science and god   an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
Science and god an automatic opposition between ultimate explanations (pres...
 
Religious belief as compensatory control (kay et al. 2010)
Religious belief as compensatory control (kay et al. 2010)Religious belief as compensatory control (kay et al. 2010)
Religious belief as compensatory control (kay et al. 2010)
 
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
Religions, values and peak experiences (maslow 1964)
 

Recently uploaded

React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App Framework
React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App FrameworkReact Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App Framework
React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App FrameworkPixlogix Infotech
 
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with Platformless
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with PlatformlessAccelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with Platformless
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with PlatformlessWSO2
 
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native development
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native developmentEmixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native development
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native developmentPim van der Noll
 
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorial
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorialKuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorial
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorialJoão Esperancinha
 
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks and Compliance Requirements i...
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks  and Compliance Requirements i...Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks  and Compliance Requirements i...
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks and Compliance Requirements i...itnewsafrica
 
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyes
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyesHow to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyes
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyesThousandEyes
 
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesQCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesBernd Ruecker
 
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...amber724300
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...BookNet Canada
 
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDF
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDFAll These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDF
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDFMichael Gough
 
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdf
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdfQMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdf
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdfROWELL MARQUINA
 
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Nikki Chapple
 
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platforms
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platformsInfrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platforms
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platformsYoss Cohen
 
A Glance At The Java Performance Toolbox
A Glance At The Java Performance ToolboxA Glance At The Java Performance Toolbox
A Glance At The Java Performance ToolboxAna-Maria Mihalceanu
 
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsPotential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsRavi Sanghani
 
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Farhan Tariq
 
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#Karmanjay Verma
 
Digital Tools & AI in Career Development
Digital Tools & AI in Career DevelopmentDigital Tools & AI in Career Development
Digital Tools & AI in Career DevelopmentMahmoud Rabie
 
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examples
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examplesTesting tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examples
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examplesKari Kakkonen
 

Recently uploaded (20)

React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App Framework
React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App FrameworkReact Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App Framework
React Native vs Ionic - The Best Mobile App Framework
 
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with Platformless
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with PlatformlessAccelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with Platformless
Accelerating Enterprise Software Engineering with Platformless
 
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native development
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native developmentEmixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native development
Emixa Mendix Meetup 11 April 2024 about Mendix Native development
 
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorial
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorialKuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorial
Kuma Meshes Part I - The basics - A tutorial
 
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks and Compliance Requirements i...
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks  and Compliance Requirements i...Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks  and Compliance Requirements i...
Abdul Kader Baba- Managing Cybersecurity Risks and Compliance Requirements i...
 
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyes
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyesHow to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyes
How to Effectively Monitor SD-WAN and SASE Environments with ThousandEyes
 
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architecturesQCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
QCon London: Mastering long-running processes in modern architectures
 
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...
JET Technology Labs White Paper for Virtualized Security and Encryption Techn...
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC SalesData and LibraryData -...
 
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDF
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDFAll These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDF
All These Sophisticated Attacks, Can We Really Detect Them - PDF
 
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdf
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdfQMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdf
QMMS Lesson 2 - Using MS Excel Formula.pdf
 
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
Microsoft 365 Copilot: How to boost your productivity with AI – Part one: Ado...
 
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platforms
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platformsInfrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platforms
Infrared simulation and processing on Nvidia platforms
 
A Glance At The Java Performance Toolbox
A Glance At The Java Performance ToolboxA Glance At The Java Performance Toolbox
A Glance At The Java Performance Toolbox
 
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and InsightsPotential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
Potential of AI (Generative AI) in Business: Learnings and Insights
 
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
Genislab builds better products and faster go-to-market with Lean project man...
 
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#
Microservices, Docker deploy and Microservices source code in C#
 
Digital Tools & AI in Career Development
Digital Tools & AI in Career DevelopmentDigital Tools & AI in Career Development
Digital Tools & AI in Career Development
 
How Tech Giants Cut Corners to Harvest Data for A.I.
How Tech Giants Cut Corners to Harvest Data for A.I.How Tech Giants Cut Corners to Harvest Data for A.I.
How Tech Giants Cut Corners to Harvest Data for A.I.
 
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examples
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examplesTesting tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examples
Testing tools and AI - ideas what to try with some tool examples
 

After religion cognitive science and the study of human behavior (pyysiäinen 2008)

  • 1. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 1 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM Prof. ILKKA PYYSIAINEN Academy Research Fellow Deputy Director, Helsinki Collegium for Advanced Studies University of Helsinki, Finland ilkka.pyysiainen @ helsinki.fi AFTER RELIGION: COGNITIVE SCIENCE AND THE STUDY OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR© In this paper, I present a multilevel mechanistic model of explanation, arguing that the cognitive science of religion can operate at many differing levels of mechanisms and is open for many kinds of cross- disciplinary cooperation. Getting rid of the old idea of an essence of religion helps understand the so-called “religious” phenomena as a natural part of human psychology, culture, and behavioral repertoire. In the beginning … In what follows, I try to explain what kind of a research program the cognitive science of religion is, as I understand it. In the end of the 19th century, the study of religion came to be called “comparative religion” because it was first inspired by an attempt to understand why certain types of beliefs and practices were recurrent in virtually all cultures [see Kippenberg 2002]. The early phenomenology of religion was by and large based on the view that such recurrence was to be explained as a human response to a holy, supernatural, or a “wholly other” reality [see Otto 1958/1917; Wiebe 1999; Jensen 2003; Masuzawa 2005, 107–120]. Mircea Eliade famously reworked this idea into a research program in which religious phenomena were interpreted as “hierophanies,” that is, profane manifestations of the “sacred” which was regarded as an “authentic” and unconditional mode of existence [see Rennie 2001, 2006]. All these attempts have had to sail between the Scylla of unfounded dogmatism and the Kharybdis of vague speculation. On the one hand, the notion of a “wholly other” reality is completely meaningless as an explanation in a scientific context, while, on the other hand, the more liberal views about the sacred as authentic existence are too vague to provide a basis for empirical research. Thus, William Paden [1996], for example, has attempted to develop a “new comparativism” which would be more in line with the spirit of science. Yet also his attempt suffers from lack of theory and thus an operationalizable measuring stick; what exactly is compared when religions or religious phenomena are compared [Lawson 1996]? There will, of course, always be room for good ethnography. But when one wants to go beyond description, an explicit research strategy is needed. Gathering data is one thing, explaining it another thing. In the latter one needs a theory that says which information is relevant and in what sense. In explaining human behavior, one should be able to keep apart what is being explained [the explanandum] and what is the explanation [the explanans]. In other words, data and theory should be independent from each other. One problem is that religion is all too easily assumed to be a coherent category with certain singly necessary and jointly sufficient criteria that make something an instance of religion [see Fitzgerald 1999]. Yet religion does not have an essence; it is rather a heterogeneous category and recognizing a phenomenon as religious is based on a comparison with prototypically religious phenomena [Saler 2000; Day 2005]. Defining religion is a notorious pseudoproblem of religionists and has received much attention with little being thereby gained [see Geertz 2004]. Religion is not easily disentangled from non-religion, and in practice the Judeo- Christian tradition has served as the prototype of religion for Western scholars [Saler 2000]. The notion of “world religions” also is a pseudocategory with varying and vague criteria on which III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 2. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 2 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM religions supposedly count as a “world religions” [Masuzawa 2005]. Usually the best strategy is simply to forget about “religion” as a general category and merely to analyze human behavior and patterns of thought. Religion and reductionism Forming a general definition of religion is neither a precondition, nor the ultimate goal of comparative religion [see Pyysiäinen in press c]. The scholar never studies religion as a whole; he or she rather focuses on some specific recurrent pattern in human thought, experience, and behavior; this pattern may be studied as an instance of the more general category of “religious phenomena,” although it is not necessary to make such explicit generalization in each and every individual study. The scholar of religion is free to study all forms of thought and behavior that can be profitably taken as “religious” in the everyday sense of the term. “Profitably” here means that the scholar is able to show some connection between his or her interpretation of the data and that of other scholars of religion. The cognitive science of religion took its first steps in 1975 and 1980, when Dan Sperber [1975] first published his book Rethinking Symbolism and Stewart Guthrie [1980] the paper “A cognitive theory of religion.” Sperber first expressed the idea that the human mental architecture seems to channel the cultural spread of religious ideas, and Guthrie argued that the nature of human perception and cognition leads us to postulate supernatural agents. The new field then truly emerged in the 1990’s with attempts at explaining how the structure of religious rituals is mentally represented and how religious concepts are culturally transmitted [Lawson and McCauley 1990; Boyer 1994; see Pyysiäinen 2008]. Later, also such issues as human evolution and the nature of emotions have been discussed [Boyer 2001; Pyysiäinen 2001; Atran 2002; Tremlin 2006]. Currently, we can find research on such areas as the mental representation of non-natural agent concepts [Barrett and Keil 1996; Barrett 2004; Bering 2006], the evolution of the neuro-cognitive systems that support the acquisition of cultural knowledge [Boyer 1994, 1998, 2001, 2003a], anthropomorphism [Guthrie 1993], and rituals [Lawson and McCauley 1990; McCauley and Lawson 2002; Whitehouse 2000, 2004; Whitehouse and McCauley 2004]. “Cognitive science of religion” is now an established term but the kinds of research it covers no longer form a homogeneous whole. There are such varying research programs as Boyer’s “standard model” [Boyer 2005], the views of religion as an adaptation [see Sanderson 2008; Bulbulia et al. 2008], Whitehouse’s [2004] “modes theory,” and also approaches that emphasize the constructive role of culture [Geertz 2008]. Three things that yet unite these approaches are their multidisciplinary nature, emphasis on explanation, and the view that human behavior must be understood in the light of the cognitive processes that support and direct it. Common objections to the cognitive science of religion are that it is “reductionist” and “explains religion away.” I have dealt with these claims elsewhere and shall here only summarize the main points [see Pyysiäinen in press a, b, c]. All scientific research is reductionist in some sense because it is always made from some specific point of view; therefore, it is always possible to say that all other perspectives have been reduced to the one perspective chosen. Thus, to argue that religion should not be reduced to anything non-religious, implies that religion only exists at some specific level and has an irreducible essence. This is a highly problematic view, as we have seen; the word “religion” names a very heterogeneous category and one person’s religion often is another person’s non-religion [superstition, culture, etc.]. There is no homogenous “religion;” ideological variation can in some instances be greater within the category of religion than between religion and non-religion. What I mean is that it is possible to find two religious discourses that contradict each other more sharply III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 3. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 3 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM than do some third religious discourse and its supposedly non-religious alternative. Buddhist and Islamic doctrines about God, for example, are in sharp contrast, whereas it may at times be difficult to say in what sense the claims presented by liberal-minded Lutheran ministers differ from those of atheists [Nielsen 1985, 37]. Therefore, there cannot be any homogenous “religious language,” notwithstanding the philosophical claims about “religious language” differing radically from all factual language. We cannot study religion in itself, pure and simple; instead, we study written texts, art, social groups and institutions, practices, the behavior of persons, and so forth. Thus, Eliade’s [1969, 132–33] claim that we do not have the right to reduce the religious creations of the human mind “to something other than what they are, namely spiritual creations,” is dubious. When we peel of history, culture, sociality, psychology, and so forth, nothing is left in the end. Thus, it is the attempt to be a true anti-reductionist that ironically leads one to explain [or understand] religion away in the sense that one no longer knows what it is that one is studying. Eliade, of course, could reply that religion is in the essence an attempt at finding authentic existence or something like that, and that it should be studied as such. This, however, is only his view and is shared neither by the millions of people who practice religion, nor by most scholars of religion. The claim that religion should not be “explained away” is based on a confusion between the explanans [that which explains something] and the explanandum [that which is to be explained]: an explanation of a religious fact is presupposed to take the place of the religious fact that is explained. Kelly Bulkeley [2003], for example, writes that “Boyer finds cause to dismiss religious understandings and replace them with cognitive scientific ones” [emphasis added]. Yet it should be clear that explaining religion is something we do as scholars; practicing religion is another matter. It might still be argued that studying religion is dangerous business because it may make religious truth claims dubious in the eyes of believers [see Dennett 2006]. However, such straightforward defense of religion is a purely political matter and should not be masked as valid methodological criticism [see Wiebe 1999]. Whether a given explanation is valid should be judged on the basis of evidence and logical coherence of the argument, not on the basis of a religious [or anti-religious] agenda. Mind matters Sometimes anti-reductionist arguments are based on the claim that religious phenomena should be understood or explained as part of “society” or “culture” and that this immediately renders all psychological arguments dubious [Durkheim 1925; Geertz 1973; cf. Pyysiäinen 2001]. However, this argument was developed before modern cognitive and developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Dan Sperber [1975; 1985; 2006] seems to have been the first to have realized that culture and psychology cannot be in this way opposed, once we understand cultures as sets of ideas that are passed on to others relying on certain mechanisms of memory. This passing on involves such sharing of ideas where everyone understands that others understand them to understand what others understand [and so on …; Dennett 1993, 243–46; see Pyysiäinen in press]. Sperber’s “epidemiology of representations” means the study of the differential spread of concepts and beliefs in populations. Some concepts and beliefs win and some lose in cultural selection because not all concepts fit the human mind equally well. Pascal Boyer has studied the evolution of the neuro-cognitive systems that support the acquisition of cultural knowledge along these lines. He has emphasized the role of the so-called intuitive ontology, that is, a set of ontological categories into which we intuitively assign all objects. The basic intuitive ontological categories are: PERSONS, ANIMALS, PLANTS, ARTIFACTS and NATURAL OBJECTS [Boyer 1994, 101; 1998, 878; 2000, 280]. Counterintuitive representations are formed by adding or deleting a feature that then violates our intuitive expectations. It is possible to add mental III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 4. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 4 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM characteristics to an artifact [e.g. a statue that hears prayers] or to deny a biological body to a person [e.g. gods], for example. Such minimally counterintuitive representations seem to be an important defining characteristic of religion [Pyysiäinen et al. 2003; see Boyer 2001; Barrett 2004]. Scott Atran [2002, 10–11] uses the image of evolutionary history as a landscape formed by different mountain ridges: just as rain converges toward a limited set of rivers and lakes, so also human experience falls into certain basic types determined by the cognitive architecture of our species. Atran refers to Conrad Waddington’s [1959] idea of developmental processes as a complex landscape of hills and branching valleys, descending from a high plateau. This plateau represents the state of the fertilized egg. The valleys are developmental pathways leading to particular end states such as a functioning eye, heart, and so on [see Jablonka and Lamb 2006, 63–65, 261–62]. A set of genes and their mutual interactions forms a developmental system producing a phenotype [as distinguished from a genotype]. Many features of the phenotype are explained by the dynamical properties of the system as a whole, not by individual alleles. Developmental canalization here means a “buffering” against both environmental and genetic parameters: single genes or features of environment cannot bias development that is bound to go a certain way because of the developmental canalization. Canalization thus not only blocks effects of environmental variation but also the effects of variation in certain alleles [Griffiths and Machery 2008, 397–99]. Atran, however, uses the idea of cognitive architecture as canalizing cultural transmission to emphasize that certain ways of thinking are a natural part of human nature. Their independence from cultural construction does not mean that they could develop without a cultural environment; it is rather that their development is not dependent on any particular culture [as Boyer 2003b, 238– 39] puts it. The crossculturally recurrent patterns of religion studied by the early phenomenologists could be explained with reference to the nature of the human mind [especially Boyer 1994]. The epidemiology of representations thus offers an interesting research program for the study of religion. We might finally be able to explain why certain patterns of belief and behavior are so contagious. Underlying the epidemiology of representations is the idea of selectionism: scholars ask why some concepts and beliefs are selected for cultural transmission. Scholars thus are asking population-level questions and their answers are statements about trends in populations, not explanations of the deepest motives of individuals [see Boyer 2001, 319; Pyysiäinen in press d]. As Justin Barrett [2008, 298] recently put it, cognitive theories have not been applied to particular problems, scholars rather studying “why religious rituals appear the way they do generally, why people believe in gods generally,” and so forth. This is also often accompanied by attempts at solving only theoretical problems and to do this by conceptual analysis alone. This is understandable given the different kinds of background of cognitive scientists of religion and the nature of methodological training at many departments of comparative religion. However, it is important to be clear about whether one tries to explain certain recurrent types of beliefs and practices, or something that characterizes the beliefs, experiences, and acts of individuals. Evolutionary explanations do not reveal the deepest unconscious motives of individuals; they only apply to population-level questions. Thus, mere epidemiology is not enough in the study of religion, because then individual-level questions would be left out of the picture. Yet we also need to be able to explain why an individual does or believes something. There is a curious tension in the program of the cognitive science of religion in the sense that, on the one hand, emphasis is shifted from culture to the individual, and yet, on the other hand, it is not so much actual individuals that are studied but individual-level cognitive mechanisms III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 5. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 5 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM [Boyer 1994]. Especially when these are studied within an evolutionary framework, as in Boyer [2001], the objects of explanation are always population-level phenomena; an evolutionary explanation does not account for the unconscious motives of an individual in a unique situation [see Ariew 2003; Pyysiäinen in press d]. Yet we must also bear in mind that mental and cultural representations are not two different types of representations. It is rather a question of two different points of view: either we view a concept or a belief as such, or explore its spread in human populations [Boyer 2003b]. Explanation When Dilthey [1924, 144] once argued in the Neokantian spirit: “Die Natur erklären wir, das Seelenleben verstehen wir,” he could not know anything about things such as cognitive science, neuropsychology, or modern philosophy of science. We no longer think that explanation is always causal and based on formulating general laws [see Pyysiäinen in press a, d]. Also the very idea of “understanding” as well as its use have been critically evaluated [Martin 2000; Ylikoski in press]. Yet some still argue that religion should be “understood” rather than “explained” [see Gothóni 2005]. I shall here not discuss this controversy; instead, I want briefly to outline a view of explanation that might help understand how religion can be explained. Unlike in the old general law model, in modern philosophy of science, explanation is often understood as based on specifying a mechanism that is responsible for the production of a phenomenon, either in an etiological or in a constitutive sense. A mechanism thus produces or supports something. It is something that exists in the real world and thus explanation is not just a deductive argument [as it was in the covering law model]. It is enough that an explanatory generalization is stable in the sense that the specified relation between cause and effect holds under a range of conditions [generally not universal]. Mechanisms are not deterministic; they rather produce a probability distribution over possible outcomes and show that the explanandum is an instance of one of those possible outcomes [Craver 2007, 40; see Railton 1978]. Mechanistic explanation consists in describing the parts, operations, and organization of a mechanism, and showing how the mechanism realizes the phenomenon to be explained. A mechanism is a “set of entities and activities organized such that they exhibit the phenomenon to be explained” [Bechtel 2008, 49; Craver 2007, 5, 99]. Carl Craver’s model of mechanistic explanation is based on the idea of causal relevance. In this view, X is causally relevant with regard to Y if an “ideal intervention I on X with respect to Y is a change in the value of X that changes Y, if at all, only via the change in X” [Craver 2007, 95– 96]. Causation is here understood in a manipulationist sense [Woodward 2003]. As explanations specify counterfactual relationships that are somehow invariant [Craver’s “stable conditions”], it is possible to say what would have happened if the cause of the event had been manipulated by an ideal intervention [Woodward 2003]. An intervention means that we manipulate a variable A in order to see if changes in A have a causal relationship with changes in the variable B. The variables must have measureable values but it is enough that the intervention or manipulation is logically or conceptually possible [Woodward 2003, 94, 114, 127-133]. If a manipulation of A introduces a change in B, A is causally relevant with regard to B. Such causal relevance is best explained using counterfactuals [that is, contrastively]. Take the question: “Did Socrates’ sipping the pint of hemlock cause his death?” If this means: “Did Socrates’ sipping of the pint of hemlock [rather than wine] cause his death?” the answer is “Yes.” But if the question is: “Did Socrates’ sipping [rather than guzzling or in some other way consuming] the pint of hemlock cause his death?” the answer is “No” [Craver 2007, 202–203]. Experimental research relies precisely on such contrastive causal claims. An experiment is arranged in order to find an answer to the question: “Did A [rather than B] cause C?” The experimental group III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 6. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 6 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM and the control group differ only with respect to the value of the putative causal variable [Craver 2007, 203]. Here the contrast serves to specify the explanandum as clearly as possible. Objective relations of causal relevance then explain the effect [Craver 2007, 204]. For the study of religion, such multilevel mechanistic explanation means that it is possible to explain different aspect of religion at differing levels by specifying mechanisms that produce and sustain various religious phenomena. There is no one level at which religion should only be explained. At which level of mechanisms we look for an explanans depends on what we have chosen as the explanandum. If we, for example, want to know why praying has diminished as a practice in this or that society, and then offer a sociological answer to this question, it might be that somebody objects, saying that the real answers are to be found in the brain. This, however, does not lead to a better answer to the original question; it is rather a strategy of changing the explanandum from a sociological phenomenon into a neural one. Sometimes we need to do this, but it is necessary to understand that when we change the explanans, also explanandum is changes [see Craver and Bechtel 2007]. As we want to know different kinds of things about saying prayers, there is room for different kinds of questions. But any specific answer given at some specific level is not necessarily an answer to another question which exists at another level. To take an example, when Pascal Boyer and Pierre Liénard [2006] presented their theory of action ritualization, some of the commentators understood them to be claiming that religious rituals are mere obsessive-compulsive disorders. As Joan Hageman [2006, 619] puts it, she wants to argue “against the theory that cultural ritual behavior is meaningless or that ritual action is solely a by- product of fearbased precautionary and action-parsing systems.” But Boyer and Liénard were not saying that all cultural ritual behavior is blindly produced by the hazard precaution system. Their argument is that actions of an individual are ritualized under specific conditions and that such action ritualization is one component making cultural ceremonies salient and memorable. Not all ritual action is ritualized action. Thus, the mechanisms supporting cultural ritual behavior exist at differing levels, from culture to individuals, cognitive systems, neural systems, and molecular-level events [see Craver 2007; Pyysiäinen in press d]. To say, for example, that “genes have nothing to do with God” is true but only in the sense that there is a component of the mechanism missing at the intermediate level between genes and the idea of God. Explanatory cognitive science of religion can operate at many differing levels of mechanisms and is open for many kinds of cross-disciplinary cooperation. Getting rid of the old idea of an essence of religion helps understand the so-called “religious” phenomena as a natural part of human psychology and behavioral repertoire. References Ariew, André. [2003]. Ernst Mayr’s ‘ultimate/proximate’ distinction reconsidered and reconstructed. Biology and Philosophy 18, 553–65. Atran, Scott. [2002]. In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. New York: Oxford University Press. Barrett, Justin L. [2004]. Why would anyone believe in God? Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Barrett, Justin L., & Frank Keil. [1996]. Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31, 219–47. Bechtel, William. [2008]. Mental mechanisms: Philosophical perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. New York: Routledge. Boyer, Pascal. [1994]. The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley: University of California Press. III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 7. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 7 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM Boyer, Pascal. [1998]. Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist 100[4], 876–89. Boyer, Pascal. [2000]. Natural epistemology or evolved metaphysics? Developmental evidence for early-developed, intuitive, category-specific, incomplete, and stubborn metaphysical presumptions. Philosophical Psychology 13[3], 277–97. Boyer, Pascal. [2001]. Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books. Boyer, Pascal. [2003a]. Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7[3], 119–24. Boyer, Pascal. [2003b]. Are ghost concepts “intuitive,” “endemic” and “innate”? Journal of Cognition and Culture 3[3], 233–43. Boyer, Pascal. [2005]. A reductionistic model of distinct modes of religious transmission. In Harvey Whitehouse & Robert N. McCauley [Eds.], Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive foundations of religiosity, 3–29. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Boyer, Pascal, & Pierre Liénard. [2006]. Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action- parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29[6], 595–650 [with commentaries]. Bulbulia, Joseph, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, & Karen Wyman [eds.]. [2008]. The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, & critiques. Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Family Foundation. Bulkeley, Kelly. [2003]. Review of Religion Explained by Pascal Boyer; How Religion Works by Ilkka Pyysiäinen. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 71, 671–74. Craver, Carl. [2007]. Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press. Craver, Carl, & William Bechtel. [2007]. Top-down causation without top-down causes. Biology and Philosophy 22, 547–63. Day, Matthew. [2005]. The undiscovered and undiscoverable essence: Species and religion after Darwin. Journal of Religion 85[1], 58–82. Dennett, Daniel C. [2006]. Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. New York: Viking. Dilthey, Wilhelm. [1924]. Die Geistige Welt. [Gesammelte Schriften; 5.] Leipzig: Teubner. Durkheim, Émile. [1925/1912]. Les formes élémentaires de la vie religieuse. Paris: Alcan. Fitzgerald, Timothy. [1999]. The ideology of religious studies. New York: Oxford University Press. Geertz, Armin. [2004]. Definition, categorization, and indecision: Or, how to get on with the study of religion. In Unterwegs. Neue Pfade in der Religionswissenschaft / New Paths in the Study of Religions: Festschrift in Honour of Michael Pye on his 65th birthday, edited by Christoph Kleine, Monika Schrimpf, & Katja Triplett, 109–118. München: Biblion. Geertz, Armin. [2008]. From apes to devils and angels: Comparing scenarios on the evolution of religion. In Bulbulia, Joseph, Richard Sosis, Erica Harris, Russell Genet, Cheryl Genet, & Karen Wyman [eds.], The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, & critiques, 43–49. Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Family Foundation. Geertz, Clifford. [1973]. The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books. III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 8. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 8 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM Gothóni, René [Ed.]. [2005]. How to do comparative religion: Three ways, many goals. [Religion and Reason, 44.] Berlin: De Gruyter. Griffiths, Paul E., & Edouard Machery. [2008]. Innateness, canalization, and ‘biologicizing the mind.’ Philosophical Psychology 21[3], 397–414. Guthrie, Stewart. [1980]. A cognitive theory of religion. Current Anthropology 21, 181–203. Guthrie, Stewart [E.]. [1993]. Faces in the clouds. New York: Oxford University Press. Hagen, Joan M: [2006]. Multicultural religious and spiritual rituals: Meaning and praxis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29[6], 619–20. Jablonka, Eva, & Marion J. Lamb. [2006/2005]. Evolution in four dimensions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Jensen, Jeppe Sinding. [2003]. The study of religion in a new key. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press. Kippenberg, Hans G. [2992]. Discovering religious history in the modern age. Tr. From the German by Barbara Harshaw. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Lawson, E. Thomas. [1996]. Theory and the new comparativism, old and new. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion 8, ????. Lawson, E. Thomas, & Robert N. McCauley. [1990]. Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McCauley, Robert N., & E. Thomas Lawson. [2002]. Bringing ritual to mind: Psychological foundations of cultural forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Martin, Michael. [2000]. Verstehen: The uses of understanding in social science. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers. Masuzawa, Tomoko. [2005]. The invention of world religions: Or, how European universalism was preserved in the language of pluralism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Nielsen, Kai. [1985]. Philosophy & atheism: In defense of atheism. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books. Otto, Rudolf. [1958/1917]. The idea of the Holy. Tr. From the German by John W. Harvey. Lodon: Oxford University Press. Paden, William E. [1996]. Elements of a new comparativism. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion 8, 5–14. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [2001]. How religion works: Towards a new cognitive science of religion. Leiden: Brill. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [2008]. Introduction: Religion, cognition, and culture. Religion 38[2], 101–108. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press a]. Reduction and explanatory pluralism in the cognitive science of religion. In Changing minds: Religion and cognition through the ages, edited by István Czachesz & Tamás Bíró. [Groningen Studies in Cultural Change.] Leuven: Peeters. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press b]. Supernatural agents: Why we believe in souls, gods, and buddhas. New York: Oxford University Press. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press c]. Religion and the brain – Cognitive science as a basis for theories of religion. In Theory/Religion/Critique, edited by Richard King. New York: Columbia University Press. III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ
  • 9. III IINTERNATIIONAL ONLIINE CONFERENCE ON RELIIGIIOUS STUDIIES III IN TERNATIO NAL ONLIN E CONFERENCE ON RELIG IO US STUDIE S NTERNAT ONAL ONL NE CONFERENCE ON REL G OUS STUD ES 9 COMPARATIIVE   RELIIGIION:::  FROM   SUBJJECT   TO   PROBLEM      COMPARATIV E R ELIG IO N  F ROM S UBJE CT T O P ROBLEM   COMPARAT VE REL G ON FROM SUB ECT TO PROBLEM Pyysiäinen, Ilkka. [In press d]. Mechanistic explanation of ritualized behavior. In Armin Geertz & Jesper Sørensen [Ed.], Religious ritual, cognition and culture. London: Equinox. Pyysiäinen, Ilkka, Marjaana Lindeman, & Timo Honkela. [2003]. Counterintuitiveness as the hallmark of religiosity. Religion 33[4], 341–55. Railton, Peter. [1978]. A deductive-nomological model of probabilistic explanation. Philosophy of Science 45, 206–226. Rennie, Bryan. [2001]. Changing religious worlds: The meaning and end of Mircea Eliade. Albany; NY: State University of New York Press. Rennie, Bryan [Ed.]. [2006]. Mircea Eliade: A critical reader. London: Equinox. Saler, Benson. [2000/1993]. Conceptualizing religion: Immanent anthropologists, transcendent natives, and unbound categories. With a new preface. New York: Berghahn Books. Sanderson, Stephen K. [2008]. Adaptation, evolution, and religion. Religion 38[2], 141–56. Sperber, Dan. [1995/1975]. Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Sperber, Dan. [1985]. Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man [N.S.] 20, 73–89. Sperber, Dan. [2006]. Conceptual tools for a naturalistic approach to cultural evolution. In Stephen C. Levinson and Pierre Jaisson [Eds.], Evolution of culture: A Fyssen Foundation symposium, 147– 65. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Tremlin, Todd. [2006]. Minds and gods: The cognitive foundations of religion. New York: Oxford University Press. Waddington, Conrad H. [1959]. Canalization of development and genetic assimilation of acquired characters. Nature 183, 1654–55. Whitehouse, Harvey. [2000]. Arguments and icons: Divergent modes of religiosity, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Whitehouse, Harvey. [2004]. Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Whitehouse, Harvey, & Robert N. McCauley [Eds]. [2005]. Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive foundations of religion. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Wiebe, Donald. [1999]. The politics of religious studies. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Woodward, James. [2003]. Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. New York: Oxford University Press. Ylikoski, Petri. [In press]. The illusion of depth of understanding in science. In Henk De Regt, Sabinelli, & Eigner [Eds.], Scientific understanding: Philosophical perspectives. Pittsburgh: Pittsburgh University Press. III   МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ   НАУЧНАЯ   ИНТЕРНЕТ‐‐КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ   ПО   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ    III М ЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ Н АУЧНАЯ И НТЕРНЕТ‐К ОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ П О Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ МЕЖДУНАРОДНАЯ НАУЧНАЯ ИНТЕРНЕТ КОНФЕРЕНЦИЯ ПО РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЮ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ   РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ:::ОТ   ПРЕДМЕТА   К   ПРОБЛЕМЕ    СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ Р ЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ О Т П РЕДМЕТА К П РОБЛЕМЕ СРАВНИТЕЛЬНОЕ РЕЛИГИОВЕДЕНИЕ ОТ ПРЕДМЕТА К ПРОБЛЕМЕ