3800 poster

The Psychological and Physiological Responses to
Frequencies in Music
Mariangela Garcia, Rose Hodson, Manpreet Kaur, Alexis Lopez, & Nicole Martinez
Department of Psychology
California State University, Stanislaus
References
Bruckner,J.B., Hess, S., Schneider,E., Schweichel, E.
(1977). Dopamine effects on the circulation and
myocardial oxygen supply. Journal of the Royal
Society of Medicine, 70, 43-47. needs completion
Kawakami, A., Furukawa, K., Katahira, C., & Okanoya, K.
(2013). Sad music induces pleasant emotion.
Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-15. doi:10.1037/t27579-
000
Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A.,
& Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine
release during anticipation and experience of peak
emtotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14(2), 257-
264. doi:10.1038/nn.2726.
Discussion
In this study, participants were exposed to different
sound audio clips: A= 432 Hz and A= 440 Hz. Audio
preference and enjoyment were assessed alongside
physiological responses – blood oxygen levels and pulse
rate. We expected to find significant changes in heart rate
and blood oxygen levels between conditions. In addition,
we predicted that the A=440 Hz frequency would have a
higher preference rating because it is what modern music
is played in.
Our study concluded that there was a significant
difference in the preference levels of both frequencies.
Participants strongly preferred an audio track played at a
frequency of A=440Hz over an audio track played at a
frequency of A=432Hz. These results show that standard
tuning was more enjoyable, which in turn, supported our
hypothesis.
The results of our study were inconclusive in
comparison to the findings of Bruckner et al. (1977), as
neither heart rate nor blood oxygen levels were accurately
recorded regardless of condition. Study limitations
included a very limited sample size and the readings of
heart rates and O2 levels from our pulse-oximeter were
inaccurate measurements.
Introduction
The relationship between auditory perception and
emotional and physiological responses warrants further
investigation, as encountering and processing sound waves
is a natural phenomenon for living organisms. The
experience we call music affects us in ways that may not
be immediately apparent, and so necessitates research and
experimentation into the various responses elicited by its
components.
explored the inherent contradiction in the enjoyment
of sad music to discover that regardless of the emotional
quality of the sound, a clip of music can produce sensations
of pleasure due to its predictable nature (Kawakami et al.,
2013) . The experience of recognition is in itself a
rewarding process in the brain. Salimpoor et al. (2010)
investigated the flow of dopamine through the brain while
listening to music, focusing on the regions that show an
increase in dopaminergic activity in response to pleasurable
music.
The connection between dopamine production and
heart rate was provided by Bruckner et al. (1977), whose
research discovered that as dopamine levels increase in the
brain, heart rate also increases. From these studies, we can
deduce that there is a strong, positive correlation between
music that is in some way predictable or familiar, thus
causing a higher production of dopamine resulting in an
increased heart rate.
Our study sought to determine any preference
between participants exposed to different frequencies of
music. Half of the participants were exposed to audio clips
played at the standard frequency A=440Hz and the second
half were exposed to audio clips played at a frequency of
A=432Hz. We hypothesized that the A=440Hz group
would show greater preference for the music they listened
to because it was played at a familiar frequency. In
addition, we hypothesized that A=440Hz condition, being
the standard for modern music, and therefore more familiar
frequency, would elicit higher heart rates and blood oxygen
levels than the A=432Hz condition.
Method
Participants
•4 Females, 2 Males.
•Median age 23.8 years, age range 19 to 40 years
•All participants registered via SONA
Design
• Independent Variable: Sound Frequency
• Levels: A=440Hz and A=432Hz
• Between Subject Design
• Dependent Variables: Heart rate and oxygen levels
measured via Pulse Oximeter, frequency preference
measured via Likert Scale to assess music enjoyability
Procedure
• Upon being seated, participants were asked to fill out a
consent form, and complete a short demographics
questionnaire on Qualtrics online survey.
• Participants were then asked to place the Tracks
headphones on their ears, and listen to a randomly
assigned test audio clip in order to adjust volume to their
liking. The test audio clip determined which frequency
condition the participants were exposed to.
• Researchers placed a pulse oximeter onto the left index
finger of the participant.
• Participants then listened to one of 2 audio clips that
were either in A-440Hz, or A- 432Hz while heart rate
and oxygen levels were being recorded.
• Once physiological effects were recorded participants
completed an online survey that recorded their
preference for the music piece.
• At the end of the study, participants were debriefed and
dismissed.
Results
Participants’ physiological response to frequency was
measured by recording the values for heart rate and
blood oxygen levels provided by a pulse oximeter and
frequency preference was assessed by comparing
averages reported on a Likert scale for music
enjoyability.
An independent samples t-test was run to determine if the
difference in average heart rate was significant. The
results showed that the 432Hz condition (M = 98.42, SD
= .51), had no significant difference t(4) = 1.42, .37, p
> .05, in comparison to the 440Hz condition (M = 97.63,
SD = .81). Both groups displayed similar heart rate
averages, as shown in Figure 1.
The results of an independent samples t-test for average
oxygen levels showed that the 432Hz condition (M =
72.12, SD = .7.24), was not significantly different t(4) =
1.69, p > .05, to the 440Hz condition (M =79.27 SD =
1.03), Figure 2. Both groups displayed similar oxygen
levels.
The results of an independent samples t-test for the Likert
scale of music enjoyability showed that A-440 Hz
condition (M = 5, SD = .00), was significantly preferred
over condition A-432 Hz (M = 3.33, SD = .58); t(4) = 5.00,
p.01 < .05, Figure 3.
Above, Figure 1 displays average pulse rate of participants between
conditions. Participant results showed no significant difference between
conditions.
Above, Figure 2 displays average participant oxygen levels between conditions.
Results showed no significant difference, with increased oxygen levels in
condition A – 432Hz.
Above, Figure 3 displays average participant enjoyment recorded on the Likert scale.
Results show that enjoyment level was higher in the A – 440Hz condition (standard
tuning).
http://www.collective-evolution.com/2013/12/21/heres-why-you-should-convert-your-
music-to-432hz/
1 de 1

Recomendados

Hallam 2014 research fair poster por
Hallam 2014 research fair posterHallam 2014 research fair poster
Hallam 2014 research fair posterAnthony Hallam
58 visualizações1 slide
Age, Music Preferences and Exercise por
Age, Music Preferences and ExerciseAge, Music Preferences and Exercise
Age, Music Preferences and ExerciseRaich143
406 visualizações1 slide
20 blogging tips you don't want to miss por
20 blogging tips you don't want to miss 20 blogging tips you don't want to miss
20 blogging tips you don't want to miss Inoshiro Design
323 visualizações22 slides
Using NFC to Crowd Curate Music Preferences por
Using NFC to Crowd Curate Music PreferencesUsing NFC to Crowd Curate Music Preferences
Using NFC to Crowd Curate Music PreferencesUniversity of Central Lancashire
496 visualizações36 slides
Music in palliative care por
Music in palliative careMusic in palliative care
Music in palliative careflorenzcruz
426 visualizações18 slides
Music and music preferences por
Music and music preferences Music and music preferences
Music and music preferences Gomez123456
505 visualizações5 slides

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Similar a 3800 poster

Research Methods Paper por
Research Methods PaperResearch Methods Paper
Research Methods PaperMadalynn Lauria
147 visualizações15 slides
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute Run por
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute RunEffect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute Run
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute RunXanderGarcialExercis
39 visualizações1 slide
The evidence on music therapy.pdf por
The evidence on music therapy.pdfThe evidence on music therapy.pdf
The evidence on music therapy.pdfelmar hasratov
15 visualizações13 slides
MORRIS_SENIOR PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm por
MORRIS_SENIOR  PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental ParadigmMORRIS_SENIOR  PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm
MORRIS_SENIOR PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental ParadigmJeremy Morris
159 visualizações1 slide
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform... por
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...IOSR Journals
370 visualizações4 slides
ARCHIVO PRUEBA por
ARCHIVO PRUEBAARCHIVO PRUEBA
ARCHIVO PRUEBAmercedespr99
43 visualizações11 slides

Similar a 3800 poster(20)

Research Methods Paper por Madalynn Lauria
Research Methods PaperResearch Methods Paper
Research Methods Paper
Madalynn Lauria147 visualizações
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute Run por XanderGarcialExercis
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute RunEffect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute Run
Effect of Music on Perceived Exertion during a 15-minute Run
XanderGarcialExercis39 visualizações
The evidence on music therapy.pdf por elmar hasratov
The evidence on music therapy.pdfThe evidence on music therapy.pdf
The evidence on music therapy.pdf
elmar hasratov15 visualizações
MORRIS_SENIOR PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm por Jeremy Morris
MORRIS_SENIOR  PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental ParadigmMORRIS_SENIOR  PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm
MORRIS_SENIOR PROJECT POSTER Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm
Jeremy Morris159 visualizações
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform... por IOSR Journals
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...
A Study to Explore the Effects of Varied Tempo music on Brisk Walking Perform...
IOSR Journals370 visualizações
ARCHIVO PRUEBA por mercedespr99
ARCHIVO PRUEBAARCHIVO PRUEBA
ARCHIVO PRUEBA
mercedespr9943 visualizações
Musicoterapia slide show por Paula Teresa Almeida
Musicoterapia slide showMusicoterapia slide show
Musicoterapia slide show
Paula Teresa Almeida471 visualizações
poster1 por James Fulks
poster1poster1
poster1
James Fulks25 visualizações
TR310 por Shaun Cavanagh
TR310TR310
TR310
Shaun Cavanagh538 visualizações
Research Proposal por Ryan Pelon
Research ProposalResearch Proposal
Research Proposal
Ryan Pelon232 visualizações
LR3 por Sara Norris
LR3LR3
LR3
Sara Norris189 visualizações
Neural Substrates of Music Learning and Emotions por Psyche Loui
Neural Substrates of Music Learning and EmotionsNeural Substrates of Music Learning and Emotions
Neural Substrates of Music Learning and Emotions
Psyche Loui569 visualizações
The Influence of Binaural Meditative Sounds on Menses-Prosex Processes por ijtsrd
The Influence of Binaural Meditative Sounds on Menses-Prosex ProcessesThe Influence of Binaural Meditative Sounds on Menses-Prosex Processes
The Influence of Binaural Meditative Sounds on Menses-Prosex Processes
ijtsrd25 visualizações
Morris_SENIOR PROJECT POWERPOINT Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm por Jeremy Morris
Morris_SENIOR PROJECT POWERPOINT Music Tempo and Experimental ParadigmMorris_SENIOR PROJECT POWERPOINT Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm
Morris_SENIOR PROJECT POWERPOINT Music Tempo and Experimental Paradigm
Jeremy Morris533 visualizações
wang2016.pdf por nershudiok
wang2016.pdfwang2016.pdf
wang2016.pdf
nershudiok2 visualizações
Mt & parkinson's disease slides for mms por Daniel Tague
Mt & parkinson's disease slides for mmsMt & parkinson's disease slides for mms
Mt & parkinson's disease slides for mms
Daniel Tague1.7K visualizações
Stanford University - Therapeutic Benefits of Musical Rhythm por Paul Quaiser
Stanford University - Therapeutic Benefits of Musical RhythmStanford University - Therapeutic Benefits of Musical Rhythm
Stanford University - Therapeutic Benefits of Musical Rhythm
Paul Quaiser159 visualizações
4 2009-changes in mlae ps following two yoga based relaxation techniques por Elsa von Licy
4 2009-changes in mlae ps following two yoga based relaxation techniques4 2009-changes in mlae ps following two yoga based relaxation techniques
4 2009-changes in mlae ps following two yoga based relaxation techniques
Elsa von Licy253 visualizações
Music Cognition EE Final por Simon Karlsson
Music Cognition EE FinalMusic Cognition EE Final
Music Cognition EE Final
Simon Karlsson1.3K visualizações

3800 poster

  • 1. The Psychological and Physiological Responses to Frequencies in Music Mariangela Garcia, Rose Hodson, Manpreet Kaur, Alexis Lopez, & Nicole Martinez Department of Psychology California State University, Stanislaus References Bruckner,J.B., Hess, S., Schneider,E., Schweichel, E. (1977). Dopamine effects on the circulation and myocardial oxygen supply. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 70, 43-47. needs completion Kawakami, A., Furukawa, K., Katahira, C., & Okanoya, K. (2013). Sad music induces pleasant emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-15. doi:10.1037/t27579- 000 Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emtotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14(2), 257- 264. doi:10.1038/nn.2726. Discussion In this study, participants were exposed to different sound audio clips: A= 432 Hz and A= 440 Hz. Audio preference and enjoyment were assessed alongside physiological responses – blood oxygen levels and pulse rate. We expected to find significant changes in heart rate and blood oxygen levels between conditions. In addition, we predicted that the A=440 Hz frequency would have a higher preference rating because it is what modern music is played in. Our study concluded that there was a significant difference in the preference levels of both frequencies. Participants strongly preferred an audio track played at a frequency of A=440Hz over an audio track played at a frequency of A=432Hz. These results show that standard tuning was more enjoyable, which in turn, supported our hypothesis. The results of our study were inconclusive in comparison to the findings of Bruckner et al. (1977), as neither heart rate nor blood oxygen levels were accurately recorded regardless of condition. Study limitations included a very limited sample size and the readings of heart rates and O2 levels from our pulse-oximeter were inaccurate measurements. Introduction The relationship between auditory perception and emotional and physiological responses warrants further investigation, as encountering and processing sound waves is a natural phenomenon for living organisms. The experience we call music affects us in ways that may not be immediately apparent, and so necessitates research and experimentation into the various responses elicited by its components. explored the inherent contradiction in the enjoyment of sad music to discover that regardless of the emotional quality of the sound, a clip of music can produce sensations of pleasure due to its predictable nature (Kawakami et al., 2013) . The experience of recognition is in itself a rewarding process in the brain. Salimpoor et al. (2010) investigated the flow of dopamine through the brain while listening to music, focusing on the regions that show an increase in dopaminergic activity in response to pleasurable music. The connection between dopamine production and heart rate was provided by Bruckner et al. (1977), whose research discovered that as dopamine levels increase in the brain, heart rate also increases. From these studies, we can deduce that there is a strong, positive correlation between music that is in some way predictable or familiar, thus causing a higher production of dopamine resulting in an increased heart rate. Our study sought to determine any preference between participants exposed to different frequencies of music. Half of the participants were exposed to audio clips played at the standard frequency A=440Hz and the second half were exposed to audio clips played at a frequency of A=432Hz. We hypothesized that the A=440Hz group would show greater preference for the music they listened to because it was played at a familiar frequency. In addition, we hypothesized that A=440Hz condition, being the standard for modern music, and therefore more familiar frequency, would elicit higher heart rates and blood oxygen levels than the A=432Hz condition. Method Participants •4 Females, 2 Males. •Median age 23.8 years, age range 19 to 40 years •All participants registered via SONA Design • Independent Variable: Sound Frequency • Levels: A=440Hz and A=432Hz • Between Subject Design • Dependent Variables: Heart rate and oxygen levels measured via Pulse Oximeter, frequency preference measured via Likert Scale to assess music enjoyability Procedure • Upon being seated, participants were asked to fill out a consent form, and complete a short demographics questionnaire on Qualtrics online survey. • Participants were then asked to place the Tracks headphones on their ears, and listen to a randomly assigned test audio clip in order to adjust volume to their liking. The test audio clip determined which frequency condition the participants were exposed to. • Researchers placed a pulse oximeter onto the left index finger of the participant. • Participants then listened to one of 2 audio clips that were either in A-440Hz, or A- 432Hz while heart rate and oxygen levels were being recorded. • Once physiological effects were recorded participants completed an online survey that recorded their preference for the music piece. • At the end of the study, participants were debriefed and dismissed. Results Participants’ physiological response to frequency was measured by recording the values for heart rate and blood oxygen levels provided by a pulse oximeter and frequency preference was assessed by comparing averages reported on a Likert scale for music enjoyability. An independent samples t-test was run to determine if the difference in average heart rate was significant. The results showed that the 432Hz condition (M = 98.42, SD = .51), had no significant difference t(4) = 1.42, .37, p > .05, in comparison to the 440Hz condition (M = 97.63, SD = .81). Both groups displayed similar heart rate averages, as shown in Figure 1. The results of an independent samples t-test for average oxygen levels showed that the 432Hz condition (M = 72.12, SD = .7.24), was not significantly different t(4) = 1.69, p > .05, to the 440Hz condition (M =79.27 SD = 1.03), Figure 2. Both groups displayed similar oxygen levels. The results of an independent samples t-test for the Likert scale of music enjoyability showed that A-440 Hz condition (M = 5, SD = .00), was significantly preferred over condition A-432 Hz (M = 3.33, SD = .58); t(4) = 5.00, p.01 < .05, Figure 3. Above, Figure 1 displays average pulse rate of participants between conditions. Participant results showed no significant difference between conditions. Above, Figure 2 displays average participant oxygen levels between conditions. Results showed no significant difference, with increased oxygen levels in condition A – 432Hz. Above, Figure 3 displays average participant enjoyment recorded on the Likert scale. Results show that enjoyment level was higher in the A – 440Hz condition (standard tuning). http://www.collective-evolution.com/2013/12/21/heres-why-you-should-convert-your- music-to-432hz/