SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 13
Baixar para ler offline
Journal of Counseling Psychology
Alliance and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for
PTSD: A Study of Within-Person Processes
Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen, Tomas Formo Langkaas, and Bruce E. Wampold
Online First Publication, August 19, 2013. doi: 10.1037/a0033604

CITATION
Hoffart, A., Øktedalen, T., Formo Langkaas, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2013, August 19). Alliance
and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD: A Study of Within-Person Processes.
Journal of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0033604
Journal of Counseling Psychology
2013, Vol. 60, No. 4, 000

© 2013 American Psychological Association
0022-0167/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0033604

Alliance and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD:
A Study of Within-Person Processes
Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen,
and Tomas Formo Langkaas

Bruce E. Wampold
University of Wisconsin Madison and Research Institute,
Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Research Institute, Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway, and
University of Oslo

The present study examined both the intraindividual relationship between alliance components (task,
goal, and bond) and subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms over the course of
therapy and the interindividual relationships between the initial level of the alliance components and
overall PTSD outcome. PTSD patients (n ϭ 65) were randomized to either standard prolonged exposure,
which includes imaginal exposure (IE) to the traumatic memory, or modified prolonged exposure, where
imagery rescripting (IR) of the memory replaced IE as the imagery component of prolonged exposure in
a 10-week residential program. They were assessed repeatedly (weekly) on alliance and PTSD symptom
measures. The centering method of detrending (Curran & Bauer, 2011) was used to separate the variance
related to the intraindividual process of change during treatment (within-person component) from the
variance related to initial individual differences (between-person component). The hypothesis of a
negative within-person effect of the alliance components agreement about the tasks of therapy and bond
on subsequent PTSD symptoms was supported for the component task agreement. As expected, this
effect was stronger in IE than in IR. Moreover, there was a negative relationship between interindividual
differences in initial Task and Bond scale scores and slope of PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy.
By contrast, within-person variations in PTSD symptoms did not predict subsequent alliance components. The present results suggest the importance of agreement about therapy tasks during the process of
IE or IR within prolonged exposure for PTSD patients, particularly in IE.
Keywords: imaginal exposure, imagery rescripting, posttraumatic stress disorder, alliance, process
research

exposure (Foa, Hembree, & Rothbaum, 2007), which has been
most extensively documented as an efficacious treatment for
PTSD, consists of imaginal exposure (IE) to the traumatic memory, repeated listening to tapes of the imagery sessions, and in vivo
exposure to avoided situations and stimuli. Thus, the patient is
asked to approach what has evoked the most anxiety and distress.
During IE, also the therapist is confronted with aversive information, which may evoke emotional responses he or she cannot
express and consequently induce therapist feelings of powerlessness (Arntz, Tiesema, & Kindt, 2007). The strains put on the
patient and the therapist potentially requires an agreement about
these tasks, which suggests that both the patient and the therapist
understand and accept the treatment rationale and believe that the
treatment is an appropriate and beneficial approach to reduce
symptoms (Keller, Zoellner, & Feeny, 2010). Furthermore, the
traumatic experience and its aftermaths often involve helplessness,
shame, guilt, and anger reactions that are difficult to reveal to
another person (Lee, Scragg, & Turner, 2001). Many clients also
fear that IE to the trauma experience will lead to loss of control and
even insanity. All this requires a development of a bond, in which
the patient trusts that the therapist understands, cares for, and
accepts him/her and believes the therapist is able to help the patient
regulate strong emotions (Wampold & Budge, 2012).
Conversely, the demands of the trauma-focused procedures may
lower many patients’ enthusiasm about engaging in the therapy

A stronger therapeutic alliance has been found to be associated
with better outcomes across a variety of treatment approaches and
mental health problems (Flückiger, Del Re, Wampold, Symonds,
& Horvath, 2012; Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, & Symonds, 2011).
According to the most widely accepted transtheoretical model,
alliance is composed of agreement about the tasks of therapy,
agreement about the goals of therapy, and the emotional bond
between patient and therapist (Bordin, 1979). These components
may have a different role and influence depending on the treatment
approach and the problem being treated (Ulvenes et al., 2012;
Webb et al., 2011). The task and bond components should be
particularly influential on outcome in trauma-focused treatments
of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) because of the demands
put on both the patient and the therapist. For instance, prolonged

Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen, and Tomas Formo Langkaas, Research
Institute, Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway, and Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Bruce E. Wampold, Department of
Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin Madison, and Research
Institute, Modum Bad.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Asle
Hoffart, Research Institute, Modum Bad, N-3370 Vikersund, Norway.
E-mail: asle.hoffart@modum-bad.no
1
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2

HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

tasks and also lead them to question the therapist’s care for them.
Therefore, there should be much variation in the levels of task
agreement and bond, both between patients and within patients
over time, which in turn might well covary with outcome. An
agreement about goals is necessary in trauma-focused therapy as
well. However, there should be more uniformity of levels of
agreement about the goals because the goals of trauma-focused
therapy—reducing the fear of the trauma memory and of the
trauma reminders (Foa et al., 2007)—should be strongly endorsed
by the patients as well as the therapists. Consequently, restricted
range reduces the possibility of finding covariation between agreements of goals and outcome. In general, task agreement has been
found to be more strongly related to outcome than goal agreement
and bond have (Horvath, 2011). With respect to PTSD patients,
early alliance has been shown to predict their adherence to prolonged exposure (Keller et al., 2010) and their emotion regulation
skills and outcome in a two-phase stabilization/skill development
and exposure therapy for childhood abuse-related PTSD (Cloitre,
Stovall-McClough, Miranda, & Chemtob, 2004). Based on this
literature, we specifically expected that task agreement in particular, and perhaps bond as well, would predict better weekly as well
as overall outcome in trauma-focused therapy of PTSD.
Although the IE component of prolonged exposure is an effective intervention for trauma-related fear through the mechanisms
of habituation and experienced nonoccurrence of feared event (Foa
et al., 2007), it may be less effective for other trauma-related
emotions such as shame, guilt, and anger. Repeated exposure to a
traumatic memory involving shame and guilt may provide little
corrective information and actually run the risk of reinforcing
these emotions (Dalgleish & Power, 2004). To address the range of
emotions in PTSD, some authors have advocated (Arntz et al.,
2007) the addition of an element of imagery rescripting (IR;
Smucker, 2005), in which an imagined change of the course of
events of the trauma memory is induced. In a randomized controlled trial (RCT), Arntz et al. (2007) compared a combination of
IE and IR to IE alone. They found no difference in reduction of
PTSD severity but did find the IE and IR combination to be more
effective for anger control, externalization of anger, hostility, and
guilt, especially at 1-month follow-up. The IR method used in this
study was to provide the patient with an opportunity to discover
and express in imagery any trauma-related inhibited emotional
responses (e.g., anger about what happened). The present study,
the data for which was obtained in an RCT, replicates and extends
the study of Arntz et al. by using a broader form of IR developed
by Smucker (2005). In this method, the patient’s current self
is—after an initial imagery reliving phase—invited to enter the
imagery at the worst moment of the trauma, bring the situation to
a solution (e.g., overpower a perpetrator), and then interact with
the traumatized self back then. The patient’s anger is used as a
resource in overpowering perpetrators and the current self–
traumatized self interaction stimulates the development of selfcompassion instead of shame, guilt, and self-critique. The empowering and relieving features of IR may put less strain on the patient
and the therapist by making them feel less helpless and distressed
compared to IE and thus help them both to engage in imagery
work. In the study by Arntz et al., therapists tended to favor the
combination of IE and IR, as it decreased their feelings of helplessness compared to IE alone. Supporting the effectiveness of the
broader form of IR, Grunert, Weis, Smucker, and Christianson

(2007) found in an open trial that IR was extremely helpful for
PTSD patients who had previously not profited from IE. The
present study does not focus on therapy outcome per se but on how
the influence of the alliance on outcome may relate to the specific
trauma-focused therapy model being applied. We expected that,
due to the empowering and relieving features of IR compared to
IE, the influence of task agreement and bond on subsequent PTSD
symptoms would be weaker in IR than in IE.
Understanding the nature of the alliance depends on the methods
used to examine it. For example, the well-established alliance/
outcome relationship is cross-sectional (i.e., bivariate observations
for each psychotherapy dyad) and is thus focused on betweenperson differences (i.e., interindividual processes). That is, variations between patients in early alliance have been found to correlate with between-patient variations in outcome at the end of
therapy (Horvath et al., 2011). However, it is also important to
consider the development of the alliance for a particular patient.
For example, the rupture-repair model (Safran & Muran, 1996)
assumes that alliance ruptures represent opportunities for patients
to learn about their problems relating to others, and repairs represent such opportunities having been taken in the here-and-now of
the therapeutic relationship. This process is indicated by marked
drops in alliance followed by a quick return to previous or higher
levels, which represents within-person variations in the alliance. In
general, therapy models, and particularly therapists, focus on
within-person relationships, which would be the case, for example,
when a change in the alliance for a particular patient leads to a
subsequent alleviation of PTSD symptoms in that patient.
The typical alliance data, collected once early in therapy, or
occasionally during therapy, are unsuitable for evaluating withinperson processes (Curran & Bauer, 2011). Only repeated measures
data allow for the proper disaggregation of between-person and
within-person effects (Curran & Bauer, 2011; Hoffman & Stawski,
2009). Such a disaggregation not only allows the study of withinperson processes separated from between-person effects, but also
is able to examine cross-level interactions of between- and withinperson effects. For instance, the effect of having a stronger alliance
than expected for a particular patient may matter more for patients
who have lower alliance in general. When the general (betweenperson) level of bond is low, for example when the patient has low
trust that the therapist wants the best for him/her and is therefore
preoccupied with this issue, a certain increase of this trust in a
particular session might be a valued event with an immediate
effect on symptoms. On the other hand, when the patient’s trust is
already high and is not an issue for him/her, the same increase
would probably have fewer consequences. That is, one should
expect within-person variations in alliance to affect PTSD symptoms more when the between-person level of alliance is low.
So far, the ability to separate these effects has not been fully
capitalized upon in alliance research. Two notable exceptions are
the studies of Tasca and Lampard (2012) and Falkenström, Granström, and Holmqvist (2013). Using latent change score modeling,
in which between- and within-person components of both the
predictor and outcome variables are separated, Tasca and Lampard
obtained evidence for a reciprocal influence of alliance to the
patient group and outcome among eating disordered individuals.
Using the disaggregation methods in multilevel models proposed
by Curran and Bauer (2011), Falkenström et al. also found evidence for a reciprocal causal model of alliance and outcome in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME

primary care psychotherapy. Based on the results of these welldesigned studies, we expected to find that over the course of
trauma-focused therapy, prior growth in task and bond would be
associated with subsequent reduction in PTSD symptoms, and
prior reduction in PTSD symptoms would be associated with
subsequent growth in task and bond.
The main purpose of the present study was to examine the role
of alliance components in the process of therapeutic time-specific
change in patients diagnosed with PTSD. The patients received
either standard prolonged exposure, which includes IE, or modified prolonged exposure, where IR replaced IE as the imagery
component of prolonged exposure, in a 10-week residential program. They were assessed repeatedly (weekly) on alliance and
PTSD symptom measures, allowing us to separate the variance
related to individual differences (between-person component) at
the start of treatment from variance related to the intraindividual
process of change during treatment (within-person component). To
summarize, we wanted to examine the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: Time-specific change in a patient’s task and
bond components of the alliance over the course of therapy are
negatively related to subsequent change in PTSD symptoms
assessed 3 days later (within-person effect). That is, when the
task agreement and bond for a given patient is higher than is
expected for that patient, subsequent symptoms will be lower.
Hypothesis 2: Time-specific change in a patient’s PTSD
symptoms over the course of therapy are negatively related to
subsequent change in task agreement and bond assessed 4
days later (within-person effect). That is, when the PTSD
symptoms for a given patient are less than is expected for that
patient, subsequent task agreement and bond will be higher.
Hypothesis 3: Individual differences in task agreement and
bond at the start of imagery therapy are negatively related to
individual differences in the rate of change of PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy (between-person effect). That
is, patients who have a higher task agreement and bond at the
start of imagery therapy will have a more negative rate of
change of PTSD symptoms.
Hypothesis 4: There is a cross-level interaction of betweenperson and within-person effects. That is, the lower the level
of task agreement and bond is at the start of imagery therapy,
the stronger the relationship between time-specific change in
alliance and subsequent change in PTSD symptoms will be
during therapy, and the higher the level of task agreement and
bond is at the start of imagery therapy, the weaker the relationship between time-specific change in alliance and subsequent change in PTSD symptoms will be during therapy.
Hypothesis 5: The within-person effect of task agreement and
bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms is stronger for IE within
prolonged exposure than for IR within prolonged exposure.
We also wanted to explore the relationships between goal agreement and PTSD symptoms but expected the magnitude of this
relation to be less than the magnitudes for task agreement and
bond.

3
Method

Participants
The participants were selected from referrals to a PTSD treatment program at a national clinic. The clinic was established for
the residential treatment of nonpsychotic patients who lack adequate local treatment opportunities or have not responded adequately to outpatient care and require more extensive and/or specialized treatment. The study eligibility was similar to treatment
eligibility, that is, all patients who were considered to potentially
benefit from the PTSD treatment were included. The inclusion
criteria were (a) satisfying Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD, (b) PTSD identified as the primary disorder in need of treatment, (c) age 18 to 67 years (regulated by the hospital), and (d) accepting withdrawal of all
psychotropic medication (regulated by the hospital—patients referred to the hospital have usually received medication without
effect). The exclusion criteria were (a) extensive dissociative
symptoms, (b) current suicidal risk, (c) current psychosis, and (d)
ongoing trauma (e.g., current involvement in an abusive relationship). The study was approved by the Regional Ethics Committee,
and the patients’ gave informed consent after the procedures had
been fully explained.
A flow chart of patients is presented in Figure 1. Seventy-one
patients were found eligible for treatment at the assessment stay
and admitted to treatment from December 2008 to November
2010. At admission, all these 71 patients were found to meet
research criteria, but three of them declined participation. One
patient dropped out from treatment before randomization because
she changed her mind about receiving trauma-focused therapy.
The remaining 67 patients were randomized, 33 to IE within
prolonged exposure and 34 to IR within prolonged exposure. Two
IE patients lost their eligibility after randomization— one was
found to need an eating disorder focus to the exclusion of imagery
work, and another was inadvertently treated by the IR protocol.
Thus, our intent-to-treat (ITT) with imagery sample consisted of
65 patients—31 IE and 34 IR patients—who signed consent, were
randomized to an imagery condition, and were not removed by the
investigators. Of these, three patients— one IE and two IR patients— dropped out within 5 to 6 weeks into the program. The
reasons for dropout were conflict with therapist in two cases and
serious somatic illness in one case. One IR patient received a
restricted dose of rescripting, as she insisted to focus on her
relationship to her parents after three sessions in accordance with
the IR manual. This left a completer sample of 61 patients—30 IE
and 31 IR patients.
The mean age of 65 patients—38 women and 27 men—was 45.2
years (SD ϭ 9.7). The mean length of time since the index trauma
was 17.5 years (SD ϭ 13.3). The most prevalent index trauma,
defined as the one experienced by the patient as currently most
distressing or most frequently reexperienced or both, among the 38
women was nonsexual assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 12;
31.6%), sexual assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 9; 23.7%), and
sexual assault by a stranger (n ϭ 8; 21.1%). Among the 27 men,
war experience was most frequent (n ϭ 7; 25.9%), followed by
assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 6; 22.2%) and accidents (n ϭ 4;
14.8%). Over half the index traumas were prolonged and/or re-
HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

4

Assessed for eligibility (N = 71)

Excluded (n = 4)
♦ Declined to participate (n = 3)
♦ Dropped out before randomization (n = 1)

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Randomized (n = 67)

Allocated to imagery exposure (n = 33)
♦ Received full intervention (n = 30)
♦
♦

Allocated to imagery rescripting (n=34)
♦ Received full intervention (n = 31)

Lost eligibility (n = 2)
Dropped out after 6 weeks (n = 1)

♦

Analyzed (n = 31)
♦ Excluded because lost eligibility (n = 2)

Figure 1.

Changed focus after 6 weeks (n = 1)

♦

Dropped out after 5 weeks (n = 2)

Analyzed (n = 34)
♦ Excluded from analysis (n = 0)

Flow of patients through the study.

peated events. Among the 65 patients, 40 (61.5%) had current
major depression or dysthymia, 44 (67.7%) had panic disorder
with or without agoraphobia or agoraphobia without a history of
panic disorder, 39 (60.0%) social phobia, 16 (24.6%) obsessive–
compulsive disorder (Axis I), 11 (16.9%) generalized anxiety
disorder, 18 (27.7%) alcohol abuse/dependence, 11 (16.9%)
avoidant personality disorder, 9 (13.9%) substance abuse/dependence, and 9 (13.9%) obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.
No other diagnosis exceeded a proportion of 10% in the present
sample. According to chi-square tests, there were no diagnostic
differences between the patients in the two treatment conditions.

Measures
PTSD Symptom Scale–Interview (PSS-I). The PSS-I (Foa,
Riggs, Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993) is a semistructured interview
consisting of 17 items corresponding to the DSM–IV PTSD symptoms. Both PTSD diagnosis and PTSD symptom severity are
assessed. Items are rated on 0 –3 scales for combined frequency
and severity in the past 2 weeks (0 ϭ not at all, 1 ϭ once per week
or less/a little bit, 2 ϭ 2 to 4 times per week/somewhat, and 3 ϭ
5 or more times per week/very much). Symptom severity is determined by the sum of the 17 ratings. The PSS-I has demonstrated
satisfactory internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ϭ .85),
high interrater agreement (interclass correlation [ICC] ϭ .97), high
1-month test–retest reliability (r ϭ .80), good concurrent validity
with other measures of psychopathology, and excellent convergent
validity with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R
(SCID; Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1988), correctly identifying the PTSD status of 94% of the studied subjects (Foa et al.,
1993). The PSS-I was translated into Norwegian (see later) and
used as the primary outcome measure in this study. Ten pretreat-

ment and 10 posttreatment PSS-I interviews were randomly selected from the total sample of interviews and scored independently. Interrater agreement for the PSS-I total score was evaluated
by means of ICC (3, 1; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979), with a value of .91
at pretreatment and .95 at posttreatment.
PTSD Symptom Scale–Self-Report (PSS-SR). The PSS-SR
(Foa et al., 1993) is a self-report version of the PSS-I and was used
as a suboutcome measure in the present study. This measure is
usually rated for the last week, but the rating period was shortened
to the last 3 days in this study. The frequency part of the criteria
was changed correspondingly (0 ϭ not at all, 1 ϭ 1 time/sometimes, 2 ϭ 2 times/half of the time, 3 ϭ 3 or more times/almost
always. As for the PSS-I, symptom severity is determined by the
sum of the 17 ratings. PSS-SR symptom severity has demonstrated
satisfactory internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ϭ .91),
high 1-month test–retest reliability (r ϭ .74), good concurrent
validity with other measures of psychopathology, and excellent
convergent validity with the SCID, correctly identifying the PTSD
status of 86% of the studied subjects (Foa et al., 1993). The PSS-I
and the PSS-SR were translated to Norwegian by the first and the
third author and back-translated to English by a native-Englishspeaking professional also competent in Norwegian, until satisfactory formulations were found. Internal consistency reliability of
the first-week PSS-SR rating was .88. One-week test–retest reliability coefficient for the PSS-SR scores from the first to the
second week (before the more active therapy components were
introduced) was .70. Concurrent validity was supported by a
correlation of .68 between the first-week PSS-SR scores and
pretreatment PSS-I scores.
Working Alliance Inventory–Short Revised (WAI-SR).
The WAI-SR (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006) is a shortened 12-item
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME

version of the original 36-item WAI (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989).
Items are rated on a 1–7 Likert-type scale, and subscale scores for
the task (four items), goal (four items) and bond (four items)
components of alliance are computed by averaging across items.
The WAI-SR has been found to differentiate well between these
three components and has shown high internal consistency reliability (subscale score alphas ranging from .85 to .90) and high
correlations with other alliance scales (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006).
The WAI-SR has been translated to Norwegian and backtranslated to English until satisfactory formulations have been
found (Horvath, 1981, 1984, 1991/2006). The internal consistency
reliabilities of the four-item Task, Goal, and Bond subscales at the
first assessment for the second week were .90, .91, and .85,
respectively, and their 1-week test–retest reliabilities from the
second to the third week were .72, .80, and .80, respectively.

Procedure
During a 3-day assessment stay, one of two research psychologists (the second and the third authors) evaluated the applicants by
conducting the PSS-I to ascertain the diagnosis of PTSD, whereas
the two individual therapists associated with the program evaluated the overall eligibility for the program. At the patients’ admission to the program (pretreatment), one of the two research psychologists conducted a comprehensive interview consisting of the
PSS-I, the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI;
Sheehan et al., 1994), and the Structural Clinical Interview for
Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Spitzer, Gibbon,
Williams, & Benjamin, 1994). The PSS-I was also conducted at
discharge (posttreatment), but this time by a psychologist not
involved in the study and blind to the patients’ treatment condition.
The alliance measure (together with other process measures not
analyzed here) was completed every Friday morning. The patients
were asked to base their ratings on their experiences during the last
4 days, that is, during the most treatment-intensive part of the
week. The PSS-SR was completed every Monday morning. The
patients were asked to base their ratings on their experiences
during the last 3 days, that is, during a less treatment-intensive
period. To control for potential expectancy bias with respect to the
alliance measure, patients were informed that the therapists were
blind to the process ratings.

Design and Randomization
The patients received 10 individual sessions lasting 90 min over
a period of 10 weeks. After 1 week of treatment (two first sessions
according to the prolonged exposure protocol), the patients were
randomized to either IE or IR as the imagery component of the
treatment. A person who was not affiliated with the research team
organized the randomization procedure. Random sequences generated from http://www.random.org were used for assignment to
conditions. A blocked randomization procedure was used in which
each therapist was assigned an equal number of cases in each
condition. The probability of every patient ending up in any of the
two conditions was kept constant at 0.5, and no measures were
taken to correct for any imbalance in numbers between the conditions due to discontinued treatments.

5

Treatment
The outpatient manuals for prolonged exposure, including IE
(Foa et al., 2007) and IR (Smucker, 2005), were used but adapted
for the inpatient setting. Essentially, it meant that milieu therapists
were available to assist in between-session assignments (in vivo
exposure, listening to tapes of the imagery work) and to provide
safety and support after intensive individual sessions. The first two
individual sessions were the same for all patients and consisted of
giving a general treatment rationale and providing trauma education (first session) and introducing and planning in vivo exposure
by constructing an exposure hierarchy (second session). Then,
before the third session, patients were stratified by therapist and
randomly allocated to either the IE or the IR condition, after which
they followed the relevant protocols for the third (occurring toward
the end of the second week of treatment) to ninth session. In the
tenth and final session, the content was again identical and consisted of imagery exposure to the total memory, a review of
progress, and suggestions of continued practice. In the sixth week,
the patients returned home to test their newly acquired skill in their
natural environment. All the time, there was one other treatment
group of anxiety patients at the ward, and the PTSD patients
participated in the ward’s general program, consisting of one
physical exercise session and one ward meeting per week.
The IE approach consisted of having participants relive the
traumatic event in their imagination and recount the memory in the
present tense. To increase vividness, patients were asked to report
as much detail as possible, including sights, sounds, smells, behaviors, bodily sensations, feelings, and thoughts. The memory
was repeated if necessary to allow total reliving for a period of 40
to 60 min. The entire memory was relived during the first two or
three sessions. In the subsequent sessions, the hot spots procedure
was usually applied, where reliving was focused on the currently
most distressing parts of the memory.
The IR approach consisted of three continuous phases. The first
phase consisted of imagery reliving of traumatic event in order to
activate the trauma memory and to identify the hot spot(s). In
Phase 2, without pause in imagery, the memory was relived from
the beginning, but this time—at the identified hot spot—the patient
was asked to imagine the current self entering the scene at the hot
spot and bringing the situation to a solution (overpowering the
perpetrators or updating the traumatized self back then with future
information). Finally, in Phase 3, patients were stimulated to
imagine an interaction between the current self and the traumatized
self back then. As in IE, the imagery was supposed to last 40 to 60
min.

Therapists
One of the individual therapists was a 57-year-old male clinical
psychologist with a PhD. The other was a 55-year-old female
psychiatric nurse with a master’s degree. The milieu therapists
were four psychiatric nurses ranging from 45 to 60 years old. All
the individual and milieu therapists had at least 10 years of
experience in the cognitive therapy programs for anxiety disorders
at the unit and had completed the cognitive therapy specialization
program provided by the Norwegian Association of Cognitive
Therapy. Of the 65 ITT patients, the psychologist treated 16 IR
patients and 15 IE patients, whereas the nurse individual therapist
treated 18 IR patients and 16 IE patients.
6

HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

Training and Supervision
All the staff received prestudy workshops and supervision by
experts Elizabeth Hembree (in prolonged exposure including IE)
and Mervin R. Smucker (in IR) during several pilot treatment
groups. Throughout the study period, all of the individual sessions
were videotaped, and each of the experts provided 90-min supervision sessions of taped imagery biweekly. In addition, the first
author provided two 60-min supervision sessions per week to the
milieu staff and individual therapists in a group format.

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Treatment Integrity
The Treatment Integrity Checklist (Foa, Hearst-Ikeda, Dancu,
Hembree, & Jaycox, 1997) contains items describing essential and
desirable ingredients of prolonged exposure therapy across the 10
sessions described in the manual (Foa et al., 2007). As we were
particularly interested in assessing the imagery component, which
was the only intended difference between the two treatment conditions, we rated the eight items of the Prolonged Exposure Sessions 4 –9, Section C: Imaginal Exposure. Three of the eight items
refer to ingredients that are obligatory (e.g., “reviews instructions
for imaginal exposure”), whereas five refer to ingredients that
should be present if needed (e.g., “titrates the experience as
needed”). Based on discussions with the originator of IR, Mervin
R. Smucker, a corresponding checklist for this method was constructed. It consisted of the same three obligatory items as for the
IE checklist, one unique obligatory item, and six unique per-asneeded items (e.g., “identifies relevant action impulses coming
from the client and helps the client to implement them within
imagery”). A score for percentage adherence during an imagery
episode is computed by dividing the number of obligatory and
per-as-needed ingredients present by the total number of items
rated. An overall adequacy (competence) rating for the episode
was given using a 1–5 scale with the anchor points poor, mediocre,
satisfactory, good, and excellent. Finally, the presence or absence
of IR elements was rated. The expert on the therapy form (Elizabeth Hembree or Mervin R. Smucker) rated the episode together
with the first and the second author, whereas the third author did
simultaneous translation of the videotape. A pilot case in each
therapy form was first rated and discussed to calibrate the ratings.
Then, 10 random cases, stratified for order of treatment group in
the trial and individual therapist, from each therapy form were
selected. From these 20 cases, the imagery part of the fifth individual session was rated. One of the cases turned out to be the one
who was inadvertently treated by IR instead of IE (see Participants), and this case was omitted from all analyses. Thus, 19
(4.3%) of the total of 440 sessions including the specific imagery
component were analyzed. The intraclass correlation (ICC [3, 2];
Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) was .69 in IE and .92 in IR for adherence
and .93 in IE and .87 in IR for adequacy.
The results are based on the expert ratings. Mean adherence
rating was 75% (SD ϭ 15%) in IE and 80% (SD ϭ 21%) in IR.
Mean adequacy rating was 2.78 (SD ϭ 1.30) in IE, corresponding
to a level a little below satisfactory, and 3.20 (SD ϭ 1.32) in IR,
corresponding to a level a little above satisfactory. One minor
protocol violation was detected in one of the IE sessions, where the
therapist asked questions typical of IR for a couple of minutes.
After the trial, we asked the individual therapists to fill in a
questionnaire about their preference for IE or IR. The psychologist

indicated no preference, whereas the psychiatric nurse reported
preference for IR because she felt patients’ experience of taking
the power from the perpetrator was particularly helpful.

Statistical Analysis
A main purpose of this study was to examine how within-person
changes in components of alliance affected subsequent withinperson changes in outcome. Such a focus on within-person processes necessitates a proper disaggregation of the within-person
and between-person components of change in the time-varying
predictor. The choice of method of disaggregating within-person
and between-person effects in a time-varying predictor depends on
how it is related to time (Curran & Bauer, 2011). Specifically, it is
important to know if this relationship is characterized by a fixed
effect of time or if it is characterized by both a fixed and random
effect of time. To estimate these parameters, we conducted several
series of mixed models using the three alliance scales (WAI-Task,
WAI-Goal, WAI-Bond) and the PTSD symptom measure (PSSSR) as dependent variables. The intent-to-treat sample was analyzed, and due to our research purposes, scores were included from
the start of the imagery part of therapy (from the second week of
treatment). Moreover, as only active treatment time was of interest,
ratings from the week at home were not included, and the home
week was not counted in the time term. The fit of these nested
models for the covariance was compared by using the likelihood
ratio test, in which the difference in model –2 log likelihood values
is divided by the difference in degrees of freedom of the models
(Fitzmaurice, Laird, & Ware, 2004). Restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used to estimate nested models with only
varying random effects (Fitzmaurice et al., 2004). Models with
different fixed effects were compared using maximum likelihood
estimation. We used an unstructured covariance structure for the
random effects, thus allowing the estimation of covariance between the random intercepts and slopes. By contrast, we used a
diagonal covariance structure for the residuals, thus allowing the
variances of the residuals to differ over time points but setting the
covariance between the residuals across time points to zero. Thus,
the correlation between the scores across assessments had to be
modeled exclusively by the random effects. We started with a
model with only a fixed intercept and no random effects, added a
random intercept, and, finally, added a random effect of week in
therapy. After the best random effects structure had been found in
this way, we tested whether another residual covariance structure
besides the diagonal—for example, a first-order autoregressive
(e.g., AR(1), Toeplitz)— could improve model fit.
We then tested whether the inclusion of a fixed linear time term
(week in therapy) and—in a second step—a fixed quadratic time
term (week2) as independent variables improved model fit. Again,
the fit of these nested models was compared by using the likelihood ratio test.
For all the alliance scales, a fixed and random intercept and a
fixed and random linear effect of time gave the best model fit.
Moreover, no alternative residual covariance structure to the diagonal turned out to improve the fit. For the PSS-SR scores as well,
a fixed and random intercept and a fixed and random effect of time
turned out to be the most appropriate model. In addition, an AR(1)
residual covariance structure improved model fit compared to the
diagonal structure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME

In order to disaggregate the within-person and between-person
variability in the time-varying alliance and symptom measures, we
utilized the statistical centering method of detrending presented by
Curran and Bauer (2011). This method was chosen because these
time-varying predictors were characterized by a fixed and random
effect of time. We created two new variables representing the
within-person change and between-person differences for bond,
task, goal, and PSS-SR scores, respectively (see the applied equations in the Appendix). First, we created the within-person predictor by regressing the variables on time separately for each individual using ordinary least squares. The resulting within-person
deviations over weeks in therapy represent the within-person component of the time-varying alliance and symptom measures. In this
way, the within-person deviations are conceptualized as the difference between a time-specific observation and the trend line for
the variable (i.e., the expected value given a linear growth in the
variable).
Due to our present research purpose to examine the effect of
between-person differences in alliance at the start of the differing
imagery therapies, we used the estimated differences on the timevarying predictors at this time point (second week of treatment) to
represent their between-person component. By setting time to zero
at this point, the between-person component of the time-varying
measures are represented by the estimated intercept at the second
week for each individual.
To correct for the possibility of Type I error, the chosen alpha
significance level of .05 was divided by the number of tests (two)
for each hypothesis, yielding a level of .025 for the individual test.
Because all of the hypotheses were directional in nature, one-tailed
tests were used. The effect size (ES) of the overall outcome was
computed as Hedges’s g for dependent samples (Borenstein,
Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). ESs of the between-person
and within-person effects were calculated as the proportion of
explained outcome variance for each predictor (Snijder & Bosker,
1999; see the applied equations in the Appendix). We used the
program SPSS 19.0.

Results
Overall Outcome
In the following ITT analyses, pretreatment PSS-I ratings substituted missing posttreatment ratings. Due to a failure in administrative routines, one IE patient missed the pretreatment PSS-I
interview, and his ratings were substituted by the first and the last
PSS-SR score. On the PSS-I, the 34 IR patients changed from
33.32 (SD ϭ 6.88) at pretreatment to 22.71 (SD ϭ 14.27) at
posttreatment, yielding an ES of Ϫ0.83, 95% CI [Ϫ0.46, Ϫ1.20].
The corresponding change among the 31 IE patients was from
35.19 (SD ϭ 8.24) to 19.90 (SD ϭ 13.76), with an ES of Ϫ1.27,
95% CI [Ϫ0.76, Ϫ1.78]. In the total sample of 65 patients, the ES
was Ϫ1.06, 95% CI [Ϫ0.74, Ϫ1.38]. A time by treatment
repeated-measures analysis of variance yielded a time effect, F(1,
63) ϭ 69.87, p Ͻ .0001, but no treatment effect, F(1, 63) ϭ 0.04,
ns., or time by treatment effect, F(1, 63) ϭ 2.27, p ϭ .137
(two-tailed).

7

Summary Statistics for the Weekly Outcome and
Alliance Measures
Missing data in the intent-to-treat sample during active imagery
treatment was 6.4% for PSS-SR scores, 9.8% for Task scores,
10.0% for Goal scores, and 10.5% for Bond scores. The mean
between-person PSS-SR score at the second week (estimated intercept) was 31.31 (SD ϭ 9.06). At the second week of treatment,
mean between-person Task score (estimated intercept) was 5.33
(SD ϭ 1.17), Goal score was 5.65 (SD ϭ 1.12), and Bond score
was 5.14 (SD ϭ 1.34). An F test for comparing variances in
correlated variables showed that the standard deviations of the
between-person Task, Goal, and Bond scores were not significantly different. The standard deviations of the within-person
Task, Goal, and Bond scores were 0.4752, 0.4367, and 0.4127,
respectively. An F test showed that Task scores had larger variances than did Goal and Bond scores (both ps Ͻ .025). The
intercorrelations for the estimated between-person alliance scores
at the second week (intercept) were high: .87 for Task and Goal,
.62 for Task and Bond, and .73 for Goal and Bond. The intercorrelations for the within-person alliance scores over the course of
imagery treatment were more moderate: .64 for Task and Goal, .46
for Task and Bond, and .51 for Goal and Bond.

Testing Hypotheses
Our weekly outcome measure—the PSS-SR—was used as dependent variable in mixed models with random intercept and slope
and an AR(1) covariance structure for the residuals (see the Statistical Analysis section). Time (week), treatment (IR vs. IE), and
the within-person and between-person components of the three
WAI scales were used as predictors. Separate analyses were conducted for each scale. To establish a temporal sequence between
predictor and outcome, within-person alliance scores were lagged
and thus related to the PSS-SR scores the following week (3 days
later).
A summary of the fixed main effects for the three alliance
components (viz., task, goal, and bond) on PTSD symptoms, as
well as the random effects, are shown in Table 1. Our first
hypothesis, about a negative within-person effect of task agreement and bond on subsequent symptoms, was supported for the
Task scale. That is, if a patient had stronger agreement on tasks in
a given week than would be predicted for that patient given his/her
general trend, then this patient’s subsequent (3 days later) symptoms were lower than would be expected. The Goal and Bond
scales showed no such within-person effect.
Unrelated to our hypotheses, Table 1 also shows that there was
a negative relationship between interindividual differences in initial Task scores and mean level of PTSD symptoms over the
course of therapy but no such relationship for the other two WAI
scales. In addition, there was a negative effect of time, which
indicates that the PSS-SR scores were reduced over the course of
therapy. There was no effect of treatment (viz., IR vs. IE) on the
mean level of PSS-SR scores over the course of therapy.
To examine our hypothesis about reciprocal causation, that is, that
the PTSD symptoms would be negatively related to subsequent task
agreement and bond, the three WAI scales were used as dependent
variables in mixed models with random intercept and slope and a
diagonal covariance structure for the residuals (see the Statistical
HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

8

Table 1
Fixed Effects Estimates and Random Effects (Variance–Covariance) Estimates for the Three
Models of the Predictors of PTSD Symptoms

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Parameter

Intercept
Week
Treatment: IR
Treatment: IE
Within-person task
Between-person task
Within-person goal
Between-person goal
Within-person bond
Between-person bond

Residual
AR(1) rho
Intercept
Week
Intercept ϫ Week
–2 log likelihood

Task

Goal

Bond

Fixed effects
40.885‫)602.6( ء‬
43.138‫)453.5( ء‬
Ϫ1.312‫)071.0( ء‬
Ϫ1.272‫)361.0( ء‬
Ϫ2.446 (2.265)
Ϫ3.470 (2.374)
0 (0)
0 (0)
Ϫ0.820‫)193.0( ء‬
Ϫ2.139‫)779.0( ء‬
Ϫ0.527 (0.426)
Ϫ1.667 (1.065)

37.054‫)179.4( ء‬
Ϫ1.316‫)771.0( ء‬
Ϫ1.110 (2.411)
0 (0)

Ϫ0.724 (0.464)
Ϫ1.174 (0.910)

Random effects
17.136‫)249.1( ء‬
16.638‫)997.1( ء‬
0.210‫)980.0( ء‬
0.193‫)680.0( ء‬
68.460‫)872.41( ء‬
79.295‫)442.61( ء‬
1.118‫)003.0( ء‬
1.235‫)433.0( ء‬
2.947 (1.617)
3.897‫)856.1( ء‬
2737.418
2727.652

16.096‫)348.1( ء‬
0.191‫)390.0( ء‬
74.918‫)367.51( ء‬
1.296‫)343.0( ء‬
2.712 (1.752)
2549.456

Note. Standard errors are in parentheses. PTSD ϭ posttraumatic stress disorder; IR ϭ imagery rescripting; IE ϭ
imaginal exposure; task ϭ agreement about tasks; goal ϭ agreement about goals; bond ϭ patient–therapist emotional
bond; AR(1) ϭ first order autoregressive.
‫ء‬
p Ͻ .05.

Analysis section). Within-person and between-person PSS-SR scores
were used as predictors. In addition, we included time and treatment
as predictors of the alliance scores. Our hypothesis that within-person
variations in PTSD symptoms would predict subsequent withinperson variations in PTSD symptoms was not supported for any of the
WAI scales. That is, there was no within-person effect of PSS-SR
scores on Task, Goal, or Bond scores (all absolute t values Ͻ 1).
To examine our third to fifth hypotheses, all the six interactions
between our four predictors were added in the three models. Our third
hypothesis, stating that higher initial task agreement and bond predicted a steeper negative slope of PTSD symptoms, was supported.
That is, there was a significant time by between-person task effect,
␤ ϭ Ϫ0.272, SE ϭ 0.136, t(55.5) ϭ Ϫ2.00, p ϭ .025, and a
significant time by between-person bond effect, ␤ ϭ Ϫ0.337, SE ϭ
0.125, t(55.6) ϭ Ϫ2.71 p Ͻ .01. As these interaction effects were
negative, they indicate that with longer time into therapy, higher initial
alliance was associated with lower PTSD symptoms. There was no
time by between-person goal effect on symptoms.
Our fourth hypothesis, that the within-person effect of alliance on
outcome is stronger with lower initial levels of alliance, was contradicted by the results for the Task scale. That is, there was a cross-level
interaction of between-person and within-person effects of task, ␤ ϭ
Ϫ0.814, SE ϭ 0.403, t(320.7) ϭ Ϫ2.02, p Ͻ .025. The negative
direction of this interaction effect shows that— opposite to what we
expected—the higher the initial task alliance, the stronger the negative
relationship between within-person variations in task alliance and
subsequent within-person variations in PTSD symptoms. No crosslevel interactions of the within- and between-person effects were
evident for the Goal and Bond scales.
Our fifth hypothesis, stating that the within-person relationship
between alliance and outcome is stronger in IE than in IR, was
supported for the Task scale. That is, treatment interacted with the

within-person effect of Task scores on PSS-SR scores. When using
IE as a baseline, there was a positive effect of IR on PSS-SR
scores, ␤ ϭ 2.031, SE ϭ 0.775, t(325.4) ϭ 2.62, p Ͻ .01.
Considering the overall negative within-person effect of Task
scores on PSS-SR scores (see Table 1), the positive direction of
this relationship in IR compared to IE shows that the relationship
is weaker in IR than in IE.
It should also be noted that there was a time by treatment effect.
In the model using task as a predictor, there was a positive effect of
IR on PSS-SR scores with time, ␤ ϭ 0.964, SE ϭ 0.317, t(54.9) ϭ
3.04, p Ͻ .01. Considering the overall negative effect of time on
PSS-SR scores (see Table 1), the positive effect of IR compared to IE
used as baseline shows that the PSS-SR scores were less reduced in IR
than in IE. There was no individual therapist effect on the rate of
change of PSS-SR, Task, Goal, or Bond scores.

The Magnitude of Effects
Compared to a baseline model including only the random effects
(intercept, time) and the fixed effect of time, residual variance was
reduced, with 4.3%, while random intercept variance was reduced,
with 5.8%, when within-person and between-person Task scores
were added in the model.

Discussion
The Role of Alliance in Varying Imagery Procedures
for PTSD
The main purpose of this study was to examine the role of
alliance components in the process of therapeutic change in PTSD
patients. Most importantly, the hypothesis of a negative within-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME

person effect of the components agreement about the tasks of
therapy and bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms was supported
for the task component. That is, when the task score for a given
patient was higher than was expected for that patient, the subsequent symptom score was lower than was expected for him/her,
explaining about 4% of the outcome variance. In any event, this
finding goes beyond previous research in the PTSD treatment field
by indicating that time-specific change in a person’s task agreement during therapy is related to this person’s subsequent change
in PTSD symptoms. On a more general level, this finding supports
and extends those of Tasca and Lampard (2012) and Falkenström
et al. (2013), who found a within-person relationship between
overall alliance and subsequent symptoms but in different treatments and patient populations.
Furthermore, the results indicate that the within-person relationship between task agreement and outcome is dependent on the
specific therapy form. As we hypothesized, this relationship was
stronger in IE than in IR.
On the other hand, within-person changes in PTSD symptoms
did not predict subsequent task agreement and bond. That is,
time-specific change in a person’s symptoms during therapy was
not related to this person’s subsequent change in task and bond.
This finding is at odds with those of Tasca and Lampard (2012)
and Falkenström et al. (2013), who found a bidirectional relationship between alliance and symptoms. A conspicuous difference
between these studies and ours is that we used standardized, highly
structured, and manual-based procedures. One may speculate that
patients’ belief in and agreement to such procedures are less
influenced by symptom variations than is their agreement to less
standardized and less clearly defined procedures.
Our hypotheses about a between-person effect of initial task
agreement and bond was supported. Initial Task and Bond scores
predicted a steeper negative slope of PTSD symptoms. These
findings are consistent with most findings in alliance research
(Horvath et al., 2011) that early alliance predicts the further course
of symptoms. The centrality of the task component in predicting
overall (between-person) outcome is consistent with the results of
Webb et al. (2011), who found therapist–patient agreement on the
tasks and goals of therapy to account for most of the outcome
variance in cognitive therapy for depression. However, our results
indicate that a good initial bond is also important for a successful
overall outcome in exposure-based therapy for PTSD. Thus, alliance components may have different roles in cognitive behavioral
therapy (CBT) for different patient populations. What results
would be obtained for forms of therapy other than CBT is unknown, as it appears that the bond works differently in dynamic
therapy than it does in CBT. For instance, the bond and therapist’s
focus on affect seem to be differently related to each other and to
outcome in these two therapies (Ulvenes et al., 2012).
We expected that task agreement and bond would be of greater
concern for those who had a lower individual level and would thus
be more influential in these persons’ process of change, but in fact
the within-person effect of task scores on subsequent PTSD symptoms was stronger in those with a higher initial task agreement.
Future research must show whether this was a chance finding or
not. However, if this effect is replicated, it would suggest a double
drawback for a patient having a low initial agreement about the
tasks of therapy. First, the patient would experience less overall
improvement over the course of therapy, and, second, greater than

9

expected levels of agreement during the process of therapy for that
patient would not be as effective.
We also explored the role of the alliance component goal
agreement. As expected, this component was unrelated to symptom change. The within-person component of goal agreement also
had less variance than the within-person component of task agreement, and this difference may have contributed to the differential
findings. Our results are consistent with Horvath (2011), who
found— on the between-person level—that the agreement on tasks
as a predictor of outcome was superior to both bond and agreement
on goals.

Strengths and Limitations of the Study
Alliance and PTSD symptoms were assessed weekly, and adequate methods were utilized to separate the within-person and
between-person effects of the time-varying predictors in the applied multilevel models. Thus, we could study within-person relationships over the course of therapy, which are of particular
relevance for psychotherapy theories. This is because therapy
theories concern such relationships, that is, how change in a
process variable relates to subsequent change in an outcome variable. Such knowledge directly informs therapists concerning what
process variables need to be affected to achieve patient improvement. By contrast, knowledge of between-person relationships—
one patient having a low initial alliance and poor outcome and
another having a high initial alliance and good outcome— does not
imply that an increase in the first patient’s alliance would lead to
a better outcome for that patient. Thus, relationships established on
a between-person level do not imply that the same relationships
hold on a within-person level. For instance, the relationship between bond and outcome obtained in the present study on the
between-person level was not replicated on the within-person
level. A further advantage of properly separating the between- and
within-person components of a time-varying predictor is the possibility of examining cross-level interactions of within- and
between-person effects. For therapists, how between-person differences in, for example, alliance or self-concept moderate withinperson relationships over the course of therapy is more directly
relevant than are the correlations of these differences with overall
outcome. Such moderating knowledge informs therapists concerning under what conditions (e.g., high task agreement relative to
other patients) certain within-person change processes are working
(e.g., higher than usual task agreement at a given time point
predicts lower than usual PTSD symptoms). A further advantage
of studying within-person relationships between process and outcome is the possibility of identifying reciprocal or even reversed
causality between process and outcome. The RCT design, where
patients were randomized to two empirically based imagery methods, allowed us to study the moderating influence of therapy form
on the within-person relationships. The studied sample had high
clinical representativeness, as research eligibility was similar to
treatment eligibility and only three (4.2%) of 71 treatment eligible
patients declined research participation. Moreover, the dropout
rate from imagery treatment was low: three (4.6%) of 65 patients.
The present study has several limitations. Although the uncontrolled effect size of Ϫ1.27 (Hedges’s g, intent-to-treat analysis)
for standard prolonged exposure (including IE as the imagery
component) is comparable to that in one of the studies conducted
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

10

HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

by the originators of prolonged exposure (e.g., Hedges’s g ϭ
Ϫ1.37 in Foa et al., 2005), the adequacy ratings were only around
a level of satisfactory for both imagery treatments. The adherence
ratings of 75% (IE) and 80% (IR) are lower than those typically
found in the original studies of prolonged exposure (e.g., 97% in
Foa et al., 2005). Thus, the varied component of treatment may
have been delivered in a less than optimal way. Moreover, less
extensive examinations of integrity (4.3% of tapes) than usual
(about 10% of tapes; Foa et al., 1997) were performed. No integrity ratings were performed for the other components of treatment
(e.g., in vivo exposure). Although well-validated measures were
used, their Norwegian translations have not undergone psychometric evaluation in previous investigations. In the present study, their
internal consistency and test–retest reliability appeared satisfactory, though. Alliance and symptom ratings were collected from
the same individual, that is, the patient, and this may have
inflated their correlation. However, halo effects were prevented
by having the ratings done 3 and 4 days apart. Furthermore,
response biases like acquiescence are supposed to cut across
ratings and may affect within-person variations—which were
the main focus of this study—to a lesser degree. We used a
passive observational design, and unmeasured third variable
confounds could have influenced the results. The power of the
study, based on about eight repeated measurements of 65 patients
(minus some missing data), may be too low to detect some withinperson relationships. We studied process on a weekly time scale,
and larger or lesser scales could be associated with different
results. The strategy of using the same therapists across therapies
has both strengths and weaknesses. The therapists may not be
equally competent and have the same preferences for both therapies. Actually, one of the therapists reported a preference for IR.
However, this bias could not explain the present results, as PTSD
symptoms measured weekly were less reduced over the course of
therapy in IR than in IE. In the context of the present study, an
advantage of crossing therapists was that the general ability to
form alliances was balanced between conditions.

Research Implications
As elaborated above, our study invites an increased focus on
within-person relationships in psychotherapy research. In highly
structured therapies like those of the present study and cognitive
therapy of depression (Webb et al., 2011), symptomatic improvement is supposed to result from the relatively specific tasks of
these therapies. Agreement about tasks may therefore be particularly important in such therapies. Moreover, the studied PTSD
sample was a severe one with a high degree of comorbidity and a
long duration of PTSD, and over half of the patients had experienced repeated and/or prolonged traumas. Future studies should
investigate the within-person relationships between alliance components and outcome across therapies and type and severity of
disorders. Furthermore, studies of within-person relationships between therapy events/therapist actions and alliance components are
needed.

Clinical Implications
The present within-person results make a firm basis for the
recommendation to monitor, increase, and restore decreases of

agreement about therapy tasks over the course of IE or IR within
prolonged exposure for PTSD patients. They also suggest that
addressing agreement about the tasks of therapy is particularly
important in IE compared to IR. Given that these exposure methods consist of confronting the feared trauma memory and feared
external situations, agreeing to their use based on an understanding
of the rationale for these methods and a belief in their efficacy
seems paramount. On the other hand, the results do not imply an
increased focus on the agreement about goals of therapy and bond
components of alliance over the course of these treatments. Our
between-person results may inform therapists using prolonged
exposure for PTSD that low initial task agreement and bond signal
a poorer outcome of therapy. Unfortunately, because the crosslevel interaction between interindividual and within-individual
task agreement was contrary to our hypothesis, clinical implications cannot be drawn from this finding.

References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Arntz, A., Tiesema, M., & Kindt, M. (2007). Treatment of PTSD: A
comparison of imaginal exposure with and without imagery rescripting.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 345–
370. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.006
Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of
the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice,
16, 252–260. doi:10.1037/h0085885
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. (2009).
Introduction to meta-analysis. Cornwall, United Kingdom: Wiley. doi:
10.1002/9780470743386
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, C., Miranda, R., & Chemtob, C. M. (2004).
Therapeutic alliance, negative mood regulation, and treatment outcome
in child abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 411– 416. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72
.3.411
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person
and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual
Review of Psychology, 62, 583– 619. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008
.100356
Dalgleish, T., & Power, M. J. (2004). Emotion-specific and emotion-nonspecific components of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Implications for a taxonomy of related psychopathology. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 42, 1069 –1088. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.05.001
Falkenström, F., Granström, F., & Holmqvist, R. (2013). Therapeutic
alliance predicts symptomatic improvement session by session. Journal
of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/
a0032258
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin,
L. (1994). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis II Personality
Disorders—Patient (SCID-II) (Version 2.0). New York, NY: New York
State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department.
Fitzmaurice, G. M., Laird, N. M., & Ware, J. H. (2004). Applied longitudinal analysis. New York, NY: Wiley.
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Symonds, D., & Horvath,
A. O. (2012). How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel
longitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 10 –
17. doi:10.1037/a0025749
Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D. E., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., & Jaycox,
L. H. (1997). Prolonged exposure (PE): Manual. Unpublished manuscript, Eastern Pennsylvania Psychiatric Institute, Allegheny University
of the Health Sciences.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E., Cahill, S. E., Rauch, S. A. M., Riggs, D. S.,
Feeney, N. C., & Yadin, E. (2005). Randomized trial of prolonged
exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder with and without cognitive
restructuring: Outcome of academic and community clinics. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 953–964. doi:10.1037/0022006X.73.5.953
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E., & Rothbaum, B. O. (2007). Prolonged exposure
therapy for PTSD: Emotional processing of traumatic experiences. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1993).
Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 459 – 473. doi:10.1002/
jts.2490060405
Grunert, B. K., Weis, J. M., Smucker, M. R., & Christianson, H. (2007).
Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy after failed prolonged
imaginal exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder following industrial
injury. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38,
317–328. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.005
Hatcher, R. L., & Gillaspy, J. A. (2006). Development and validation of a
short version of the Working Alliance Inventory. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 12–25. doi:10.1080/10503300500352500
Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Evaluating
between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development, 6, 97–120. doi:10.1080/
15427600902911189
Horvath, A. O. (2006). The Working Alliance Inventory: Norwegian translation of items generated by the factor analyses of Hatcher & Gillaspy
(2006). M. H. Rønnestad, A. von der Lippe, E. Axelsen, S. E. Gullestad,
H. Haavind, S. Reichelt, & O. A. Tjersland (Trans.). Unpublished
manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway. (Original works published 1981, 1984, 1991)
Horvath, A. O. (2011, July). The complex world of alliance assessments:
Will the real alliance please stand up? Paper presented at the Society for
Psychotherapy Research Conference, Bern.
Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011).
Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48, 9 –16. doi:
10.1037/a0022186
Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of
the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,
223–233. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.36.2.223
Keller, S. M., Zoellner, L. A., & Feeny, N. C. (2010). Understanding
factors associated with early therapeutic alliance in PTSD treatment:

11

Adherence, childhood sexual abuse history, and social support. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 974 –979. doi:10.1037/
a0020758
Lee, D. A., Scragg, P., & Turner, S. (2001). The role of shame and guilt in
traumatic events: A clinical model of shame-based and guilt-based
PTSD. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 74, 451– 466. doi:
10.1348/000711201161109
Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (1996). The resolution of ruptures in the
therapeutic alliance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64,
447– 458. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.447
Sheehan, D., Janavs, J., Baker, R., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Knapp, E., &
Sheehan, M. (1994). M.I.N.I. (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview). Tampa, FL: University of South Florida.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in
assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420 – 428. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
Smucker, M. R. (2005). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy
(IRRT): A treatment manual for adult survivors of childhood sexual
abuse experiencing PTSD. Milwaukee, WI: International Trauma Institute.
Snijder, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel modeling: An introduction to
basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London, England: Sage.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1988).
Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM–III–R—Patient Version
(SCID-P). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Tasca, G. A., & Lampard, A. M. (2012). Reciprocal influence of alliance
to the group and outcome in day treatment for eating disorders. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 59, 507–517. doi:10.1037/a0029947
Ulvenes, P. G., Berggraf, L., Hoffart, A., Stiles, T. C., Svartberg, M.,
McCullough, L., & Wampold, B. E. (2012). Different processes for
different therapies: Therapist actions, therapeutic bond, and outcome.
Psychotherapy, 49, 291–302. doi:10.1037/a0027895
Wampold, B. E., & Budge, S. L. (2012). The 2011 Leona Tyler Award
Address: The relationship—And its relationship to the common and
specific factors of psychotherapy. Counseling Psychologist, 40, 601–
623. doi:10.1177/0011000011432709
Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., Amsterdam, J. D., Shelton, R. C., Hollon,
S. D., & Dimidjian, S. (2011). Two aspects of the therapeutic alliance:
Differential relations with depressive symptom change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 279 –283. doi:10.1037/a0023252

(Appendix follows)
HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD

12

Appendix
Equations Used in the Statistical Analyses
␤1i ϭ ␥10 ϩ u1i

Equations for the Multilevel Models
We begin with the Level 1 model:
yti ϭ ␤0i ϩ ␤1i x ti ϩ eti

u0i ϭ (zi Ϫ ␥00) Ϫ (␥10 ϩ u1i)xi
៮
(A1)

Composite:

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

eti ϭ zti Ϫ ␤0i Ϫ ␤1i x ti .

Ei(zti) ϭ (␥00 ϩ u0i) ϩ ␥10Ei(x ti) ϩ Ei(u1i x 0i)

Here the individual-specific value of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms (yti) is a function of an individual intercept
(␤0i), the slope coefficient of the time score for time t for individual i (␤1i xti), and the residual symptoms of PTDS (eti) on time t for
individual i. The equation term eti is computed by deviating the
time-specific predictor (zti) from the regression line (␤1i xti) estimated separately (case by case) for each individual in the sample.
The deviated measure, eti, is then the residual (i.e., the observed
score minus expected value) from the regression of the timevarying predictor on time computed separately for each individual
case, which then represents the variable for the within-person level
of each predictor (i.e., Task, Goal, or Bond).
The Level 2 between-person predictor represents variance due
to interindividual differences in the time-varying predictor at the
start of treatment, as shown in Equation A2:

ϭ(␥00 ϩ u0i) ϩ (␥10 ϩ u1i)Ei(x ti).

zbi ϭ ␤1i x 0i .

In the Level 2 model, zbi is the between-person component
of the time-varying predictor and is a function of individual
differences in the time-varying predictor at the start of treatment (␤1i x0i).
The equations for the model with main effects of the betweenperson and within-person predictors are presented in Equation A3:
Level 1:

៮
៮
eti ϭ (zti Ϫ zi) Ϫ (␥10 ϩ u1i)(x ti Ϫ x i)
Level 2:
␤0i ϭ ␥00 ϩ u0i

Equations for Proportion Reduction of
Error at Each Level
The proportion reduction of error for predicting the Level 1
outcome is

(A2)

R

zti ϭ ␤0i ϩ ␤1i x ti ϩ eti

In the Level 1 model, the individual-specific value of symptoms
of PTSD (yti) is a function of an individual intercept (␤0i), the
within-person effects of the time-varying predictor (␤1i xti), and
the residual PTSD symptoms (eti) on time t for individual i. In the
Level 2 model, the individual intercept (␤0i) is a function of a fixed
intercept (␥00) and an individual-specific random intercept (u0i).
The individual effects of slope (␤1i) is a function of the fixed
effects in rate of change (␥10) and person-specific slope (u1i).

(A3)

R2 ϭ 1 –
L1

ͩ

residual variance more ϩ intercept variance more
residual variance fewer ϩ intercept variance fewer

ͪ

.

The proportion reduction of error for predicting the Level 2
outcome is

R2 ϭ 1 –
L2

΂

residual variance more
#Level 1 units
residual variance fewer
#Level 1 units

ϩ intercept variance more
ϩ intercept variance fewer

΃

.

Received January 9, 2013
Revision received May 13, 2013
Accepted May 13, 2013 Ⅲ

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-Support
Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-SupportDoctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-Support
Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-SupportReina Salazar
 
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPRule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPSarah Kenehan
 
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3Barbara Babcock, ACC
 
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen Flor
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen FlorDeterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen Flor
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen FlorScott Miller
 
GAD Term Paper Psych Article Draft
GAD Term Paper Psych Article DraftGAD Term Paper Psych Article Draft
GAD Term Paper Psych Article DraftKendra Medor
 
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...sipij
 
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson PublishersThe Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson PublishersCrimsonpublishersPPrs
 
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPY
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPYAttitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPY
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPYThangamani Ramalingam
 
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka Jayamini D Samarathunge
 
Achieving Clinical Excellence Handouts
Achieving Clinical Excellence HandoutsAchieving Clinical Excellence Handouts
Achieving Clinical Excellence HandoutsScott Miller
 
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...James Tobin, Ph.D.
 
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social Construction
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social ConstructionFIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social Construction
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social ConstructionScott Miller
 
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Guillem Feixas
 
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014Fonzo et al. JAD 2014
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014Greg Fonzo
 

Mais procurados (19)

Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-Support
Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-SupportDoctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-Support
Doctoral Capstone Project: Sensi-Support
 
chronic pain management
chronic pain managementchronic pain management
chronic pain management
 
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAPRule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
Rule Order Manipulation and the IRAP
 
Therapist Effects
Therapist EffectsTherapist Effects
Therapist Effects
 
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3
BBabcock - Research Summary - Coaching and Chronic Conditions - May 2013 v3
 
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen Flor
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen FlorDeterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen Flor
Deterioration in Psychotherapy: A Summary of Research by Jorgen Flor
 
TheoryofChange
TheoryofChangeTheoryofChange
TheoryofChange
 
Psychological first aid1
Psychological first aid1Psychological first aid1
Psychological first aid1
 
GAD Term Paper Psych Article Draft
GAD Term Paper Psych Article DraftGAD Term Paper Psych Article Draft
GAD Term Paper Psych Article Draft
 
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...
INDIVIDUAL EMOTION RECOGNITION AND SUBGROUP ANALYSIS FROM PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL...
 
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson PublishersThe Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
 
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPY
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPYAttitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPY
Attitude & BEHAVIOUR MODIFICATION THERAPY
 
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka
Personality and Stress - Quantitative Study in Sri Lanka
 
Achieving Clinical Excellence Handouts
Achieving Clinical Excellence HandoutsAchieving Clinical Excellence Handouts
Achieving Clinical Excellence Handouts
 
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...
The Shift from "Ordinary" to "Extraordinary" Experience in Psychodynamic Supe...
 
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social Construction
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social ConstructionFIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social Construction
FIT: Evidence-based Practice meets Social Construction
 
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
 
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014Fonzo et al. JAD 2014
Fonzo et al. JAD 2014
 
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & AnxietyScientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
Scientifi c Journal of Depression & Anxiety
 

Destaque

Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)
Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)
Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)Scott Miller
 
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...Scott Miller
 
The outcome of psychotherapy yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...
The outcome of psychotherapy  yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...The outcome of psychotherapy  yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...
The outcome of psychotherapy yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...Scott Miller
 
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)Scott Miller
 
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)Scott Miller
 
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...Scott Miller
 
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013Scott Miller
 
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)Behavior Therapist (April 2009)
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)Scott Miller
 
Feedback informed treatment 2013
Feedback informed treatment 2013Feedback informed treatment 2013
Feedback informed treatment 2013Scott Miller
 
Responding to new scientific objections to the ors
Responding to new scientific objections to the orsResponding to new scientific objections to the ors
Responding to new scientific objections to the orsScott Miller
 
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)Scott Miller
 
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)Scott Miller
 
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a Therapist
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a TherapistSeven Qualities Most Desired in a Therapist
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a TherapistAnnual Conference
 
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...Scott Miller
 
Delrapport ii slutversion
Delrapport ii slutversionDelrapport ii slutversion
Delrapport ii slutversionScott Miller
 
Bargman Nye Veje For Evidensbegrebet
Bargman Nye Veje For EvidensbegrebetBargman Nye Veje For Evidensbegrebet
Bargman Nye Veje For EvidensbegrebetScott Miller
 
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...Scott Miller
 
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...Scott Miller
 
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in Agreement
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in AgreementExpanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in Agreement
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in AgreementScott Miller
 

Destaque (20)

Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)
Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)
Why most therapists are average (german, 2014)
 
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...
How do psychotherapists experience the use of outcome questionnaires ors and ...
 
The outcome of psychotherapy yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...
The outcome of psychotherapy  yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...The outcome of psychotherapy  yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...
The outcome of psychotherapy yesterday, today, and tomorrow (psychotherapy m...
 
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
The art of being a failure as a therapist (haley, 1969)
 
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)
The Carlat Psychiatry Report (Interview with Scott Miller, April 2015)
 
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...
The need for empirically supported psychology training standards (psychothera...
 
Chaste Living
Chaste LivingChaste Living
Chaste Living
 
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013
North & south counselling outcomes article march 2013
 
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)Behavior Therapist (April 2009)
Behavior Therapist (April 2009)
 
Feedback informed treatment 2013
Feedback informed treatment 2013Feedback informed treatment 2013
Feedback informed treatment 2013
 
Responding to new scientific objections to the ors
Responding to new scientific objections to the orsResponding to new scientific objections to the ors
Responding to new scientific objections to the ors
 
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)
Shared Decision Making (Miller, 2013)
 
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)
Snatching Victory From The Jaws Of Defeat (Handouts)
 
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a Therapist
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a TherapistSeven Qualities Most Desired in a Therapist
Seven Qualities Most Desired in a Therapist
 
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...
Article from the National Psychologist about Scott Miller's speech at Evoluti...
 
Delrapport ii slutversion
Delrapport ii slutversionDelrapport ii slutversion
Delrapport ii slutversion
 
Bargman Nye Veje For Evidensbegrebet
Bargman Nye Veje For EvidensbegrebetBargman Nye Veje For Evidensbegrebet
Bargman Nye Veje For Evidensbegrebet
 
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...
Model for Dealing with Ruptures in the Therapeutic Alliance via Session by Se...
 
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...
Utilizing the ORS in a Community Mental Health Clinic (student outcomes etc) ...
 
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in Agreement
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in AgreementExpanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in Agreement
Expanding the Lens of EBP: A Common Factors in Agreement
 

Semelhante a The Relationship between Alliance & Outcome in PTSD

Running head ARTICLE REVIEW .docx
Running head  ARTICLE REVIEW                                 .docxRunning head  ARTICLE REVIEW                                 .docx
Running head ARTICLE REVIEW .docxtoddr4
 
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docxCOUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docxvoversbyobersby
 
Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Brianna Bradley
 
ESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperSharon Hui
 
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived Stress
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived StressA Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived Stress
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived StressNatasha Grant
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Stud
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Studhttpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Stud
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case StudPazSilviapm
 
Gingerich-Coping with Stress
Gingerich-Coping with StressGingerich-Coping with Stress
Gingerich-Coping with StressMarisa Gingerich
 
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docxlanagore871
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BSarah M
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Lossijtsrd
 
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docx
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docxSpecificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docx
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docxrafbolet0
 
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docxstandfordabbot
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxdaniahendric
 
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016Christine Leclerc-Sherling
 
Tseng et al., 2015
Tseng et al., 2015Tseng et al., 2015
Tseng et al., 2015Angela Tseng
 

Semelhante a The Relationship between Alliance & Outcome in PTSD (20)

Investigation of Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post ...
Investigation of Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post ...Investigation of Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post ...
Investigation of Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post ...
 
Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post Traumatic Stress ...
Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post Traumatic Stress ...Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post Traumatic Stress ...
Horticultural Therapy as a Complementary Treatment for Post Traumatic Stress ...
 
Running head ARTICLE REVIEW .docx
Running head  ARTICLE REVIEW                                 .docxRunning head  ARTICLE REVIEW                                 .docx
Running head ARTICLE REVIEW .docx
 
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docxCOUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
 
Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535Bradley_Research Paper_535
Bradley_Research Paper_535
 
ESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study Paper
 
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived Stress
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived StressA Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived Stress
A Literature Review On Emotional Competency And Perceived Stress
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Stud
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Studhttpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Stud
httpsdoi.org10.11771534650120954275Clinical Case Stud
 
Gingerich-Coping with Stress
Gingerich-Coping with StressGingerich-Coping with Stress
Gingerich-Coping with Stress
 
IFTA - October 2015
IFTA - October 2015IFTA - October 2015
IFTA - October 2015
 
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
·Response GuidelinesReply to the posts of two peers in thi.docx
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
 
Comp 9
Comp 9Comp 9
Comp 9
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
 
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docx
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docxSpecificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docx
Specificity of Treatment Effects Cognitive Therapy and Relaxa.docx
 
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx
5Relationship Between Depression (from heartbreak).docx
 
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docxAdult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
Adult Attachment as a Moderator of Treatment Outcome for Gener.docx
 
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016
CLeclerc-Sherling _Additional litterature 03192016
 
Tseng et al., 2015
Tseng et al., 2015Tseng et al., 2015
Tseng et al., 2015
 
Final Paper
Final PaperFinal Paper
Final Paper
 

Mais de Scott Miller

Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015
Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015
Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015Scott Miller
 
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)Scott Miller
 
Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Scott Miller
 
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Scott Miller
 
Deliberate Practice at Stangehjelp
Deliberate Practice at StangehjelpDeliberate Practice at Stangehjelp
Deliberate Practice at StangehjelpScott Miller
 
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance Ruptures
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance RupturesFinal Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance Ruptures
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance RupturesScott Miller
 
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session Feedback
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session FeedbackThe Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session Feedback
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session FeedbackScott Miller
 
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift MitchellScott Miller
 
Effect size of common versus specific factors
Effect size of common versus specific factorsEffect size of common versus specific factors
Effect size of common versus specific factorsScott Miller
 
Beyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringBeyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringScott Miller
 
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015Scott Miller
 
Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Scott Miller
 
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press release
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press releaseMedipex innovation awards 2015 press release
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press releaseScott Miller
 
Duration of Psychological Therapy
Duration of Psychological TherapyDuration of Psychological Therapy
Duration of Psychological TherapyScott Miller
 
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...Scott Miller
 
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach Us
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach UsBurnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach Us
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach UsScott Miller
 
Resolving our Identity Crisis
Resolving our Identity CrisisResolving our Identity Crisis
Resolving our Identity CrisisScott Miller
 
ORS and SRS in Penelope
ORS and SRS in PenelopeORS and SRS in Penelope
ORS and SRS in PenelopeScott Miller
 
Thought Reform and Totalism
Thought Reform and TotalismThought Reform and Totalism
Thought Reform and TotalismScott Miller
 
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)Scott Miller
 

Mais de Scott Miller (20)

Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015
Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015
Trajectories of Change (Clinicians Research Digest version) 2015
 
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)
Classifying happiness as a psychiatric disorder (richard bentall, 1992)
 
Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016Measures and feedback 2016
Measures and feedback 2016
 
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
Feedback informed treatment (fit) achieving(apa ip miller hubble seidel chow ...
 
Deliberate Practice at Stangehjelp
Deliberate Practice at StangehjelpDeliberate Practice at Stangehjelp
Deliberate Practice at Stangehjelp
 
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance Ruptures
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance RupturesFinal Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance Ruptures
Final Rational Empirical Model for Identifying and Addressing Alliance Ruptures
 
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session Feedback
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session FeedbackThe Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session Feedback
The Therapeutic Alliance, Ruptures, and Session-by-Session Feedback
 
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell
"I Don't Know--The Three Most Common Words in Psychotherapy" Clift Mitchell
 
Effect size of common versus specific factors
Effect size of common versus specific factorsEffect size of common versus specific factors
Effect size of common versus specific factors
 
Beyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoringBeyond measures and monitoring
Beyond measures and monitoring
 
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015
Rehab Guarantee Official Report 2015
 
Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)Do therapists improve (preprint)
Do therapists improve (preprint)
 
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press release
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press releaseMedipex innovation awards 2015 press release
Medipex innovation awards 2015 press release
 
Duration of Psychological Therapy
Duration of Psychological TherapyDuration of Psychological Therapy
Duration of Psychological Therapy
 
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...
Does publication bias inflate the apparent efficacy of psychological treatmen...
 
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach Us
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach UsBurnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach Us
Burnout Reconsidered: What Supershrinks Can Teach Us
 
Resolving our Identity Crisis
Resolving our Identity CrisisResolving our Identity Crisis
Resolving our Identity Crisis
 
ORS and SRS in Penelope
ORS and SRS in PenelopeORS and SRS in Penelope
ORS and SRS in Penelope
 
Thought Reform and Totalism
Thought Reform and TotalismThought Reform and Totalism
Thought Reform and Totalism
 
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)
Practice-based Evdience (Michael Barkham, 2014)
 

Último

Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfJemuel Francisco
 
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and Film
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and FilmOppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and Film
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and FilmStan Meyer
 
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 Database
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 DatabaseHow to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 Database
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 DatabaseCeline George
 
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWMythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWQuiz Club NITW
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Seán Kennedy
 
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...DhatriParmar
 
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptxmary850239
 
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptxmary850239
 
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4JOYLYNSAMANIEGO
 
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing Postmodern Elements in Literature.pptx
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing  Postmodern Elements in  Literature.pptxUnraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing  Postmodern Elements in  Literature.pptx
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing Postmodern Elements in Literature.pptxDhatriParmar
 
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdf
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdfICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdf
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdfVanessa Camilleri
 
Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemChristalin Nelson
 
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdf
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdfNarcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdf
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdfPrerana Jadhav
 
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWQ-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWQuiz Club NITW
 
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptx
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptxDecoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptx
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptxDhatriParmar
 
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnv
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnvESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnv
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnvRicaMaeCastro1
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptxmary850239
 
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHS
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHSTextual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHS
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHSMae Pangan
 

Último (20)

Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdfGrade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
Grade 9 Quarter 4 Dll Grade 9 Quarter 4 DLL.pdf
 
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and Film
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and FilmOppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and Film
Oppenheimer Film Discussion for Philosophy and Film
 
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 Database
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 DatabaseHow to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 Database
How to Make a Duplicate of Your Odoo 17 Database
 
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWMythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Mythology Quiz-4th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
 
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...
Blowin' in the Wind of Caste_ Bob Dylan's Song as a Catalyst for Social Justi...
 
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx
4.16.24 Poverty and Precarity--Desmond.pptx
 
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx
4.11.24 Mass Incarceration and the New Jim Crow.pptx
 
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4
Daily Lesson Plan in Mathematics Quarter 4
 
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...
31 ĐỀ THI THỬ VÀO LỚP 10 - TIẾNG ANH - FORM MỚI 2025 - 40 CÂU HỎI - BÙI VĂN V...
 
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing Postmodern Elements in Literature.pptx
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing  Postmodern Elements in  Literature.pptxUnraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing  Postmodern Elements in  Literature.pptx
Unraveling Hypertext_ Analyzing Postmodern Elements in Literature.pptx
 
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdf
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdfICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdf
ICS2208 Lecture6 Notes for SL spaces.pdf
 
Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management System
 
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdf
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdfNarcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdf
Narcotic and Non Narcotic Analgesic..pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Large Language Models"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Large Language Models"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Large Language Models"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Large Language Models"
 
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITWQ-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
Q-Factor HISPOL Quiz-6th April 2024, Quiz Club NITW
 
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptx
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptxDecoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptx
Decoding the Tweet _ Practical Criticism in the Age of Hashtag.pptx
 
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnv
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnvESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnv
ESP 4-EDITED.pdfmmcncncncmcmmnmnmncnmncmnnjvnnv
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
 
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHS
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHSTextual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHS
Textual Evidence in Reading and Writing of SHS
 

The Relationship between Alliance & Outcome in PTSD

  • 1. Journal of Counseling Psychology Alliance and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD: A Study of Within-Person Processes Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen, Tomas Formo Langkaas, and Bruce E. Wampold Online First Publication, August 19, 2013. doi: 10.1037/a0033604 CITATION Hoffart, A., Øktedalen, T., Formo Langkaas, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2013, August 19). Alliance and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD: A Study of Within-Person Processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0033604
  • 2. Journal of Counseling Psychology 2013, Vol. 60, No. 4, 000 © 2013 American Psychological Association 0022-0167/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0033604 Alliance and Outcome in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD: A Study of Within-Person Processes Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen, and Tomas Formo Langkaas Bruce E. Wampold University of Wisconsin Madison and Research Institute, Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Research Institute, Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway, and University of Oslo The present study examined both the intraindividual relationship between alliance components (task, goal, and bond) and subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms over the course of therapy and the interindividual relationships between the initial level of the alliance components and overall PTSD outcome. PTSD patients (n ϭ 65) were randomized to either standard prolonged exposure, which includes imaginal exposure (IE) to the traumatic memory, or modified prolonged exposure, where imagery rescripting (IR) of the memory replaced IE as the imagery component of prolonged exposure in a 10-week residential program. They were assessed repeatedly (weekly) on alliance and PTSD symptom measures. The centering method of detrending (Curran & Bauer, 2011) was used to separate the variance related to the intraindividual process of change during treatment (within-person component) from the variance related to initial individual differences (between-person component). The hypothesis of a negative within-person effect of the alliance components agreement about the tasks of therapy and bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms was supported for the component task agreement. As expected, this effect was stronger in IE than in IR. Moreover, there was a negative relationship between interindividual differences in initial Task and Bond scale scores and slope of PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy. By contrast, within-person variations in PTSD symptoms did not predict subsequent alliance components. The present results suggest the importance of agreement about therapy tasks during the process of IE or IR within prolonged exposure for PTSD patients, particularly in IE. Keywords: imaginal exposure, imagery rescripting, posttraumatic stress disorder, alliance, process research exposure (Foa, Hembree, & Rothbaum, 2007), which has been most extensively documented as an efficacious treatment for PTSD, consists of imaginal exposure (IE) to the traumatic memory, repeated listening to tapes of the imagery sessions, and in vivo exposure to avoided situations and stimuli. Thus, the patient is asked to approach what has evoked the most anxiety and distress. During IE, also the therapist is confronted with aversive information, which may evoke emotional responses he or she cannot express and consequently induce therapist feelings of powerlessness (Arntz, Tiesema, & Kindt, 2007). The strains put on the patient and the therapist potentially requires an agreement about these tasks, which suggests that both the patient and the therapist understand and accept the treatment rationale and believe that the treatment is an appropriate and beneficial approach to reduce symptoms (Keller, Zoellner, & Feeny, 2010). Furthermore, the traumatic experience and its aftermaths often involve helplessness, shame, guilt, and anger reactions that are difficult to reveal to another person (Lee, Scragg, & Turner, 2001). Many clients also fear that IE to the trauma experience will lead to loss of control and even insanity. All this requires a development of a bond, in which the patient trusts that the therapist understands, cares for, and accepts him/her and believes the therapist is able to help the patient regulate strong emotions (Wampold & Budge, 2012). Conversely, the demands of the trauma-focused procedures may lower many patients’ enthusiasm about engaging in the therapy A stronger therapeutic alliance has been found to be associated with better outcomes across a variety of treatment approaches and mental health problems (Flückiger, Del Re, Wampold, Symonds, & Horvath, 2012; Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, & Symonds, 2011). According to the most widely accepted transtheoretical model, alliance is composed of agreement about the tasks of therapy, agreement about the goals of therapy, and the emotional bond between patient and therapist (Bordin, 1979). These components may have a different role and influence depending on the treatment approach and the problem being treated (Ulvenes et al., 2012; Webb et al., 2011). The task and bond components should be particularly influential on outcome in trauma-focused treatments of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) because of the demands put on both the patient and the therapist. For instance, prolonged Asle Hoffart, Tuva Øktedalen, and Tomas Formo Langkaas, Research Institute, Modum Bad, Vikersund, Norway, and Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Bruce E. Wampold, Department of Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin Madison, and Research Institute, Modum Bad. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Asle Hoffart, Research Institute, Modum Bad, N-3370 Vikersund, Norway. E-mail: asle.hoffart@modum-bad.no 1
  • 3. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 2 HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD tasks and also lead them to question the therapist’s care for them. Therefore, there should be much variation in the levels of task agreement and bond, both between patients and within patients over time, which in turn might well covary with outcome. An agreement about goals is necessary in trauma-focused therapy as well. However, there should be more uniformity of levels of agreement about the goals because the goals of trauma-focused therapy—reducing the fear of the trauma memory and of the trauma reminders (Foa et al., 2007)—should be strongly endorsed by the patients as well as the therapists. Consequently, restricted range reduces the possibility of finding covariation between agreements of goals and outcome. In general, task agreement has been found to be more strongly related to outcome than goal agreement and bond have (Horvath, 2011). With respect to PTSD patients, early alliance has been shown to predict their adherence to prolonged exposure (Keller et al., 2010) and their emotion regulation skills and outcome in a two-phase stabilization/skill development and exposure therapy for childhood abuse-related PTSD (Cloitre, Stovall-McClough, Miranda, & Chemtob, 2004). Based on this literature, we specifically expected that task agreement in particular, and perhaps bond as well, would predict better weekly as well as overall outcome in trauma-focused therapy of PTSD. Although the IE component of prolonged exposure is an effective intervention for trauma-related fear through the mechanisms of habituation and experienced nonoccurrence of feared event (Foa et al., 2007), it may be less effective for other trauma-related emotions such as shame, guilt, and anger. Repeated exposure to a traumatic memory involving shame and guilt may provide little corrective information and actually run the risk of reinforcing these emotions (Dalgleish & Power, 2004). To address the range of emotions in PTSD, some authors have advocated (Arntz et al., 2007) the addition of an element of imagery rescripting (IR; Smucker, 2005), in which an imagined change of the course of events of the trauma memory is induced. In a randomized controlled trial (RCT), Arntz et al. (2007) compared a combination of IE and IR to IE alone. They found no difference in reduction of PTSD severity but did find the IE and IR combination to be more effective for anger control, externalization of anger, hostility, and guilt, especially at 1-month follow-up. The IR method used in this study was to provide the patient with an opportunity to discover and express in imagery any trauma-related inhibited emotional responses (e.g., anger about what happened). The present study, the data for which was obtained in an RCT, replicates and extends the study of Arntz et al. by using a broader form of IR developed by Smucker (2005). In this method, the patient’s current self is—after an initial imagery reliving phase—invited to enter the imagery at the worst moment of the trauma, bring the situation to a solution (e.g., overpower a perpetrator), and then interact with the traumatized self back then. The patient’s anger is used as a resource in overpowering perpetrators and the current self– traumatized self interaction stimulates the development of selfcompassion instead of shame, guilt, and self-critique. The empowering and relieving features of IR may put less strain on the patient and the therapist by making them feel less helpless and distressed compared to IE and thus help them both to engage in imagery work. In the study by Arntz et al., therapists tended to favor the combination of IE and IR, as it decreased their feelings of helplessness compared to IE alone. Supporting the effectiveness of the broader form of IR, Grunert, Weis, Smucker, and Christianson (2007) found in an open trial that IR was extremely helpful for PTSD patients who had previously not profited from IE. The present study does not focus on therapy outcome per se but on how the influence of the alliance on outcome may relate to the specific trauma-focused therapy model being applied. We expected that, due to the empowering and relieving features of IR compared to IE, the influence of task agreement and bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms would be weaker in IR than in IE. Understanding the nature of the alliance depends on the methods used to examine it. For example, the well-established alliance/ outcome relationship is cross-sectional (i.e., bivariate observations for each psychotherapy dyad) and is thus focused on betweenperson differences (i.e., interindividual processes). That is, variations between patients in early alliance have been found to correlate with between-patient variations in outcome at the end of therapy (Horvath et al., 2011). However, it is also important to consider the development of the alliance for a particular patient. For example, the rupture-repair model (Safran & Muran, 1996) assumes that alliance ruptures represent opportunities for patients to learn about their problems relating to others, and repairs represent such opportunities having been taken in the here-and-now of the therapeutic relationship. This process is indicated by marked drops in alliance followed by a quick return to previous or higher levels, which represents within-person variations in the alliance. In general, therapy models, and particularly therapists, focus on within-person relationships, which would be the case, for example, when a change in the alliance for a particular patient leads to a subsequent alleviation of PTSD symptoms in that patient. The typical alliance data, collected once early in therapy, or occasionally during therapy, are unsuitable for evaluating withinperson processes (Curran & Bauer, 2011). Only repeated measures data allow for the proper disaggregation of between-person and within-person effects (Curran & Bauer, 2011; Hoffman & Stawski, 2009). Such a disaggregation not only allows the study of withinperson processes separated from between-person effects, but also is able to examine cross-level interactions of between- and withinperson effects. For instance, the effect of having a stronger alliance than expected for a particular patient may matter more for patients who have lower alliance in general. When the general (betweenperson) level of bond is low, for example when the patient has low trust that the therapist wants the best for him/her and is therefore preoccupied with this issue, a certain increase of this trust in a particular session might be a valued event with an immediate effect on symptoms. On the other hand, when the patient’s trust is already high and is not an issue for him/her, the same increase would probably have fewer consequences. That is, one should expect within-person variations in alliance to affect PTSD symptoms more when the between-person level of alliance is low. So far, the ability to separate these effects has not been fully capitalized upon in alliance research. Two notable exceptions are the studies of Tasca and Lampard (2012) and Falkenström, Granström, and Holmqvist (2013). Using latent change score modeling, in which between- and within-person components of both the predictor and outcome variables are separated, Tasca and Lampard obtained evidence for a reciprocal influence of alliance to the patient group and outcome among eating disordered individuals. Using the disaggregation methods in multilevel models proposed by Curran and Bauer (2011), Falkenström et al. also found evidence for a reciprocal causal model of alliance and outcome in
  • 4. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME primary care psychotherapy. Based on the results of these welldesigned studies, we expected to find that over the course of trauma-focused therapy, prior growth in task and bond would be associated with subsequent reduction in PTSD symptoms, and prior reduction in PTSD symptoms would be associated with subsequent growth in task and bond. The main purpose of the present study was to examine the role of alliance components in the process of therapeutic time-specific change in patients diagnosed with PTSD. The patients received either standard prolonged exposure, which includes IE, or modified prolonged exposure, where IR replaced IE as the imagery component of prolonged exposure, in a 10-week residential program. They were assessed repeatedly (weekly) on alliance and PTSD symptom measures, allowing us to separate the variance related to individual differences (between-person component) at the start of treatment from variance related to the intraindividual process of change during treatment (within-person component). To summarize, we wanted to examine the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: Time-specific change in a patient’s task and bond components of the alliance over the course of therapy are negatively related to subsequent change in PTSD symptoms assessed 3 days later (within-person effect). That is, when the task agreement and bond for a given patient is higher than is expected for that patient, subsequent symptoms will be lower. Hypothesis 2: Time-specific change in a patient’s PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy are negatively related to subsequent change in task agreement and bond assessed 4 days later (within-person effect). That is, when the PTSD symptoms for a given patient are less than is expected for that patient, subsequent task agreement and bond will be higher. Hypothesis 3: Individual differences in task agreement and bond at the start of imagery therapy are negatively related to individual differences in the rate of change of PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy (between-person effect). That is, patients who have a higher task agreement and bond at the start of imagery therapy will have a more negative rate of change of PTSD symptoms. Hypothesis 4: There is a cross-level interaction of betweenperson and within-person effects. That is, the lower the level of task agreement and bond is at the start of imagery therapy, the stronger the relationship between time-specific change in alliance and subsequent change in PTSD symptoms will be during therapy, and the higher the level of task agreement and bond is at the start of imagery therapy, the weaker the relationship between time-specific change in alliance and subsequent change in PTSD symptoms will be during therapy. Hypothesis 5: The within-person effect of task agreement and bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms is stronger for IE within prolonged exposure than for IR within prolonged exposure. We also wanted to explore the relationships between goal agreement and PTSD symptoms but expected the magnitude of this relation to be less than the magnitudes for task agreement and bond. 3 Method Participants The participants were selected from referrals to a PTSD treatment program at a national clinic. The clinic was established for the residential treatment of nonpsychotic patients who lack adequate local treatment opportunities or have not responded adequately to outpatient care and require more extensive and/or specialized treatment. The study eligibility was similar to treatment eligibility, that is, all patients who were considered to potentially benefit from the PTSD treatment were included. The inclusion criteria were (a) satisfying Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for PTSD, (b) PTSD identified as the primary disorder in need of treatment, (c) age 18 to 67 years (regulated by the hospital), and (d) accepting withdrawal of all psychotropic medication (regulated by the hospital—patients referred to the hospital have usually received medication without effect). The exclusion criteria were (a) extensive dissociative symptoms, (b) current suicidal risk, (c) current psychosis, and (d) ongoing trauma (e.g., current involvement in an abusive relationship). The study was approved by the Regional Ethics Committee, and the patients’ gave informed consent after the procedures had been fully explained. A flow chart of patients is presented in Figure 1. Seventy-one patients were found eligible for treatment at the assessment stay and admitted to treatment from December 2008 to November 2010. At admission, all these 71 patients were found to meet research criteria, but three of them declined participation. One patient dropped out from treatment before randomization because she changed her mind about receiving trauma-focused therapy. The remaining 67 patients were randomized, 33 to IE within prolonged exposure and 34 to IR within prolonged exposure. Two IE patients lost their eligibility after randomization— one was found to need an eating disorder focus to the exclusion of imagery work, and another was inadvertently treated by the IR protocol. Thus, our intent-to-treat (ITT) with imagery sample consisted of 65 patients—31 IE and 34 IR patients—who signed consent, were randomized to an imagery condition, and were not removed by the investigators. Of these, three patients— one IE and two IR patients— dropped out within 5 to 6 weeks into the program. The reasons for dropout were conflict with therapist in two cases and serious somatic illness in one case. One IR patient received a restricted dose of rescripting, as she insisted to focus on her relationship to her parents after three sessions in accordance with the IR manual. This left a completer sample of 61 patients—30 IE and 31 IR patients. The mean age of 65 patients—38 women and 27 men—was 45.2 years (SD ϭ 9.7). The mean length of time since the index trauma was 17.5 years (SD ϭ 13.3). The most prevalent index trauma, defined as the one experienced by the patient as currently most distressing or most frequently reexperienced or both, among the 38 women was nonsexual assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 12; 31.6%), sexual assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 9; 23.7%), and sexual assault by a stranger (n ϭ 8; 21.1%). Among the 27 men, war experience was most frequent (n ϭ 7; 25.9%), followed by assault by a familiar person (n ϭ 6; 22.2%) and accidents (n ϭ 4; 14.8%). Over half the index traumas were prolonged and/or re-
  • 5. HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD 4 Assessed for eligibility (N = 71) Excluded (n = 4) ♦ Declined to participate (n = 3) ♦ Dropped out before randomization (n = 1) This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Randomized (n = 67) Allocated to imagery exposure (n = 33) ♦ Received full intervention (n = 30) ♦ ♦ Allocated to imagery rescripting (n=34) ♦ Received full intervention (n = 31) Lost eligibility (n = 2) Dropped out after 6 weeks (n = 1) ♦ Analyzed (n = 31) ♦ Excluded because lost eligibility (n = 2) Figure 1. Changed focus after 6 weeks (n = 1) ♦ Dropped out after 5 weeks (n = 2) Analyzed (n = 34) ♦ Excluded from analysis (n = 0) Flow of patients through the study. peated events. Among the 65 patients, 40 (61.5%) had current major depression or dysthymia, 44 (67.7%) had panic disorder with or without agoraphobia or agoraphobia without a history of panic disorder, 39 (60.0%) social phobia, 16 (24.6%) obsessive– compulsive disorder (Axis I), 11 (16.9%) generalized anxiety disorder, 18 (27.7%) alcohol abuse/dependence, 11 (16.9%) avoidant personality disorder, 9 (13.9%) substance abuse/dependence, and 9 (13.9%) obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. No other diagnosis exceeded a proportion of 10% in the present sample. According to chi-square tests, there were no diagnostic differences between the patients in the two treatment conditions. Measures PTSD Symptom Scale–Interview (PSS-I). The PSS-I (Foa, Riggs, Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993) is a semistructured interview consisting of 17 items corresponding to the DSM–IV PTSD symptoms. Both PTSD diagnosis and PTSD symptom severity are assessed. Items are rated on 0 –3 scales for combined frequency and severity in the past 2 weeks (0 ϭ not at all, 1 ϭ once per week or less/a little bit, 2 ϭ 2 to 4 times per week/somewhat, and 3 ϭ 5 or more times per week/very much). Symptom severity is determined by the sum of the 17 ratings. The PSS-I has demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ϭ .85), high interrater agreement (interclass correlation [ICC] ϭ .97), high 1-month test–retest reliability (r ϭ .80), good concurrent validity with other measures of psychopathology, and excellent convergent validity with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R (SCID; Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1988), correctly identifying the PTSD status of 94% of the studied subjects (Foa et al., 1993). The PSS-I was translated into Norwegian (see later) and used as the primary outcome measure in this study. Ten pretreat- ment and 10 posttreatment PSS-I interviews were randomly selected from the total sample of interviews and scored independently. Interrater agreement for the PSS-I total score was evaluated by means of ICC (3, 1; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979), with a value of .91 at pretreatment and .95 at posttreatment. PTSD Symptom Scale–Self-Report (PSS-SR). The PSS-SR (Foa et al., 1993) is a self-report version of the PSS-I and was used as a suboutcome measure in the present study. This measure is usually rated for the last week, but the rating period was shortened to the last 3 days in this study. The frequency part of the criteria was changed correspondingly (0 ϭ not at all, 1 ϭ 1 time/sometimes, 2 ϭ 2 times/half of the time, 3 ϭ 3 or more times/almost always. As for the PSS-I, symptom severity is determined by the sum of the 17 ratings. PSS-SR symptom severity has demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ϭ .91), high 1-month test–retest reliability (r ϭ .74), good concurrent validity with other measures of psychopathology, and excellent convergent validity with the SCID, correctly identifying the PTSD status of 86% of the studied subjects (Foa et al., 1993). The PSS-I and the PSS-SR were translated to Norwegian by the first and the third author and back-translated to English by a native-Englishspeaking professional also competent in Norwegian, until satisfactory formulations were found. Internal consistency reliability of the first-week PSS-SR rating was .88. One-week test–retest reliability coefficient for the PSS-SR scores from the first to the second week (before the more active therapy components were introduced) was .70. Concurrent validity was supported by a correlation of .68 between the first-week PSS-SR scores and pretreatment PSS-I scores. Working Alliance Inventory–Short Revised (WAI-SR). The WAI-SR (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006) is a shortened 12-item
  • 6. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME version of the original 36-item WAI (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). Items are rated on a 1–7 Likert-type scale, and subscale scores for the task (four items), goal (four items) and bond (four items) components of alliance are computed by averaging across items. The WAI-SR has been found to differentiate well between these three components and has shown high internal consistency reliability (subscale score alphas ranging from .85 to .90) and high correlations with other alliance scales (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006). The WAI-SR has been translated to Norwegian and backtranslated to English until satisfactory formulations have been found (Horvath, 1981, 1984, 1991/2006). The internal consistency reliabilities of the four-item Task, Goal, and Bond subscales at the first assessment for the second week were .90, .91, and .85, respectively, and their 1-week test–retest reliabilities from the second to the third week were .72, .80, and .80, respectively. Procedure During a 3-day assessment stay, one of two research psychologists (the second and the third authors) evaluated the applicants by conducting the PSS-I to ascertain the diagnosis of PTSD, whereas the two individual therapists associated with the program evaluated the overall eligibility for the program. At the patients’ admission to the program (pretreatment), one of the two research psychologists conducted a comprehensive interview consisting of the PSS-I, the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1994), and the Structural Clinical Interview for Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, Williams, & Benjamin, 1994). The PSS-I was also conducted at discharge (posttreatment), but this time by a psychologist not involved in the study and blind to the patients’ treatment condition. The alliance measure (together with other process measures not analyzed here) was completed every Friday morning. The patients were asked to base their ratings on their experiences during the last 4 days, that is, during the most treatment-intensive part of the week. The PSS-SR was completed every Monday morning. The patients were asked to base their ratings on their experiences during the last 3 days, that is, during a less treatment-intensive period. To control for potential expectancy bias with respect to the alliance measure, patients were informed that the therapists were blind to the process ratings. Design and Randomization The patients received 10 individual sessions lasting 90 min over a period of 10 weeks. After 1 week of treatment (two first sessions according to the prolonged exposure protocol), the patients were randomized to either IE or IR as the imagery component of the treatment. A person who was not affiliated with the research team organized the randomization procedure. Random sequences generated from http://www.random.org were used for assignment to conditions. A blocked randomization procedure was used in which each therapist was assigned an equal number of cases in each condition. The probability of every patient ending up in any of the two conditions was kept constant at 0.5, and no measures were taken to correct for any imbalance in numbers between the conditions due to discontinued treatments. 5 Treatment The outpatient manuals for prolonged exposure, including IE (Foa et al., 2007) and IR (Smucker, 2005), were used but adapted for the inpatient setting. Essentially, it meant that milieu therapists were available to assist in between-session assignments (in vivo exposure, listening to tapes of the imagery work) and to provide safety and support after intensive individual sessions. The first two individual sessions were the same for all patients and consisted of giving a general treatment rationale and providing trauma education (first session) and introducing and planning in vivo exposure by constructing an exposure hierarchy (second session). Then, before the third session, patients were stratified by therapist and randomly allocated to either the IE or the IR condition, after which they followed the relevant protocols for the third (occurring toward the end of the second week of treatment) to ninth session. In the tenth and final session, the content was again identical and consisted of imagery exposure to the total memory, a review of progress, and suggestions of continued practice. In the sixth week, the patients returned home to test their newly acquired skill in their natural environment. All the time, there was one other treatment group of anxiety patients at the ward, and the PTSD patients participated in the ward’s general program, consisting of one physical exercise session and one ward meeting per week. The IE approach consisted of having participants relive the traumatic event in their imagination and recount the memory in the present tense. To increase vividness, patients were asked to report as much detail as possible, including sights, sounds, smells, behaviors, bodily sensations, feelings, and thoughts. The memory was repeated if necessary to allow total reliving for a period of 40 to 60 min. The entire memory was relived during the first two or three sessions. In the subsequent sessions, the hot spots procedure was usually applied, where reliving was focused on the currently most distressing parts of the memory. The IR approach consisted of three continuous phases. The first phase consisted of imagery reliving of traumatic event in order to activate the trauma memory and to identify the hot spot(s). In Phase 2, without pause in imagery, the memory was relived from the beginning, but this time—at the identified hot spot—the patient was asked to imagine the current self entering the scene at the hot spot and bringing the situation to a solution (overpowering the perpetrators or updating the traumatized self back then with future information). Finally, in Phase 3, patients were stimulated to imagine an interaction between the current self and the traumatized self back then. As in IE, the imagery was supposed to last 40 to 60 min. Therapists One of the individual therapists was a 57-year-old male clinical psychologist with a PhD. The other was a 55-year-old female psychiatric nurse with a master’s degree. The milieu therapists were four psychiatric nurses ranging from 45 to 60 years old. All the individual and milieu therapists had at least 10 years of experience in the cognitive therapy programs for anxiety disorders at the unit and had completed the cognitive therapy specialization program provided by the Norwegian Association of Cognitive Therapy. Of the 65 ITT patients, the psychologist treated 16 IR patients and 15 IE patients, whereas the nurse individual therapist treated 18 IR patients and 16 IE patients.
  • 7. 6 HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD Training and Supervision All the staff received prestudy workshops and supervision by experts Elizabeth Hembree (in prolonged exposure including IE) and Mervin R. Smucker (in IR) during several pilot treatment groups. Throughout the study period, all of the individual sessions were videotaped, and each of the experts provided 90-min supervision sessions of taped imagery biweekly. In addition, the first author provided two 60-min supervision sessions per week to the milieu staff and individual therapists in a group format. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Treatment Integrity The Treatment Integrity Checklist (Foa, Hearst-Ikeda, Dancu, Hembree, & Jaycox, 1997) contains items describing essential and desirable ingredients of prolonged exposure therapy across the 10 sessions described in the manual (Foa et al., 2007). As we were particularly interested in assessing the imagery component, which was the only intended difference between the two treatment conditions, we rated the eight items of the Prolonged Exposure Sessions 4 –9, Section C: Imaginal Exposure. Three of the eight items refer to ingredients that are obligatory (e.g., “reviews instructions for imaginal exposure”), whereas five refer to ingredients that should be present if needed (e.g., “titrates the experience as needed”). Based on discussions with the originator of IR, Mervin R. Smucker, a corresponding checklist for this method was constructed. It consisted of the same three obligatory items as for the IE checklist, one unique obligatory item, and six unique per-asneeded items (e.g., “identifies relevant action impulses coming from the client and helps the client to implement them within imagery”). A score for percentage adherence during an imagery episode is computed by dividing the number of obligatory and per-as-needed ingredients present by the total number of items rated. An overall adequacy (competence) rating for the episode was given using a 1–5 scale with the anchor points poor, mediocre, satisfactory, good, and excellent. Finally, the presence or absence of IR elements was rated. The expert on the therapy form (Elizabeth Hembree or Mervin R. Smucker) rated the episode together with the first and the second author, whereas the third author did simultaneous translation of the videotape. A pilot case in each therapy form was first rated and discussed to calibrate the ratings. Then, 10 random cases, stratified for order of treatment group in the trial and individual therapist, from each therapy form were selected. From these 20 cases, the imagery part of the fifth individual session was rated. One of the cases turned out to be the one who was inadvertently treated by IR instead of IE (see Participants), and this case was omitted from all analyses. Thus, 19 (4.3%) of the total of 440 sessions including the specific imagery component were analyzed. The intraclass correlation (ICC [3, 2]; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) was .69 in IE and .92 in IR for adherence and .93 in IE and .87 in IR for adequacy. The results are based on the expert ratings. Mean adherence rating was 75% (SD ϭ 15%) in IE and 80% (SD ϭ 21%) in IR. Mean adequacy rating was 2.78 (SD ϭ 1.30) in IE, corresponding to a level a little below satisfactory, and 3.20 (SD ϭ 1.32) in IR, corresponding to a level a little above satisfactory. One minor protocol violation was detected in one of the IE sessions, where the therapist asked questions typical of IR for a couple of minutes. After the trial, we asked the individual therapists to fill in a questionnaire about their preference for IE or IR. The psychologist indicated no preference, whereas the psychiatric nurse reported preference for IR because she felt patients’ experience of taking the power from the perpetrator was particularly helpful. Statistical Analysis A main purpose of this study was to examine how within-person changes in components of alliance affected subsequent withinperson changes in outcome. Such a focus on within-person processes necessitates a proper disaggregation of the within-person and between-person components of change in the time-varying predictor. The choice of method of disaggregating within-person and between-person effects in a time-varying predictor depends on how it is related to time (Curran & Bauer, 2011). Specifically, it is important to know if this relationship is characterized by a fixed effect of time or if it is characterized by both a fixed and random effect of time. To estimate these parameters, we conducted several series of mixed models using the three alliance scales (WAI-Task, WAI-Goal, WAI-Bond) and the PTSD symptom measure (PSSSR) as dependent variables. The intent-to-treat sample was analyzed, and due to our research purposes, scores were included from the start of the imagery part of therapy (from the second week of treatment). Moreover, as only active treatment time was of interest, ratings from the week at home were not included, and the home week was not counted in the time term. The fit of these nested models for the covariance was compared by using the likelihood ratio test, in which the difference in model –2 log likelihood values is divided by the difference in degrees of freedom of the models (Fitzmaurice, Laird, & Ware, 2004). Restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used to estimate nested models with only varying random effects (Fitzmaurice et al., 2004). Models with different fixed effects were compared using maximum likelihood estimation. We used an unstructured covariance structure for the random effects, thus allowing the estimation of covariance between the random intercepts and slopes. By contrast, we used a diagonal covariance structure for the residuals, thus allowing the variances of the residuals to differ over time points but setting the covariance between the residuals across time points to zero. Thus, the correlation between the scores across assessments had to be modeled exclusively by the random effects. We started with a model with only a fixed intercept and no random effects, added a random intercept, and, finally, added a random effect of week in therapy. After the best random effects structure had been found in this way, we tested whether another residual covariance structure besides the diagonal—for example, a first-order autoregressive (e.g., AR(1), Toeplitz)— could improve model fit. We then tested whether the inclusion of a fixed linear time term (week in therapy) and—in a second step—a fixed quadratic time term (week2) as independent variables improved model fit. Again, the fit of these nested models was compared by using the likelihood ratio test. For all the alliance scales, a fixed and random intercept and a fixed and random linear effect of time gave the best model fit. Moreover, no alternative residual covariance structure to the diagonal turned out to improve the fit. For the PSS-SR scores as well, a fixed and random intercept and a fixed and random effect of time turned out to be the most appropriate model. In addition, an AR(1) residual covariance structure improved model fit compared to the diagonal structure.
  • 8. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME In order to disaggregate the within-person and between-person variability in the time-varying alliance and symptom measures, we utilized the statistical centering method of detrending presented by Curran and Bauer (2011). This method was chosen because these time-varying predictors were characterized by a fixed and random effect of time. We created two new variables representing the within-person change and between-person differences for bond, task, goal, and PSS-SR scores, respectively (see the applied equations in the Appendix). First, we created the within-person predictor by regressing the variables on time separately for each individual using ordinary least squares. The resulting within-person deviations over weeks in therapy represent the within-person component of the time-varying alliance and symptom measures. In this way, the within-person deviations are conceptualized as the difference between a time-specific observation and the trend line for the variable (i.e., the expected value given a linear growth in the variable). Due to our present research purpose to examine the effect of between-person differences in alliance at the start of the differing imagery therapies, we used the estimated differences on the timevarying predictors at this time point (second week of treatment) to represent their between-person component. By setting time to zero at this point, the between-person component of the time-varying measures are represented by the estimated intercept at the second week for each individual. To correct for the possibility of Type I error, the chosen alpha significance level of .05 was divided by the number of tests (two) for each hypothesis, yielding a level of .025 for the individual test. Because all of the hypotheses were directional in nature, one-tailed tests were used. The effect size (ES) of the overall outcome was computed as Hedges’s g for dependent samples (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). ESs of the between-person and within-person effects were calculated as the proportion of explained outcome variance for each predictor (Snijder & Bosker, 1999; see the applied equations in the Appendix). We used the program SPSS 19.0. Results Overall Outcome In the following ITT analyses, pretreatment PSS-I ratings substituted missing posttreatment ratings. Due to a failure in administrative routines, one IE patient missed the pretreatment PSS-I interview, and his ratings were substituted by the first and the last PSS-SR score. On the PSS-I, the 34 IR patients changed from 33.32 (SD ϭ 6.88) at pretreatment to 22.71 (SD ϭ 14.27) at posttreatment, yielding an ES of Ϫ0.83, 95% CI [Ϫ0.46, Ϫ1.20]. The corresponding change among the 31 IE patients was from 35.19 (SD ϭ 8.24) to 19.90 (SD ϭ 13.76), with an ES of Ϫ1.27, 95% CI [Ϫ0.76, Ϫ1.78]. In the total sample of 65 patients, the ES was Ϫ1.06, 95% CI [Ϫ0.74, Ϫ1.38]. A time by treatment repeated-measures analysis of variance yielded a time effect, F(1, 63) ϭ 69.87, p Ͻ .0001, but no treatment effect, F(1, 63) ϭ 0.04, ns., or time by treatment effect, F(1, 63) ϭ 2.27, p ϭ .137 (two-tailed). 7 Summary Statistics for the Weekly Outcome and Alliance Measures Missing data in the intent-to-treat sample during active imagery treatment was 6.4% for PSS-SR scores, 9.8% for Task scores, 10.0% for Goal scores, and 10.5% for Bond scores. The mean between-person PSS-SR score at the second week (estimated intercept) was 31.31 (SD ϭ 9.06). At the second week of treatment, mean between-person Task score (estimated intercept) was 5.33 (SD ϭ 1.17), Goal score was 5.65 (SD ϭ 1.12), and Bond score was 5.14 (SD ϭ 1.34). An F test for comparing variances in correlated variables showed that the standard deviations of the between-person Task, Goal, and Bond scores were not significantly different. The standard deviations of the within-person Task, Goal, and Bond scores were 0.4752, 0.4367, and 0.4127, respectively. An F test showed that Task scores had larger variances than did Goal and Bond scores (both ps Ͻ .025). The intercorrelations for the estimated between-person alliance scores at the second week (intercept) were high: .87 for Task and Goal, .62 for Task and Bond, and .73 for Goal and Bond. The intercorrelations for the within-person alliance scores over the course of imagery treatment were more moderate: .64 for Task and Goal, .46 for Task and Bond, and .51 for Goal and Bond. Testing Hypotheses Our weekly outcome measure—the PSS-SR—was used as dependent variable in mixed models with random intercept and slope and an AR(1) covariance structure for the residuals (see the Statistical Analysis section). Time (week), treatment (IR vs. IE), and the within-person and between-person components of the three WAI scales were used as predictors. Separate analyses were conducted for each scale. To establish a temporal sequence between predictor and outcome, within-person alliance scores were lagged and thus related to the PSS-SR scores the following week (3 days later). A summary of the fixed main effects for the three alliance components (viz., task, goal, and bond) on PTSD symptoms, as well as the random effects, are shown in Table 1. Our first hypothesis, about a negative within-person effect of task agreement and bond on subsequent symptoms, was supported for the Task scale. That is, if a patient had stronger agreement on tasks in a given week than would be predicted for that patient given his/her general trend, then this patient’s subsequent (3 days later) symptoms were lower than would be expected. The Goal and Bond scales showed no such within-person effect. Unrelated to our hypotheses, Table 1 also shows that there was a negative relationship between interindividual differences in initial Task scores and mean level of PTSD symptoms over the course of therapy but no such relationship for the other two WAI scales. In addition, there was a negative effect of time, which indicates that the PSS-SR scores were reduced over the course of therapy. There was no effect of treatment (viz., IR vs. IE) on the mean level of PSS-SR scores over the course of therapy. To examine our hypothesis about reciprocal causation, that is, that the PTSD symptoms would be negatively related to subsequent task agreement and bond, the three WAI scales were used as dependent variables in mixed models with random intercept and slope and a diagonal covariance structure for the residuals (see the Statistical
  • 9. HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD 8 Table 1 Fixed Effects Estimates and Random Effects (Variance–Covariance) Estimates for the Three Models of the Predictors of PTSD Symptoms This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Parameter Intercept Week Treatment: IR Treatment: IE Within-person task Between-person task Within-person goal Between-person goal Within-person bond Between-person bond Residual AR(1) rho Intercept Week Intercept ϫ Week –2 log likelihood Task Goal Bond Fixed effects 40.885‫)602.6( ء‬ 43.138‫)453.5( ء‬ Ϫ1.312‫)071.0( ء‬ Ϫ1.272‫)361.0( ء‬ Ϫ2.446 (2.265) Ϫ3.470 (2.374) 0 (0) 0 (0) Ϫ0.820‫)193.0( ء‬ Ϫ2.139‫)779.0( ء‬ Ϫ0.527 (0.426) Ϫ1.667 (1.065) 37.054‫)179.4( ء‬ Ϫ1.316‫)771.0( ء‬ Ϫ1.110 (2.411) 0 (0) Ϫ0.724 (0.464) Ϫ1.174 (0.910) Random effects 17.136‫)249.1( ء‬ 16.638‫)997.1( ء‬ 0.210‫)980.0( ء‬ 0.193‫)680.0( ء‬ 68.460‫)872.41( ء‬ 79.295‫)442.61( ء‬ 1.118‫)003.0( ء‬ 1.235‫)433.0( ء‬ 2.947 (1.617) 3.897‫)856.1( ء‬ 2737.418 2727.652 16.096‫)348.1( ء‬ 0.191‫)390.0( ء‬ 74.918‫)367.51( ء‬ 1.296‫)343.0( ء‬ 2.712 (1.752) 2549.456 Note. Standard errors are in parentheses. PTSD ϭ posttraumatic stress disorder; IR ϭ imagery rescripting; IE ϭ imaginal exposure; task ϭ agreement about tasks; goal ϭ agreement about goals; bond ϭ patient–therapist emotional bond; AR(1) ϭ first order autoregressive. ‫ء‬ p Ͻ .05. Analysis section). Within-person and between-person PSS-SR scores were used as predictors. In addition, we included time and treatment as predictors of the alliance scores. Our hypothesis that within-person variations in PTSD symptoms would predict subsequent withinperson variations in PTSD symptoms was not supported for any of the WAI scales. That is, there was no within-person effect of PSS-SR scores on Task, Goal, or Bond scores (all absolute t values Ͻ 1). To examine our third to fifth hypotheses, all the six interactions between our four predictors were added in the three models. Our third hypothesis, stating that higher initial task agreement and bond predicted a steeper negative slope of PTSD symptoms, was supported. That is, there was a significant time by between-person task effect, ␤ ϭ Ϫ0.272, SE ϭ 0.136, t(55.5) ϭ Ϫ2.00, p ϭ .025, and a significant time by between-person bond effect, ␤ ϭ Ϫ0.337, SE ϭ 0.125, t(55.6) ϭ Ϫ2.71 p Ͻ .01. As these interaction effects were negative, they indicate that with longer time into therapy, higher initial alliance was associated with lower PTSD symptoms. There was no time by between-person goal effect on symptoms. Our fourth hypothesis, that the within-person effect of alliance on outcome is stronger with lower initial levels of alliance, was contradicted by the results for the Task scale. That is, there was a cross-level interaction of between-person and within-person effects of task, ␤ ϭ Ϫ0.814, SE ϭ 0.403, t(320.7) ϭ Ϫ2.02, p Ͻ .025. The negative direction of this interaction effect shows that— opposite to what we expected—the higher the initial task alliance, the stronger the negative relationship between within-person variations in task alliance and subsequent within-person variations in PTSD symptoms. No crosslevel interactions of the within- and between-person effects were evident for the Goal and Bond scales. Our fifth hypothesis, stating that the within-person relationship between alliance and outcome is stronger in IE than in IR, was supported for the Task scale. That is, treatment interacted with the within-person effect of Task scores on PSS-SR scores. When using IE as a baseline, there was a positive effect of IR on PSS-SR scores, ␤ ϭ 2.031, SE ϭ 0.775, t(325.4) ϭ 2.62, p Ͻ .01. Considering the overall negative within-person effect of Task scores on PSS-SR scores (see Table 1), the positive direction of this relationship in IR compared to IE shows that the relationship is weaker in IR than in IE. It should also be noted that there was a time by treatment effect. In the model using task as a predictor, there was a positive effect of IR on PSS-SR scores with time, ␤ ϭ 0.964, SE ϭ 0.317, t(54.9) ϭ 3.04, p Ͻ .01. Considering the overall negative effect of time on PSS-SR scores (see Table 1), the positive effect of IR compared to IE used as baseline shows that the PSS-SR scores were less reduced in IR than in IE. There was no individual therapist effect on the rate of change of PSS-SR, Task, Goal, or Bond scores. The Magnitude of Effects Compared to a baseline model including only the random effects (intercept, time) and the fixed effect of time, residual variance was reduced, with 4.3%, while random intercept variance was reduced, with 5.8%, when within-person and between-person Task scores were added in the model. Discussion The Role of Alliance in Varying Imagery Procedures for PTSD The main purpose of this study was to examine the role of alliance components in the process of therapeutic change in PTSD patients. Most importantly, the hypothesis of a negative within-
  • 10. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME person effect of the components agreement about the tasks of therapy and bond on subsequent PTSD symptoms was supported for the task component. That is, when the task score for a given patient was higher than was expected for that patient, the subsequent symptom score was lower than was expected for him/her, explaining about 4% of the outcome variance. In any event, this finding goes beyond previous research in the PTSD treatment field by indicating that time-specific change in a person’s task agreement during therapy is related to this person’s subsequent change in PTSD symptoms. On a more general level, this finding supports and extends those of Tasca and Lampard (2012) and Falkenström et al. (2013), who found a within-person relationship between overall alliance and subsequent symptoms but in different treatments and patient populations. Furthermore, the results indicate that the within-person relationship between task agreement and outcome is dependent on the specific therapy form. As we hypothesized, this relationship was stronger in IE than in IR. On the other hand, within-person changes in PTSD symptoms did not predict subsequent task agreement and bond. That is, time-specific change in a person’s symptoms during therapy was not related to this person’s subsequent change in task and bond. This finding is at odds with those of Tasca and Lampard (2012) and Falkenström et al. (2013), who found a bidirectional relationship between alliance and symptoms. A conspicuous difference between these studies and ours is that we used standardized, highly structured, and manual-based procedures. One may speculate that patients’ belief in and agreement to such procedures are less influenced by symptom variations than is their agreement to less standardized and less clearly defined procedures. Our hypotheses about a between-person effect of initial task agreement and bond was supported. Initial Task and Bond scores predicted a steeper negative slope of PTSD symptoms. These findings are consistent with most findings in alliance research (Horvath et al., 2011) that early alliance predicts the further course of symptoms. The centrality of the task component in predicting overall (between-person) outcome is consistent with the results of Webb et al. (2011), who found therapist–patient agreement on the tasks and goals of therapy to account for most of the outcome variance in cognitive therapy for depression. However, our results indicate that a good initial bond is also important for a successful overall outcome in exposure-based therapy for PTSD. Thus, alliance components may have different roles in cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for different patient populations. What results would be obtained for forms of therapy other than CBT is unknown, as it appears that the bond works differently in dynamic therapy than it does in CBT. For instance, the bond and therapist’s focus on affect seem to be differently related to each other and to outcome in these two therapies (Ulvenes et al., 2012). We expected that task agreement and bond would be of greater concern for those who had a lower individual level and would thus be more influential in these persons’ process of change, but in fact the within-person effect of task scores on subsequent PTSD symptoms was stronger in those with a higher initial task agreement. Future research must show whether this was a chance finding or not. However, if this effect is replicated, it would suggest a double drawback for a patient having a low initial agreement about the tasks of therapy. First, the patient would experience less overall improvement over the course of therapy, and, second, greater than 9 expected levels of agreement during the process of therapy for that patient would not be as effective. We also explored the role of the alliance component goal agreement. As expected, this component was unrelated to symptom change. The within-person component of goal agreement also had less variance than the within-person component of task agreement, and this difference may have contributed to the differential findings. Our results are consistent with Horvath (2011), who found— on the between-person level—that the agreement on tasks as a predictor of outcome was superior to both bond and agreement on goals. Strengths and Limitations of the Study Alliance and PTSD symptoms were assessed weekly, and adequate methods were utilized to separate the within-person and between-person effects of the time-varying predictors in the applied multilevel models. Thus, we could study within-person relationships over the course of therapy, which are of particular relevance for psychotherapy theories. This is because therapy theories concern such relationships, that is, how change in a process variable relates to subsequent change in an outcome variable. Such knowledge directly informs therapists concerning what process variables need to be affected to achieve patient improvement. By contrast, knowledge of between-person relationships— one patient having a low initial alliance and poor outcome and another having a high initial alliance and good outcome— does not imply that an increase in the first patient’s alliance would lead to a better outcome for that patient. Thus, relationships established on a between-person level do not imply that the same relationships hold on a within-person level. For instance, the relationship between bond and outcome obtained in the present study on the between-person level was not replicated on the within-person level. A further advantage of properly separating the between- and within-person components of a time-varying predictor is the possibility of examining cross-level interactions of within- and between-person effects. For therapists, how between-person differences in, for example, alliance or self-concept moderate withinperson relationships over the course of therapy is more directly relevant than are the correlations of these differences with overall outcome. Such moderating knowledge informs therapists concerning under what conditions (e.g., high task agreement relative to other patients) certain within-person change processes are working (e.g., higher than usual task agreement at a given time point predicts lower than usual PTSD symptoms). A further advantage of studying within-person relationships between process and outcome is the possibility of identifying reciprocal or even reversed causality between process and outcome. The RCT design, where patients were randomized to two empirically based imagery methods, allowed us to study the moderating influence of therapy form on the within-person relationships. The studied sample had high clinical representativeness, as research eligibility was similar to treatment eligibility and only three (4.2%) of 71 treatment eligible patients declined research participation. Moreover, the dropout rate from imagery treatment was low: three (4.6%) of 65 patients. The present study has several limitations. Although the uncontrolled effect size of Ϫ1.27 (Hedges’s g, intent-to-treat analysis) for standard prolonged exposure (including IE as the imagery component) is comparable to that in one of the studies conducted
  • 11. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 10 HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD by the originators of prolonged exposure (e.g., Hedges’s g ϭ Ϫ1.37 in Foa et al., 2005), the adequacy ratings were only around a level of satisfactory for both imagery treatments. The adherence ratings of 75% (IE) and 80% (IR) are lower than those typically found in the original studies of prolonged exposure (e.g., 97% in Foa et al., 2005). Thus, the varied component of treatment may have been delivered in a less than optimal way. Moreover, less extensive examinations of integrity (4.3% of tapes) than usual (about 10% of tapes; Foa et al., 1997) were performed. No integrity ratings were performed for the other components of treatment (e.g., in vivo exposure). Although well-validated measures were used, their Norwegian translations have not undergone psychometric evaluation in previous investigations. In the present study, their internal consistency and test–retest reliability appeared satisfactory, though. Alliance and symptom ratings were collected from the same individual, that is, the patient, and this may have inflated their correlation. However, halo effects were prevented by having the ratings done 3 and 4 days apart. Furthermore, response biases like acquiescence are supposed to cut across ratings and may affect within-person variations—which were the main focus of this study—to a lesser degree. We used a passive observational design, and unmeasured third variable confounds could have influenced the results. The power of the study, based on about eight repeated measurements of 65 patients (minus some missing data), may be too low to detect some withinperson relationships. We studied process on a weekly time scale, and larger or lesser scales could be associated with different results. The strategy of using the same therapists across therapies has both strengths and weaknesses. The therapists may not be equally competent and have the same preferences for both therapies. Actually, one of the therapists reported a preference for IR. However, this bias could not explain the present results, as PTSD symptoms measured weekly were less reduced over the course of therapy in IR than in IE. In the context of the present study, an advantage of crossing therapists was that the general ability to form alliances was balanced between conditions. Research Implications As elaborated above, our study invites an increased focus on within-person relationships in psychotherapy research. In highly structured therapies like those of the present study and cognitive therapy of depression (Webb et al., 2011), symptomatic improvement is supposed to result from the relatively specific tasks of these therapies. Agreement about tasks may therefore be particularly important in such therapies. Moreover, the studied PTSD sample was a severe one with a high degree of comorbidity and a long duration of PTSD, and over half of the patients had experienced repeated and/or prolonged traumas. Future studies should investigate the within-person relationships between alliance components and outcome across therapies and type and severity of disorders. Furthermore, studies of within-person relationships between therapy events/therapist actions and alliance components are needed. Clinical Implications The present within-person results make a firm basis for the recommendation to monitor, increase, and restore decreases of agreement about therapy tasks over the course of IE or IR within prolonged exposure for PTSD patients. They also suggest that addressing agreement about the tasks of therapy is particularly important in IE compared to IR. Given that these exposure methods consist of confronting the feared trauma memory and feared external situations, agreeing to their use based on an understanding of the rationale for these methods and a belief in their efficacy seems paramount. On the other hand, the results do not imply an increased focus on the agreement about goals of therapy and bond components of alliance over the course of these treatments. Our between-person results may inform therapists using prolonged exposure for PTSD that low initial task agreement and bond signal a poorer outcome of therapy. Unfortunately, because the crosslevel interaction between interindividual and within-individual task agreement was contrary to our hypothesis, clinical implications cannot be drawn from this finding. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Arntz, A., Tiesema, M., & Kindt, M. (2007). Treatment of PTSD: A comparison of imaginal exposure with and without imagery rescripting. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 345– 370. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.006 Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 16, 252–260. doi:10.1037/h0085885 Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Cornwall, United Kingdom: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386 Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, C., Miranda, R., & Chemtob, C. M. (2004). Therapeutic alliance, negative mood regulation, and treatment outcome in child abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 411– 416. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72 .3.411 Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 583– 619. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008 .100356 Dalgleish, T., & Power, M. J. (2004). Emotion-specific and emotion-nonspecific components of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Implications for a taxonomy of related psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1069 –1088. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.05.001 Falkenström, F., Granström, F., & Holmqvist, R. (2013). Therapeutic alliance predicts symptomatic improvement session by session. Journal of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/ a0032258 First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. (1994). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis II Personality Disorders—Patient (SCID-II) (Version 2.0). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department. Fitzmaurice, G. M., Laird, N. M., & Ware, J. H. (2004). Applied longitudinal analysis. New York, NY: Wiley. Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Symonds, D., & Horvath, A. O. (2012). How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel longitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 10 – 17. doi:10.1037/a0025749 Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D. E., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., & Jaycox, L. H. (1997). Prolonged exposure (PE): Manual. Unpublished manuscript, Eastern Pennsylvania Psychiatric Institute, Allegheny University of the Health Sciences.
  • 12. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. WITHIN-PERSON EFFECTS OF ALLIANCE ON OUTCOME Foa, E. B., Hembree, E., Cahill, S. E., Rauch, S. A. M., Riggs, D. S., Feeney, N. C., & Yadin, E. (2005). Randomized trial of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder with and without cognitive restructuring: Outcome of academic and community clinics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 953–964. doi:10.1037/0022006X.73.5.953 Foa, E. B., Hembree, E., & Rothbaum, B. O. (2007). Prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD: Emotional processing of traumatic experiences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 459 – 473. doi:10.1002/ jts.2490060405 Grunert, B. K., Weis, J. M., Smucker, M. R., & Christianson, H. (2007). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy after failed prolonged imaginal exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder following industrial injury. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 317–328. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.005 Hatcher, R. L., & Gillaspy, J. A. (2006). Development and validation of a short version of the Working Alliance Inventory. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 12–25. doi:10.1080/10503300500352500 Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Evaluating between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development, 6, 97–120. doi:10.1080/ 15427600902911189 Horvath, A. O. (2006). The Working Alliance Inventory: Norwegian translation of items generated by the factor analyses of Hatcher & Gillaspy (2006). M. H. Rønnestad, A. von der Lippe, E. Axelsen, S. E. Gullestad, H. Haavind, S. Reichelt, & O. A. Tjersland (Trans.). Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway. (Original works published 1981, 1984, 1991) Horvath, A. O. (2011, July). The complex world of alliance assessments: Will the real alliance please stand up? Paper presented at the Society for Psychotherapy Research Conference, Bern. Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011). Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48, 9 –16. doi: 10.1037/a0022186 Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.36.2.223 Keller, S. M., Zoellner, L. A., & Feeny, N. C. (2010). Understanding factors associated with early therapeutic alliance in PTSD treatment: 11 Adherence, childhood sexual abuse history, and social support. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 974 –979. doi:10.1037/ a0020758 Lee, D. A., Scragg, P., & Turner, S. (2001). The role of shame and guilt in traumatic events: A clinical model of shame-based and guilt-based PTSD. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 74, 451– 466. doi: 10.1348/000711201161109 Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (1996). The resolution of ruptures in the therapeutic alliance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 447– 458. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.447 Sheehan, D., Janavs, J., Baker, R., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Knapp, E., & Sheehan, M. (1994). M.I.N.I. (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview). Tampa, FL: University of South Florida. Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420 – 428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420 Smucker, M. R. (2005). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy (IRRT): A treatment manual for adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse experiencing PTSD. Milwaukee, WI: International Trauma Institute. Snijder, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel modeling: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London, England: Sage. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1988). Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM–III–R—Patient Version (SCID-P). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute. Tasca, G. A., & Lampard, A. M. (2012). Reciprocal influence of alliance to the group and outcome in day treatment for eating disorders. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 507–517. doi:10.1037/a0029947 Ulvenes, P. G., Berggraf, L., Hoffart, A., Stiles, T. C., Svartberg, M., McCullough, L., & Wampold, B. E. (2012). Different processes for different therapies: Therapist actions, therapeutic bond, and outcome. Psychotherapy, 49, 291–302. doi:10.1037/a0027895 Wampold, B. E., & Budge, S. L. (2012). The 2011 Leona Tyler Award Address: The relationship—And its relationship to the common and specific factors of psychotherapy. Counseling Psychologist, 40, 601– 623. doi:10.1177/0011000011432709 Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., Amsterdam, J. D., Shelton, R. C., Hollon, S. D., & Dimidjian, S. (2011). Two aspects of the therapeutic alliance: Differential relations with depressive symptom change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 279 –283. doi:10.1037/a0023252 (Appendix follows)
  • 13. HOFFART, ØKTEDALEN, LANGKAAS, AND WAMPOLD 12 Appendix Equations Used in the Statistical Analyses ␤1i ϭ ␥10 ϩ u1i Equations for the Multilevel Models We begin with the Level 1 model: yti ϭ ␤0i ϩ ␤1i x ti ϩ eti u0i ϭ (zi Ϫ ␥00) Ϫ (␥10 ϩ u1i)xi ៮ (A1) Composite: This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. eti ϭ zti Ϫ ␤0i Ϫ ␤1i x ti . Ei(zti) ϭ (␥00 ϩ u0i) ϩ ␥10Ei(x ti) ϩ Ei(u1i x 0i) Here the individual-specific value of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms (yti) is a function of an individual intercept (␤0i), the slope coefficient of the time score for time t for individual i (␤1i xti), and the residual symptoms of PTDS (eti) on time t for individual i. The equation term eti is computed by deviating the time-specific predictor (zti) from the regression line (␤1i xti) estimated separately (case by case) for each individual in the sample. The deviated measure, eti, is then the residual (i.e., the observed score minus expected value) from the regression of the timevarying predictor on time computed separately for each individual case, which then represents the variable for the within-person level of each predictor (i.e., Task, Goal, or Bond). The Level 2 between-person predictor represents variance due to interindividual differences in the time-varying predictor at the start of treatment, as shown in Equation A2: ϭ(␥00 ϩ u0i) ϩ (␥10 ϩ u1i)Ei(x ti). zbi ϭ ␤1i x 0i . In the Level 2 model, zbi is the between-person component of the time-varying predictor and is a function of individual differences in the time-varying predictor at the start of treatment (␤1i x0i). The equations for the model with main effects of the betweenperson and within-person predictors are presented in Equation A3: Level 1: ៮ ៮ eti ϭ (zti Ϫ zi) Ϫ (␥10 ϩ u1i)(x ti Ϫ x i) Level 2: ␤0i ϭ ␥00 ϩ u0i Equations for Proportion Reduction of Error at Each Level The proportion reduction of error for predicting the Level 1 outcome is (A2) R zti ϭ ␤0i ϩ ␤1i x ti ϩ eti In the Level 1 model, the individual-specific value of symptoms of PTSD (yti) is a function of an individual intercept (␤0i), the within-person effects of the time-varying predictor (␤1i xti), and the residual PTSD symptoms (eti) on time t for individual i. In the Level 2 model, the individual intercept (␤0i) is a function of a fixed intercept (␥00) and an individual-specific random intercept (u0i). The individual effects of slope (␤1i) is a function of the fixed effects in rate of change (␥10) and person-specific slope (u1i). (A3) R2 ϭ 1 – L1 ͩ residual variance more ϩ intercept variance more residual variance fewer ϩ intercept variance fewer ͪ . The proportion reduction of error for predicting the Level 2 outcome is R2 ϭ 1 – L2 ΂ residual variance more #Level 1 units residual variance fewer #Level 1 units ϩ intercept variance more ϩ intercept variance fewer ΃ . Received January 9, 2013 Revision received May 13, 2013 Accepted May 13, 2013 Ⅲ