SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 20
Download to read offline
Journal of School Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 1, pp. 7–26, 2002
Copyright © 2002 Society for the Study of School Psychology
Printed in the USA
0022-4405/02 $–see front matter
PII S0022-4405(01)00092-9
7
The Prevention of Reading Difficulties
Joseph K. Torgesen
Florida State University
The purpose of this article is to provide practical advice about methods to prevent
reading failure that is grounded in the new knowledge we have acquired about
reading and learning to read over the past 2 decades. Recent research on reading
is used to establish a set of facts about reading and reading growth that is relevant
to establishing instructional objectives and methods for the prevention of reading
difficulties. Within the context of our current understanding of the reasons many
children find it difficult to learn to read, the article also identifies the instructional
conditions that need to be in place to prevent the development of reading difficul-
ties in all but a very small proportion of children. The article concludes with a dis-
cussion of issues and procedures for the early identification of children who are
likely to experience difficulties learning to read. © 2002 Society for the Study of
School Psychology. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd
Keywords: Reading disabilities, Prevention, Early identification, Assessment,
Reading instruction.
The purpose of this article is to provide practical advice about methods to
prevent reading failure that is grounded in the new knowledge we have ac-
quired about reading and learning to read over the past two decades. Most
of this new knowledge about reading has been reported in two recent con-
sensus documents. One report (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), titled Pre-
venting Reading Difficulties in Young Children, was prepared by the National
Research Council and published by the National Academy of Sciences.
The other document, titled Teaching Children to Read (National Reading
Panel, 2000) was commissioned by the U.S. Congress and supported in de-
velopment by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
ment and the U.S. Office of Education. Both documents were written by
committees of professionals who were asked to identify the findings about
reading and reading instruction that were most consistently supported in
recent research.
These are interesting and challenging times for anyone whose profes-
sional responsibilities are related in any way to literacy outcomes among
school children. For, in spite of all our new knowledge about reading and
Received June 19, 2001; accepted October 30, 2001.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Joseph K. Torgesen, Department of Psy-
chology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306-1270. Phone: (850) 644-1707; fax:
(850) 644-7739; E-mail: torgesen@psy.fsu.edu
8 Journal of School Psychology
reading instruction, there is a wide-spread concern that public education is
not as effective as it should be in teaching all children to read. Fueled by
such facts as the statistic that 37% of fourth-grade school children cannot
read well enough to effectively accomplish grade-level work (National Cen-
ter for Education Statistics, 2001), there is an emerging sense of urgency
about improving reading instruction and literacy outcomes in our country.
The report of the National Research Council pointed out that these con-
cerns about literacy derive not from declining levels of literacy in our
schools, but rather from recognition that the demands for high levels of lit-
eracy are rapidly accelerating in our society. Clearly, children who become
adults with low levels of literacy are at an increasing disadvantage in a soci-
ety that is creating ever-higher demands for effective reading skills within
the workplace. These rising demands can only be met by changing the way
we work at teaching reading so that we produce better literacy outcomes
for more children than ever before.
A major aspect of change in the way we work to teach reading to all chil-
dren must involve allocation of resources for early identification and pre-
ventive instruction. The costs of waiting until mid-elementary school to
identify children in need of special instruction in reading are simply too
great. We know, for example, that delayed development of reading skills
affects vocabulary growth (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998), alters chil-
dren’s attitudes and motivation to read (Oka & Paris, 1986), and leads to
missed opportunities to develop comprehension strategies (Brown, Palinc-
sar, & Purcell, 1986). If children fall seriously behind in the growth of criti-
cal early reading skills, they have fewer opportunities to practice reading.
Recent evidence (Torgesen, Rashotte, & Alexander, 2001) suggests that these
lost practice opportunities make it extremely difficult for children who re-
main poor readers during the first three years of elementary school to ever
acquire average levels of reading fluency. Finally, there is the very sobering
fact obtained in several longitudinal studies that children who are poor
readers at the end of first grade almost never acquire average-level reading
skills by the end of elementary school (Francis, Shaywitz, Stuebing, Shay-
witz, & Fletcher, 1996; Juel, 1988; Torgesen & Burgess, 1998).
This article is organized in three sections. The first section discusses a set
of facts about reading and reading growth that is relevant to establishing
instructional objectives and methods for the prevention of reading difficul-
ties. These are also facts about reading that every school psychologist should
know, and they represent a fundamental departure from views about read-
ing that underlie many “wholistic” approaches to reading instruction cur-
rently in use. The next section discusses the instructional conditions that
need to be in place to prevent the development of reading difficulties in all
but a very small proportion of children. The article concludes with a dis-
cussion of issues and procedures for the early identification of children
who are likely to experience difficulties learning to read. Information in
Torgesen 9
the second section should be helpful to school psychologists in their roles
as consultants about effective educational practices, and the final section
contains information of potentially direct use by school psychologists to
enhance the effectiveness of their assessment work in schools.
FACTS ABOUT READING THAT ARE RELEVANT TO THE PREVENTION
OF READING DIFFICULTIES
It is technically incorrect to call the ideas presented in this section “facts”
about reading. Rather, they are conclusions about reading and reading
growth that are now assumed to be true based on consistent research find-
ings. All of the ideas presented here are consistent with the conclusions re-
ported in the two recent consensus documents described in the introduc-
tion to this article. The first idea is not so much a “fact,” but is a self-evident
value judgment about reading for which there is wide-spread agreement.
The Ultimate Goal of Reading Instruction Is to Help Children Acquire all
the Skills Necessary to Comprehend the Meaning of Text
In other words, the goal of literacy instruction is to help children acquire
the skills that enable learning from, understanding, and enjoyment of writ-
ten language. This is not a “controversial” assumption. No matter what
one’s instructional orientation may be, the long-term goal of reading in-
struction is to provide children with the skills necessary to construct, or
comprehend, the meaning of text in its many forms and genres.
Two General Types of Skill and Knowledge Are Required for Good
Reading Comprehension
To be able to construct meaning from text, a child must have (a) general
language comprehension skills and (b) the ability to accurately and fluently
identify the individual words in print. Knowledge of and active application of
specific reading strategies are also helpful to maximize reading comprehen-
sion (Snow et al., 1998), but most of the variability among children and
adults in comprehension of written material can be accounted for by mea-
suring the two broad families of skills identified in Gough’s simple view of
reading (Gough, 1996; Hoover & Gough, 1990). That is, good general lan-
guage comprehension and good word reading skills are the most critical
skills required for effective comprehension of written material.
The fact that reading comprehension is the joint product of language
comprehension ability and word identification skills must be considered
when we set goals for the reading attainment of all children. In other
words, we must recognize the fact that general cognitive ability (specifically
verbal ability and knowledge) strongly influences reading comprehension
10 Journal of School Psychology
at the higher grade levels in elementary school (Adams, 1990). For this rea-
son, I usually qualify the goal for reading instruction in the following way:
The ultimate goal of reading instruction is to help children acquire the
knowledge and skills necessary to comprehend printed material at a level
that is consistent with their general verbal ability or language comprehension skills.
If we were to adopt a strict grade-level reading comprehension criteria
(i.e., every child will be able to fully comprehend material written at grade
level in fourth grade), this would imply an expectation for all children to
have at least average verbal ability. Decades of cognitive intervention re-
search suggest that it is unrealistic to expect all children to attain verbal
ability estimates within the average range as a result of special instruction
(Lee, Brooks-Gunn, Schnur, & Liaw, 1990). Thus, it seems unrealistic to
expect reading teachers to accomplish this goal starting as late as kinder-
garten or first grade. This statement does not ignore the fact that the ver-
bal ability of many children can be dramatically increased by effective read-
ing instruction (Torgesen, Alexander, et al., 2001); it is just meant to
acknowledge the fact that this may not be possible for all children.
A Critical Problem for Most Children Who Experience Reading Difficulties
Involves Early and Continuing Problems Acquiring Accurate and Fluent
Word Identification Skills
When asked to read grade-level text, the typical poor reader in third or
fourth grade will show two kinds of word-level reading difficulties. First,
when they encounter a word they are not familiar with, they tend to place
too much reliance on guessing the word based on the context or meaning
of the passage (Briggs, Austin, & Underwood, 1984; Simpson, Lorsbach, &
Whitehouse, 1983), which produces a high rate of word-level errors in
their reading. Their phonemic analysis skills, or ability to use “phonics” to
assist in the word identification process, is usually severely impaired
(Bruck, 1990; Siegel, 1989). Second, most children who are having diffi-
culty learning to read encounter many more words in grade-level text that
they cannot read “by sight” than do average readers. Compared with chil-
dren of the same age who are learning to read normally, the number of
words that children with reading problems can recognize fluently and eas-
ily as “sight words” is usually quite limited (Manis, Custodio, & Szeszulski,
1993; Torgesen, Alexander, et al., 2001).
Difficulties learning to read words accurately are manifest from the very
earliest stages of reading instruction. First, children who are destined to be
poor readers in fourth grade almost invariably have difficulties under-
standing and applying the alphabetic principle in deciphering unfamiliar
words. Their difficulties developing good analytic strategies for identifying
unknown words makes it difficult for them to read independently, and it
also produces far too many word reading errors. Both of these latter conse-
Torgesen 11
quences of failure to acquire good phonemic decoding skills affect the de-
velopment of fluent word reading ability, which depends heavily on learn-
ing to identify large numbers of words by sight (Torgesen, Rashotte, &
Alexander, 2001). Because words do not become sight words until they are
read accurately a number of times, both inaccurate reading and dimin-
ished reading practice cause slow growth of fluent word-identification
skills. Furthermore, the strongest current theories of reading growth link
phonemic and sight word reading skills together by showing how good
phonemic decoding skills are necessary in the formation of accurate mem-
ory for the spelling patterns that are the basis of sight word recognition
(Ehri, 1998).
The difficulties acquiring accurate and fluent word reading skills shown
by most poor readers are extremely important in light of the new informa-
tion we have about the way word-level processes operate during skilled read-
ing. Skilled readers do not “skim and scan” text as they read for meaning,
but rather they directly fixate and process a very high proportion of all the
words in text. Furthermore, they accurately identify most of the words in
text by processing information about all, or almost all, the letters in words
(Adams, 1990). In other words, skilled word recognition is heavily depen-
dent upon very detailed knowledge of the letters used to spell individual
words. Skilled readers do not guess at the identity of specific words in text
by relying on context; rather, they are able to accurately and fluently iden-
tify words on the basis of their written spellings. Adams (1991) summarized
these facts about word-recognition processes in skilled readers this way:
It has been proven beyond any shade of doubt that skillful readers process vir-
tually each and every word and letter of text as they read. This is extremely
counter-intuitive. For sure, skillful readers neither look nor feel as if that’s
what they do. But that’s because they do it so quickly and effortlessly. Almost
automatically; with almost no conscious attention whatsoever, skillful readers
recognize words by drawing on deep and ready knowledge of spellings and
their connections to speech and meaning. (p. 207)
One of the keys to becoming a skilled reader is to acquire a large vocab-
ulary of words that can be recognized fluently and accurately in text. We
also understand now that early development of accurate phonemic decod-
ing skills plays in important supportive role in helping children acquire the
very specific memories for words that are required for automatic recogni-
tion (Ehri, 1998). Both of these facts underline the critical role of careful
and explicit instruction in word-level reading processes as part of early
reading instruction for children at risk for reading difficulties. Programs
that are not sufficient to “normalize” the development of at-risk children
in phonemic decoding skill and fluent word recognition have not accom-
plished one of the most critical goals of preventive reading instruction.
12 Journal of School Psychology
The Most Common Cause of Children’s Early Difficulties in Acquiring
Accurate and Fluent Word Recognition Skills Involves Individual
Differences in Their Phonological Knowledge and Skill
This is one of the most important discoveries about reading difficulties in
the last 20 years (Liberman, Shankweiler, & Liberman, 1989; Torgesen &
Mathes, 2000). Children who enter first grade low in knowledge about the
phonological features of words or who have difficulties processing the pho-
nological features of words are at high risk for difficulties responding to
early reading instruction. The tasks most commonly used to measure chil-
dren’s knowledge and processing skill for the phonological features of
words are referred to as measures of phonological, or phonemic, aware-
ness. These tasks require children to identify or manipulate the phonemes
in words that are presented orally. Phonemic awareness tests do not in-
volve letters. For example, a simple task in this domain would ask children
to say which of three words (bat, car, fork) begins with the same sound as
bike. A more difficult task might ask the child to pronounce the first sound
in the word bike, and a still more difficult task might ask the child to say
what word was left when the word card was pronounced without saying the
/d/ sound. Both conscious awareness of the phonemes in words and ability
to accurately identify them within words is necessary in learning to phone-
mically decode words in print (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994; Wag-
ner et al., 1997). Children who are delayed in the development of phone-
mic awareness have a very difficult time making sense out of “phonics”
instruction, and they certainly have little chance to notice the phonemic
patterns in written words on their own. A simple way to say this is that for
individual children, phonemic awareness is what makes phonics instruc-
tion meaningful. If a child has little awareness that even simple words like
cat and car are composed of small “chunks” that are combined in different
ways to make words, our alphabetic way of writing makes no sense.
Discovery of the core phonological problems associated with early read-
ing difficulties has had at least one unanticipated consequence. The ability
to assess these core language problems directly has led to the discovery that
the early word reading difficulties of children with relatively low general in-
telligence and verbal ability are associated with the same factors that inter-
fere with early reading growth in children who have general intelligence in
the average range (Fletcher et al., 1994; Share & Stanovich, 1995; Stanov-
ich & Siegel, 1994). Weaknesses in phonemic awareness characterize chil-
dren with reading problems across a broad span of general verbal ability.
On the one hand, many children enter school with adequate general ver-
bal ability and cognitive weaknesses limited to the phonological/language
domain. Their primary problem in learning to read involves learning to
translate between printed and oral language. On the other hand, another
significant group of poor readers, composed largely of children from fami-
Torgesen 13
lies of lower socioeconomic or minority status, enter school significantly
delayed in a much broader range of prereading skills (Whitehurst & Loni-
gan, 1998). Because these children are delayed not only in phonological
but also general oral language skills, they are deficient in both of the criti-
cal kinds of knowledge and skill required for good reading comprehension
(Gough, 1996). Even if these children can acquire adequate word reading
skill, their ability to comprehend the meaning of what they read may be
limited by their weak general verbal abilities.
Children with general oral language weaknesses will require special in-
struction in a broader range of knowledge and skills than those who come
to school impaired only in phonological ability. What is well established at
this point, though, is that both kinds of children will require special sup-
port in the growth of early word reading skills if they are to make adequate
progress in learning to read.
INSTRUCTIONAL METHODS AND ELEMENTS THAT CAN HELP TO
PREVENT READING DIFFICULTIES IN YOUNG CHILDREN
To adequately address the fundamental problem that too many children
are leaving elementary school with reading skills inadequate for the next
higher level of instruction, we must change the way we work to teach read-
ing in three ways. First, we must insure that classroom instruction in kin-
dergarten through Grade 3 is skillfully delivered with a balanced emphasis
on word-level and reading comprehension skills. Second, we must have
procedures in place to accurately identify children who fall behind in early
reading growth, even when they are provided excellent classroom instruc-
tion. Third, we must provide these at-risk children with reading instruction
that is more intensive, more explicit, and more supportive than can be pro-
vided in a classroom of 20 to 30 children.
Critical Elements of Regular Classroom Instruction
The report from the National Research Council (Snow et al., 1998) con-
cluded that the most efficient way to prevent reading difficulties was to in-
sure that every child received high quality balanced reading instruction in
the early elementary grades. If any elementary school is producing high
numbers of children in fourth and fifth grade who cannot read well
enough to do grade-level work, the first place to suggest change is in the
regular classroom reading curriculum in kindergarten through Grade 3.
Both of our recent consensus documents (National Reading Panel, 2000;
Snow et al., 1998) identified the critical components of early reading in-
struction as including explicit teaching to build phonemic awareness and
phonemic decoding skills, fluency in word recognition and text process-
ing, reading comprehension strategies, oral language vocabulary, spelling,
14 Journal of School Psychology
and writing skills. Instruction that includes these elements delivered in a
consistent and skillful way is consistently more effective than instruction
that does not contain these components.
In the introduction to this article, I mentioned that the most recent Na-
tional Assessment of Educational Progress (National Center for Education
Statistics, 2001) indicated that 37% of fourth-grade school children nation-
ally do not have adequate reading skills for academic work at their grade
level. This does not mean, of course, that 37% of all children in the United
States have a reading disability and need special education. Rather, it sug-
gests the need for strengthening the instructional environment in early el-
ementary school by more consistent and skillful instruction in the critical
elements identified in recent summaries of research on reading. For exam-
ple, Foorman, Francis, Fletcher, Schatschneider, and Mehta (1998) dem-
onstrated that well-balanced and skilled classroom instruction can dramati-
cally reduce the incidence of reading failure in first- and second-grade
classrooms without special interventions for most children.
Explicit instruction and practice to build phonemic awareness and phone-
mic decoding skills are particularly important for children who enter first
grade low in talent or preparation for learning to read. Both Foorman et al.
(1998) and Juel and Minden-Cupp (2000) found that explicit instruction and
opportunities for extended practice with phonemically decodable texts were
particularly beneficial for children at risk for reading failure. In the former
study, the most phonemically explicit instructional condition produced the
strongest reading growth for all children, but the effects were particularly
striking for children with the weakest phonological skills entering first grade.
One of the arguments that is frequently made against increasing the ex-
plicitness of phonics instruction in early elementary school is that not all
children need the same amount or explicitness of instruction in this area.
This is, in fact, true. Many children enter school with excellent phonologi-
cal processing skills and a strong beginning understanding of the alpha-
betic principle. For these children, most of the knowledge that must be ac-
quired to become a skilled reader can be discovered by the child during
interactions with print. As these children read, they notice useful generali-
zations about print–sound relationships, and they acquire a great deal of
word-specific knowledge as well (i.e., the sight words that are required for
fluent reading; Share & Stanovich, 1995). As Moats (1999) pointed out,
“although some children will learn to read in spite of incidental teaching,
others never learn unless they are taught in an organized, systematic, effi-
cient way by a knowledgeable teacher using a well-designed instructional
approach” (p. 7). What the data from studies such as the ones considered
earlier (Foorman et al., 1998; Juel & Minden-Cupp, 2000) suggest is that
explicit phonics instruction can help all children during the early stages of
learning to read, but there will be individual differences in the amount of
such instruction that is required.
Torgesen 15
Critical Elements of Instruction for Children at Risk for
Reading Difficulties
A point that is clear from recent research is that traditional approaches to
reading instruction in the early elementary grades have substantially un-
derestimated the variability among children in their talent and preparation
for learning to read. For example, Hart and Risley (1995) documented
enormous differences among children from different socioeconomic strata
in preschool opportunities to acquire oral language vocabulary. We also
know that there are very significant differences among entering school
children in their knowledge about letters, print conventions, and phono-
logical sensitivity (Adams, 1990). Further, we know that the differences in
knowledge and skill that make children more or less prepared to profit
from reading instruction in first grade can be the result of either neurobio-
logical factors that are genetically transmitted and constitutionally based,
or they can be caused by a lack of adequate instruction and language expe-
rience in the child’s preschool or home environment (Neisser et al., 1996;
Olson, Wise, Johnson, & Ring, 1997). At present, one of the biggest chal-
lenges for schools is to provide a range of instructional opportunities in
reading that matches the huge diversity in children’s talent and prepara-
tion for learning to read. That is, if the diversity among children in talent
and preparation for learning to read varies across a 100-point scale, and
the range of instructional opportunities varies across only a 70-point scale,
it is obvious that many children will not receive the instruction they require
to become good readers.
Instruction for at-risk children must be more explicit than for other
children. This point has already been made to some extent as we consid-
ered the differential effects of explicit instruction for children entering
first grade more and less prepared for learning to read. Children who en-
ter first grade with weaknesses in knowledge about letters, letter-sound cor-
respondences, and phonological awareness require explicit and systematic
instruction to help them acquire the knowledge and strategies necessary
for decoding print. As Gaskins, Ehri, Cress, O’Hara, and Donnelly (1997)
pointed out, “first graders who are at risk for failure in learning to read do
not discover what teachers leave unsaid about the complexities of word
learning. As a result, it is important to teach them procedures for learning
words”(p. 325).
An illustration of this point is provided in a recent study of preventive in-
struction given to a group of highly at-risk children during kindergarten,
first grade, and second grade (Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al.,
1999). Of three interventions that were tested on children who were se-
lected because of phonological weaknesses, the most phonemically explicit
one produced the strongest growth in word reading ability. In fact, of the
16 Journal of School Psychology
three interventions tested, only the most explicit intervention produced a
reliable difference in the growth of word reading ability over children who
were not provided with any special interventions. Other studies (Brown &
Felton, 1990; Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; Iversen & Tunmer, 1993) com-
bine with this one to suggest that schools must be prepared to provide very
explicit and systematic instruction in beginning word reading skills to
some of their students if they expect all children to acquire word reading
skills at grade level by third grade.
Instruction for at-risk children must be more intensive than for other
children. The fact that instruction must be more explicit and compre-
hensive for these children implies that more skills and knowledge must be
directly taught, which logically requires a greater number of teaching/
learning opportunities. To say that instruction for at-risk children must be
more intense than for other children simply means that it must contain
more teaching/learning opportunities per day than typical classroom in-
struction. If at-risk children do not receive more teaching/learning oppor-
tunities per day than other children, they will acquire reading skills more
slowly, and thus will experience the disadvantages outlined in the introduc-
tion of this article. Another factor that underlines the need for more inten-
sive instruction is the fact that children who come to school with weak-
nesses in talent for learning to read learn more slowly than other children
and will thus require more repetition in order to solidly establish critical
word reading and comprehension skills. Although children whose risk sta-
tus is determined primarily by lack of instructional opportunities in the
preschool environment may learn at average rates, they have much more
to learn than children who come to school with typical levels of prepara-
tion (Hart & Risley, 1995) and thus must be given more intensive instruc-
tion if they are to keep pace in reading growth with their age peers.
There are actually many different ways to effectively increase instructional
intensity for children at risk for reading failure. For example, Greenwood
and his colleagues (Greenwood, 1996) used the ClassWide Peer Tutoring
model to increase amount of academic engaged time, and this increase has
been consistently associated with improvements in learning outcomes in
reading. In a similar vein, Doug and Lynn Fuchs reported success (Fuchs,
Fuchs, Mathes, & Simmons, 1997) in using peer-assisted learning strategies
to improve reading skills in mid-elementary school, and Mathes developed
successful procedures that allow the use of peer tutoring for basic reading
skills in first-grade classrooms (Mathes, Torgesen, & Allor, in press). For
both older and younger children, the interventions provided by the peer-
assisted procedures are both more explicit and more intensive than the in-
struction that is typically provided by the classroom teacher.
Another practical method for providing greater instructional intensity
for at-risk children is to use special education or reading resource teachers
Torgesen 17
to provide small group (3–4 children) instruction in addition to the regu-
lar classroom instruction the children receive. There can be no question
that children with reading difficulties, or children at risk for these difficul-
ties, will learn more rapidly under conditions of greater instructional in-
tensity than they learn in typical classroom settings. Meta-analyses consis-
tently show positive effects of grouping practices that increase instructional
intensity (Elbaum, Vaughn, Hughes, & Moody, 1999). One interesting
finding that has emerged from these analyses is that, so far, one-to-one in-
terventions in reading have not been shown to be more effective than
small group interventions (Elbaum et al., 1999; National Reading Panel,
2000). Although Torgesen, Alexander, et al. (2001) recently demonstrated
very powerful instructional effects for one-to-one instruction (as opposed
to the larger group instruction typically provided in special education re-
source rooms), other studies have shown similar rates of growth for read-
ing-disabled children using small groups of 3 and 4 children at a time
(Rashotte, MacPhee, & Torgesen, 2001; Wise, Ring, & Olson, 1999).
Instruction for at-risk children must be more supportive than for other
children. The needs of at-risk children for more positive emotional sup-
port in the form of encouragement, feedback, and positive reinforcement
is widely understood. However, their potential need for more cognitive
support, in the form of carefully “scaffolded” instruction, is less widely ap-
preciated. Instruction for at-risk children should involve two types of scaf-
folding. One type of scaffolding involves careful sequencing so that skills
build very gradually: The child is always systematically taught and practiced
on the skills required for any task they are asked to do (Swanson, 1999).
Another type of scaffolding involves teacher–student dialog that directly
shows the child what kind of processing or thinking needs to be done in or-
der to complete the task successfully. This type of scaffolding in instruction
usually involves four elements: (a) the student is presented with a task such
as reading or spelling a word, or making a paragraph summary (i.e., tries to
spell the word “flat”); (b) the student makes a response that is incorrect in
some way, or indicates that he/she doesn’t know how to proceed (i.e.,
spells it “fat”); (c) the teacher asks a question that focuses the child’s atten-
tion on a first step in the solution process, or that draws attention to a re-
quired piece of information (“If you read that word, what does it say?”
Child responds, “fat.” “So, what do you need to add to make it say flat?” No
answer. “When you say flat, what do you hear coming right after the begin-
ning sound /f/?”); and (d) another response from the child (“I hear the /l/
sound.”). This kind of interaction between student and child continues un-
til the child had been led to successfully accomplish the task. The point of
this type of instructional interaction is that the child is led to discover the
information or strategies that are critical to accomplishing the task, rather
than simply being told what to do. As Juel (1996) recently showed, the abil-
18 Journal of School Psychology
ity to offer scaffolded support while children are acquiring reading skills
may have increasing importance as the severity of the child’s disability in-
creases.
I have described three broad ways that instruction for children who are
at risk for reading failure needs to be different from the instruction that is
typically provided to all children in the classroom. Insuring that all three of
these elements are part of the instruction for our most at-risk children rep-
resents an enormous challenge for our schools. The requirement for more
explicit and supportive instruction demands a higher level of training and
skill for teachers than is usually provided at present (Moats, 1994). The re-
quirement for more intensive instruction for at-risk children must involve a
reallocation of resources to make more teacher time available for preven-
tive instruction and, in many cases, will probably require entirely new re-
sources to adequately meet the instructional needs of all children who are
at risk for reading failure.
What Do We Know About the Effectiveness of Early
Preventive Instruction?
An obvious question that must be addressed before schools reallocate or in-
vest new resources for preventive instruction is whether the procedures out-
lined above are actually effective in preventing reading difficulties in most
children. Two kinds of information are required to provide a complete an-
swer to this question. First, we must know to what extent appropriate preven-
tive instruction can bring word reading and comprehension skills to ade-
quate levels during the period of the intervention. Second, we must
understand the long-term effects of preventive instruction on subsequent
reading growth. Answering this latter question is complicated, because long-
term outcomes can be influenced by many factors. For example, long-term
outcomes can certainly be influenced by the extent to which the preventive
instruction helps children acquire all the reading skills required as a founda-
tion for further growth. However, these outcomes may also depend on the
support for reading improvement (both in the school and in the home) that
is provided after the preventive intervention is concluded. When the solid
foundations for reading growth are established in the early primary grades,
further reading growth is dependent almost entirely on the breadth and
depth of the child’s reading experience and practice (Snow et al., 1998).
In terms of immediate outcomes from preventive instruction, we know
that it is possible to bring the word-level reading skills of children at risk
for reading failure within the average range by the end of first or second
grade when the intervention group is considered as a whole (Torgesen, in
press). Children who achieve these average-level word reading skills can
also comprehend text within the average range (Foorman et al., 1998) or
at a level consistent with their general verbal ability (Torgesen, Wagner,
Torgesen 19
Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). However, there are large individual differ-
ences in response to the early interventions examined in research, and not
all children show satisfactory outcomes.
Torgesen (2000) recently examined the outcomes from five prevention
studies that all used similar measures to assess reading outcomes. These stud-
ies all contained at least one instructional condition that offered skilled de-
livery of explicit and systematic instruction in phonemic awareness, phone-
mic decoding, and fluent text reading. The children who received the
preventive instruction were selected to be at risk for reading failure on the
basis of either weak phonological processing skills or weak development of
early word reading ability, and the preventive instruction was provided at
some point during kindergarten, first grade, or second grade. The number
of hours of special instruction varied between 340 hr of first- and second-
grade instruction delivered to groups of 8 (Brown & Felton, 1990) and
35–65 hr of one-on-one instruction delivered in the second semester of first
grade and the first semester of second grade (Vellutino et al., 1996).
Outcomes from these studies were analyzed to estimate the proportion
of the population that would remain below the 30th percentile (by current
norms) in word reading ability if the interventions were available to all chil-
dren who needed them. The proportions varied between 6% of the popu-
lation in the study that provided 35–65 hr of one-on-one instruction to 2%
of the population in a study that provided 92 hr of computer-assisted tutor-
ing to groups of 3 during first grade (Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, & Her-
ron, 2001). These varying proportions of “treatment resisters” cannot be
used to directly compare effectiveness of the different methods in these
studies because the samples were selected by different methods, the popu-
lations from which they were selected also differed, and the background
classroom instruction the children received also differed. The important
point is that none of the interventions were sufficient to produce adequate
reading growth in all children. Across all the interventions, the average es-
timate of “population failure rate” was 5%. It is interesting that this 5% fig-
ure roughly corresponds to the percentage of children who are currently
identified as learning disabled, most of whom are identified because of
problems learning to read. As a counterpoint to this estimate of popula-
tion failure rate in early intervention studies, Scanlon, Vellutino, Small,
and Fanuele (2000) recently reported a study in which the failure rate was
essentially zero in their most effective condition. This condition involved a
combination of small group intervention in kindergarten and one-on-one
instruction in first grade, and it suggests that it may be possible to improve
on past results with multilayered interventions in the early grades.
Of course, there are many reasons why outcomes from highly controlled
research studies may be either unrealistically high or unrealistically low.
One reason that they may sometimes be low is that the preventive instruc-
tion may be offered in the context of classroom instruction that does not
20 Journal of School Psychology
reinforce or encourage the skills being taught to the at-risk learners (i.e.,
Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). As was mentioned earlier,
to maximize reading growth, children at risk for reading difficulties must
receive both strong classroom instruction in reading and more intensive,
explicit, and supportive preventive instruction. A reason that research
studies may produce unrealistically strong outcomes is that teachers may
be more highly trained and supervised in these settings than usually occurs
in schools. For these reasons, it is instructive to examine reading outcomes
in schools where sound preventive procedures are implemented.
One such school is Hartsfield Elementary in Tallahassee, Florida (King &
Torgesen, 2001). Over a 5-year period, this school worked to implement a
balanced reading curriculum in kindergarten through Grade 3 and to estab-
lish significant amounts of preventive reading instruction for children who
were performing below grade level in the first and second grade. The school
serves a population of children who are about 65% minority (mostly African
American), and 60% of the children are eligible for free or reduced lunch
support. In the first year of the project, the new classroom reading instruc-
tion was only partially implemented, but by the end of the 5-year period, it
was fully implemented in all primary-grade classrooms. The preventive in-
struction was phased in gradually beginning in the second year of the project
as new resources for providing the instruction were identified. The results
for word-level reading skills are presented graphically in Figure 1. The test
used was a nationally standardized measure of word reading ability, and it
was given by individuals other than the children’s teachers at the end of each
year to all children. The figure shows the percentage of children who ended
first and second grade performing below the 25th percentile, and it also de-
scribes the change in average percentile for all children. As can be seen from
the figure, during the 5-year implementation period, the percentage of chil-
dren performing below the 25th percentile at the end of first grade dropped
from 31.8% to 3.7%. At the end of the implementation period, the percent-
age of children (using only those children at Hartsfield with at least 2 years
of instruction) still weak in word-level reading skills at the end of second
grade was only 2.4%. In terms of the long-term impact of early intervention
at this school, during the same period of time, Hartsfield achieved the larg-
est growth of any of the 20 elementary schools in its district on the state-
administered standardized reading test administered at the end of third
grade. Median percentile in reading achievement for third-grade children
on the California Achievement Test improved from 49 at the end of 1994 to
73 at the end of 1999.
IDENTIFYING CHILDREN IN NEED OF PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS
It is beyond the scope of this article to provide a detailed discussion of as-
sessment procedures to identify children in need of special preventive in-
Torgesen 21
struction in reading. That topic is covered in another article in this issue
(Fletcher et al.). However, for the sake of completeness, and because sys-
tematic assessment for early identification must be an integral part of any
school-wide program to prevent reading disabilities, several critical points
will be made here.
1. It does not take a lengthy assessment to approach the limits of accuracy in
identifying children who require instruction that is more intensive, ex-
plicit, and supportive than that provided by the classroom teacher. In kin-
dergarten, a reliable assessment of phonemic awareness, letter-sound
knowledge, and vocabulary will identify most children at risk for reading
failure. Children low in the first two measures will need special support
Figure 1. Changes in year-end reading performance of children during period of rapid curric-
ulum changes in reading.
22 Journal of School Psychology
for the development of word reading ability, and children low in all three
areas will require additional support in oral language development.
2. Unless screening criteria are set to identify high proportions of children
as at risk, most early identification procedures at present produce a
fairly high proportion of “false negatives” (i.e., children who pass the
screening but later show difficulties learning to read; Scarborough,
1998). The simple reason for this is that reading growth is predicted by
a variety of factors other than cognitive abilities and knowledge (Torge-
sen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). However, the number of false
negatives during the period from the start of school to third grade can
be reduced to virtually zero if screening for reading difficulties is con-
ducted regularly in first, second, and third grade. I would recommend
assessment to monitor reading growth occurs at least three times a year
during first, second, and third grade.
3. At the beginning of first grade, at-risk children can best be identified us-
ing procedures similar to those for children in kindergarten. However,
after reading instruction begins in first grade, the best way to identify
children who are falling behind in the ability to read words accurately
and fluently is to measure that skill directly. We (Torgesen, Wagner, &
Rashotte, 1999) recently published a test called the Test of Word Reading
Efficiency that requires only 45 s to obtain a reliable measure of phone-
mic decoding efficiency, and another 45 s to measure the growth of
sight word vocabulary. In first grade, I would recommend assessment of
comprehension-related skills using a measure of oral language vocabu-
lary rather than reading comprehension because comprehension is so
heavily dependent on word reading ability in the early primary grades
(Stanovich, Cunningham, & Feeman, 1984).
4. In second and third grade, the development of word-level reading abil-
ity should continue to be monitored using direct assessments to identify
children who are falling behind their peers in these critical skills. At this
point, group- or individually administered measures of reading compre-
hension may prove useful in identifying children who can continue to
profit from more intensive work to build vocabulary and reading com-
prehension strategies.
Concluding Comments
Although we do not yet understand the conditions that must be in place to
prevent reading difficulties in all children, we do know what must be done
to very substantially reduce the number of children who fail to acquire ad-
equate reading skills during the primary grades of elementary school. The
key to taking advantage of this knowledge is finding the will to change the
way we work at teaching reading. We must work more effectively to insure
that classroom teachers acquire the skills and knowledge to teach reading
Torgesen 23
to children who do not learn easily. We must work to develop and institute
procedures to identify children in need of extra instruction in a timely and
accurate manner. Finally, we must work to find sufficient instructional re-
sources to provide more intensive, explicit, and supportive instruction to
the children who need it. We have numerous examples showing that work-
ing effectively in all these areas will dramatically increase our success in
teaching all children to read well during elementary school.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This study was supported, in part, by grant HD30988, “Prevention and Re-
mediation of Reading Disabilities” and grant HD23340, “Reading-Related
Phonological Processes,” from the National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development.
REFERENCES
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Adams, M. J. (1991). A talk with Marilyn Adams. Language Arts, 68, 206–212.
Briggs, A., Austin, R., & Underwood, G. (1984). Phonological coding in good and poor
readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 54–66.
Brown, A. L., Palincsar, A. S., & Purcell, L. (l986). Poor readers: Teach, don’t label. In
U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority children: New perspectives (pp. 105–
143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brown, I. S., & Felton, R. H. (l990). Effects of instruction on beginning reading skills in
children at risk for reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,
2, 223–241.
Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dys-
lexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What reading does for the mind. Ameri-
can Educator, 22(Spring/Summer), 8–15.
Ehri, L. C. (1998). Grapheme–phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read
words in English. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning reading
(pp. 3–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Elbaum, B., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M. T., & Moody, S. W. (1999). Grouping practices and
reading outcomes for students with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 65, 399–415.
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., Steubing, K. K.,
Francis, D. J., Fowler, A. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading
disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 86, 6–23.
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998).
The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk chil-
dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 37–55.
Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996).
Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individ-
ual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 3–17.
Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. G., & Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-assisted learning
strategies: Making classrooms more responsive to academic diversity. American Educa-
tional Research Journal, 34, 174–206.
24 Journal of School Psychology
Gaskins, I. W., Ehri, L. C., Cress, C., O’Hara, C., & Donnelly, K. (1997). Procedures for
word learning: Making discoveries about words. The Reading Teacher, 50, 312–327.
Gough, P. B. (1996). How children learn to read and why they fail. Annals of Dyslexia, 46,
3–20.
Greenwood, C. R. (1996). Research on the practices and behavior of effective teachers
at the Juniper Gardens Children’s Project: Implications for the education of diverse
learners. In D. L. Speece & B. K. Keogh (Eds.), Research on classroom ecologies (pp. 39–68).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Hatcher, P., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by inte-
grating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage
hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57.
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing,
2, 127–160.
Iversen, S., & Tunmer, W. E. (1993). Phonological processing skills and the reading re-
covery program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 112–126.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from
first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447.
Juel, C. (1996). What makes literacy tutoring effective? Reading Research Quarterly, 31,
268–289.
Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and in-
structional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458–492.
King, R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Improving the effectiveness of reading instruction in one el-
ementary school: A description of the process. Unpublished manuscript, Florida State Uni-
versity, Tallahassee.
Lee, V., Brooks-Gunn, J., Schnur, E., & Liaw, F. (1990). Are Head Start effects sustained?
A longitudinal follow-up comparison of disadvantaged children attending Head
Start, no preschool, and other pre-school programs. Child Development, 61, 495–507.
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle
and learning to read. In Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.), Phonology and read-
ing disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 1–33). Ann Arbor, MI: U. of Michigan Press.
Manis, F. R., Custodio, R., & Szeszulski, P. A. (1993). Development of phonological and
orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experi-
mental Child Psychology, 56, 64–86.
Mathes, P. G., Torgesen, J. K., & Allor, J. H. (in press). The effects of Peer-Assisted Liter-
acy Strategies for first-grade readers with and without additional computer assisted
instruction in phonological awareness. American Educational Research Journal.
Moats, L. C. (1994). The missing foundation in teacher education: Knowledge of the
structure of spoken and written language. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 81–102.
Moats, L. C. (1999). Teaching reading is rocket science. Washington, D.C.: American Feder-
ation of Teachers.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2001). NAEP 2000 Reading. A report card for the
nation and the states. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of
the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Wash-
ington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Eci, S. J., Halpern, D. F.,
Loehlen, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns
and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.
Oka, E., & Paris, S. (1986). Patterns of motivation and reading skills in underachieving
children. In S. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of
learning disabilities (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Torgesen 25
Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Johnson, M., & Ring, J. (1997). The etiology and remediation of
phonologically based word recognition and spelling disabilities: Are phonological
deficits the “hole” story? In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of Reading Acquisition.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rashotte, C. A., MacPhee, K., & Torgesen, J. K. (in press). The effectiveness of a group
reading instruction program with poor readers in multiple grades. Learning Disabili-
ties Quarterly, 24, 119–134.
Scanlon, D. M., Vellutino, F. R., Small, S. G., & Fanuele, D. P. (2000). Severe reading
difficulties—Can they be prevented? A comparison of prevention and intervention
approaches. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational
Research Association, New Orleans, LA, April.
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabili-
ties: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro,
P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp.
75–120). Timonium, MD: York Press.
Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading develop-
ment: A model of acquisition and individual differences. Issues in Education: Contribu-
tions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.
Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 22, 469–479.
Simpson, G. B., Lorsbach, T., & Whitehouse, D. (1983). Encoding and contextual com-
ponents of word recognition in good and poor readers. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 35, 161–171.
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in
young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (l984). Intelligence, cognitive
skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 278–303.
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading-
disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-differ-
ence model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.
Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading research for students with LD: A meta-analysis of inter-
vention outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 504–532.
Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in read-
ing: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and
Practice, 15, 55–64.
Torgesen, J. K. (in press). Lessons learned from intervention research in reading: A way to
go before we rest. In R. Stainthorpe (Ed.), Literacy: Learning and teaching. Monograph of
the British Journal of Educational Psychology, London: British Psychological Association.
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K., Conway,
T., & Rose, E. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading
disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 33–58.
Torgesen, J. K., & Burgess, S. R. (1998). Consistency of reading-related phonological
processes throughout early childhood: Evidence from longitudinal-correlational and
instructional studies. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning read-
ing (pp. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Torgesen, J. K., & Mathes, P. (2000). A basic guide to understanding, assessing, and teaching
phonological awareness. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruc-
tion in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf
(Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–355). Parkton, MD: York Press.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency.
Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
26 Journal of School Psychology
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Herron, J. (2001). A comparison of two
computer assisted approaches to the prevention of reading disabilities in young children. Manu-
script in preparation.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Lindamood, P.,
Conway, T., & Garvin, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with
phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction.
Journal of Educational Psycholog, 91, 579–593.
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E. R., Small, S. G., Pratt, A., Chen, R., & Denckla,
M. B. (1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated
poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive
and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 88, 601–638.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-
related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality
from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R.,
Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing causal relations between phonological
processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to
fluent readers: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy.
Child Development, 69, 335–357.
Wise, B. W., Ring, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and
without explicit attention to articulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72,
271–304.

More Related Content

What's hot

Phonemic awareness phonics and fluency
Phonemic awareness phonics and fluencyPhonemic awareness phonics and fluency
Phonemic awareness phonics and fluencyJenniferPankowskiBlu
 
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!!
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!! Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!!
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!! Arifa Abid
 
Role of teaching in assessment.
Role of teaching in assessment.Role of teaching in assessment.
Role of teaching in assessment.Catherine Matias
 
Use of Computers in Education
Use of Computers in EducationUse of Computers in Education
Use of Computers in EducationAbbie Laudato
 
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...Chinly Ruth Alberto
 
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay Dunn
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay DunnPhonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay Dunn
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay DunnLindsay Dunn
 
Reading writing connections
Reading writing connectionsReading writing connections
Reading writing connectionsYusuf Ozdemir
 
Speaking Activities
Speaking ActivitiesSpeaking Activities
Speaking ActivitiesMagda Castro
 
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activities
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activitiesUnit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activities
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activitiesambreenzahid3
 
Teaching Vocabulary[1]
Teaching Vocabulary[1]Teaching Vocabulary[1]
Teaching Vocabulary[1]Mar Mendoza
 
Phonological awareness presentation
Phonological awareness presentationPhonological awareness presentation
Phonological awareness presentationAnn Noonen
 
Kimberly wauters effective reading strategies power point
Kimberly wauters   effective reading strategies power pointKimberly wauters   effective reading strategies power point
Kimberly wauters effective reading strategies power pointkwauters09
 
The relationship of reading & writing development
The relationship of reading & writing development The relationship of reading & writing development
The relationship of reading & writing development Qhairunn Nadia
 

What's hot (20)

Learner profile
Learner profileLearner profile
Learner profile
 
Oral language
Oral language Oral language
Oral language
 
Phonemic awareness phonics and fluency
Phonemic awareness phonics and fluencyPhonemic awareness phonics and fluency
Phonemic awareness phonics and fluency
 
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!!
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!! Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!!
Vocabulary skills In Linguistics!!
 
Feedback assessment
Feedback assessmentFeedback assessment
Feedback assessment
 
Role of teaching in assessment.
Role of teaching in assessment.Role of teaching in assessment.
Role of teaching in assessment.
 
Oral Language
Oral LanguageOral Language
Oral Language
 
Use of Computers in Education
Use of Computers in EducationUse of Computers in Education
Use of Computers in Education
 
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...
Theories of learning and early literacy (definition, characteristics, transit...
 
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay Dunn
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay DunnPhonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay Dunn
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics by Lindsay Dunn
 
Reading writing connections
Reading writing connectionsReading writing connections
Reading writing connections
 
Speaking Activities
Speaking ActivitiesSpeaking Activities
Speaking Activities
 
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activities
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activitiesUnit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activities
Unit 8 Day to day concerns topic school day activities
 
Word recognition
Word recognitionWord recognition
Word recognition
 
Teaching Vocabulary[1]
Teaching Vocabulary[1]Teaching Vocabulary[1]
Teaching Vocabulary[1]
 
Phonological awareness presentation
Phonological awareness presentationPhonological awareness presentation
Phonological awareness presentation
 
Kimberly wauters effective reading strategies power point
Kimberly wauters   effective reading strategies power pointKimberly wauters   effective reading strategies power point
Kimberly wauters effective reading strategies power point
 
Integration of ICT in Teaching and Learning
Integration  of  ICT in Teaching and LearningIntegration  of  ICT in Teaching and Learning
Integration of ICT in Teaching and Learning
 
E-Portfolio
E-Portfolio E-Portfolio
E-Portfolio
 
The relationship of reading & writing development
The relationship of reading & writing development The relationship of reading & writing development
The relationship of reading & writing development
 

Similar to The Prevention of Reading Difficulties

Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...
Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...
Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...AnthonyOkoye5
 
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...EvaniaYafie
 
Read aloudwhitepaper
Read aloudwhitepaperRead aloudwhitepaper
Read aloudwhitepapernyia hill
 
Lifespan psychology lecture 4.2
Lifespan psychology lecture   4.2Lifespan psychology lecture   4.2
Lifespan psychology lecture 4.2kclancy
 
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...Read Australia (Wiring Brains Education)
 
Age and motivation in l2 learning
Age and motivation in l2 learningAge and motivation in l2 learning
Age and motivation in l2 learningInvisible_Vision
 
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docx
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docxDeep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docx
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docxvickeryr87
 
Week Six Discussion Presentation
Week Six Discussion PresentationWeek Six Discussion Presentation
Week Six Discussion PresentationTiffany Harris
 
Berger Ls 7e Ch 12
Berger Ls 7e  Ch 12Berger Ls 7e  Ch 12
Berger Ls 7e Ch 12mara bentley
 
Effective vocabulary-instruction
Effective vocabulary-instructionEffective vocabulary-instruction
Effective vocabulary-instructionADONISCheryl
 
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research Conference
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research ConferenceLindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research Conference
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research ConferenceLindamood-Bell Learning Processes
 
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...Alexander Decker
 
Teaching reading as an EFA priority
Teaching reading as an EFA priorityTeaching reading as an EFA priority
Teaching reading as an EFA priorityIAU-HEEFA
 

Similar to The Prevention of Reading Difficulties (20)

Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...
Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...
Approaches in teaching reading to children with intellectual disability by An...
 
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
 
Read aloudwhitepaper
Read aloudwhitepaperRead aloudwhitepaper
Read aloudwhitepaper
 
Review lecture 12 chapter 12
Review lecture 12   chapter 12Review lecture 12   chapter 12
Review lecture 12 chapter 12
 
Lifespan psychology lecture 4.2
Lifespan psychology lecture   4.2Lifespan psychology lecture   4.2
Lifespan psychology lecture 4.2
 
Psy johnston
Psy johnstonPsy johnston
Psy johnston
 
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...
Reading Whisperer Advice: Three Cueing System, Guided Reading, Levelled Reade...
 
Age and motivation in l2 learning
Age and motivation in l2 learningAge and motivation in l2 learning
Age and motivation in l2 learning
 
Ba einar g
Ba einar gBa einar g
Ba einar g
 
Chapt 12
Chapt 12Chapt 12
Chapt 12
 
Chapt 12
Chapt 12Chapt 12
Chapt 12
 
Chapt 12
Chapt 12Chapt 12
Chapt 12
 
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docx
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docxDeep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docx
Deep Reading, CostBenefit, and the Construction of Meaning E.docx
 
Week Six Discussion Presentation
Week Six Discussion PresentationWeek Six Discussion Presentation
Week Six Discussion Presentation
 
Berger Ls 7e Ch 12
Berger Ls 7e  Ch 12Berger Ls 7e  Ch 12
Berger Ls 7e Ch 12
 
Effective vocabulary-instruction
Effective vocabulary-instructionEffective vocabulary-instruction
Effective vocabulary-instruction
 
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research Conference
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research ConferenceLindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research Conference
Lindamood-Bell's 17th Annual International Research Conference
 
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...
Teaching reading to children ages 7 8 years to early light tunagrahita and th...
 
Teaching reading as an EFA priority
Teaching reading as an EFA priorityTeaching reading as an EFA priority
Teaching reading as an EFA priority
 
Prof ed 9a
Prof ed 9aProf ed 9a
Prof ed 9a
 

More from rathx039

Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431rathx039
 
Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431rathx039
 
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3rathx039
 
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need rathx039
 
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessments
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading AssessmentsOrganizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessments
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessmentsrathx039
 
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...rathx039
 
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...rathx039
 
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIDiagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIrathx039
 

More from rathx039 (8)

Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431
 
Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431Assessmentand identification 5431
Assessmentand identification 5431
 
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3
Paris, hoffman reading assessments k 3
 
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need
Behind the Test Scores: What Struggling Readers Really Need
 
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessments
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading AssessmentsOrganizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessments
Organizing and Evaluating Results from Multiple Reading Assessments
 
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...
Reading Assessments in Kindergarten through Third Grade: Findings from the Ce...
 
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...
Guided Reading: A Research-Based Response to the Challenges of Early Reading ...
 
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIDiagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
 

Recently uploaded

Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...Postal Advocate Inc.
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONHumphrey A Beña
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parentsnavabharathschool99
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17Celine George
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxAshokKarra1
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceSamikshaHamane
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 

The Prevention of Reading Difficulties

  • 1. Journal of School Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 1, pp. 7–26, 2002 Copyright © 2002 Society for the Study of School Psychology Printed in the USA 0022-4405/02 $–see front matter PII S0022-4405(01)00092-9 7 The Prevention of Reading Difficulties Joseph K. Torgesen Florida State University The purpose of this article is to provide practical advice about methods to prevent reading failure that is grounded in the new knowledge we have acquired about reading and learning to read over the past 2 decades. Recent research on reading is used to establish a set of facts about reading and reading growth that is relevant to establishing instructional objectives and methods for the prevention of reading difficulties. Within the context of our current understanding of the reasons many children find it difficult to learn to read, the article also identifies the instructional conditions that need to be in place to prevent the development of reading difficul- ties in all but a very small proportion of children. The article concludes with a dis- cussion of issues and procedures for the early identification of children who are likely to experience difficulties learning to read. © 2002 Society for the Study of School Psychology. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Keywords: Reading disabilities, Prevention, Early identification, Assessment, Reading instruction. The purpose of this article is to provide practical advice about methods to prevent reading failure that is grounded in the new knowledge we have ac- quired about reading and learning to read over the past two decades. Most of this new knowledge about reading has been reported in two recent con- sensus documents. One report (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), titled Pre- venting Reading Difficulties in Young Children, was prepared by the National Research Council and published by the National Academy of Sciences. The other document, titled Teaching Children to Read (National Reading Panel, 2000) was commissioned by the U.S. Congress and supported in de- velopment by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop- ment and the U.S. Office of Education. Both documents were written by committees of professionals who were asked to identify the findings about reading and reading instruction that were most consistently supported in recent research. These are interesting and challenging times for anyone whose profes- sional responsibilities are related in any way to literacy outcomes among school children. For, in spite of all our new knowledge about reading and Received June 19, 2001; accepted October 30, 2001. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Joseph K. Torgesen, Department of Psy- chology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306-1270. Phone: (850) 644-1707; fax: (850) 644-7739; E-mail: torgesen@psy.fsu.edu
  • 2. 8 Journal of School Psychology reading instruction, there is a wide-spread concern that public education is not as effective as it should be in teaching all children to read. Fueled by such facts as the statistic that 37% of fourth-grade school children cannot read well enough to effectively accomplish grade-level work (National Cen- ter for Education Statistics, 2001), there is an emerging sense of urgency about improving reading instruction and literacy outcomes in our country. The report of the National Research Council pointed out that these con- cerns about literacy derive not from declining levels of literacy in our schools, but rather from recognition that the demands for high levels of lit- eracy are rapidly accelerating in our society. Clearly, children who become adults with low levels of literacy are at an increasing disadvantage in a soci- ety that is creating ever-higher demands for effective reading skills within the workplace. These rising demands can only be met by changing the way we work at teaching reading so that we produce better literacy outcomes for more children than ever before. A major aspect of change in the way we work to teach reading to all chil- dren must involve allocation of resources for early identification and pre- ventive instruction. The costs of waiting until mid-elementary school to identify children in need of special instruction in reading are simply too great. We know, for example, that delayed development of reading skills affects vocabulary growth (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998), alters chil- dren’s attitudes and motivation to read (Oka & Paris, 1986), and leads to missed opportunities to develop comprehension strategies (Brown, Palinc- sar, & Purcell, 1986). If children fall seriously behind in the growth of criti- cal early reading skills, they have fewer opportunities to practice reading. Recent evidence (Torgesen, Rashotte, & Alexander, 2001) suggests that these lost practice opportunities make it extremely difficult for children who re- main poor readers during the first three years of elementary school to ever acquire average levels of reading fluency. Finally, there is the very sobering fact obtained in several longitudinal studies that children who are poor readers at the end of first grade almost never acquire average-level reading skills by the end of elementary school (Francis, Shaywitz, Stuebing, Shay- witz, & Fletcher, 1996; Juel, 1988; Torgesen & Burgess, 1998). This article is organized in three sections. The first section discusses a set of facts about reading and reading growth that is relevant to establishing instructional objectives and methods for the prevention of reading difficul- ties. These are also facts about reading that every school psychologist should know, and they represent a fundamental departure from views about read- ing that underlie many “wholistic” approaches to reading instruction cur- rently in use. The next section discusses the instructional conditions that need to be in place to prevent the development of reading difficulties in all but a very small proportion of children. The article concludes with a dis- cussion of issues and procedures for the early identification of children who are likely to experience difficulties learning to read. Information in
  • 3. Torgesen 9 the second section should be helpful to school psychologists in their roles as consultants about effective educational practices, and the final section contains information of potentially direct use by school psychologists to enhance the effectiveness of their assessment work in schools. FACTS ABOUT READING THAT ARE RELEVANT TO THE PREVENTION OF READING DIFFICULTIES It is technically incorrect to call the ideas presented in this section “facts” about reading. Rather, they are conclusions about reading and reading growth that are now assumed to be true based on consistent research find- ings. All of the ideas presented here are consistent with the conclusions re- ported in the two recent consensus documents described in the introduc- tion to this article. The first idea is not so much a “fact,” but is a self-evident value judgment about reading for which there is wide-spread agreement. The Ultimate Goal of Reading Instruction Is to Help Children Acquire all the Skills Necessary to Comprehend the Meaning of Text In other words, the goal of literacy instruction is to help children acquire the skills that enable learning from, understanding, and enjoyment of writ- ten language. This is not a “controversial” assumption. No matter what one’s instructional orientation may be, the long-term goal of reading in- struction is to provide children with the skills necessary to construct, or comprehend, the meaning of text in its many forms and genres. Two General Types of Skill and Knowledge Are Required for Good Reading Comprehension To be able to construct meaning from text, a child must have (a) general language comprehension skills and (b) the ability to accurately and fluently identify the individual words in print. Knowledge of and active application of specific reading strategies are also helpful to maximize reading comprehen- sion (Snow et al., 1998), but most of the variability among children and adults in comprehension of written material can be accounted for by mea- suring the two broad families of skills identified in Gough’s simple view of reading (Gough, 1996; Hoover & Gough, 1990). That is, good general lan- guage comprehension and good word reading skills are the most critical skills required for effective comprehension of written material. The fact that reading comprehension is the joint product of language comprehension ability and word identification skills must be considered when we set goals for the reading attainment of all children. In other words, we must recognize the fact that general cognitive ability (specifically verbal ability and knowledge) strongly influences reading comprehension
  • 4. 10 Journal of School Psychology at the higher grade levels in elementary school (Adams, 1990). For this rea- son, I usually qualify the goal for reading instruction in the following way: The ultimate goal of reading instruction is to help children acquire the knowledge and skills necessary to comprehend printed material at a level that is consistent with their general verbal ability or language comprehension skills. If we were to adopt a strict grade-level reading comprehension criteria (i.e., every child will be able to fully comprehend material written at grade level in fourth grade), this would imply an expectation for all children to have at least average verbal ability. Decades of cognitive intervention re- search suggest that it is unrealistic to expect all children to attain verbal ability estimates within the average range as a result of special instruction (Lee, Brooks-Gunn, Schnur, & Liaw, 1990). Thus, it seems unrealistic to expect reading teachers to accomplish this goal starting as late as kinder- garten or first grade. This statement does not ignore the fact that the ver- bal ability of many children can be dramatically increased by effective read- ing instruction (Torgesen, Alexander, et al., 2001); it is just meant to acknowledge the fact that this may not be possible for all children. A Critical Problem for Most Children Who Experience Reading Difficulties Involves Early and Continuing Problems Acquiring Accurate and Fluent Word Identification Skills When asked to read grade-level text, the typical poor reader in third or fourth grade will show two kinds of word-level reading difficulties. First, when they encounter a word they are not familiar with, they tend to place too much reliance on guessing the word based on the context or meaning of the passage (Briggs, Austin, & Underwood, 1984; Simpson, Lorsbach, & Whitehouse, 1983), which produces a high rate of word-level errors in their reading. Their phonemic analysis skills, or ability to use “phonics” to assist in the word identification process, is usually severely impaired (Bruck, 1990; Siegel, 1989). Second, most children who are having diffi- culty learning to read encounter many more words in grade-level text that they cannot read “by sight” than do average readers. Compared with chil- dren of the same age who are learning to read normally, the number of words that children with reading problems can recognize fluently and eas- ily as “sight words” is usually quite limited (Manis, Custodio, & Szeszulski, 1993; Torgesen, Alexander, et al., 2001). Difficulties learning to read words accurately are manifest from the very earliest stages of reading instruction. First, children who are destined to be poor readers in fourth grade almost invariably have difficulties under- standing and applying the alphabetic principle in deciphering unfamiliar words. Their difficulties developing good analytic strategies for identifying unknown words makes it difficult for them to read independently, and it also produces far too many word reading errors. Both of these latter conse-
  • 5. Torgesen 11 quences of failure to acquire good phonemic decoding skills affect the de- velopment of fluent word reading ability, which depends heavily on learn- ing to identify large numbers of words by sight (Torgesen, Rashotte, & Alexander, 2001). Because words do not become sight words until they are read accurately a number of times, both inaccurate reading and dimin- ished reading practice cause slow growth of fluent word-identification skills. Furthermore, the strongest current theories of reading growth link phonemic and sight word reading skills together by showing how good phonemic decoding skills are necessary in the formation of accurate mem- ory for the spelling patterns that are the basis of sight word recognition (Ehri, 1998). The difficulties acquiring accurate and fluent word reading skills shown by most poor readers are extremely important in light of the new informa- tion we have about the way word-level processes operate during skilled read- ing. Skilled readers do not “skim and scan” text as they read for meaning, but rather they directly fixate and process a very high proportion of all the words in text. Furthermore, they accurately identify most of the words in text by processing information about all, or almost all, the letters in words (Adams, 1990). In other words, skilled word recognition is heavily depen- dent upon very detailed knowledge of the letters used to spell individual words. Skilled readers do not guess at the identity of specific words in text by relying on context; rather, they are able to accurately and fluently iden- tify words on the basis of their written spellings. Adams (1991) summarized these facts about word-recognition processes in skilled readers this way: It has been proven beyond any shade of doubt that skillful readers process vir- tually each and every word and letter of text as they read. This is extremely counter-intuitive. For sure, skillful readers neither look nor feel as if that’s what they do. But that’s because they do it so quickly and effortlessly. Almost automatically; with almost no conscious attention whatsoever, skillful readers recognize words by drawing on deep and ready knowledge of spellings and their connections to speech and meaning. (p. 207) One of the keys to becoming a skilled reader is to acquire a large vocab- ulary of words that can be recognized fluently and accurately in text. We also understand now that early development of accurate phonemic decod- ing skills plays in important supportive role in helping children acquire the very specific memories for words that are required for automatic recogni- tion (Ehri, 1998). Both of these facts underline the critical role of careful and explicit instruction in word-level reading processes as part of early reading instruction for children at risk for reading difficulties. Programs that are not sufficient to “normalize” the development of at-risk children in phonemic decoding skill and fluent word recognition have not accom- plished one of the most critical goals of preventive reading instruction.
  • 6. 12 Journal of School Psychology The Most Common Cause of Children’s Early Difficulties in Acquiring Accurate and Fluent Word Recognition Skills Involves Individual Differences in Their Phonological Knowledge and Skill This is one of the most important discoveries about reading difficulties in the last 20 years (Liberman, Shankweiler, & Liberman, 1989; Torgesen & Mathes, 2000). Children who enter first grade low in knowledge about the phonological features of words or who have difficulties processing the pho- nological features of words are at high risk for difficulties responding to early reading instruction. The tasks most commonly used to measure chil- dren’s knowledge and processing skill for the phonological features of words are referred to as measures of phonological, or phonemic, aware- ness. These tasks require children to identify or manipulate the phonemes in words that are presented orally. Phonemic awareness tests do not in- volve letters. For example, a simple task in this domain would ask children to say which of three words (bat, car, fork) begins with the same sound as bike. A more difficult task might ask the child to pronounce the first sound in the word bike, and a still more difficult task might ask the child to say what word was left when the word card was pronounced without saying the /d/ sound. Both conscious awareness of the phonemes in words and ability to accurately identify them within words is necessary in learning to phone- mically decode words in print (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994; Wag- ner et al., 1997). Children who are delayed in the development of phone- mic awareness have a very difficult time making sense out of “phonics” instruction, and they certainly have little chance to notice the phonemic patterns in written words on their own. A simple way to say this is that for individual children, phonemic awareness is what makes phonics instruc- tion meaningful. If a child has little awareness that even simple words like cat and car are composed of small “chunks” that are combined in different ways to make words, our alphabetic way of writing makes no sense. Discovery of the core phonological problems associated with early read- ing difficulties has had at least one unanticipated consequence. The ability to assess these core language problems directly has led to the discovery that the early word reading difficulties of children with relatively low general in- telligence and verbal ability are associated with the same factors that inter- fere with early reading growth in children who have general intelligence in the average range (Fletcher et al., 1994; Share & Stanovich, 1995; Stanov- ich & Siegel, 1994). Weaknesses in phonemic awareness characterize chil- dren with reading problems across a broad span of general verbal ability. On the one hand, many children enter school with adequate general ver- bal ability and cognitive weaknesses limited to the phonological/language domain. Their primary problem in learning to read involves learning to translate between printed and oral language. On the other hand, another significant group of poor readers, composed largely of children from fami-
  • 7. Torgesen 13 lies of lower socioeconomic or minority status, enter school significantly delayed in a much broader range of prereading skills (Whitehurst & Loni- gan, 1998). Because these children are delayed not only in phonological but also general oral language skills, they are deficient in both of the criti- cal kinds of knowledge and skill required for good reading comprehension (Gough, 1996). Even if these children can acquire adequate word reading skill, their ability to comprehend the meaning of what they read may be limited by their weak general verbal abilities. Children with general oral language weaknesses will require special in- struction in a broader range of knowledge and skills than those who come to school impaired only in phonological ability. What is well established at this point, though, is that both kinds of children will require special sup- port in the growth of early word reading skills if they are to make adequate progress in learning to read. INSTRUCTIONAL METHODS AND ELEMENTS THAT CAN HELP TO PREVENT READING DIFFICULTIES IN YOUNG CHILDREN To adequately address the fundamental problem that too many children are leaving elementary school with reading skills inadequate for the next higher level of instruction, we must change the way we work to teach read- ing in three ways. First, we must insure that classroom instruction in kin- dergarten through Grade 3 is skillfully delivered with a balanced emphasis on word-level and reading comprehension skills. Second, we must have procedures in place to accurately identify children who fall behind in early reading growth, even when they are provided excellent classroom instruc- tion. Third, we must provide these at-risk children with reading instruction that is more intensive, more explicit, and more supportive than can be pro- vided in a classroom of 20 to 30 children. Critical Elements of Regular Classroom Instruction The report from the National Research Council (Snow et al., 1998) con- cluded that the most efficient way to prevent reading difficulties was to in- sure that every child received high quality balanced reading instruction in the early elementary grades. If any elementary school is producing high numbers of children in fourth and fifth grade who cannot read well enough to do grade-level work, the first place to suggest change is in the regular classroom reading curriculum in kindergarten through Grade 3. Both of our recent consensus documents (National Reading Panel, 2000; Snow et al., 1998) identified the critical components of early reading in- struction as including explicit teaching to build phonemic awareness and phonemic decoding skills, fluency in word recognition and text process- ing, reading comprehension strategies, oral language vocabulary, spelling,
  • 8. 14 Journal of School Psychology and writing skills. Instruction that includes these elements delivered in a consistent and skillful way is consistently more effective than instruction that does not contain these components. In the introduction to this article, I mentioned that the most recent Na- tional Assessment of Educational Progress (National Center for Education Statistics, 2001) indicated that 37% of fourth-grade school children nation- ally do not have adequate reading skills for academic work at their grade level. This does not mean, of course, that 37% of all children in the United States have a reading disability and need special education. Rather, it sug- gests the need for strengthening the instructional environment in early el- ementary school by more consistent and skillful instruction in the critical elements identified in recent summaries of research on reading. For exam- ple, Foorman, Francis, Fletcher, Schatschneider, and Mehta (1998) dem- onstrated that well-balanced and skilled classroom instruction can dramati- cally reduce the incidence of reading failure in first- and second-grade classrooms without special interventions for most children. Explicit instruction and practice to build phonemic awareness and phone- mic decoding skills are particularly important for children who enter first grade low in talent or preparation for learning to read. Both Foorman et al. (1998) and Juel and Minden-Cupp (2000) found that explicit instruction and opportunities for extended practice with phonemically decodable texts were particularly beneficial for children at risk for reading failure. In the former study, the most phonemically explicit instructional condition produced the strongest reading growth for all children, but the effects were particularly striking for children with the weakest phonological skills entering first grade. One of the arguments that is frequently made against increasing the ex- plicitness of phonics instruction in early elementary school is that not all children need the same amount or explicitness of instruction in this area. This is, in fact, true. Many children enter school with excellent phonologi- cal processing skills and a strong beginning understanding of the alpha- betic principle. For these children, most of the knowledge that must be ac- quired to become a skilled reader can be discovered by the child during interactions with print. As these children read, they notice useful generali- zations about print–sound relationships, and they acquire a great deal of word-specific knowledge as well (i.e., the sight words that are required for fluent reading; Share & Stanovich, 1995). As Moats (1999) pointed out, “although some children will learn to read in spite of incidental teaching, others never learn unless they are taught in an organized, systematic, effi- cient way by a knowledgeable teacher using a well-designed instructional approach” (p. 7). What the data from studies such as the ones considered earlier (Foorman et al., 1998; Juel & Minden-Cupp, 2000) suggest is that explicit phonics instruction can help all children during the early stages of learning to read, but there will be individual differences in the amount of such instruction that is required.
  • 9. Torgesen 15 Critical Elements of Instruction for Children at Risk for Reading Difficulties A point that is clear from recent research is that traditional approaches to reading instruction in the early elementary grades have substantially un- derestimated the variability among children in their talent and preparation for learning to read. For example, Hart and Risley (1995) documented enormous differences among children from different socioeconomic strata in preschool opportunities to acquire oral language vocabulary. We also know that there are very significant differences among entering school children in their knowledge about letters, print conventions, and phono- logical sensitivity (Adams, 1990). Further, we know that the differences in knowledge and skill that make children more or less prepared to profit from reading instruction in first grade can be the result of either neurobio- logical factors that are genetically transmitted and constitutionally based, or they can be caused by a lack of adequate instruction and language expe- rience in the child’s preschool or home environment (Neisser et al., 1996; Olson, Wise, Johnson, & Ring, 1997). At present, one of the biggest chal- lenges for schools is to provide a range of instructional opportunities in reading that matches the huge diversity in children’s talent and prepara- tion for learning to read. That is, if the diversity among children in talent and preparation for learning to read varies across a 100-point scale, and the range of instructional opportunities varies across only a 70-point scale, it is obvious that many children will not receive the instruction they require to become good readers. Instruction for at-risk children must be more explicit than for other children. This point has already been made to some extent as we consid- ered the differential effects of explicit instruction for children entering first grade more and less prepared for learning to read. Children who en- ter first grade with weaknesses in knowledge about letters, letter-sound cor- respondences, and phonological awareness require explicit and systematic instruction to help them acquire the knowledge and strategies necessary for decoding print. As Gaskins, Ehri, Cress, O’Hara, and Donnelly (1997) pointed out, “first graders who are at risk for failure in learning to read do not discover what teachers leave unsaid about the complexities of word learning. As a result, it is important to teach them procedures for learning words”(p. 325). An illustration of this point is provided in a recent study of preventive in- struction given to a group of highly at-risk children during kindergarten, first grade, and second grade (Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). Of three interventions that were tested on children who were se- lected because of phonological weaknesses, the most phonemically explicit one produced the strongest growth in word reading ability. In fact, of the
  • 10. 16 Journal of School Psychology three interventions tested, only the most explicit intervention produced a reliable difference in the growth of word reading ability over children who were not provided with any special interventions. Other studies (Brown & Felton, 1990; Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; Iversen & Tunmer, 1993) com- bine with this one to suggest that schools must be prepared to provide very explicit and systematic instruction in beginning word reading skills to some of their students if they expect all children to acquire word reading skills at grade level by third grade. Instruction for at-risk children must be more intensive than for other children. The fact that instruction must be more explicit and compre- hensive for these children implies that more skills and knowledge must be directly taught, which logically requires a greater number of teaching/ learning opportunities. To say that instruction for at-risk children must be more intense than for other children simply means that it must contain more teaching/learning opportunities per day than typical classroom in- struction. If at-risk children do not receive more teaching/learning oppor- tunities per day than other children, they will acquire reading skills more slowly, and thus will experience the disadvantages outlined in the introduc- tion of this article. Another factor that underlines the need for more inten- sive instruction is the fact that children who come to school with weak- nesses in talent for learning to read learn more slowly than other children and will thus require more repetition in order to solidly establish critical word reading and comprehension skills. Although children whose risk sta- tus is determined primarily by lack of instructional opportunities in the preschool environment may learn at average rates, they have much more to learn than children who come to school with typical levels of prepara- tion (Hart & Risley, 1995) and thus must be given more intensive instruc- tion if they are to keep pace in reading growth with their age peers. There are actually many different ways to effectively increase instructional intensity for children at risk for reading failure. For example, Greenwood and his colleagues (Greenwood, 1996) used the ClassWide Peer Tutoring model to increase amount of academic engaged time, and this increase has been consistently associated with improvements in learning outcomes in reading. In a similar vein, Doug and Lynn Fuchs reported success (Fuchs, Fuchs, Mathes, & Simmons, 1997) in using peer-assisted learning strategies to improve reading skills in mid-elementary school, and Mathes developed successful procedures that allow the use of peer tutoring for basic reading skills in first-grade classrooms (Mathes, Torgesen, & Allor, in press). For both older and younger children, the interventions provided by the peer- assisted procedures are both more explicit and more intensive than the in- struction that is typically provided by the classroom teacher. Another practical method for providing greater instructional intensity for at-risk children is to use special education or reading resource teachers
  • 11. Torgesen 17 to provide small group (3–4 children) instruction in addition to the regu- lar classroom instruction the children receive. There can be no question that children with reading difficulties, or children at risk for these difficul- ties, will learn more rapidly under conditions of greater instructional in- tensity than they learn in typical classroom settings. Meta-analyses consis- tently show positive effects of grouping practices that increase instructional intensity (Elbaum, Vaughn, Hughes, & Moody, 1999). One interesting finding that has emerged from these analyses is that, so far, one-to-one in- terventions in reading have not been shown to be more effective than small group interventions (Elbaum et al., 1999; National Reading Panel, 2000). Although Torgesen, Alexander, et al. (2001) recently demonstrated very powerful instructional effects for one-to-one instruction (as opposed to the larger group instruction typically provided in special education re- source rooms), other studies have shown similar rates of growth for read- ing-disabled children using small groups of 3 and 4 children at a time (Rashotte, MacPhee, & Torgesen, 2001; Wise, Ring, & Olson, 1999). Instruction for at-risk children must be more supportive than for other children. The needs of at-risk children for more positive emotional sup- port in the form of encouragement, feedback, and positive reinforcement is widely understood. However, their potential need for more cognitive support, in the form of carefully “scaffolded” instruction, is less widely ap- preciated. Instruction for at-risk children should involve two types of scaf- folding. One type of scaffolding involves careful sequencing so that skills build very gradually: The child is always systematically taught and practiced on the skills required for any task they are asked to do (Swanson, 1999). Another type of scaffolding involves teacher–student dialog that directly shows the child what kind of processing or thinking needs to be done in or- der to complete the task successfully. This type of scaffolding in instruction usually involves four elements: (a) the student is presented with a task such as reading or spelling a word, or making a paragraph summary (i.e., tries to spell the word “flat”); (b) the student makes a response that is incorrect in some way, or indicates that he/she doesn’t know how to proceed (i.e., spells it “fat”); (c) the teacher asks a question that focuses the child’s atten- tion on a first step in the solution process, or that draws attention to a re- quired piece of information (“If you read that word, what does it say?” Child responds, “fat.” “So, what do you need to add to make it say flat?” No answer. “When you say flat, what do you hear coming right after the begin- ning sound /f/?”); and (d) another response from the child (“I hear the /l/ sound.”). This kind of interaction between student and child continues un- til the child had been led to successfully accomplish the task. The point of this type of instructional interaction is that the child is led to discover the information or strategies that are critical to accomplishing the task, rather than simply being told what to do. As Juel (1996) recently showed, the abil-
  • 12. 18 Journal of School Psychology ity to offer scaffolded support while children are acquiring reading skills may have increasing importance as the severity of the child’s disability in- creases. I have described three broad ways that instruction for children who are at risk for reading failure needs to be different from the instruction that is typically provided to all children in the classroom. Insuring that all three of these elements are part of the instruction for our most at-risk children rep- resents an enormous challenge for our schools. The requirement for more explicit and supportive instruction demands a higher level of training and skill for teachers than is usually provided at present (Moats, 1994). The re- quirement for more intensive instruction for at-risk children must involve a reallocation of resources to make more teacher time available for preven- tive instruction and, in many cases, will probably require entirely new re- sources to adequately meet the instructional needs of all children who are at risk for reading failure. What Do We Know About the Effectiveness of Early Preventive Instruction? An obvious question that must be addressed before schools reallocate or in- vest new resources for preventive instruction is whether the procedures out- lined above are actually effective in preventing reading difficulties in most children. Two kinds of information are required to provide a complete an- swer to this question. First, we must know to what extent appropriate preven- tive instruction can bring word reading and comprehension skills to ade- quate levels during the period of the intervention. Second, we must understand the long-term effects of preventive instruction on subsequent reading growth. Answering this latter question is complicated, because long- term outcomes can be influenced by many factors. For example, long-term outcomes can certainly be influenced by the extent to which the preventive instruction helps children acquire all the reading skills required as a founda- tion for further growth. However, these outcomes may also depend on the support for reading improvement (both in the school and in the home) that is provided after the preventive intervention is concluded. When the solid foundations for reading growth are established in the early primary grades, further reading growth is dependent almost entirely on the breadth and depth of the child’s reading experience and practice (Snow et al., 1998). In terms of immediate outcomes from preventive instruction, we know that it is possible to bring the word-level reading skills of children at risk for reading failure within the average range by the end of first or second grade when the intervention group is considered as a whole (Torgesen, in press). Children who achieve these average-level word reading skills can also comprehend text within the average range (Foorman et al., 1998) or at a level consistent with their general verbal ability (Torgesen, Wagner,
  • 13. Torgesen 19 Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). However, there are large individual differ- ences in response to the early interventions examined in research, and not all children show satisfactory outcomes. Torgesen (2000) recently examined the outcomes from five prevention studies that all used similar measures to assess reading outcomes. These stud- ies all contained at least one instructional condition that offered skilled de- livery of explicit and systematic instruction in phonemic awareness, phone- mic decoding, and fluent text reading. The children who received the preventive instruction were selected to be at risk for reading failure on the basis of either weak phonological processing skills or weak development of early word reading ability, and the preventive instruction was provided at some point during kindergarten, first grade, or second grade. The number of hours of special instruction varied between 340 hr of first- and second- grade instruction delivered to groups of 8 (Brown & Felton, 1990) and 35–65 hr of one-on-one instruction delivered in the second semester of first grade and the first semester of second grade (Vellutino et al., 1996). Outcomes from these studies were analyzed to estimate the proportion of the population that would remain below the 30th percentile (by current norms) in word reading ability if the interventions were available to all chil- dren who needed them. The proportions varied between 6% of the popu- lation in the study that provided 35–65 hr of one-on-one instruction to 2% of the population in a study that provided 92 hr of computer-assisted tutor- ing to groups of 3 during first grade (Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, & Her- ron, 2001). These varying proportions of “treatment resisters” cannot be used to directly compare effectiveness of the different methods in these studies because the samples were selected by different methods, the popu- lations from which they were selected also differed, and the background classroom instruction the children received also differed. The important point is that none of the interventions were sufficient to produce adequate reading growth in all children. Across all the interventions, the average es- timate of “population failure rate” was 5%. It is interesting that this 5% fig- ure roughly corresponds to the percentage of children who are currently identified as learning disabled, most of whom are identified because of problems learning to read. As a counterpoint to this estimate of popula- tion failure rate in early intervention studies, Scanlon, Vellutino, Small, and Fanuele (2000) recently reported a study in which the failure rate was essentially zero in their most effective condition. This condition involved a combination of small group intervention in kindergarten and one-on-one instruction in first grade, and it suggests that it may be possible to improve on past results with multilayered interventions in the early grades. Of course, there are many reasons why outcomes from highly controlled research studies may be either unrealistically high or unrealistically low. One reason that they may sometimes be low is that the preventive instruc- tion may be offered in the context of classroom instruction that does not
  • 14. 20 Journal of School Psychology reinforce or encourage the skills being taught to the at-risk learners (i.e., Torgesen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). As was mentioned earlier, to maximize reading growth, children at risk for reading difficulties must receive both strong classroom instruction in reading and more intensive, explicit, and supportive preventive instruction. A reason that research studies may produce unrealistically strong outcomes is that teachers may be more highly trained and supervised in these settings than usually occurs in schools. For these reasons, it is instructive to examine reading outcomes in schools where sound preventive procedures are implemented. One such school is Hartsfield Elementary in Tallahassee, Florida (King & Torgesen, 2001). Over a 5-year period, this school worked to implement a balanced reading curriculum in kindergarten through Grade 3 and to estab- lish significant amounts of preventive reading instruction for children who were performing below grade level in the first and second grade. The school serves a population of children who are about 65% minority (mostly African American), and 60% of the children are eligible for free or reduced lunch support. In the first year of the project, the new classroom reading instruc- tion was only partially implemented, but by the end of the 5-year period, it was fully implemented in all primary-grade classrooms. The preventive in- struction was phased in gradually beginning in the second year of the project as new resources for providing the instruction were identified. The results for word-level reading skills are presented graphically in Figure 1. The test used was a nationally standardized measure of word reading ability, and it was given by individuals other than the children’s teachers at the end of each year to all children. The figure shows the percentage of children who ended first and second grade performing below the 25th percentile, and it also de- scribes the change in average percentile for all children. As can be seen from the figure, during the 5-year implementation period, the percentage of chil- dren performing below the 25th percentile at the end of first grade dropped from 31.8% to 3.7%. At the end of the implementation period, the percent- age of children (using only those children at Hartsfield with at least 2 years of instruction) still weak in word-level reading skills at the end of second grade was only 2.4%. In terms of the long-term impact of early intervention at this school, during the same period of time, Hartsfield achieved the larg- est growth of any of the 20 elementary schools in its district on the state- administered standardized reading test administered at the end of third grade. Median percentile in reading achievement for third-grade children on the California Achievement Test improved from 49 at the end of 1994 to 73 at the end of 1999. IDENTIFYING CHILDREN IN NEED OF PREVENTIVE INTERVENTIONS It is beyond the scope of this article to provide a detailed discussion of as- sessment procedures to identify children in need of special preventive in-
  • 15. Torgesen 21 struction in reading. That topic is covered in another article in this issue (Fletcher et al.). However, for the sake of completeness, and because sys- tematic assessment for early identification must be an integral part of any school-wide program to prevent reading disabilities, several critical points will be made here. 1. It does not take a lengthy assessment to approach the limits of accuracy in identifying children who require instruction that is more intensive, ex- plicit, and supportive than that provided by the classroom teacher. In kin- dergarten, a reliable assessment of phonemic awareness, letter-sound knowledge, and vocabulary will identify most children at risk for reading failure. Children low in the first two measures will need special support Figure 1. Changes in year-end reading performance of children during period of rapid curric- ulum changes in reading.
  • 16. 22 Journal of School Psychology for the development of word reading ability, and children low in all three areas will require additional support in oral language development. 2. Unless screening criteria are set to identify high proportions of children as at risk, most early identification procedures at present produce a fairly high proportion of “false negatives” (i.e., children who pass the screening but later show difficulties learning to read; Scarborough, 1998). The simple reason for this is that reading growth is predicted by a variety of factors other than cognitive abilities and knowledge (Torge- sen, Wagner, Rashotte, Rose, et al., 1999). However, the number of false negatives during the period from the start of school to third grade can be reduced to virtually zero if screening for reading difficulties is con- ducted regularly in first, second, and third grade. I would recommend assessment to monitor reading growth occurs at least three times a year during first, second, and third grade. 3. At the beginning of first grade, at-risk children can best be identified us- ing procedures similar to those for children in kindergarten. However, after reading instruction begins in first grade, the best way to identify children who are falling behind in the ability to read words accurately and fluently is to measure that skill directly. We (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999) recently published a test called the Test of Word Reading Efficiency that requires only 45 s to obtain a reliable measure of phone- mic decoding efficiency, and another 45 s to measure the growth of sight word vocabulary. In first grade, I would recommend assessment of comprehension-related skills using a measure of oral language vocabu- lary rather than reading comprehension because comprehension is so heavily dependent on word reading ability in the early primary grades (Stanovich, Cunningham, & Feeman, 1984). 4. In second and third grade, the development of word-level reading abil- ity should continue to be monitored using direct assessments to identify children who are falling behind their peers in these critical skills. At this point, group- or individually administered measures of reading compre- hension may prove useful in identifying children who can continue to profit from more intensive work to build vocabulary and reading com- prehension strategies. Concluding Comments Although we do not yet understand the conditions that must be in place to prevent reading difficulties in all children, we do know what must be done to very substantially reduce the number of children who fail to acquire ad- equate reading skills during the primary grades of elementary school. The key to taking advantage of this knowledge is finding the will to change the way we work at teaching reading. We must work more effectively to insure that classroom teachers acquire the skills and knowledge to teach reading
  • 17. Torgesen 23 to children who do not learn easily. We must work to develop and institute procedures to identify children in need of extra instruction in a timely and accurate manner. Finally, we must work to find sufficient instructional re- sources to provide more intensive, explicit, and supportive instruction to the children who need it. We have numerous examples showing that work- ing effectively in all these areas will dramatically increase our success in teaching all children to read well during elementary school. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was supported, in part, by grant HD30988, “Prevention and Re- mediation of Reading Disabilities” and grant HD23340, “Reading-Related Phonological Processes,” from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. REFERENCES Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Adams, M. J. (1991). A talk with Marilyn Adams. Language Arts, 68, 206–212. Briggs, A., Austin, R., & Underwood, G. (1984). Phonological coding in good and poor readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 54–66. Brown, A. L., Palincsar, A. S., & Purcell, L. (l986). Poor readers: Teach, don’t label. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority children: New perspectives (pp. 105– 143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, I. S., & Felton, R. H. (l990). Effects of instruction on beginning reading skills in children at risk for reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 223–241. Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dys- lexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454. Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What reading does for the mind. Ameri- can Educator, 22(Spring/Summer), 8–15. Ehri, L. C. (1998). Grapheme–phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning reading (pp. 3–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Elbaum, B., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M. T., & Moody, S. W. (1999). Grouping practices and reading outcomes for students with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 65, 399–415. Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., Steubing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Fowler, A. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 6–23. Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk chil- dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 37–55. Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individ- ual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 3–17. Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. G., & Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-assisted learning strategies: Making classrooms more responsive to academic diversity. American Educa- tional Research Journal, 34, 174–206.
  • 18. 24 Journal of School Psychology Gaskins, I. W., Ehri, L. C., Cress, C., O’Hara, C., & Donnelly, K. (1997). Procedures for word learning: Making discoveries about words. The Reading Teacher, 50, 312–327. Gough, P. B. (1996). How children learn to read and why they fail. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 3–20. Greenwood, C. R. (1996). Research on the practices and behavior of effective teachers at the Juniper Gardens Children’s Project: Implications for the education of diverse learners. In D. L. Speece & B. K. Keogh (Eds.), Research on classroom ecologies (pp. 39–68). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Hatcher, P., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by inte- grating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57. Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–160. Iversen, S., & Tunmer, W. E. (1993). Phonological processing skills and the reading re- covery program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 112–126. Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447. Juel, C. (1996). What makes literacy tutoring effective? Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 268–289. Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and in- structional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458–492. King, R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Improving the effectiveness of reading instruction in one el- ementary school: A description of the process. Unpublished manuscript, Florida State Uni- versity, Tallahassee. Lee, V., Brooks-Gunn, J., Schnur, E., & Liaw, F. (1990). Are Head Start effects sustained? A longitudinal follow-up comparison of disadvantaged children attending Head Start, no preschool, and other pre-school programs. Child Development, 61, 495–507. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.), Phonology and read- ing disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 1–33). Ann Arbor, MI: U. of Michigan Press. Manis, F. R., Custodio, R., & Szeszulski, P. A. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experi- mental Child Psychology, 56, 64–86. Mathes, P. G., Torgesen, J. K., & Allor, J. H. (in press). The effects of Peer-Assisted Liter- acy Strategies for first-grade readers with and without additional computer assisted instruction in phonological awareness. American Educational Research Journal. Moats, L. C. (1994). The missing foundation in teacher education: Knowledge of the structure of spoken and written language. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 81–102. Moats, L. C. (1999). Teaching reading is rocket science. Washington, D.C.: American Feder- ation of Teachers. National Center for Education Statistics. (2001). NAEP 2000 Reading. A report card for the nation and the states. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education. National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Wash- ington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Eci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlen, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101. Oka, E., & Paris, S. (1986). Patterns of motivation and reading skills in underachieving children. In S. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • 19. Torgesen 25 Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Johnson, M., & Ring, J. (1997). The etiology and remediation of phonologically based word recognition and spelling disabilities: Are phonological deficits the “hole” story? In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of Reading Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rashotte, C. A., MacPhee, K., & Torgesen, J. K. (in press). The effectiveness of a group reading instruction program with poor readers in multiple grades. Learning Disabili- ties Quarterly, 24, 119–134. Scanlon, D. M., Vellutino, F. R., Small, S. G., & Fanuele, D. P. (2000). Severe reading difficulties—Can they be prevented? A comparison of prevention and intervention approaches. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA, April. Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabili- ties: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–120). Timonium, MD: York Press. Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading develop- ment: A model of acquisition and individual differences. Issues in Education: Contribu- tions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57. Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 469–479. Simpson, G. B., Lorsbach, T., & Whitehouse, D. (1983). Encoding and contextual com- ponents of word recognition in good and poor readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 161–171. Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (l984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 278–303. Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading- disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-differ- ence model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53. Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading research for students with LD: A meta-analysis of inter- vention outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 504–532. Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in read- ing: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 55–64. Torgesen, J. K. (in press). Lessons learned from intervention research in reading: A way to go before we rest. In R. Stainthorpe (Ed.), Literacy: Learning and teaching. Monograph of the British Journal of Educational Psychology, London: British Psychological Association. Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K., Conway, T., & Rose, E. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 33–58. Torgesen, J. K., & Burgess, S. R. (1998). Consistency of reading-related phonological processes throughout early childhood: Evidence from longitudinal-correlational and instructional studies. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning read- ing (pp. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Torgesen, J. K., & Mathes, P. (2000). A basic guide to understanding, assessing, and teaching phonological awareness. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruc- tion in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–355). Parkton, MD: York Press. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
  • 20. 26 Journal of School Psychology Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Herron, J. (2001). A comparison of two computer assisted approaches to the prevention of reading disabilities in young children. Manu- script in preparation. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., & Garvin, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psycholog, 91, 579–593. Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E. R., Small, S. G., Pratt, A., Chen, R., & Denckla, M. B. (1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educa- tional Psychology, 88, 601–638. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading- related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing causal relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to fluent readers: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479. Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 335–357. Wise, B. W., Ring, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and without explicit attention to articulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 271–304.