SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 134
Understanding Color
Chapter 7 Color Theory: A Brief History
Questions and ideas about color
 have a long history. This search
has produced an enormous library
of writing known as color theory .
The earliest known
writers on color were
            the Greek
    philosophers who
were intrigued by the
     elusive nature of
                color.
In ancient philosophy, all meaning in the larger universe
     was related somehow to mathematical order.
This idea that beauty and harmony are the natural result
of mathematical order is a premise that is still very much
                     in place today.
Pythagorus (c. 569–
490 BC) is credited
with originating the
concept of the
“harmony of the
spheres.”




Raphael, 1509
This theory
postulated that the
planets are
separated from each
other by intervals
that correspond to
the musical scale.
Aristotle (c. 384-322
BC) was the most
influential of the
earliest writers on
color and addressed
the subject both
philosophically and
scientifically.
Aristotle thought that all
colors derive from black
and white, or darkness
and light, and this idea
was accepted as fact
until the 18th century.




 Raphael, 1509
During the
      Renaissance,
writers like Leonard
 da Vinci and others
         wrote about
everything from the
     practicalities of
 mixing pigments to
   the philosophical
           and moral
meanings of colors.
But it was overall
a rather obscure
 subject until the
18th century and
   the studies of
Isaac Newton .
Newton was a
      product of the
      Enlightenment
     during the 18th
 century. This “Age
 of Reason” sought
     to give rational
   explanations for
natural phenomena
  to replace the old
    mystical beliefs.
However, this
search for
absolutes
determined by
science was as
rigid and
uncompromising in
its way as the
demands of
absolute faith that
preceded it.
Only the source of
authority had
changed, from God
and his earthly
representatives, the
clergy, to reason
and its earthly
representatives,
men.
The intellectual world
of the 18th century was
      quite fluid. People
            didn’t think of
 themselves as writers,
             biologists, or
 mathematicians but as
 “natural philosophers,”
     and wrote about all
   sorts of scientific and
    philosophical topics.
Two themes dominated 18th, 19th, and
early 20th century color study:



   the search for a comprehensive color-order
   system, including an appropriate format for
   visualizing it. AND...

   the laws of color harmony
Two towering and and very different figures
 dominate the beginnings of color theory:
Isaac Newton
 (1642–1727)
Johann Wolfgang von Goethe (1749–1832)
Newton, working at Cambridge in the late 1690’s, first
split sunlight into its component wavelengths by passing
                      it through a prism.
He observed that as each wavelength enters a
         prism it bends or refracts .
Because the glass of a prism slows each wavelength down at
a slightly different rate, each emerges as a separate beam of
                               light.
Newton was able to recombine the separated
beams with a lens and reconstituted white light.
From this he hypothesized the nature of light and
          the origins of perceived color.
He published his
results in a treatise
called Opticks, in
1703.
Newton’s
conclusion that
light alone
generates color
remains a basis of
modern physics.
Newton separated
the spectral hues
into seven colors:
red, orange yellow,
green blue, indigo,
and violet.
Most people cannot
detect the blue-
violet that Newton
calls “indigo.”
Perhaps he was influenced by 17th century thinking that
gave mystical importance to the number 7, or he may have
    had unusual visual acuity in the blue-violet range.
Although the spectrum of light is linear...
...Newton originated the concept of colors a a continuous
  experience. He diagramed the seven hues as a circle,
              linking spectral red and violet.
This first known illustration of
colors as a closed circle made
     of arcs of individual color
           appeared in Opticks.
Newton’s contemporaries viewed Opticks as a work on
  the nature of color , not on the nature of light . The
 ideas in it generated tremendous controversy all over
                         Europe.
At the same time,
        the natural
philosophers were
  considering light,
   more pragmatic
people were trying
to discover how to
        predictably
 produce colors by
   mixing paints or
             dyes.
Jacques Christophe
Le Blon (1667–1741)
was a French
printmaker who
identified the primary
nature of red, yellow
and blue while mixing
pigments for printing.
Le Blon’s treatise,
Coloritto (c. 1730)
offers the first
concept of three
subtractive
primary colors.
His work attracted a great
                                                                     deal of attention and
                                                                     acceptance because it
                                                                     addressed the practical
                                                                     aspects of using color.



Jacques Christophe Le Blon, Van Dyck Self Portrait. Three-color
mezzotint, 61.2 x 36.0 cm., c. 1720s. Yale Center for British Art,
Paul Mellon Collection.
The CMYK color
space used in
printing today is
derived from his
work.
Moses Harris
(1731–1785) was an
entomologist and
engraver who was
fascinated by the
color of the insects
he studied.
He published the
first known color
circle in 1766.
Harris believed that
red, yellow, and
blue were the most
different from each
other and should be
placed at the
greatest possible
distances apart on
the circle.
To accomplish this,
he discarded
Newton’s indigo and
created an
expanded color
circle based on
equal intervals of
color and multiples
of three.
Goethe later
adopted Harris’
organization of
color.
Johann Wolfgang
von Goethe was
fascinated by color.
Goethe spent a
great deal of energy
 trying to prove that
      Newton’s color
       theories were
              wrong.
He rejected
Newton’s assertion
   that color comes
      from light and
  insisted it was an
experienced reality.



                       Goethe’s Color Wheel
He also proposed
  notions about the
association of color
    and beauty with
 morality. He even
said that there were
   sinful and chaste
              colors.

                        Goethe’s Color Wheel
Goethe was first to recognize the importance of the
interaction between complementary colors. He called
              them “completing colors.”
Goethe also reported
       extensively on
simultaneous contrast
      and afterimage.
Otto Philip Runge
(1770–1840) shared
Goethe’s belief in
the importance of
complementary
colors.
Runge was a
painter and
developed the first
three dimensional
model for color, the
Color Sphere.
It was published in a book he wrote
        called Farken Kugel.
Goethe’s observations were so wide-ranging and
fundamental that almost every concept in modern color
          study can be found in his writing.
Goethe’s most familiar contribution to color study is
            the six-hue color circle.
Even though he first believed that the only primaries
             were blue and yellow!!!
Goethe’s French
   contemporary was
       Michel Eugene
Chevreul (1786–1889).
Chevreul was Master of the Gobelin Tapestry Works
and was mostly concerned with the practical difficulties
           with producing consistent dyes.
He accepted the three primary colors theory and also
observed the phenomenon of simultaneous contrast.




               Chevreul at age 100 in his studio-laboratory.
His 1839
treatise had a
profound
influence on
the
Impressionist
movement in
painting.
The battle between Goethe’s six-hue
spectrum and Newton’s seven-color
  model was unnecessary because
 both are valid but describe different
                reality:
Newton was looking at causes .
Goethe was looking at effects .
In the past, science students and art
 students were not usually the same
students, so the difference in the two
    ideas rarely came into conflict.
Today’s designers and students of design must
understand both cause and effect and be able to
    work within–and between–both realities.
Ostwald                    Munsell




After Goethe and Chevreul, most of the late 19th and
 early 20th century color theorists worked in rigidly
             formal scientific systems.
Ostwald                     Munsell




 The stress was on rules, control and order: the goal
 was to create a comprehensive color-order system
and to find within it immutable laws of color harmony.
Albert Munsell
 (1858–1918)
developed his
3-dimensional
color model in
        1921.
Published in A Grammar
       of Color, Munsell
 proposed a “color tree”
    with infinite room for
               expansion.
Munsell’s color space is constructed as progressive
intervals of hue that rotate around a vertical axis of value
                    from black to white.
In Munsell’s theory
every possible color
cannot be shown but
each has an
assigned place on an
alphanumeric scale.
Chroma is Munsell’s
word for saturation.
The problem with this system is that it does not take into
  account the tertiaries . Nowhere on the color tree can you
find colors mixed with their complements to reduce saturation.
In the Munsell System, reduction of saturation is achieved
     by mixing the hues with gray in graduated steps.
Color numbering
systems have great
value when they are
used to communicate
color information
between individuals
who have reference to
the same set of
standards.
But these systems are
meaningless as an
aid to
understanding
color.
Munsell was not
        immune to the
            moralizing
   associations of his
         predecessors
 asserting that certain
  colors implied good
taste and others poor
                 taste.
His system is still in
     use today, but
without the original
      commentary.
A hypothetical color
       solid (or color
       displayed in 3-
     dimensions) is a
   frequent theme in
scholarly color-order
             systems.
German chemist
    Wilhelm Ostwald
(1853–1932) brought
 the conceptual color
    solid to full-blown
      theory in Color
              Science.
The Color Primer with
its eight-hue spectrum
    became mandatory
       for color study in
  German schools and
in many English ones.
It was a strong
influence on artists of
          the Bauhaus
            movement.
Wilhelm von Bezold
         (1837–1932)
contributed scientific
  fact to the growing
body of color writing.
Bezold is best known for the “Bezold Effect” which
says that forms enclosed by light lines appear lighter
                  and vice versa.
Ludwig Von
Helmholtz (1821–
1894) also
contributed to color
studies.
He is known for his
mathematics of the
eye, the
ories of vision, ideas
on the visual
perception of
space, and his color
vision research.
By the early
20th century,
color study had
become an
enormous and
wide-ranging
topic, positioned
uncomfortably
with one foot in
the sciences
and other in the
arts.
It remained for the artists and designers of the
       Bauhaus to end this ambiguity.
The Bauhaus was a
design group
founded in 1919 by
German architect
Walter Gropius
(1883–1969).
The Bauhaus
 group brought
    the study of
color to a level
of attention not
     seen since
       Goethe’s
   challenge to
       Newton.
These Bauhaus
master students
of color and
color theory
approached
color from new
directions with
intelligence, wit,
and energy...
Lyonel Feininger
Paul Klee
Wassily Kandinsky
Johannes Itten
Josef Albers
Oskar Schlemmer
These artists and writers made the definitive break
between the study of color as science and the study of
              color as art and aesthetics.
Johannes Itten
           (1888–1967)
    followed Goethe in
   exploring color as a
      series of contrast
systems and opposing
                 forces.
He theorized
seven
contrasts of
color based
on
perceptions
alone:
Contrast of:
saturation

hue

value

warm & cool contrast

complementary contrast

simultaneous contrast

extension (area)
Itten codified color harmonies as a series of chords based
on the complementary relationship and diagramed them as
                      geometric forms.
Itten’s approach,
              although
mathematically based,
is much less rigid than
many that preceded it.
Itten’s focus was as
  much on individual
    perception as on
       mathematical
        relationships.
His major work is entitled, The Art of Color
Color-order systems were the first concern of theorists
because a formal system establishes a structured field in which
            to search for laws of color harmony.
The primary focus of that search was on the relationship
between hues. Value and saturation took a back seat to hue.
Among all the major figures in color study, there was
agreement that balance between complementary colors
      was the first principle of color harmony.
The ancient ideal
of mathematical
balance was so
much a part of the
search for laws of
harmony that hues
were frequently
associated with
numbers or
geometric forms.
Arthur Schopenhauer
 (1788–1860) theorized
        that equal light-
reflectance in spectrum
     colors is inherently
           harmonious.
Schopenhauer’s Circle of Color Harmony is made up of
unequal arcs. Each complementary pair is meant to be
equal in light-reflectance to each of the other two pairs.
Every color is assigned a number representing its
light-reflectance (or value) in relation to the others.
The total of all the numbers added together
    is 36, or 360 degrees, a full circle.
Schopenhauer’s theory can be deceptive;
a large area of violet does not necessarily reflect
  the same amount of light as a smaller area of
                       yellow.
But we do sense value differences between pure
                    colors.
Schopenhauer’s theory can be illustrated as striped tee
  shirts. In order for each shirt to be harmonious, the
complementary pairs must have different ratios: 1 to 1,
                     1 to 2, and 1 to 3.
In Itten’s quest for
   color harmony, he
        superimposed
     geometric forms
(squares, rectangles,
         triangles, and
  hexagons) over the
  artists’ spectrum to
demonstrate what he
  called “harmonious
               chords.”
Each color chord
            illustrates
complementary colors
  in some measurable
           proportion.
The geometric points
are called the “notes”
and no “chord” strays
              from the
      complementary
         relationship.
The person who made
the final break with the
color-order tradition
was Josef Albers
(1888–1976), a
colleague of Itten’s
who also taught at the
Bauhaus school.
Albers fled Nazi
Germany in the early
 1930’s with his wife,
    the weaver Anni
              Albers.
They taught first at the Black Mountain
School in North Carolina and later at Yale.
Albers
became the
most
influential
name in color
theory in the
United States.
However, his
1963 book
Interaction of
Colors contained
nothing like the
usual charts or
systems.
Albers taught that
true
understanding of
color comes from
an intuitive
approach to studio
exercises.
He stressed the instability and relativity of perceived
       colors and the power of visual training.
At the same time, he taught that even within this unstable
field, there are effects that can be predicted and controlled.
In Interaction of Colors, Albers casually discounts the
       generations of theory that preceded him.
“This
book...reverses
this order and
places practice
before theory,
which is, after all,
the conclusion of
practice,” he wrote.
Albers was not the
first to recognize
that the visual
experience, more
than conscious
choice, determines
how we perceive
colors, but he was
the first to assert
the primacy of the
visual experience
over structure or
intellectual
considerations.
For Albers, the
visual experience,
   not theory, was
       paramount.
The late 20th century saw the focus of color study
move from philosophical inquiry to a greater interest in
  psychological and motivational effects of colors.
There is an entire industry, for example, that is devoted
to determining current and future consumer preferences
            in colors and color combinations.
At the same time, color theorists continue to search
                   for absolutes.
There is an enduring
 assumption–or perhaps, a
  hope–that those elusive,
timeless, and absolute laws
for pleasing combinations of
  colors really do exist and
   simply await discovery.

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Semelhante a Chapter 7

Colour interaction and dynamics
Colour interaction and dynamicsColour interaction and dynamics
Colour interaction and dynamicsPrashant Tripathi
 
Isaac newton
Isaac newtonIsaac newton
Isaac newtonanoop kp
 
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docx
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docxPost ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docx
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docxharrisonhoward80223
 
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)Sarah Lee
 
Renaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkRenaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkbarbaramasters
 
Renaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkRenaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkbarbaramasters
 
Colour Study Part 2
 Colour Study Part 2 Colour Study Part 2
Colour Study Part 2Ranjan Joshi
 
How humanities changed_the_world
How humanities changed_the_worldHow humanities changed_the_world
How humanities changed_the_worldCSR
 
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th Century
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th CenturyMathematics and science 17th and 18th Century
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th CenturyJasonDelaCruz20
 
Modernism by Monir Hossen
Modernism by Monir HossenModernism by Monir Hossen
Modernism by Monir HossenMonir Hossen
 
The evolution of colour theory and the still life
The evolution of colour theory and the still lifeThe evolution of colour theory and the still life
The evolution of colour theory and the still lifeLibby Bourne
 
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & Gauguin
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & GauguinReading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & Gauguin
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & GauguinAzmiSuhaimi
 

Semelhante a Chapter 7 (20)

Colour interaction and dynamics
Colour interaction and dynamicsColour interaction and dynamics
Colour interaction and dynamics
 
Isaac newton
Isaac newtonIsaac newton
Isaac newton
 
Georges seurat
Georges seuratGeorges seurat
Georges seurat
 
Basic color theory
Basic color theoryBasic color theory
Basic color theory
 
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docx
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docxPost ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docx
Post ImpressionismThe Starry Night, Vincent van Gogh, 1889.docx
 
Isaac Newton
Isaac NewtonIsaac Newton
Isaac Newton
 
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)
TOK - Theory of knowledge essay (what counts as knowledge in the arts)
 
Renaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkRenaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walk
 
Ren museum walk
Ren museum walkRen museum walk
Ren museum walk
 
Renaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walkRenaissance museum walk
Renaissance museum walk
 
The Color Wheele and Beyond
The Color Wheele and BeyondThe Color Wheele and Beyond
The Color Wheele and Beyond
 
Isaac newton sunum
Isaac newton sunumIsaac newton sunum
Isaac newton sunum
 
Colour Study Part 2
 Colour Study Part 2 Colour Study Part 2
Colour Study Part 2
 
How humanities changed_the_world
How humanities changed_the_worldHow humanities changed_the_world
How humanities changed_the_world
 
Isaac newton
Isaac newton Isaac newton
Isaac newton
 
The Renaissance Essay
The Renaissance EssayThe Renaissance Essay
The Renaissance Essay
 
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th Century
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th CenturyMathematics and science 17th and 18th Century
Mathematics and science 17th and 18th Century
 
Modernism by Monir Hossen
Modernism by Monir HossenModernism by Monir Hossen
Modernism by Monir Hossen
 
The evolution of colour theory and the still life
The evolution of colour theory and the still lifeThe evolution of colour theory and the still life
The evolution of colour theory and the still life
 
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & Gauguin
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & GauguinReading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & Gauguin
Reading Visual Images: Post-Impressionism & Gauguin
 

Mais de Mary Haas

White balance
White balanceWhite balance
White balanceMary Haas
 
Mid term sp.2013ppt
Mid term sp.2013pptMid term sp.2013ppt
Mid term sp.2013pptMary Haas
 
Schindler's list
Schindler's listSchindler's list
Schindler's listMary Haas
 
Light and exposure
Light and exposureLight and exposure
Light and exposureMary Haas
 
Chapters 1 & 2 test
Chapters 1 & 2 testChapters 1 & 2 test
Chapters 1 & 2 testMary Haas
 
Saturation studies ass
Saturation studies assSaturation studies ass
Saturation studies assMary Haas
 
Color assignment 1
Color assignment 1Color assignment 1
Color assignment 1Mary Haas
 
Controlling Your Camera: Apertures
Controlling Your Camera: AperturesControlling Your Camera: Apertures
Controlling Your Camera: AperturesMary Haas
 
Controlling your camera shutter.ppt
Controlling your camera shutter.pptControlling your camera shutter.ppt
Controlling your camera shutter.pptMary Haas
 
Controlling your camera shutter
Controlling your camera shutterControlling your camera shutter
Controlling your camera shutterMary Haas
 
How cameras work
How cameras workHow cameras work
How cameras workMary Haas
 

Mais de Mary Haas (20)

White balance
White balanceWhite balance
White balance
 
Chapter 8
Chapter 8Chapter 8
Chapter 8
 
Chapter 6
Chapter 6Chapter 6
Chapter 6
 
Mid term
Mid termMid term
Mid term
 
Mid term sp.2013ppt
Mid term sp.2013pptMid term sp.2013ppt
Mid term sp.2013ppt
 
Schindler's list
Schindler's listSchindler's list
Schindler's list
 
Chapter 5
Chapter 5Chapter 5
Chapter 5
 
Light and exposure
Light and exposureLight and exposure
Light and exposure
 
Chapters 1 & 2 test
Chapters 1 & 2 testChapters 1 & 2 test
Chapters 1 & 2 test
 
Saturation studies ass
Saturation studies assSaturation studies ass
Saturation studies ass
 
Color assignment 1
Color assignment 1Color assignment 1
Color assignment 1
 
Chapter 4
Chapter 4Chapter 4
Chapter 4
 
Chapter 3
Chapter 3Chapter 3
Chapter 3
 
Focus
FocusFocus
Focus
 
Controlling Your Camera: Apertures
Controlling Your Camera: AperturesControlling Your Camera: Apertures
Controlling Your Camera: Apertures
 
Controlling your camera shutter.ppt
Controlling your camera shutter.pptControlling your camera shutter.ppt
Controlling your camera shutter.ppt
 
Chapter 2
Chapter 2Chapter 2
Chapter 2
 
Controlling your camera shutter
Controlling your camera shutterControlling your camera shutter
Controlling your camera shutter
 
Chapter 1
Chapter 1Chapter 1
Chapter 1
 
How cameras work
How cameras workHow cameras work
How cameras work
 

Chapter 7

  • 1. Understanding Color Chapter 7 Color Theory: A Brief History
  • 2. Questions and ideas about color have a long history. This search has produced an enormous library of writing known as color theory .
  • 3. The earliest known writers on color were the Greek philosophers who were intrigued by the elusive nature of color.
  • 4. In ancient philosophy, all meaning in the larger universe was related somehow to mathematical order.
  • 5. This idea that beauty and harmony are the natural result of mathematical order is a premise that is still very much in place today.
  • 6. Pythagorus (c. 569– 490 BC) is credited with originating the concept of the “harmony of the spheres.” Raphael, 1509
  • 7. This theory postulated that the planets are separated from each other by intervals that correspond to the musical scale.
  • 8. Aristotle (c. 384-322 BC) was the most influential of the earliest writers on color and addressed the subject both philosophically and scientifically.
  • 9. Aristotle thought that all colors derive from black and white, or darkness and light, and this idea was accepted as fact until the 18th century. Raphael, 1509
  • 10. During the Renaissance, writers like Leonard da Vinci and others wrote about everything from the practicalities of mixing pigments to the philosophical and moral meanings of colors.
  • 11. But it was overall a rather obscure subject until the 18th century and the studies of Isaac Newton .
  • 12. Newton was a product of the Enlightenment during the 18th century. This “Age of Reason” sought to give rational explanations for natural phenomena to replace the old mystical beliefs.
  • 13. However, this search for absolutes determined by science was as rigid and uncompromising in its way as the demands of absolute faith that preceded it.
  • 14. Only the source of authority had changed, from God and his earthly representatives, the clergy, to reason and its earthly representatives, men.
  • 15. The intellectual world of the 18th century was quite fluid. People didn’t think of themselves as writers, biologists, or mathematicians but as “natural philosophers,” and wrote about all sorts of scientific and philosophical topics.
  • 16. Two themes dominated 18th, 19th, and early 20th century color study: the search for a comprehensive color-order system, including an appropriate format for visualizing it. AND... the laws of color harmony
  • 17. Two towering and and very different figures dominate the beginnings of color theory:
  • 19. Johann Wolfgang von Goethe (1749–1832)
  • 20. Newton, working at Cambridge in the late 1690’s, first split sunlight into its component wavelengths by passing it through a prism.
  • 21. He observed that as each wavelength enters a prism it bends or refracts .
  • 22. Because the glass of a prism slows each wavelength down at a slightly different rate, each emerges as a separate beam of light.
  • 23. Newton was able to recombine the separated beams with a lens and reconstituted white light.
  • 24. From this he hypothesized the nature of light and the origins of perceived color.
  • 25. He published his results in a treatise called Opticks, in 1703.
  • 26. Newton’s conclusion that light alone generates color remains a basis of modern physics.
  • 27. Newton separated the spectral hues into seven colors: red, orange yellow, green blue, indigo, and violet.
  • 28. Most people cannot detect the blue- violet that Newton calls “indigo.”
  • 29. Perhaps he was influenced by 17th century thinking that gave mystical importance to the number 7, or he may have had unusual visual acuity in the blue-violet range.
  • 30. Although the spectrum of light is linear...
  • 31. ...Newton originated the concept of colors a a continuous experience. He diagramed the seven hues as a circle, linking spectral red and violet.
  • 32. This first known illustration of colors as a closed circle made of arcs of individual color appeared in Opticks.
  • 33. Newton’s contemporaries viewed Opticks as a work on the nature of color , not on the nature of light . The ideas in it generated tremendous controversy all over Europe.
  • 34. At the same time, the natural philosophers were considering light, more pragmatic people were trying to discover how to predictably produce colors by mixing paints or dyes.
  • 35. Jacques Christophe Le Blon (1667–1741) was a French printmaker who identified the primary nature of red, yellow and blue while mixing pigments for printing.
  • 36. Le Blon’s treatise, Coloritto (c. 1730) offers the first concept of three subtractive primary colors.
  • 37. His work attracted a great deal of attention and acceptance because it addressed the practical aspects of using color. Jacques Christophe Le Blon, Van Dyck Self Portrait. Three-color mezzotint, 61.2 x 36.0 cm., c. 1720s. Yale Center for British Art, Paul Mellon Collection.
  • 38. The CMYK color space used in printing today is derived from his work.
  • 39. Moses Harris (1731–1785) was an entomologist and engraver who was fascinated by the color of the insects he studied.
  • 40. He published the first known color circle in 1766.
  • 41. Harris believed that red, yellow, and blue were the most different from each other and should be placed at the greatest possible distances apart on the circle.
  • 42. To accomplish this, he discarded Newton’s indigo and created an expanded color circle based on equal intervals of color and multiples of three.
  • 44. Johann Wolfgang von Goethe was fascinated by color.
  • 45. Goethe spent a great deal of energy trying to prove that Newton’s color theories were wrong.
  • 46. He rejected Newton’s assertion that color comes from light and insisted it was an experienced reality. Goethe’s Color Wheel
  • 47. He also proposed notions about the association of color and beauty with morality. He even said that there were sinful and chaste colors. Goethe’s Color Wheel
  • 48. Goethe was first to recognize the importance of the interaction between complementary colors. He called them “completing colors.”
  • 49. Goethe also reported extensively on simultaneous contrast and afterimage.
  • 50. Otto Philip Runge (1770–1840) shared Goethe’s belief in the importance of complementary colors.
  • 51. Runge was a painter and developed the first three dimensional model for color, the Color Sphere.
  • 52. It was published in a book he wrote called Farken Kugel.
  • 53. Goethe’s observations were so wide-ranging and fundamental that almost every concept in modern color study can be found in his writing.
  • 54. Goethe’s most familiar contribution to color study is the six-hue color circle.
  • 55. Even though he first believed that the only primaries were blue and yellow!!!
  • 56. Goethe’s French contemporary was Michel Eugene Chevreul (1786–1889).
  • 57. Chevreul was Master of the Gobelin Tapestry Works and was mostly concerned with the practical difficulties with producing consistent dyes.
  • 58. He accepted the three primary colors theory and also observed the phenomenon of simultaneous contrast. Chevreul at age 100 in his studio-laboratory.
  • 59. His 1839 treatise had a profound influence on the Impressionist movement in painting.
  • 60. The battle between Goethe’s six-hue spectrum and Newton’s seven-color model was unnecessary because both are valid but describe different reality:
  • 61. Newton was looking at causes .
  • 62. Goethe was looking at effects .
  • 63. In the past, science students and art students were not usually the same students, so the difference in the two ideas rarely came into conflict.
  • 64. Today’s designers and students of design must understand both cause and effect and be able to work within–and between–both realities.
  • 65. Ostwald Munsell After Goethe and Chevreul, most of the late 19th and early 20th century color theorists worked in rigidly formal scientific systems.
  • 66. Ostwald Munsell The stress was on rules, control and order: the goal was to create a comprehensive color-order system and to find within it immutable laws of color harmony.
  • 67. Albert Munsell (1858–1918) developed his 3-dimensional color model in 1921.
  • 68. Published in A Grammar of Color, Munsell proposed a “color tree” with infinite room for expansion.
  • 69. Munsell’s color space is constructed as progressive intervals of hue that rotate around a vertical axis of value from black to white.
  • 70. In Munsell’s theory every possible color cannot be shown but each has an assigned place on an alphanumeric scale.
  • 71. Chroma is Munsell’s word for saturation.
  • 72. The problem with this system is that it does not take into account the tertiaries . Nowhere on the color tree can you find colors mixed with their complements to reduce saturation.
  • 73. In the Munsell System, reduction of saturation is achieved by mixing the hues with gray in graduated steps.
  • 74. Color numbering systems have great value when they are used to communicate color information between individuals who have reference to the same set of standards.
  • 75. But these systems are meaningless as an aid to understanding color.
  • 76. Munsell was not immune to the moralizing associations of his predecessors asserting that certain colors implied good taste and others poor taste.
  • 77. His system is still in use today, but without the original commentary.
  • 78. A hypothetical color solid (or color displayed in 3- dimensions) is a frequent theme in scholarly color-order systems.
  • 79. German chemist Wilhelm Ostwald (1853–1932) brought the conceptual color solid to full-blown theory in Color Science.
  • 80. The Color Primer with its eight-hue spectrum became mandatory for color study in German schools and in many English ones.
  • 81. It was a strong influence on artists of the Bauhaus movement.
  • 82. Wilhelm von Bezold (1837–1932) contributed scientific fact to the growing body of color writing.
  • 83. Bezold is best known for the “Bezold Effect” which says that forms enclosed by light lines appear lighter and vice versa.
  • 84. Ludwig Von Helmholtz (1821– 1894) also contributed to color studies.
  • 85. He is known for his mathematics of the eye, the ories of vision, ideas on the visual perception of space, and his color vision research.
  • 86. By the early 20th century, color study had become an enormous and wide-ranging topic, positioned uncomfortably with one foot in the sciences and other in the arts.
  • 87. It remained for the artists and designers of the Bauhaus to end this ambiguity.
  • 88. The Bauhaus was a design group founded in 1919 by German architect Walter Gropius (1883–1969).
  • 89. The Bauhaus group brought the study of color to a level of attention not seen since Goethe’s challenge to Newton.
  • 90. These Bauhaus master students of color and color theory approached color from new directions with intelligence, wit, and energy...
  • 97. These artists and writers made the definitive break between the study of color as science and the study of color as art and aesthetics.
  • 98. Johannes Itten (1888–1967) followed Goethe in exploring color as a series of contrast systems and opposing forces.
  • 99. He theorized seven contrasts of color based on perceptions alone:
  • 100. Contrast of: saturation hue value warm & cool contrast complementary contrast simultaneous contrast extension (area)
  • 101. Itten codified color harmonies as a series of chords based on the complementary relationship and diagramed them as geometric forms.
  • 102. Itten’s approach, although mathematically based, is much less rigid than many that preceded it.
  • 103. Itten’s focus was as much on individual perception as on mathematical relationships.
  • 104. His major work is entitled, The Art of Color
  • 105. Color-order systems were the first concern of theorists because a formal system establishes a structured field in which to search for laws of color harmony.
  • 106. The primary focus of that search was on the relationship between hues. Value and saturation took a back seat to hue.
  • 107. Among all the major figures in color study, there was agreement that balance between complementary colors was the first principle of color harmony.
  • 108. The ancient ideal of mathematical balance was so much a part of the search for laws of harmony that hues were frequently associated with numbers or geometric forms.
  • 109. Arthur Schopenhauer (1788–1860) theorized that equal light- reflectance in spectrum colors is inherently harmonious.
  • 110. Schopenhauer’s Circle of Color Harmony is made up of unequal arcs. Each complementary pair is meant to be equal in light-reflectance to each of the other two pairs.
  • 111. Every color is assigned a number representing its light-reflectance (or value) in relation to the others.
  • 112. The total of all the numbers added together is 36, or 360 degrees, a full circle.
  • 113. Schopenhauer’s theory can be deceptive; a large area of violet does not necessarily reflect the same amount of light as a smaller area of yellow.
  • 114. But we do sense value differences between pure colors.
  • 115. Schopenhauer’s theory can be illustrated as striped tee shirts. In order for each shirt to be harmonious, the complementary pairs must have different ratios: 1 to 1, 1 to 2, and 1 to 3.
  • 116. In Itten’s quest for color harmony, he superimposed geometric forms (squares, rectangles, triangles, and hexagons) over the artists’ spectrum to demonstrate what he called “harmonious chords.”
  • 117. Each color chord illustrates complementary colors in some measurable proportion.
  • 118. The geometric points are called the “notes” and no “chord” strays from the complementary relationship.
  • 119. The person who made the final break with the color-order tradition was Josef Albers (1888–1976), a colleague of Itten’s who also taught at the Bauhaus school.
  • 120. Albers fled Nazi Germany in the early 1930’s with his wife, the weaver Anni Albers.
  • 121. They taught first at the Black Mountain School in North Carolina and later at Yale.
  • 122. Albers became the most influential name in color theory in the United States.
  • 123. However, his 1963 book Interaction of Colors contained nothing like the usual charts or systems.
  • 124. Albers taught that true understanding of color comes from an intuitive approach to studio exercises.
  • 125. He stressed the instability and relativity of perceived colors and the power of visual training.
  • 126. At the same time, he taught that even within this unstable field, there are effects that can be predicted and controlled.
  • 127. In Interaction of Colors, Albers casually discounts the generations of theory that preceded him.
  • 128. “This book...reverses this order and places practice before theory, which is, after all, the conclusion of practice,” he wrote.
  • 129. Albers was not the first to recognize that the visual experience, more than conscious choice, determines how we perceive colors, but he was the first to assert the primacy of the visual experience over structure or intellectual considerations.
  • 130. For Albers, the visual experience, not theory, was paramount.
  • 131. The late 20th century saw the focus of color study move from philosophical inquiry to a greater interest in psychological and motivational effects of colors.
  • 132. There is an entire industry, for example, that is devoted to determining current and future consumer preferences in colors and color combinations.
  • 133. At the same time, color theorists continue to search for absolutes.
  • 134. There is an enduring assumption–or perhaps, a hope–that those elusive, timeless, and absolute laws for pleasing combinations of colors really do exist and simply await discovery.