SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161
DOI 10.1007/s00221-009-1901-7

 R ES EA R C H A R TI CLE



Can illusory deviant stimuli be used as attentional distractors
to record vMMN in a passive three stimulus oddball paradigm?
Maria Flynn · Alki Liasis · Mark Gardner ·
Stewart Boyd · Tony Towell




Received: 23 September 2008 / Accepted: 9 June 2009 / Published online: 24 June 2009
© Springer-Verlag 2009


Abstract A passive three stimulus oddball paradigm was                 an embedded active attention task, we conWrmed the exis-
used to investigate Visual Mismatch Negativity (vMMN) a                tence of an earlier (150–170 ms) and attenuated vMMN.
component of the Event Related Potential (ERP) believed                Recordings from an intracranial case study conWrmed sepa-
to represent a central pre-attentive change mechanism.                 ration of N1 and discrimination components to posterior
Responses to a change in orientation were recorded to                  and anterior occipital areas, respectively. We conclude that
monochrome stimuli presented to subjects on a computer                 although the illusory Wgure captured spatial attention in its
screen. One of the infrequent stimuli formed an illusory               own right it did not draw suYcient attentional resources
Wgure (Kanizsa Square) aimed to capture spatial attention              from the standard–deviant comparison as revealed when
in the absence of an active task. Nineteen electrodes (10–20           using a concurrent active task.
system) were used to record the electroencephalogram in
fourteen subjects (ten females) mean age 34.5 years. ERPs              Keywords Event-related potential · Visual mismatch
to all stimuli consisted of a positive negative positive com-          negativity · Kanizsa Wgure · Orientation
plex recorded maximally over lateral occipital areas. The
negative component was greater for deviant and illusory
deviant compared to standard stimuli in a time window of               Introduction
170–190 ms. A P3a component over frontal/central elec-
trodes to the illusory deviant but not to the deviant stimulus         The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) is deWned as a compo-
suggests the illusory Wgure was able to capture attention              nent of the Event Related Potential (ERP) that can be
and orientate subjects to the recording. Subtraction wave-             evoked to stimulus change in the absence of attention. The
forms revealed visual discrimination responses at occipital            MMN is usually elicited when a deviant stimulus is pre-
electrodes, which may represent vMMN. In a control study               sented within a sequence of standard stimuli. The auditory
with 13 subjects (11 females; mean age 29.23 years), using             MMN has been identiWed as a negative deXection usually
                                                                       peaking at 150–200 ms from change onset and is related to
                                                                       automatic discrimination processing and sensory memory
M. Flynn · A. Liasis · M. Gardner · T. Towell (&)
                                                                       mechanisms (see Näätänen et al. 2005, 1997; Schröger
Department of Psychology, University of Westminster,
309 Regent Street, London W1B 2UW, UK                                  1997, for reviews).
e-mail: towella@wmin.ac.uk                                                 A number of studies have identiWed visual Mismatch
                                                                       Negativity (vMMN), as a negative deXection 100–250 ms
A. Liasis
                                                                       post-stimulus change onset (see Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003
Department of Ophthalmology,
Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children,                             for a review). These studies have reported vMMN either to
London WC1N 3JH, UK                                                    ‘match’ and ‘non-match’ tasks where the stimuli are pre-
                                                                       sented with equiprobability to control the eVects of global
S. Boyd
                                                                       stimulus presentation (Fu et al. 2003) for changes in orien-
Department of Clinical Neurophysiology,
Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children,                             tation and spatial frequency (Kimura et al. 2006) for
London WC1N 3JH, UK                                                    changes in spatial frequency), or within an oddball


                                                                                                                         123
154                                                                                             Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161


paradigm, whereby a variety of dimensions of the visual           Marsalek 2003; Polich 2003; Hagen et al. 2006 for
stimulus that are known to be important in early visual           reviews). The P3 can be further divided into the subcompo-
processing are manipulated. These include changes in spa-         nents P3a and P3b. P3a originates from frontal attention
tial frequency (Maekawa et al. 2005), motion (Kremlacek           mechanisms to task novelty and/or distractors whilst the
et al. 2006), colour (Czigler et al. 2004; Czigler et al. 2002;   P3b is generated in more temporal/parietal regions and is
Horimoto et al. 2002), form (Berti and Schroger 2004;             associated with context updating and memory storage oper-
Besle et al. 2005; Stagg et al. 2004) and orientation             ations (Polich 2007). We therefore set out to validate the
(Astikainen et al. 2004, 2008; Czigler and Csibra 1992).          use of illusory deviant stimuli in orienting attention in a
   Similar to the auditory MMN the vMMN is thought to             passive and active task in the context of a vMMN para-
reXect the memory based detection of deviant stimuli rather       digm. It was predicted that in the passive paradigm with no
than refractoriness (see Czigler et al. 2007 for a detailed       task conditions that discrimination components possibly
discussion). However, in contrast to the correlation seen         reXecting MMN would be evoked by both the deviant and
between auditory MMN and behavioural detection of devi-           illusory deviant stimuli while a P3a component would only
ants (Winkler et al. 1993) there appears to be no such rela-      be evident to illusory deviant stimuli that captured attention.
tionship in the visual modality. The amplitude of vMMN                To test the use of the illusory deviant stimulus in orien-
does not increase beyond 40 ms stimulus onset asynchrony          tating attention from the standard–deviant discrimination a
(SOA) of a masking stimulus whilst detection performance          control study was carried out containing an embedded
of deviant stimuli and RT improve up to 174 ms SOA                active task. Last, the generator sources of the visual ERP
(Czigler et al. 2007). These Wndings strongly suggest that        components were explored using intracranial recordings in
overt detection of visual deviance is not the sole mechanism      a subject undergoing presurgical evaluation for epilepsy
underlying vMMN.                                                  surgery.
   In comparison to other ERP change components, such as
N2 and P3, the MMN can be elicited in the absence of
attention (Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003). Therefore, in order to      Methods
diVerentiate between MMN and other ERP change compo-
nents the subjects’ attention is typically drawn away from        Participants
the test stimuli, employing a variety of behavioural tasks.
For example, Stagg et al. (2004) and Tales et al. (1999)          Study 1 and 2
required participants to press a button in response to target
stimuli, Astikainen et al. (2004) used an auditory distrac-       With ethical approval and informed consent 14 healthy
tion task whereby participants were required to focus their       adults (mean age 34.5 § 8.6 years (10 females) were
attention on counting the number of words in a story whilst       recruited for study 1 and 13 healthy adults (mean age
being presented with visual stimuli.                              29.23 § 8.8 years (11 females) for study 2. Subjects
   It has generally been understood that a concurrent active      reported no history of neurological disease and had normal
task is mandatory in eliciting vMMN to control for the            or corrected-to-normal visual acuity.
eVects of attention so that resources are allocated away
from the standard–deviant discrimination towards the              Study 3
active task (Heslenfeld 2003; Czigler 2007). However, not
all patient populations can meet the demands of an active         With hospital ethical approval and patient and parental con-
task. Therefore, in the present study a three-stimulus pas-       sent, a 15-year-old male with focal epilepsy undergoing
sive oddball paradigm was developed. Stimuli diVered with         pre-surgical evaluation for resection of a R anterior parietal
regard to orientation of local endline type pacman Wgures         lesion provided the opportunity to examine whether there
and their information/entropy content. So in addition to          was a dissociation of detection and discrimination compo-
standard and deviant stimuli, an infrequent illusory deviant      nents of the visual ERP.
stimulus was introduced in order to investigate the eVects
of attention. The illusory deviant stimulus was a Kanizsa         Stimuli and procedure
Wgure (Kanizsa 1976) which formed an illusory square, a
salient event thought to demand attention to reconstruct          Study 1
contours that are absent from visual images (Kaiser et al.
2004).                                                            Three monochrome endline type stimuli based on pacman
   A consistent Wnding in ERP research is that the P3 wave,       Wgures were employed in a behaviourally silent oddball
a positive deXection occurring from 280 to 400 ms post-           paradigm where the ratio of standards to deviants and illu-
stimulus indicates attentional processing (see Hruby and          sory deviants was 8:1:1. The stimuli in (Fig. 1a), diVered

123
Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161                                                                                         155


Fig. 1 a Stimuli presented in               a)                                b)         -8µV                c)
oddball paradigm with pseudo-
                                                                         N1
random sequence of 8:1:1,
respectively. (i) standard, (ii)      i)                       O1                                  O2
deviant and (iii) illusory deviant                                                              100ms
forming a Kanizsa square.
b, c Grand average waveforms
referenced to Fz at O1 and O2,
respectively for (i) Standard                                                 P2
                                                                    P1
(dashed line), deviant (solid        ii)
line) and illusory deviant stimuli
(dotted line) (ii) Deviant minus
standard (iii) Illusory deviant
minus standard. Note the dis-
crimination responses in ii) and
iii) with an additional negative
component in (iii) corresponding     iii)
to an inverted P3




from each other only in terms of the orientation of elements      In study 3, the two blocks of the stimuli were presented
(which were oriented unsystematically around their axes        with no embedded active attention task.
for the standard and deviant stimuli and formed an illusory
Kanizsa Wgure for the illusory deviant stimulus. The stimuli   Electroencephalogram recording and data analysis
were generated employing STIM software (Neuroscan-
STIM version 4; Compumedics USA, Ltd., El Paso, TX,            Study 1 and 2
USA) and presented on a computer screen subtending 4°.
The stimuli appeared on the screen for 400 ms with an          Nineteen silver–silver chloride electrodes were used to
inter-stimulus interval of 600 ms. Subjects were seated        record the electroencephalogram (EEG) activity and were
comfortably in a darkened room 1 m away from the screen        positioned at sites in accordance with the International 10–
and requested to Wxate on a small red dot in the centre of     20 system (Fz, F3, F4, Cz, C3, C4, T3, T4, Pz, P3, P4, Oz,
the screen that was present throughout recording. Within       O1, O2, T5, T6, VEOG, M1, M2). The reference electrode
the oddball paradigm stimuli were presented in a pseudo-       and the ground electrode were placed at the right and left
random sequence ensuring that deviant and illusory deviant     mastoid, respectively. An electrode was placed above the
stimuli were interspersed with standard stimuli. In study 1,   left eye to enable online artefact rejection of eye blinks.
the stimuli were presented in Wve blocks of 225 stimuli with   Continuous EEG was collected using Neuroscan-SCAN
up to a minute break between blocks. At the end of the odd-    version 4.3; Compumedics USA, Ltd., El Paso, TX, USA at
ball recording blocks of 64 deviants and illusory deviants     a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz, with a low pass of 100 Hz and
‘alone’ were presented.                                        a high pass of 1 Hz and stored on a computer for oZine
                                                               analysis.
Study 2                                                           Continuous EEG data were epoched oZine ¡100 ms
                                                               pre-stimulus to +500 ms post-stimulus. The epochs were
The same stimuli and procedure as in Study 1 were utilized     digitally Wltered with a band pass 1–30 Hz and baseline cor-
with the exception that an active attention task was embed-    rected. Epochs containing transients greater than §150 V
ded in the three stimulus oddball paradigm. Within the         were excluded from further analysis. For each subject, ERPs
blocks of 225 stimuli, during the interstimulus interval       were averaged separately for standard, deviant and illusory
(ISI), a small red square replaced the small red Wxation dot   deviant stimuli employing Fz as a reference and grand aver-
on 22 trials chosen at random. The red square appeared at      age waveforms were constructed. Additional ERPs were
the start of the 600 ms ISI and stayed on the screen for       constructed in study 2 for the red Wxation dot and for the red
200 ms. Subjects were instructed to focus their attention on   square that replaced the Wxation dot on a number of trials.
the red Wxation dot and press the right button of a mouse as   ERPs to standard stimuli were constructed from epochs that
quickly as possible whenever the red square appeared.          preceded deviant stimuli. As in previous studies (Stagg et al.
Inclusion criteria were based on participants achieving 90%    2004; Tales et al. 1999), averaged mastoids were employed
or more correct responses, excluding false positives.          as a reference to investigate P3 activity.

                                                                                                                  123
156                                                                                                Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161


   From the grand average waveforms MMN-like diVer-               Table 1 Mean ERP amplitude ( V) and standard deviation (SD) for
ences were identiWed on the basis of known negative polar-        each stimulus type at electrode sites for the 170–190 ms time window
ity, known emergence over posterior electrode positions           for the passive task (n = 14)
and typical latency range (100–250 ms post-stimulus: Pazo-        Electrode       Mean amplitude ( V) and standard
Alvarez et al. 2003). In each study, the maximal diVerence        site            deviation (§SD)
between ERPs to standards and deviants was identiWed at                           Stimulus
occipital sites and a 20 ms time window was centred at this
                                                                                  Standard         Deviant           Illusory deviant
latency for electrodes P3, P4, O1, O2, T5, T6 (Astikainen
et al. 2008). Mean amplitudes for the time windows were           O1              2.98 § 2.32      5.67 § 3.36       7.10 § 4.10
calculated relative to the mean voltage of a 100 ms pre-          O2              3.11 § 2.73      5.66 § 3.38       7.33 § 4.38
stimulus baseline for each participant for the standard, devi-    P3              1.99 § 1.68      3.75 § 2.85       5.03 § 3.49
ant and illusory deviant stimuli. The mean amplitudes were        P4              1.90 § 1.91      3.43 § 2.27       5.08 § 3.12
analysed using ANOVA. In addition, subtraction wave-              T5              2.36 § 2.03      4.59 § 2.75       5.42 § 3.07
forms were constructed of deviant minus standard and illu-        T6              2.62 § 1.83      4.70 § 2.24       5.13 § 2.75
sory deviant minus standard.

Study 3                                                           P < 0.001], indicating that the amplitude of the deviant
                                                                  stimulus was greater than the standard stimulus at occipital
The patient was implanted with a 32-contact sub-dural plat-       (t = 4.004; df = 13; P = 0.002) and temporal electrodes
inum grid straddling the parietal and pre-motor gyri and a        (t = 4.552; df = 13; P = 0.001) and that the amplitude of the
6-contact strip extending posteriorly over the inferior parie-    illusory deviant was greater than the standard at occipital
tal cortex such that the most distal contact (S1) overlay the     (t = 4.507; df = 13; P = 0.001), temporal (t = 4.552;
R occipital cortex (Fig. 3a).                                     df = 13; P = 0.001) and parietal electrodes (t = 4.276;
                                                                  df = 13; P = 0.001).
                                                                      DiVerence waveforms of deviant minus standard and
Results                                                           illusory deviant minus standard both revealed vMMN com-
                                                                  ponents (Fig. 1b, c). When comparing the deviant to the
Study 1                                                           standard ERP, using the point-by-point t test algorithm
                                                                  (P < 0.05; one-tailed) against baseline there were signiW-
A visual response was recorded in all subjects in all trials      cant diVerences at O1 between 173 and 217 ms (181–
consisting of a P1–N1–P2 waveform. Grand average wave-            203 ms; P < 0.01) and at O2 between 178 and 208 ms
forms were constructed for the standard, deviant and illu-        (185–196 ms); P < 0.01). Comparing the illusory deviant to
sory deviant stimuli (see Fig. 1 for waveforms at O1 and          the standard ERP against baseline, there were signiWcant
O2). The maximal diVerence between ERPs to standards              diVerences (P < 0.05; one-tailed) at O1 between 164 and
and deviants was at approximately 180 ms post-stimulus at         212 ms (175–203 ms; P < 0.01) and at O2 between 169 and
occipital electrodes. A 20 ms time window was centred at          212 ms (181–199 ms; P < 0.01).
this latency for electrodes O1, O2, P3, P4, T5, T6 and, for           Illusory deviant stimuli evoked an additional late nega-
each participant, mean amplitudes for this time window cal-       tive component at 234 ms at Oz. To examine whether this
culated relative to the mean voltage of a 100 ms pre-stimu-       component corresponded to an inverted P3 component the
lus baseline for standards, deviants and illusory deviants.       waveforms were re-referenced to averaged mastoids. We
Mean amplitudes and standard deviations for the standard,         were able to reveal a positive component over the fronto-
deviant and illusory deviant are shown in Table 1.                central electrode sites corresponding to P3a. At Fz this
    A three-way within subjects ANOVA was used to ana-            component had an onset latency of 244 ms, SD = 13 ms
lyse the mean amplitude data in the 170–190 ms time win-          and a peak latency of 290 ms, SD = 27 ms with a peak
dow. Pairwise comparison of means was carried out using           amplitude of 4.19 V, SD = 2.06 V.
bonferroni corrected t tests. Factors were location (occipi-          To examine whether the diVerences observed in the sub-
tal, parietal, temporal), hemisphere (left, right) and stimulus   traction waveforms were confounded by pure stimulus
(standard, deviant and illusory deviant). The amplitude           diVerences we compared the discrimination waveform to
diVered signiWcantly with location [F(2,26) = 11.880;             the deviant stimulus to the discrimination waveform when
P < 0.001] and stimulus type [F(2,26) = 15.886; P < 0.001]        that same stimulus was presented alone, i.e. out of context
but not with hemisphere [F(1,13) = 0.233; P = 0.794].             and not in an oddball paradigm. Point-by-point t tests
There was a statistically signiWcant interaction between          revealed no signiWcant diVerences between the deviant–
location and stimulus [F(4.52) = 6.503; P = 0.001,                standard and deviant alone-deviant waveforms suggesting

123
Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161                                                                                                           157


that when the deviant stimulus was presented alone and out                 Table 2 Mean ERP amplitude ( V) and standard deviation for each
of context it behaved in a similar way to the standard stimu-              stimulus type at electrode sites at 150–170 ms for the active task
lus even though it was physically diVerent. The same proce-                (n = 13)
dure was used to compare the illusory deviant stimulus in                  Electrode      Mean amplitude ( V) and standard
the context of an oddball paradigm with the illusory deviant               site           deviation (§SD)
stimulus presented alone. Similarly, there were no signiW-                                Stimulus
cant diVerences between the illusory deviant–standard and
                                                                                          Standard         Deviant           Illusory deviant
illusory deviant alone-illusory deviant waveforms.
                                                                           O1             4.12 § 2.22      5.12 § 2.10       7.24 § 2.79
Study 2                                                                    O2             5.20 § 3.40      6.25 § 3.43       9.27 § 5.20
                                                                           P3             2.32 § 1.76      2.79 § 1.83       3.51 § 2.22
As in study 1, a visual response was recorded for all sub-                 P4             3.50 § 2.33      4.07 § 2.01       5.56 § 3.28
jects consisting of a P1–N1–P2 waveform. Grand average                     T5             3.23 § 1.75      4.18 § 1.82       5.20 § 2.00
waveforms were constructed for the standard, deviant and                   T6             4.90 § 2.35      5.6 4 § 2.36      7.48 § 3.92
illusory deviant stimuli (see Fig. 2 for waveforms at O1 and
O2). The maximal diVerence between ERPs to standards
and deviants was at approximately 160 ms post-stimulus at                  mean voltage of a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline for stan-
occipital sites. A 20-ms time window was centred at this                   dards, deviants and illusory deviants for each participant.
latency for electrodes O1, O2, P3, P4, T5, T6 and mean                     Mean amplitudes and standard deviations for the standard,
amplitudes for this time window calculated relative to the                 deviant and illusory deviant are shown in Table 2.
                                                                               A three-way within subjects ANOVA was used to ana-
                                                                           lyse the mean amplitude data of the 150–170 ms time win-
                                                                           dow. Pairwise comparison of means was carried out using
          O1                                   O2
                 N1                                                        bonferroni corrected t tests. Factors were location (occipi-
                                                                           tal, parietal, temporal), hemisphere (left, right) and stimulus
                                                                           (standard, deviant and illusory deviant). The amplitude
                                                                           diVered signiWcantly with location [F(2,24) = 16.874;
a)                                                                         P < 0.001], hemisphere [F(2,24) = 7.059; P = 0.021] and
                                                                           stimulus type [F(2,24) = 14.254; P < 0.001]. There was a
            P1
                       P2
                                                                           signiWcant interaction between location and stimulus
                                                                           [F(4.48) = 10.636; P < 0.001] indicating that the amplitude
                                                                           of the deviant stimulus was greater than the standard stimu-
                                                                           lus at occipital (t = 3.796; df = 12; P = 0.003) and temporal
                                    -8µV
                                                                           (t = 3.147; df = 12; P = 0.008) electrodes. The amplitude of
 b)                                                                        the illusory deviant stimulus was greater than the standard
                                                                           stimulus at occipital (t = 4.494; df = 12; P = 0.001), tempo-
                                           100ms                           ral (t = 4.425; df = 12; P = 0.001) and parietal (t = 4.105;
                                                                           df = 12; P = 0.001) electrodes. There was a signiWcant
                                                                           interaction between hemisphere and stimulus [F(2,24) =
c)                                                                         3.402; P = 0.050] indicating that in the left hemisphere the
                                                                           mean amplitude was greater for the deviant (t = 4.194;
                                                                           df = 12; P = 0.001) and illusory deviant (t = 5.536; df = 12;
                                                                           P < 0.001) than for the standard. In the right hemisphere the
                                                                           mean amplitude of the illusory deviant (t = 5.944; df = 12;
 d)                                                                        P < 0.001) was greater than the standard as was the deviant
                      P3                                                   but to a lesser extent (t = 2.952; df = 12; P = 0.012).
                                                                               DiVerence waveforms of deviant minus standard and
Fig. 2 a Grand average waveforms referenced to Fz for standard             illusory deviant minus standard both revealed attenuated
(dashed line), deviant (solid line) and illusory deviant stimuli (dotted   vMMN components (Fig. 2b, c). When comparing the devi-
line) at O1 and O2. Note the P3a component seen only to illusory devi-     ant to the standard ERP, using the point-by-point t test
ant stimuli. b Deviant minus standard. c Illusory deviant minus stan-
                                                                           algorithm (P < 0.05; one-tailed) against baseline, there
dard. d Grand average waveforms for the rarely occurring red Wxation
square (solid line) and for the central Wxation dot (dotted line). Note:   were signiWcant diVerences at O1 between 138 and 176 ms
the attenuated vMMN in b and the P3b wave to the task in d                 but no signiWcant diVerences were apparent at O2. When


                                                                                                                                 123
158                                                                                                      Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161


comparing the illusory deviant to the standard ERP against                  mismatch) were recorded more anteriorly at S3 and S4 and
baseline, there were signiWcant diVerences (P < 0.05; one-                  were characterised by enhanced positivities at about 90 ms,
tailed) at O1 between 142 and 178 ms and at O2 between                      42.32 V and 219 ms, 87.21 V for S3 and enhanced
147 and 177 ms.                                                             positivities at 88 ms, 28.55 V and 237 ms, 45.93 for S4,
    Illusory deviant stimuli evoked an additional late nega-                either side of the major negative component (Fig. 3b).
tive component at occipital electrodes. When re-referenced                  Subtraction waveforms (deviant–standard) revealed dis-
to averaged mastoids this component was positive over the                   crimination responses with positive peaks at 86 and 219 ms
fronto-central electrode sites. At Fz this had an onset                     for electrode S3 and 93 and 233 ms at electrode S4. In
latency of 223 ms, SD = 18 ms and a peak latency of                         addition, a later positive response to the illusory deviant
282 ms, SD = 22 ms with a peak amplitude of 5.17 V,                         stimulus was seen at around 386 ms over pre-motor regions
SD = 2.72 V.                                                                (G9 > G17 and G18), suggesting activation of a frontal sys-
    All participants completed the active task (pressing the                tem to stimulus novelty and/or target detection (Fig. 3c).
mouse when the red Wxation dot was replaced with a red
Wxation square) within the limits of the inclusion criteria.
As expected, the active task evoked a P3b component                         Discussion
showing that the participants’ attention was engaged with
the task (See Fig. 2d).                                                     The main result from this study is that visual discrimination
                                                                            responses including vMMN components have been
Study 3: intracranial recording                                             recorded in a behaviourally silent oddball paradigm to a
                                                                            change in orientation. The stimuli utilised in this study
A negative positive negative complex was recorded maxi-                     evoked a response that was more negative to the deviant
mally to all stimuli at the most posterior electrode site (S1)              stimuli than to the standard stimuli in the period 150–
(Fig. 3b). The latency and amplitude of the Wrst major neg-                 200 ms after stimulus onset. Whilst we acknowledge that
ative component (N1) was similar for standard, deviant                      there were physical diVerences between the stimuli, and
stimuli and illusory deviant stimuli (153 ms and ¡32.39 V,                  that these changes were not equal between standard–devi-
153 ms and ¡48.54 V, 162 ms and ¡45.50 V, respec-                           ant and standard–illusory deviant comparisons, the employ-
tively). Responses to stimulus discrimination (visual                       ment of a ‘deviant alone’ and ‘illusory deviant alone’
                                                                            conditions served as controls. Subsequent subtraction
                                                                            waveforms using the subtraction method suggested by
                                                                            Kraus et al. (1995) for delineating the MMN reveals that
                                                                            the diVerence in negativity was attributable not to physical
                                                G32                         diVerences in the stimuli themselves, but by the context in
b)    s1                            a)                xx
                                                           ++
                                                                G17
                                                                 G9
                                                                 G1
                                                                            which the stimuli were presented.
      s2                                   S1
                                                 S6
                                                                                The presence of a P3a over frontal/central electrodes for
                                                                            the illusory deviant grand average waveform but not for the
      s3                                                                    standard or deviant grand average waveforms, suggests that
                                     c)
                        -30uV
                                          G9
                                                                            the Kanizsa square captured attention. Without the control
                            100ms
      s4
                                                                            task this would imply that the enhanced negativity exhib-
                                          G17                               ited by the deviant compared to the standard may not
      s5                                                                    depend on attention. However, the use of the illusory Wgure
                                          G18                               is supported by Senkowski et al. (2005) who found that
                                                                            Kanizsa Wgures automatically capture spatial attention
      s6



Fig. 3 a Co-registered sub-dural electrode locations, dotted eclipse
                                                                            when used as visual cues and Wallach and Slaughter (1988)
denotes surface visible lesion, x seizure onset zone, + somatosensory       who found that the familiarity of the illusory shape
ERP localised. b Standard and deviant waveforms from the six strip          increases the likelihood that the shape will be perceived. In
contacts—dashed waveform represents the standard ERP, solid wave-           addition, a number of clinical studies show that the
form the deviant. At S1 and S2 the illusory deviant waveform did not
diVer from the deviant or standard and no consistent changes were seen      response to Kanizsa Wgures is robust. In an ERP study,
at S3 and S4. For reasons of clarity the illusory deviant waveform is not   Grice et al. (2003) examined perceptual completion in par-
shown. Peak amplitude of the N1 and inverted discrimination compo-          ticipants with Williams Syndrome—a genetic disorder in
nent shown by the shaded area of the waveform. c Anterior grid elec-        which visuo-spatial performance is poor, and found that
trodes revealing a later positive response to the illusory deviant
stimulus shown by the shaded area of the waveform. Dashed, solid and        although the underlying neural mechanisms of the partici-
dotted lines represent ERPs to standard, deviant and illusory deviant       pants with Williams Syndrome may be diVerent to controls,
stimuli, respectively                                                       their ability to perceive illusory contours was apparently

123
Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161                                                                                          159


normal. Milne and Scope, in their 2007 study, suggested         of arc also reveals the contribution of the parvocellular sys-
that the perception of illusory contours in participants with   tem and ventral stream in detecting diVerences in the
Autistic Spectrum Disorder was intact.                          sequence of unattended central stimuli. The parvocellular
   Observation and statistical analysis of the waveforms        system is particularly adapted to colour and high-contrast
and the discrimination components reveals that the ampli-       black and white detailed information.
tude component N1 for the illusory deviant at the lateral          Besle et al. (2005) using the deformation of a circle as a
occipital electrodes was greater than for the standard or       deviant stimulus embedded within an active task presented
deviant stimuli. Many previous studies have demonstrated        within 2° of arc demonstrated bilateral vMMN responses at
an enhanced visual N1 amplitude component to attended-          216 ms being maximal at electrode PO3 and PO4. In an
location stimuli (see Vogel and Luck 2000 for a review)         active geometric shape discrimination task P1, N1 and P2
and this evidence further suggests that the illusory deviant    components were identiWed at 80, 140 and 200 ms, respec-
stimulus captured attention whilst the standard and deviant     tively, and the N1 and P2 components became less sharp
stimuli did not.                                                and more diVuse as stimulus presentation changed between
   As with several previous studies (e.g. Tales et al. 1999)    4°, 8° and 12° of arc (Shoji and Ozaki 2006).
we also engaged an active control task that required partici-      Extra deviant stimuli conceptulised as distractor stimuli
pants to press a button at the occurrence of a change in        have also been used in the auditory modality to manipulate
shape of the central Wxation dot. The understanding here is     attention. For instance, Schroger et al. (2000) and Schroger
that attentional resources are drawn from the standard–         and WolV (1998) in an auditory duration discrimination
deviant discrimination to the active task. Under these con-     task found that task irrelevant distractors in the form of
ditions we were able to conWrm the existence of vMMN            small changes in frequency prolonged reaction times and
responses although they were signiWcantly reduced in            elicited MMN and P3a components, reXecting orientation
amplitude. The underlying mechanisms of vMMN are still          towards the distractor.
to be resolved although a number of studies have suggested         Recordings from the intracranial case study support the
a memory based rather than refractoriness explanation (see      separation of detection and discrimination processes within
Czigler 2007). Such a visual based memory system would          the visual cortex. The N1 component at 153 ms located at
rely on the representation of regularity following repeated     the more posterior electrodes corresponds to the scalp
exposure to identical frequent stimuli. The violation of such   recorded N1 at 167 ms. The waveforms were similar for the
regularity following the presentation of a deviant stimulus     standard, deviant and illusory deviant stimuli. However, at
would elicit an enhanced posterior negativity commonly          adjacent posterior electrodes (S3 and S4) the deviant stim-
seen as vMMN in subtraction waveforms. However, in this         uli evoked early and later positivities that probably contrib-
model, it appears that longer sequences (10–15) of fre-         ute to the scalp recorded MMN. With respect to scalp
quent/standard and identical unattended stimuli will inXu-      recordings these potentials to stimulus discrimination are
ence the generation of vMMN (Czigler and Pato 2009). In         inverted in polarity and the Wrst positive component is seen
the current study, the median number of continuous stan-        relatively early at around 90 ms. These Wnding are consis-
dard sequences was 4 and this may account for the rela-         tent with an MEG study showing strong activation of the
tively low amplitude of the vMMN responses in study 2.          lateral occipital cortex at around 155 ms post-stimulus
The latency of the responses in the current study are consis-   (Halgren et al. 2003). In MEG studies comparison of illu-
tent within the general window for vMMN responses of            sory Kanizsa stimuli with control stimuli reveals activation
between 100 and 250 ms (Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003)               between 100 and 350 ms post-stimulus (Kaiser et al. 2004)
although it is known that latency and duration of vMMN          and at around 280 ms (Halgren et al. 2003). It is believed
will diVer according to stimulus characteristics and task       that illusory contour sensitivity may Wrst occur in middle to
complexity with less salient changes and more complex           higher order visual processing areas and that feedback
rules resulting in longer latency and less phasic vMMN          modulation from lateral occipital areas will activate V1 and
responses (Czigler et al. 2006).                                V2 areas (Kaiser et al. 2004).
   Previous studies on vMMN have tended to engage active           Early cortical processing in the visual cortex has also
tasks embedded in more peripheral areas of the visual Weld      been reported from MEG studies using Xash stimuli that
and one study speciWcally set out to assess the contribution    reach medial occipital areas around 47 ms (Inui and Kakigi
of the magnocellular system (Kremlacek et al. 2006). This       2006). Recent studies using intracranial recordings demon-
pathway forms the dorsal stream and is not sensitive to col-    strate activation in the superior parietal lobule at 75 ms to
our or detail but is thought to be responsible for pre-atten-   coloured disc stimuli presented in the macular Weld
tive detection of motion stimuli. Whilst in the present study   (Molholm et al. 2006) and recordings from the striate
we cannot exclude the contribution of the magnocellular         cortex to alternating stimuli have been reported as a P55
system, our Wndings of a vMMN in the macular Weld at 4°         followed by a more consistent N75 (Farrell et al. 2007).

                                                                                                                   123
160                                                                                                               Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161


    Polarity inversions between the cortex and the scalp can                   Czigler I, Csibra G (1992) Event-related potentials and the identiWca-
indicate local generator sources in that region of cortex. As                       tion of deviant visual stimuli. Psychophysiology 29:471–485
                                                                               Czigler I, Pato L (2009) Unnoticed regularity violation elicts change-
these scalp recorded N1 and MMN Welds are interactions of                           related brain activity. Biol Psychol 80:339–347
the super imposition of several bilateral generators it is                     Czigler I, Balázs L, Winkler I (2002) Memory-based detection of task-
diYcult to understand how focal intracranial potentials con-                        irrelevant visual changes. Psychophysiology 39:869–873
tribute to the scalp recorded N1 and MMN. The inverted                         Czigler I, Balázs L, Pató LG (2004) Visual change detection: event-
                                                                                    related potentials are dependent on stimulus location in humans.
biWd positive discrimination component may well represent                           Neurosci Lett 364:149–153
the existence of one or more local generator sources to                        Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I (2006) ERPs and deviance detection:
change detection. Complex and widespread activation has                             visual mismatch negativity to repeated visual stimuli. Neurosci
also been recorded to alternating and on/oV stimuli from the                        Lett 401:178–182
                                                                               Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I (2007) Backward masking and visual
striate cortex and visual association areas (Farrell et al.                         mismatch negativity: Electrophysiological evidence for memory-
2007) further supporting the view that it is diYcult to                             based detection of deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 44:610–619
entangle the generator sources that contribute to responses                    Farrell DF, Leeman S, Ojemann A (2007) Study of the human visual
measured at the scalp.                                                              cortex: direct cortical evoked potentials and stimulation. J Clin
                                                                                    Neurophysiol 24:1–10
    Intracerebral potentials to rare distractor visual and audi-               Fu S, Fan S, Chen L (2003) Event-related potentials reveal involuntary
tory stimuli have been recorded from frontal regions as a                           processing of orientation changes in the visual modality. Psycho-
widespread negative–positive–negative waveform at                                   physiology 40:770–775
approximate latencies of 210–280–390 ms, respectively                          Grice SJ, de Hahn M, Halit H, Johnson MH, Csibra G, Grant J,
                                                                                    KarmiloV-Smith A (2003) ERP abnormalities of illusory contour
(Baudena et al. 1995). It is believed that this waveform cor-                       perception in Williams Syndrome. Neuroreport 14:1773–1777
responds with the scalp recorded N2a/P3a/slow wave that is                     Hagen GF, Gatherwright JR, Lopez BA, Polich J (2006) P3a from
associated with orienting. In the present study the later pos-                      visual stimuli: task diYculty eVects. Int J Psychophysiol 59:8–14
itivity to the illusory deviant stimulus seen at around                        Halgren E, Dale AM, Mendola J, Chong CDR (2003) Cortical activa-
                                                                                    tion to illusory shapes as measured with magnetoencephalogra-
386 ms over pre-motor regions may correspond to this nov-                           phy. Neuroimage 18:1001–1009
elty orienting process.                                                        Heslenfeld DJ (2003) Visual mismatch negativity. In: Polich J (ed)
    In conclusion, we suggest that visual discrimination                            Detection of change: event-related potential and fMRI Wndings.
potentials containing vMMN components can be elicited                               Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 41–60
                                                                               Horimoto R, Inagaki M, Yano T, Sata Y, Kaga M (2002) Mismatch
using a paradigm with no task demands. The inclusion of                             negativity of the color modality during selective attention task to
an illusory square was intended to capture the subject’s                            auditory stimuli in children with mental retardation. Brain Dev
attention and therefore orientate them to the recording. The                        24:703–709
existence of attenuated vMMN when subjects engaged in                          Hruby T, Marsalek P (2003) Event-related potentials—the P3 Wave.
                                                                                    Acta Neurobiol Exp 63:55–63
an active distractor task supports the contention that the                     Inui K, Kakigi R (2006) Temporal analysis of the Xow from V1 to the
illusory square was unable to command all resources away                            extrastriate cortex in humans. J Neurophysiol 96:775–784
from the standard–deviant comparison.                                          Kaiser J, Bühler M, Lutzenberger W (2004) Magnetoencephalographic
                                                                                    gamma-band responses to illusory triangles in humans. Neuroim-
Acknowledgments We are grateful to the anonymous referees for                       age 23:551–560
their critical comments and suggestions for improvements on earlier            Kanizsa G (1976) Subjective contours. Sci Am 234:48–52
versions of the manuscript.                                                    Kimura M, Murohashi H, Ji Katayama (2006) An ERP study of visual
                                                                                    change detection: EVects of magnitude of spatial frequency
                                                                                    changes on the change-related posterior positivity. Int J Psycho-
                                                                                    physiol 62:14–23
References                                                                     Kraus N, Sharma A, McGee T, Carrell TD (1995) Neurophysiologic
                                                                                    bases of speech discrimination. Ear Hear 16:19–37
Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Wikgren J, Korhonen T (2004) The human             Kremlacek J, Kuba M, Kubova Z, Langrova J (2006) Visual mismatch
     brain processes visual changes that are not cued by attended audi-             negativity elicited by magnocellular system activation. Vision
     tory stimulation. Neurosci Lett 368:231–234                                    Res 46:485–490
Astikainen P, Lillstrang E, Ruusuvirta T (2008) Visual mismatch neg-           Maekawa T, Taniwaki T, Kinukawa N, Kanba S, Goto Y, Tobimatsu
     ativity for changes in orientation—a sensory memory-dependent                  S (2005) Functional characterization of mismatch negativity to a
     response. Eur J Neurosci 28:2319–2324                                          visual stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol 116:2392–2402
Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G, Clark JM (1995) Intracerebral potentials         Milne E, Scope A (2007) Are children with autistic spectrum disorders
     to rare target and distractor auditory and visula stimuli. III. Frontal        susceptible to contour illusions? Br J Dev Psychol 26:91–102
     cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 94:251–264                   Molholm S, Sehatpour P, Mehta AD, Shpaner M, Gomez-Ramirez M,
Berti S, Schroger E (2004) Distraction eVects in vision: behavioural                Ortigue S, Dyke JP, Schwartz TH, Foxe JJ (2006) Audio-visual
     and event-related potential indices. Neuroreport 15:665–669                    multisensory integration in superior parietal lobule revealed by
Besle J, Fort A, Giard MH (2005) Is the auditory sensory memory sen-                human intracranial recordings. J Neurophysiol 96:721–729
     sitive to visual information. Exp Brain Res 166:337–344                   Näätänen R, Lehtokoski A, Lennes M, Cheour M (1997) Language-
Czigler I (2007) Visual mismatch negativity. Violation of nonattended               speciWc phoneme representations revealed by electric and mag-
     environmental regularities. J Psychophysiol 21:224–230                         netic brain responses. Nature 385:432–434



123
Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161                                                                                                               161


Näätänen R, Jacobsen T, Winkler I (2005) Memory-based or aVerent          Senkowski D, Rottger S, Grimm S, Foxe JJ, Herrmann CS (2005) Kan-
     processes in mismatch negativity (MMN): a review of the evi-              izsa subjective Wgures capture visual spatial attention: evidence
     dence. Psychophysiology 42:25–32                                          from electrophysiological and behavioral data. Neuropsychologia
Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E (2003) MMN in the visual                43:872–886
     modality: a review. Biol Psychol 63:199–236                          Shoji H, Ozaki H (2006) Topographic change in ERP due to discrimi-
Polich J (2003) Theoretical overview of P3a and P3b. In: Polich J (ed)         nation of geometric Wgures in the peripheral visual Weld. Int J Psy-
     Detection of change: event-related potential and fMRI Wndings.            chophysiol 62:115–121
     Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 83–98                                          Stagg C, Hindley P, Tales A, Butler S (2004) Visual mismatch nega-
Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b.           tivity: the detection of stimulus change. Neuroreport 15:659–663
     Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128–2148                                      Tales A, Newton P, Troscianko T, Butler S (1999) Mismatch negativ-
Schröger E (1997) On the detection of auditory deviations: a pre-atten-        ity in the visual modality. Neuroreport 10:3363–3367
     tive activation model. Psychophysiology 34:245–257                   Vogel EK, Luck SJ (2000) The visual N1 component as an index of a
Schroger E, WolV C (1998) Attentional orienting and reorienting is             discrimination process. Psychophysiology 37:190–203
     indicated by human event-related brain potentials. Neuroreport       Wallach H, Slaughter V (1988) The role of memory in perceiving sub-
     9:3355–3358                                                               jective contours. Percept Psychophys 43:101–106
Schroger E, Giard MH, WolV C (2000) Auditory distraction: event-re-       Winkler I, Reinikainen K, Naatanen R (1993) Event-related brain
     lated potential and behavioral indices. Clin Neurophysiol                 potentials reXect traces of echoic memory in humans. Percept
     111:1450–1460                                                             Psychophys 53:443–449




                                                                                                                                      123

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Pokrovskii Reading
Pokrovskii ReadingPokrovskii Reading
Pokrovskii Readingguest63a177
 
142 Presentation
142 Presentation142 Presentation
142 PresentationJferSanders
 
Bobrovnikov Reading
Bobrovnikov ReadingBobrovnikov Reading
Bobrovnikov Readingguest63a177
 
Glass and Ceramic Works
Glass and Ceramic WorksGlass and Ceramic Works
Glass and Ceramic WorksNandini Datta
 
Daffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkDaffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkNandini Datta
 
Daffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkDaffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkNandini Datta
 
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09Evaluating Websites ULearn 09
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09P H
 
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...guest5e4294
 
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2Tsendsuren Munkhdalai
 
Software for Learning - ULearn09
Software for Learning - ULearn09Software for Learning - ULearn09
Software for Learning - ULearn09P H
 
Hargobind Khorana
Hargobind KhoranaHargobind Khorana
Hargobind KhoranaVRC Reddy
 
PPT 7th grade math
PPT 7th grade mathPPT 7th grade math
PPT 7th grade mathSara
 

Viewers also liked (17)

Pokrovskii Reading
Pokrovskii ReadingPokrovskii Reading
Pokrovskii Reading
 
142 Presentation
142 Presentation142 Presentation
142 Presentation
 
Rev
RevRev
Rev
 
Soccer09
Soccer09Soccer09
Soccer09
 
Bobrovnikov Reading
Bobrovnikov ReadingBobrovnikov Reading
Bobrovnikov Reading
 
Glass and Ceramic Works
Glass and Ceramic WorksGlass and Ceramic Works
Glass and Ceramic Works
 
Daffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkDaffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of Work
 
Daffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of WorkDaffodils Archive Of Work
Daffodils Archive Of Work
 
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09Evaluating Websites ULearn 09
Evaluating Websites ULearn 09
 
Soccer09
Soccer09Soccer09
Soccer09
 
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...
Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V M...
 
motivation
motivationmotivation
motivation
 
motivation
motivationmotivation
motivation
 
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2
Improvement of no sql technology for relational databases v2
 
Software for Learning - ULearn09
Software for Learning - ULearn09Software for Learning - ULearn09
Software for Learning - ULearn09
 
Hargobind Khorana
Hargobind KhoranaHargobind Khorana
Hargobind Khorana
 
PPT 7th grade math
PPT 7th grade mathPPT 7th grade math
PPT 7th grade math
 

Similar to Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V Mmn In A Passive Three Stimulus Oddball Paradigm

A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in the
A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in theA multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in the
A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in thefizyoloji12345
 
VEMPs in a Pediatric Population
VEMPs in a Pediatric PopulationVEMPs in a Pediatric Population
VEMPs in a Pediatric PopulationAndrew Fenn
 
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9Yesenia Castillo Salinas
 
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...Giuseppe Fineschi
 
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Taruna Ikrar
 
Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Jacob Sheu
 
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Dimitris Vlastos
 
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015Cormac Assink
 
Ben Turner - MRI workshop
Ben Turner -  MRI workshopBen Turner -  MRI workshop
Ben Turner - MRI workshopMS Trust
 
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docx
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docxAbnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docx
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docxdaniahendric
 
Final Progress Report- Farzana
Final Progress Report- FarzanaFinal Progress Report- Farzana
Final Progress Report- FarzanaFarzana Ali
 
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimaging
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric NeuroimagingThe Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimaging
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimagingwef
 
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awareness
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awarenessHaggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awareness
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awarenessTochy
 

Similar to Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V Mmn In A Passive Three Stimulus Oddball Paradigm (20)

The human thalamus is crucially involved in executive control operations
The human thalamus is crucially involved in executive control operationsThe human thalamus is crucially involved in executive control operations
The human thalamus is crucially involved in executive control operations
 
A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in the
A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in theA multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in the
A multimodal brain imaging study of repetition suppression in the
 
NCP_Thesis_Francesca_Bocca.pdf
NCP_Thesis_Francesca_Bocca.pdfNCP_Thesis_Francesca_Bocca.pdf
NCP_Thesis_Francesca_Bocca.pdf
 
VEMPs in a Pediatric Population
VEMPs in a Pediatric PopulationVEMPs in a Pediatric Population
VEMPs in a Pediatric Population
 
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
 
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...
Automatic eye fixations identification based on analysis of variance and cova...
 
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
Nogo receptor1confinesadisinhibitorymicrocircuittothe criticalperiodinvisualc...
 
Collier et al., 2014_SME
Collier et al., 2014_SMECollier et al., 2014_SME
Collier et al., 2014_SME
 
Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003Mc intosh 2003
Mc intosh 2003
 
Posner task results
Posner task resultsPosner task results
Posner task results
 
Journalcompneuro
JournalcompneuroJournalcompneuro
Journalcompneuro
 
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
 
SPRING WEBINAR WITH DR. BRUCE DONOFF
SPRING WEBINAR WITH DR. BRUCE DONOFFSPRING WEBINAR WITH DR. BRUCE DONOFF
SPRING WEBINAR WITH DR. BRUCE DONOFF
 
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015
Assink Lockwood Duffy poster 7-26-2015
 
Ben Turner - MRI workshop
Ben Turner -  MRI workshopBen Turner -  MRI workshop
Ben Turner - MRI workshop
 
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docx
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docxAbnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docx
Abnormal changes in cortical activity in women with migraine bet.docx
 
Final Progress Report- Farzana
Final Progress Report- FarzanaFinal Progress Report- Farzana
Final Progress Report- Farzana
 
Facial nerve palsy
Facial nerve palsyFacial nerve palsy
Facial nerve palsy
 
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimaging
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric NeuroimagingThe Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimaging
The Baby and the Bathwater: Signal and Noise in Psychiatric Neuroimaging
 
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awareness
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awarenessHaggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awareness
Haggard (2002) voluntary action and conscious awareness
 

Can Illusory Deviant Stimuli Be Used As Attentional Distractors To Record V Mmn In A Passive Three Stimulus Oddball Paradigm

  • 1. Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 DOI 10.1007/s00221-009-1901-7 R ES EA R C H A R TI CLE Can illusory deviant stimuli be used as attentional distractors to record vMMN in a passive three stimulus oddball paradigm? Maria Flynn · Alki Liasis · Mark Gardner · Stewart Boyd · Tony Towell Received: 23 September 2008 / Accepted: 9 June 2009 / Published online: 24 June 2009 © Springer-Verlag 2009 Abstract A passive three stimulus oddball paradigm was an embedded active attention task, we conWrmed the exis- used to investigate Visual Mismatch Negativity (vMMN) a tence of an earlier (150–170 ms) and attenuated vMMN. component of the Event Related Potential (ERP) believed Recordings from an intracranial case study conWrmed sepa- to represent a central pre-attentive change mechanism. ration of N1 and discrimination components to posterior Responses to a change in orientation were recorded to and anterior occipital areas, respectively. We conclude that monochrome stimuli presented to subjects on a computer although the illusory Wgure captured spatial attention in its screen. One of the infrequent stimuli formed an illusory own right it did not draw suYcient attentional resources Wgure (Kanizsa Square) aimed to capture spatial attention from the standard–deviant comparison as revealed when in the absence of an active task. Nineteen electrodes (10–20 using a concurrent active task. system) were used to record the electroencephalogram in fourteen subjects (ten females) mean age 34.5 years. ERPs Keywords Event-related potential · Visual mismatch to all stimuli consisted of a positive negative positive com- negativity · Kanizsa Wgure · Orientation plex recorded maximally over lateral occipital areas. The negative component was greater for deviant and illusory deviant compared to standard stimuli in a time window of Introduction 170–190 ms. A P3a component over frontal/central elec- trodes to the illusory deviant but not to the deviant stimulus The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) is deWned as a compo- suggests the illusory Wgure was able to capture attention nent of the Event Related Potential (ERP) that can be and orientate subjects to the recording. Subtraction wave- evoked to stimulus change in the absence of attention. The forms revealed visual discrimination responses at occipital MMN is usually elicited when a deviant stimulus is pre- electrodes, which may represent vMMN. In a control study sented within a sequence of standard stimuli. The auditory with 13 subjects (11 females; mean age 29.23 years), using MMN has been identiWed as a negative deXection usually peaking at 150–200 ms from change onset and is related to automatic discrimination processing and sensory memory M. Flynn · A. Liasis · M. Gardner · T. Towell (&) mechanisms (see Näätänen et al. 2005, 1997; Schröger Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, 309 Regent Street, London W1B 2UW, UK 1997, for reviews). e-mail: towella@wmin.ac.uk A number of studies have identiWed visual Mismatch Negativity (vMMN), as a negative deXection 100–250 ms A. Liasis post-stimulus change onset (see Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003 Department of Ophthalmology, Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children, for a review). These studies have reported vMMN either to London WC1N 3JH, UK ‘match’ and ‘non-match’ tasks where the stimuli are pre- sented with equiprobability to control the eVects of global S. Boyd stimulus presentation (Fu et al. 2003) for changes in orien- Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children, tation and spatial frequency (Kimura et al. 2006) for London WC1N 3JH, UK changes in spatial frequency), or within an oddball 123
  • 2. 154 Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 paradigm, whereby a variety of dimensions of the visual Marsalek 2003; Polich 2003; Hagen et al. 2006 for stimulus that are known to be important in early visual reviews). The P3 can be further divided into the subcompo- processing are manipulated. These include changes in spa- nents P3a and P3b. P3a originates from frontal attention tial frequency (Maekawa et al. 2005), motion (Kremlacek mechanisms to task novelty and/or distractors whilst the et al. 2006), colour (Czigler et al. 2004; Czigler et al. 2002; P3b is generated in more temporal/parietal regions and is Horimoto et al. 2002), form (Berti and Schroger 2004; associated with context updating and memory storage oper- Besle et al. 2005; Stagg et al. 2004) and orientation ations (Polich 2007). We therefore set out to validate the (Astikainen et al. 2004, 2008; Czigler and Csibra 1992). use of illusory deviant stimuli in orienting attention in a Similar to the auditory MMN the vMMN is thought to passive and active task in the context of a vMMN para- reXect the memory based detection of deviant stimuli rather digm. It was predicted that in the passive paradigm with no than refractoriness (see Czigler et al. 2007 for a detailed task conditions that discrimination components possibly discussion). However, in contrast to the correlation seen reXecting MMN would be evoked by both the deviant and between auditory MMN and behavioural detection of devi- illusory deviant stimuli while a P3a component would only ants (Winkler et al. 1993) there appears to be no such rela- be evident to illusory deviant stimuli that captured attention. tionship in the visual modality. The amplitude of vMMN To test the use of the illusory deviant stimulus in orien- does not increase beyond 40 ms stimulus onset asynchrony tating attention from the standard–deviant discrimination a (SOA) of a masking stimulus whilst detection performance control study was carried out containing an embedded of deviant stimuli and RT improve up to 174 ms SOA active task. Last, the generator sources of the visual ERP (Czigler et al. 2007). These Wndings strongly suggest that components were explored using intracranial recordings in overt detection of visual deviance is not the sole mechanism a subject undergoing presurgical evaluation for epilepsy underlying vMMN. surgery. In comparison to other ERP change components, such as N2 and P3, the MMN can be elicited in the absence of attention (Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003). Therefore, in order to Methods diVerentiate between MMN and other ERP change compo- nents the subjects’ attention is typically drawn away from Participants the test stimuli, employing a variety of behavioural tasks. For example, Stagg et al. (2004) and Tales et al. (1999) Study 1 and 2 required participants to press a button in response to target stimuli, Astikainen et al. (2004) used an auditory distrac- With ethical approval and informed consent 14 healthy tion task whereby participants were required to focus their adults (mean age 34.5 § 8.6 years (10 females) were attention on counting the number of words in a story whilst recruited for study 1 and 13 healthy adults (mean age being presented with visual stimuli. 29.23 § 8.8 years (11 females) for study 2. Subjects It has generally been understood that a concurrent active reported no history of neurological disease and had normal task is mandatory in eliciting vMMN to control for the or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. eVects of attention so that resources are allocated away from the standard–deviant discrimination towards the Study 3 active task (Heslenfeld 2003; Czigler 2007). However, not all patient populations can meet the demands of an active With hospital ethical approval and patient and parental con- task. Therefore, in the present study a three-stimulus pas- sent, a 15-year-old male with focal epilepsy undergoing sive oddball paradigm was developed. Stimuli diVered with pre-surgical evaluation for resection of a R anterior parietal regard to orientation of local endline type pacman Wgures lesion provided the opportunity to examine whether there and their information/entropy content. So in addition to was a dissociation of detection and discrimination compo- standard and deviant stimuli, an infrequent illusory deviant nents of the visual ERP. stimulus was introduced in order to investigate the eVects of attention. The illusory deviant stimulus was a Kanizsa Stimuli and procedure Wgure (Kanizsa 1976) which formed an illusory square, a salient event thought to demand attention to reconstruct Study 1 contours that are absent from visual images (Kaiser et al. 2004). Three monochrome endline type stimuli based on pacman A consistent Wnding in ERP research is that the P3 wave, Wgures were employed in a behaviourally silent oddball a positive deXection occurring from 280 to 400 ms post- paradigm where the ratio of standards to deviants and illu- stimulus indicates attentional processing (see Hruby and sory deviants was 8:1:1. The stimuli in (Fig. 1a), diVered 123
  • 3. Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 155 Fig. 1 a Stimuli presented in a) b) -8µV c) oddball paradigm with pseudo- N1 random sequence of 8:1:1, respectively. (i) standard, (ii) i) O1 O2 deviant and (iii) illusory deviant 100ms forming a Kanizsa square. b, c Grand average waveforms referenced to Fz at O1 and O2, respectively for (i) Standard P2 P1 (dashed line), deviant (solid ii) line) and illusory deviant stimuli (dotted line) (ii) Deviant minus standard (iii) Illusory deviant minus standard. Note the dis- crimination responses in ii) and iii) with an additional negative component in (iii) corresponding iii) to an inverted P3 from each other only in terms of the orientation of elements In study 3, the two blocks of the stimuli were presented (which were oriented unsystematically around their axes with no embedded active attention task. for the standard and deviant stimuli and formed an illusory Kanizsa Wgure for the illusory deviant stimulus. The stimuli Electroencephalogram recording and data analysis were generated employing STIM software (Neuroscan- STIM version 4; Compumedics USA, Ltd., El Paso, TX, Study 1 and 2 USA) and presented on a computer screen subtending 4°. The stimuli appeared on the screen for 400 ms with an Nineteen silver–silver chloride electrodes were used to inter-stimulus interval of 600 ms. Subjects were seated record the electroencephalogram (EEG) activity and were comfortably in a darkened room 1 m away from the screen positioned at sites in accordance with the International 10– and requested to Wxate on a small red dot in the centre of 20 system (Fz, F3, F4, Cz, C3, C4, T3, T4, Pz, P3, P4, Oz, the screen that was present throughout recording. Within O1, O2, T5, T6, VEOG, M1, M2). The reference electrode the oddball paradigm stimuli were presented in a pseudo- and the ground electrode were placed at the right and left random sequence ensuring that deviant and illusory deviant mastoid, respectively. An electrode was placed above the stimuli were interspersed with standard stimuli. In study 1, left eye to enable online artefact rejection of eye blinks. the stimuli were presented in Wve blocks of 225 stimuli with Continuous EEG was collected using Neuroscan-SCAN up to a minute break between blocks. At the end of the odd- version 4.3; Compumedics USA, Ltd., El Paso, TX, USA at ball recording blocks of 64 deviants and illusory deviants a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz, with a low pass of 100 Hz and ‘alone’ were presented. a high pass of 1 Hz and stored on a computer for oZine analysis. Study 2 Continuous EEG data were epoched oZine ¡100 ms pre-stimulus to +500 ms post-stimulus. The epochs were The same stimuli and procedure as in Study 1 were utilized digitally Wltered with a band pass 1–30 Hz and baseline cor- with the exception that an active attention task was embed- rected. Epochs containing transients greater than §150 V ded in the three stimulus oddball paradigm. Within the were excluded from further analysis. For each subject, ERPs blocks of 225 stimuli, during the interstimulus interval were averaged separately for standard, deviant and illusory (ISI), a small red square replaced the small red Wxation dot deviant stimuli employing Fz as a reference and grand aver- on 22 trials chosen at random. The red square appeared at age waveforms were constructed. Additional ERPs were the start of the 600 ms ISI and stayed on the screen for constructed in study 2 for the red Wxation dot and for the red 200 ms. Subjects were instructed to focus their attention on square that replaced the Wxation dot on a number of trials. the red Wxation dot and press the right button of a mouse as ERPs to standard stimuli were constructed from epochs that quickly as possible whenever the red square appeared. preceded deviant stimuli. As in previous studies (Stagg et al. Inclusion criteria were based on participants achieving 90% 2004; Tales et al. 1999), averaged mastoids were employed or more correct responses, excluding false positives. as a reference to investigate P3 activity. 123
  • 4. 156 Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 From the grand average waveforms MMN-like diVer- Table 1 Mean ERP amplitude ( V) and standard deviation (SD) for ences were identiWed on the basis of known negative polar- each stimulus type at electrode sites for the 170–190 ms time window ity, known emergence over posterior electrode positions for the passive task (n = 14) and typical latency range (100–250 ms post-stimulus: Pazo- Electrode Mean amplitude ( V) and standard Alvarez et al. 2003). In each study, the maximal diVerence site deviation (§SD) between ERPs to standards and deviants was identiWed at Stimulus occipital sites and a 20 ms time window was centred at this Standard Deviant Illusory deviant latency for electrodes P3, P4, O1, O2, T5, T6 (Astikainen et al. 2008). Mean amplitudes for the time windows were O1 2.98 § 2.32 5.67 § 3.36 7.10 § 4.10 calculated relative to the mean voltage of a 100 ms pre- O2 3.11 § 2.73 5.66 § 3.38 7.33 § 4.38 stimulus baseline for each participant for the standard, devi- P3 1.99 § 1.68 3.75 § 2.85 5.03 § 3.49 ant and illusory deviant stimuli. The mean amplitudes were P4 1.90 § 1.91 3.43 § 2.27 5.08 § 3.12 analysed using ANOVA. In addition, subtraction wave- T5 2.36 § 2.03 4.59 § 2.75 5.42 § 3.07 forms were constructed of deviant minus standard and illu- T6 2.62 § 1.83 4.70 § 2.24 5.13 § 2.75 sory deviant minus standard. Study 3 P < 0.001], indicating that the amplitude of the deviant stimulus was greater than the standard stimulus at occipital The patient was implanted with a 32-contact sub-dural plat- (t = 4.004; df = 13; P = 0.002) and temporal electrodes inum grid straddling the parietal and pre-motor gyri and a (t = 4.552; df = 13; P = 0.001) and that the amplitude of the 6-contact strip extending posteriorly over the inferior parie- illusory deviant was greater than the standard at occipital tal cortex such that the most distal contact (S1) overlay the (t = 4.507; df = 13; P = 0.001), temporal (t = 4.552; R occipital cortex (Fig. 3a). df = 13; P = 0.001) and parietal electrodes (t = 4.276; df = 13; P = 0.001). DiVerence waveforms of deviant minus standard and Results illusory deviant minus standard both revealed vMMN com- ponents (Fig. 1b, c). When comparing the deviant to the Study 1 standard ERP, using the point-by-point t test algorithm (P < 0.05; one-tailed) against baseline there were signiW- A visual response was recorded in all subjects in all trials cant diVerences at O1 between 173 and 217 ms (181– consisting of a P1–N1–P2 waveform. Grand average wave- 203 ms; P < 0.01) and at O2 between 178 and 208 ms forms were constructed for the standard, deviant and illu- (185–196 ms); P < 0.01). Comparing the illusory deviant to sory deviant stimuli (see Fig. 1 for waveforms at O1 and the standard ERP against baseline, there were signiWcant O2). The maximal diVerence between ERPs to standards diVerences (P < 0.05; one-tailed) at O1 between 164 and and deviants was at approximately 180 ms post-stimulus at 212 ms (175–203 ms; P < 0.01) and at O2 between 169 and occipital electrodes. A 20 ms time window was centred at 212 ms (181–199 ms; P < 0.01). this latency for electrodes O1, O2, P3, P4, T5, T6 and, for Illusory deviant stimuli evoked an additional late nega- each participant, mean amplitudes for this time window cal- tive component at 234 ms at Oz. To examine whether this culated relative to the mean voltage of a 100 ms pre-stimu- component corresponded to an inverted P3 component the lus baseline for standards, deviants and illusory deviants. waveforms were re-referenced to averaged mastoids. We Mean amplitudes and standard deviations for the standard, were able to reveal a positive component over the fronto- deviant and illusory deviant are shown in Table 1. central electrode sites corresponding to P3a. At Fz this A three-way within subjects ANOVA was used to ana- component had an onset latency of 244 ms, SD = 13 ms lyse the mean amplitude data in the 170–190 ms time win- and a peak latency of 290 ms, SD = 27 ms with a peak dow. Pairwise comparison of means was carried out using amplitude of 4.19 V, SD = 2.06 V. bonferroni corrected t tests. Factors were location (occipi- To examine whether the diVerences observed in the sub- tal, parietal, temporal), hemisphere (left, right) and stimulus traction waveforms were confounded by pure stimulus (standard, deviant and illusory deviant). The amplitude diVerences we compared the discrimination waveform to diVered signiWcantly with location [F(2,26) = 11.880; the deviant stimulus to the discrimination waveform when P < 0.001] and stimulus type [F(2,26) = 15.886; P < 0.001] that same stimulus was presented alone, i.e. out of context but not with hemisphere [F(1,13) = 0.233; P = 0.794]. and not in an oddball paradigm. Point-by-point t tests There was a statistically signiWcant interaction between revealed no signiWcant diVerences between the deviant– location and stimulus [F(4.52) = 6.503; P = 0.001, standard and deviant alone-deviant waveforms suggesting 123
  • 5. Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 157 that when the deviant stimulus was presented alone and out Table 2 Mean ERP amplitude ( V) and standard deviation for each of context it behaved in a similar way to the standard stimu- stimulus type at electrode sites at 150–170 ms for the active task lus even though it was physically diVerent. The same proce- (n = 13) dure was used to compare the illusory deviant stimulus in Electrode Mean amplitude ( V) and standard the context of an oddball paradigm with the illusory deviant site deviation (§SD) stimulus presented alone. Similarly, there were no signiW- Stimulus cant diVerences between the illusory deviant–standard and Standard Deviant Illusory deviant illusory deviant alone-illusory deviant waveforms. O1 4.12 § 2.22 5.12 § 2.10 7.24 § 2.79 Study 2 O2 5.20 § 3.40 6.25 § 3.43 9.27 § 5.20 P3 2.32 § 1.76 2.79 § 1.83 3.51 § 2.22 As in study 1, a visual response was recorded for all sub- P4 3.50 § 2.33 4.07 § 2.01 5.56 § 3.28 jects consisting of a P1–N1–P2 waveform. Grand average T5 3.23 § 1.75 4.18 § 1.82 5.20 § 2.00 waveforms were constructed for the standard, deviant and T6 4.90 § 2.35 5.6 4 § 2.36 7.48 § 3.92 illusory deviant stimuli (see Fig. 2 for waveforms at O1 and O2). The maximal diVerence between ERPs to standards and deviants was at approximately 160 ms post-stimulus at mean voltage of a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline for stan- occipital sites. A 20-ms time window was centred at this dards, deviants and illusory deviants for each participant. latency for electrodes O1, O2, P3, P4, T5, T6 and mean Mean amplitudes and standard deviations for the standard, amplitudes for this time window calculated relative to the deviant and illusory deviant are shown in Table 2. A three-way within subjects ANOVA was used to ana- lyse the mean amplitude data of the 150–170 ms time win- dow. Pairwise comparison of means was carried out using O1 O2 N1 bonferroni corrected t tests. Factors were location (occipi- tal, parietal, temporal), hemisphere (left, right) and stimulus (standard, deviant and illusory deviant). The amplitude diVered signiWcantly with location [F(2,24) = 16.874; a) P < 0.001], hemisphere [F(2,24) = 7.059; P = 0.021] and stimulus type [F(2,24) = 14.254; P < 0.001]. There was a P1 P2 signiWcant interaction between location and stimulus [F(4.48) = 10.636; P < 0.001] indicating that the amplitude of the deviant stimulus was greater than the standard stimu- lus at occipital (t = 3.796; df = 12; P = 0.003) and temporal -8µV (t = 3.147; df = 12; P = 0.008) electrodes. The amplitude of b) the illusory deviant stimulus was greater than the standard stimulus at occipital (t = 4.494; df = 12; P = 0.001), tempo- 100ms ral (t = 4.425; df = 12; P = 0.001) and parietal (t = 4.105; df = 12; P = 0.001) electrodes. There was a signiWcant interaction between hemisphere and stimulus [F(2,24) = c) 3.402; P = 0.050] indicating that in the left hemisphere the mean amplitude was greater for the deviant (t = 4.194; df = 12; P = 0.001) and illusory deviant (t = 5.536; df = 12; P < 0.001) than for the standard. In the right hemisphere the mean amplitude of the illusory deviant (t = 5.944; df = 12; d) P < 0.001) was greater than the standard as was the deviant P3 but to a lesser extent (t = 2.952; df = 12; P = 0.012). DiVerence waveforms of deviant minus standard and Fig. 2 a Grand average waveforms referenced to Fz for standard illusory deviant minus standard both revealed attenuated (dashed line), deviant (solid line) and illusory deviant stimuli (dotted vMMN components (Fig. 2b, c). When comparing the devi- line) at O1 and O2. Note the P3a component seen only to illusory devi- ant to the standard ERP, using the point-by-point t test ant stimuli. b Deviant minus standard. c Illusory deviant minus stan- algorithm (P < 0.05; one-tailed) against baseline, there dard. d Grand average waveforms for the rarely occurring red Wxation square (solid line) and for the central Wxation dot (dotted line). Note: were signiWcant diVerences at O1 between 138 and 176 ms the attenuated vMMN in b and the P3b wave to the task in d but no signiWcant diVerences were apparent at O2. When 123
  • 6. 158 Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 comparing the illusory deviant to the standard ERP against mismatch) were recorded more anteriorly at S3 and S4 and baseline, there were signiWcant diVerences (P < 0.05; one- were characterised by enhanced positivities at about 90 ms, tailed) at O1 between 142 and 178 ms and at O2 between 42.32 V and 219 ms, 87.21 V for S3 and enhanced 147 and 177 ms. positivities at 88 ms, 28.55 V and 237 ms, 45.93 for S4, Illusory deviant stimuli evoked an additional late nega- either side of the major negative component (Fig. 3b). tive component at occipital electrodes. When re-referenced Subtraction waveforms (deviant–standard) revealed dis- to averaged mastoids this component was positive over the crimination responses with positive peaks at 86 and 219 ms fronto-central electrode sites. At Fz this had an onset for electrode S3 and 93 and 233 ms at electrode S4. In latency of 223 ms, SD = 18 ms and a peak latency of addition, a later positive response to the illusory deviant 282 ms, SD = 22 ms with a peak amplitude of 5.17 V, stimulus was seen at around 386 ms over pre-motor regions SD = 2.72 V. (G9 > G17 and G18), suggesting activation of a frontal sys- All participants completed the active task (pressing the tem to stimulus novelty and/or target detection (Fig. 3c). mouse when the red Wxation dot was replaced with a red Wxation square) within the limits of the inclusion criteria. As expected, the active task evoked a P3b component Discussion showing that the participants’ attention was engaged with the task (See Fig. 2d). The main result from this study is that visual discrimination responses including vMMN components have been Study 3: intracranial recording recorded in a behaviourally silent oddball paradigm to a change in orientation. The stimuli utilised in this study A negative positive negative complex was recorded maxi- evoked a response that was more negative to the deviant mally to all stimuli at the most posterior electrode site (S1) stimuli than to the standard stimuli in the period 150– (Fig. 3b). The latency and amplitude of the Wrst major neg- 200 ms after stimulus onset. Whilst we acknowledge that ative component (N1) was similar for standard, deviant there were physical diVerences between the stimuli, and stimuli and illusory deviant stimuli (153 ms and ¡32.39 V, that these changes were not equal between standard–devi- 153 ms and ¡48.54 V, 162 ms and ¡45.50 V, respec- ant and standard–illusory deviant comparisons, the employ- tively). Responses to stimulus discrimination (visual ment of a ‘deviant alone’ and ‘illusory deviant alone’ conditions served as controls. Subsequent subtraction waveforms using the subtraction method suggested by Kraus et al. (1995) for delineating the MMN reveals that the diVerence in negativity was attributable not to physical G32 diVerences in the stimuli themselves, but by the context in b) s1 a) xx ++ G17 G9 G1 which the stimuli were presented. s2 S1 S6 The presence of a P3a over frontal/central electrodes for the illusory deviant grand average waveform but not for the s3 standard or deviant grand average waveforms, suggests that c) -30uV G9 the Kanizsa square captured attention. Without the control 100ms s4 task this would imply that the enhanced negativity exhib- G17 ited by the deviant compared to the standard may not s5 depend on attention. However, the use of the illusory Wgure G18 is supported by Senkowski et al. (2005) who found that Kanizsa Wgures automatically capture spatial attention s6 Fig. 3 a Co-registered sub-dural electrode locations, dotted eclipse when used as visual cues and Wallach and Slaughter (1988) denotes surface visible lesion, x seizure onset zone, + somatosensory who found that the familiarity of the illusory shape ERP localised. b Standard and deviant waveforms from the six strip increases the likelihood that the shape will be perceived. In contacts—dashed waveform represents the standard ERP, solid wave- addition, a number of clinical studies show that the form the deviant. At S1 and S2 the illusory deviant waveform did not diVer from the deviant or standard and no consistent changes were seen response to Kanizsa Wgures is robust. In an ERP study, at S3 and S4. For reasons of clarity the illusory deviant waveform is not Grice et al. (2003) examined perceptual completion in par- shown. Peak amplitude of the N1 and inverted discrimination compo- ticipants with Williams Syndrome—a genetic disorder in nent shown by the shaded area of the waveform. c Anterior grid elec- which visuo-spatial performance is poor, and found that trodes revealing a later positive response to the illusory deviant stimulus shown by the shaded area of the waveform. Dashed, solid and although the underlying neural mechanisms of the partici- dotted lines represent ERPs to standard, deviant and illusory deviant pants with Williams Syndrome may be diVerent to controls, stimuli, respectively their ability to perceive illusory contours was apparently 123
  • 7. Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 159 normal. Milne and Scope, in their 2007 study, suggested of arc also reveals the contribution of the parvocellular sys- that the perception of illusory contours in participants with tem and ventral stream in detecting diVerences in the Autistic Spectrum Disorder was intact. sequence of unattended central stimuli. The parvocellular Observation and statistical analysis of the waveforms system is particularly adapted to colour and high-contrast and the discrimination components reveals that the ampli- black and white detailed information. tude component N1 for the illusory deviant at the lateral Besle et al. (2005) using the deformation of a circle as a occipital electrodes was greater than for the standard or deviant stimulus embedded within an active task presented deviant stimuli. Many previous studies have demonstrated within 2° of arc demonstrated bilateral vMMN responses at an enhanced visual N1 amplitude component to attended- 216 ms being maximal at electrode PO3 and PO4. In an location stimuli (see Vogel and Luck 2000 for a review) active geometric shape discrimination task P1, N1 and P2 and this evidence further suggests that the illusory deviant components were identiWed at 80, 140 and 200 ms, respec- stimulus captured attention whilst the standard and deviant tively, and the N1 and P2 components became less sharp stimuli did not. and more diVuse as stimulus presentation changed between As with several previous studies (e.g. Tales et al. 1999) 4°, 8° and 12° of arc (Shoji and Ozaki 2006). we also engaged an active control task that required partici- Extra deviant stimuli conceptulised as distractor stimuli pants to press a button at the occurrence of a change in have also been used in the auditory modality to manipulate shape of the central Wxation dot. The understanding here is attention. For instance, Schroger et al. (2000) and Schroger that attentional resources are drawn from the standard– and WolV (1998) in an auditory duration discrimination deviant discrimination to the active task. Under these con- task found that task irrelevant distractors in the form of ditions we were able to conWrm the existence of vMMN small changes in frequency prolonged reaction times and responses although they were signiWcantly reduced in elicited MMN and P3a components, reXecting orientation amplitude. The underlying mechanisms of vMMN are still towards the distractor. to be resolved although a number of studies have suggested Recordings from the intracranial case study support the a memory based rather than refractoriness explanation (see separation of detection and discrimination processes within Czigler 2007). Such a visual based memory system would the visual cortex. The N1 component at 153 ms located at rely on the representation of regularity following repeated the more posterior electrodes corresponds to the scalp exposure to identical frequent stimuli. The violation of such recorded N1 at 167 ms. The waveforms were similar for the regularity following the presentation of a deviant stimulus standard, deviant and illusory deviant stimuli. However, at would elicit an enhanced posterior negativity commonly adjacent posterior electrodes (S3 and S4) the deviant stim- seen as vMMN in subtraction waveforms. However, in this uli evoked early and later positivities that probably contrib- model, it appears that longer sequences (10–15) of fre- ute to the scalp recorded MMN. With respect to scalp quent/standard and identical unattended stimuli will inXu- recordings these potentials to stimulus discrimination are ence the generation of vMMN (Czigler and Pato 2009). In inverted in polarity and the Wrst positive component is seen the current study, the median number of continuous stan- relatively early at around 90 ms. These Wnding are consis- dard sequences was 4 and this may account for the rela- tent with an MEG study showing strong activation of the tively low amplitude of the vMMN responses in study 2. lateral occipital cortex at around 155 ms post-stimulus The latency of the responses in the current study are consis- (Halgren et al. 2003). In MEG studies comparison of illu- tent within the general window for vMMN responses of sory Kanizsa stimuli with control stimuli reveals activation between 100 and 250 ms (Pazo-Alvarez et al. 2003) between 100 and 350 ms post-stimulus (Kaiser et al. 2004) although it is known that latency and duration of vMMN and at around 280 ms (Halgren et al. 2003). It is believed will diVer according to stimulus characteristics and task that illusory contour sensitivity may Wrst occur in middle to complexity with less salient changes and more complex higher order visual processing areas and that feedback rules resulting in longer latency and less phasic vMMN modulation from lateral occipital areas will activate V1 and responses (Czigler et al. 2006). V2 areas (Kaiser et al. 2004). Previous studies on vMMN have tended to engage active Early cortical processing in the visual cortex has also tasks embedded in more peripheral areas of the visual Weld been reported from MEG studies using Xash stimuli that and one study speciWcally set out to assess the contribution reach medial occipital areas around 47 ms (Inui and Kakigi of the magnocellular system (Kremlacek et al. 2006). This 2006). Recent studies using intracranial recordings demon- pathway forms the dorsal stream and is not sensitive to col- strate activation in the superior parietal lobule at 75 ms to our or detail but is thought to be responsible for pre-atten- coloured disc stimuli presented in the macular Weld tive detection of motion stimuli. Whilst in the present study (Molholm et al. 2006) and recordings from the striate we cannot exclude the contribution of the magnocellular cortex to alternating stimuli have been reported as a P55 system, our Wndings of a vMMN in the macular Weld at 4° followed by a more consistent N75 (Farrell et al. 2007). 123
  • 8. 160 Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 Polarity inversions between the cortex and the scalp can Czigler I, Csibra G (1992) Event-related potentials and the identiWca- indicate local generator sources in that region of cortex. As tion of deviant visual stimuli. Psychophysiology 29:471–485 Czigler I, Pato L (2009) Unnoticed regularity violation elicts change- these scalp recorded N1 and MMN Welds are interactions of related brain activity. Biol Psychol 80:339–347 the super imposition of several bilateral generators it is Czigler I, Balázs L, Winkler I (2002) Memory-based detection of task- diYcult to understand how focal intracranial potentials con- irrelevant visual changes. Psychophysiology 39:869–873 tribute to the scalp recorded N1 and MMN. The inverted Czigler I, Balázs L, Pató LG (2004) Visual change detection: event- related potentials are dependent on stimulus location in humans. biWd positive discrimination component may well represent Neurosci Lett 364:149–153 the existence of one or more local generator sources to Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I (2006) ERPs and deviance detection: change detection. Complex and widespread activation has visual mismatch negativity to repeated visual stimuli. Neurosci also been recorded to alternating and on/oV stimuli from the Lett 401:178–182 Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I (2007) Backward masking and visual striate cortex and visual association areas (Farrell et al. mismatch negativity: Electrophysiological evidence for memory- 2007) further supporting the view that it is diYcult to based detection of deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 44:610–619 entangle the generator sources that contribute to responses Farrell DF, Leeman S, Ojemann A (2007) Study of the human visual measured at the scalp. cortex: direct cortical evoked potentials and stimulation. J Clin Neurophysiol 24:1–10 Intracerebral potentials to rare distractor visual and audi- Fu S, Fan S, Chen L (2003) Event-related potentials reveal involuntary tory stimuli have been recorded from frontal regions as a processing of orientation changes in the visual modality. Psycho- widespread negative–positive–negative waveform at physiology 40:770–775 approximate latencies of 210–280–390 ms, respectively Grice SJ, de Hahn M, Halit H, Johnson MH, Csibra G, Grant J, KarmiloV-Smith A (2003) ERP abnormalities of illusory contour (Baudena et al. 1995). It is believed that this waveform cor- perception in Williams Syndrome. Neuroreport 14:1773–1777 responds with the scalp recorded N2a/P3a/slow wave that is Hagen GF, Gatherwright JR, Lopez BA, Polich J (2006) P3a from associated with orienting. In the present study the later pos- visual stimuli: task diYculty eVects. Int J Psychophysiol 59:8–14 itivity to the illusory deviant stimulus seen at around Halgren E, Dale AM, Mendola J, Chong CDR (2003) Cortical activa- tion to illusory shapes as measured with magnetoencephalogra- 386 ms over pre-motor regions may correspond to this nov- phy. Neuroimage 18:1001–1009 elty orienting process. Heslenfeld DJ (2003) Visual mismatch negativity. In: Polich J (ed) In conclusion, we suggest that visual discrimination Detection of change: event-related potential and fMRI Wndings. potentials containing vMMN components can be elicited Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 41–60 Horimoto R, Inagaki M, Yano T, Sata Y, Kaga M (2002) Mismatch using a paradigm with no task demands. The inclusion of negativity of the color modality during selective attention task to an illusory square was intended to capture the subject’s auditory stimuli in children with mental retardation. Brain Dev attention and therefore orientate them to the recording. The 24:703–709 existence of attenuated vMMN when subjects engaged in Hruby T, Marsalek P (2003) Event-related potentials—the P3 Wave. Acta Neurobiol Exp 63:55–63 an active distractor task supports the contention that the Inui K, Kakigi R (2006) Temporal analysis of the Xow from V1 to the illusory square was unable to command all resources away extrastriate cortex in humans. J Neurophysiol 96:775–784 from the standard–deviant comparison. Kaiser J, Bühler M, Lutzenberger W (2004) Magnetoencephalographic gamma-band responses to illusory triangles in humans. Neuroim- Acknowledgments We are grateful to the anonymous referees for age 23:551–560 their critical comments and suggestions for improvements on earlier Kanizsa G (1976) Subjective contours. Sci Am 234:48–52 versions of the manuscript. Kimura M, Murohashi H, Ji Katayama (2006) An ERP study of visual change detection: EVects of magnitude of spatial frequency changes on the change-related posterior positivity. Int J Psycho- physiol 62:14–23 References Kraus N, Sharma A, McGee T, Carrell TD (1995) Neurophysiologic bases of speech discrimination. Ear Hear 16:19–37 Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Wikgren J, Korhonen T (2004) The human Kremlacek J, Kuba M, Kubova Z, Langrova J (2006) Visual mismatch brain processes visual changes that are not cued by attended audi- negativity elicited by magnocellular system activation. Vision tory stimulation. Neurosci Lett 368:231–234 Res 46:485–490 Astikainen P, Lillstrang E, Ruusuvirta T (2008) Visual mismatch neg- Maekawa T, Taniwaki T, Kinukawa N, Kanba S, Goto Y, Tobimatsu ativity for changes in orientation—a sensory memory-dependent S (2005) Functional characterization of mismatch negativity to a response. Eur J Neurosci 28:2319–2324 visual stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol 116:2392–2402 Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G, Clark JM (1995) Intracerebral potentials Milne E, Scope A (2007) Are children with autistic spectrum disorders to rare target and distractor auditory and visula stimuli. III. Frontal susceptible to contour illusions? Br J Dev Psychol 26:91–102 cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 94:251–264 Molholm S, Sehatpour P, Mehta AD, Shpaner M, Gomez-Ramirez M, Berti S, Schroger E (2004) Distraction eVects in vision: behavioural Ortigue S, Dyke JP, Schwartz TH, Foxe JJ (2006) Audio-visual and event-related potential indices. Neuroreport 15:665–669 multisensory integration in superior parietal lobule revealed by Besle J, Fort A, Giard MH (2005) Is the auditory sensory memory sen- human intracranial recordings. J Neurophysiol 96:721–729 sitive to visual information. Exp Brain Res 166:337–344 Näätänen R, Lehtokoski A, Lennes M, Cheour M (1997) Language- Czigler I (2007) Visual mismatch negativity. Violation of nonattended speciWc phoneme representations revealed by electric and mag- environmental regularities. J Psychophysiol 21:224–230 netic brain responses. Nature 385:432–434 123
  • 9. Exp Brain Res (2009) 197:153–161 161 Näätänen R, Jacobsen T, Winkler I (2005) Memory-based or aVerent Senkowski D, Rottger S, Grimm S, Foxe JJ, Herrmann CS (2005) Kan- processes in mismatch negativity (MMN): a review of the evi- izsa subjective Wgures capture visual spatial attention: evidence dence. Psychophysiology 42:25–32 from electrophysiological and behavioral data. Neuropsychologia Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E (2003) MMN in the visual 43:872–886 modality: a review. Biol Psychol 63:199–236 Shoji H, Ozaki H (2006) Topographic change in ERP due to discrimi- Polich J (2003) Theoretical overview of P3a and P3b. In: Polich J (ed) nation of geometric Wgures in the peripheral visual Weld. Int J Psy- Detection of change: event-related potential and fMRI Wndings. chophysiol 62:115–121 Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 83–98 Stagg C, Hindley P, Tales A, Butler S (2004) Visual mismatch nega- Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. tivity: the detection of stimulus change. Neuroreport 15:659–663 Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128–2148 Tales A, Newton P, Troscianko T, Butler S (1999) Mismatch negativ- Schröger E (1997) On the detection of auditory deviations: a pre-atten- ity in the visual modality. Neuroreport 10:3363–3367 tive activation model. Psychophysiology 34:245–257 Vogel EK, Luck SJ (2000) The visual N1 component as an index of a Schroger E, WolV C (1998) Attentional orienting and reorienting is discrimination process. Psychophysiology 37:190–203 indicated by human event-related brain potentials. Neuroreport Wallach H, Slaughter V (1988) The role of memory in perceiving sub- 9:3355–3358 jective contours. Percept Psychophys 43:101–106 Schroger E, Giard MH, WolV C (2000) Auditory distraction: event-re- Winkler I, Reinikainen K, Naatanen R (1993) Event-related brain lated potential and behavioral indices. Clin Neurophysiol potentials reXect traces of echoic memory in humans. Percept 111:1450–1460 Psychophys 53:443–449 123