SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 11
Baixar para ler offline
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
VOLUME 1 NUMBER 1, 2007
Effects of Labeling Students “Learning Disabled”:
Emergent Themes in the Research Literature 1970 through
2000
Karen Osterholm, PhD
Assistant Professor
Department of Educational Leadership and
Counseling
The Whitelowe R. Green College of Education
Prairie View A&M University
Member of the Texas A&M University System
William R. Nash, EdD
Professor
Department of Educational Psychology
Texas A&M University
Member of the Texas A&M University System
William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
Professor
PhD Program in Educational Leadership
The Whitelowe R. Green College of Education
Prairie View A&M University
Member of the Texas A&M University System
Visiting Lecturer (2005)
Oxford Round Table
University of Oxford, Oxford, England
Distinguished Alumnus (2004)
College of Education and Professional Studies
Central Washington University
ABSTRACT
Using an iterative process similar to the constant comparative method, 34 studies
addressing the impact of the learning disabled label were synthesized. Four overlapping
primary themes emerged: expectancies, stereotypes, and attitudes; stigmatization,
rejection, and social distance; action versus attitude toward labeled individuals; and
differential influence of the LD label when other salient information is provided.
See: www.nationalforum.com
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
2___________________________________________________________________________________________
Introduction
The confusion and conflict surrounding definition, diagnosis, and treatment of learning
disabilities is well documented. Debate over definition is common and surprisingly heated.
Learning disabilities manifest in diverse ways, making diagnosis difficult. Lacking associated
physical signs or symptoms, learning disabilities comprise an invisible disability. As a result,
students so labeled are frequently considered simply lacking in motivation or commitment. Ong-
Dean (2005) points out that society may view invisible disabilities as “illegitimate excuses for
failure to conform” (p. 148) to achievement standards and other expectations. Alternately, the
LD diagnosis is sometimes misconstrued by laypersons as a form of mental retardation, with
myriad misconceptions deriving from this error. Labeled students, their co-learners, and teachers
often experience considerable frustration in negotiating the legal, ethical, educational, and social
mazes that learning disabilities present.
When ambiguity and the attendant professional disagreement regarding a disorder are
ongoing, the question of whether the designation should be used at all inevitably arises. Does
such a diagnosis hinder more than help those so labeled? Labeling theory, a sociological model,
proposes that labeling of individuals as “different” in the negative connotation of the word
creates a potentially distorted reality for those who bear the label, as well as for their teachers,
parents, and peers (Hebding & Glick, 1987). Labeling theory further predicts that once an
individual is labeled, “the social group seems to assign to that person a new identity and a new
role, a new set of expectations. The social group then responds to the individual according to
those expectations, thus reinforcing the label and affecting all future interactions” (p. 136).
The labeled individual’s self-perception is inextricably bound to others’ perceptions and
reactions. As Poole, Regoli, and Pogrebin (1986) suggest, individuals “are not passive recipients
of negative labels; rather, they are actively managing or coping with these labels” (p. 347). A
self-fulfilling prophecy might result when a person accepts the label and incorporates it into her
or his self-conceptualization, with reduced performance expectations and damaged self-esteem
as unfortunate byproducts (McGrew & Evans, 2003; Rosenthal, 2002). Reduced effort and lower
achievement might logically follow.
Others engaged in the debate believe that labels serve useful purposes with a net positive
result. Keogh (1987) opines that the LD label serves “as a focus for advocacy and for ensuring
attention to the problem, as a category or mechanism for providing services and as a condition or
set of conditions that require scientific study” (pp. 4-5). Kuther (1994) adds that diagnostic labels
facilitate research for practitioners, which hopefully results in tailored interventions and program
improvement. Opposing those who support inclusion and elimination of the LD label, Hallahan
and Kauffman (1994) go so far as to endorse development of a “culture of disability.” They
suggest that reduced stigma and enhanced learning might be achieved by “developing esprit de
corps among congregations of people with disabilities” (p. 505).
The Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act 2004 (IDEIA, 2004)
speaks to the labeling issue repeatedly. Particularly germane to the concerns of this paper, the
new statute prescribes “early intervening services to reduce the need to label children as disabled
in order to address the learning and behavioral needs of such children.” The statement is first
seen in the 2004 statute, bearing subtle testimony to the recognized dangers of label application.
KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS
____________________________________________________________________________3
Purpose of the Article
Divergent opinions on the ramifications of the “learning disabled” label abound. The
purpose of this study is a synthesis of research related to the effects of the label. Herein, “label”
will connote a designation assignment by qualified school personnel, psychologists, and other
diagnostic professionals. The reader should be cognizant that informal labeling – as well as self-
identification – might certainly impact the labeled individual as well.
Research Questions
Study selection, examination, and synthesis were guided by the following research questions:
1. How does being formally labeled as learning disabled affect labeled students?
2. Are teachers’ perceptions, attitudes, expectations, and behaviors different for labeled and
nonlabeled students?
3. How do nonlabeled co-learners perceive and react to peers identified as learning
disabled?
Methodology
Using all professional databases available, conscientious efforts were made to identify,
locate, and collect the entire population of relevant studies from 1970 through 2000. Several
promising studies failed to delineate students’ diagnoses precisely, while others did not
adequately distinguish the influence of the label from the cumulative impact of experiences
germinated by the symptoms of the disorder. The term “learning disabilities” denotes very
different phenomena in different countries (Hart, 1999). In response to these and other
idiosyncrasies of this particular literature, the following selection criteria evolved:
1. Studies must report data specific to the LD label as a distinct phenomenon, rather
than data based on aggregated diagnostic labels.
2. Studies must directly address the formal LD label as distinguished from the actual
disorder.
3. Studies involving two or more diagnoses within a single individual described in case
studies or presented in vignettes were excluded.
4. Due to differential use of relevant labels across national boundaries, only studies
conducted in the United States were utilized in this synthesis.
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
4___________________________________________________________________________________________
Application of these selection criteria yielded 33 studies. Twenty-eight employed quantitative
methodologies, and five were qualitative. One quantitative research effort involved two
independent reports, yielding a total of 34 studies.
The decision to include both quantitative and qualitative studies was a carefully
considered one. Despite the obvious philosophical and other distinctions between the two
research types, synthesis of diverse approaches has become increasingly utilized and appreciated
in recent years (Collins, Onwuegbuzie, & Sutton, 2006). Miles and Huberman (1994) offer
strong support for this approach when they state, “The careful measurement, generalizable
samples, experiment control, and statistical tools of good quantitative studies are precious assets.
When they are combined with the up-close, real-world contexts that characterize good qualitative
studies, we have a very powerful mix (p. 42).
Specific methods used to analyze and combine results from the 34 identified reports were
necessarily driven by the characteristics of the data collected. An iterative approach similar to the
constant comparative method was employed (Boeije, 2002; Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Descriptive
synopses were developed for each study. The synthesist was alert to potential commonalities and
patterns, analogous to the concept of emerging themes in the qualitative literature. She kept an
informal record of such intuitions and insights, generating tentative labels for individual clusters.
Methodological, theoretical, temporal, and subject-related patterns were of interest, though
receptivity to other potential commonalities was a hallmark of the iterative process. Each new
synopsis presented potential for addition to and reorganization of groupings previously
considered. During this phase, data collection and data analysis proceeded simultaneously and
interactively.
After completion of the 34 descriptive synopses, all were reviewed by one of the authors
and another faculty member in psychology, and sorted with regard to the final iteration of
clusters. Many studies fell into multiple clusters. Examination of clusters yielded four
overlapping themes or patterns. Again, studies often contributed to more than one theme.
Results and Discussion
The four overlapping themes or patterns that eventually emerged follow.
1. Expectancies, stereotypes, and attitudes related to the LD label
2. Stigmatization, rejection, and social distance related to the LD label
3. Action versus attitude
4. Differential influence of the LD label given other salient information
Each of the four themes is addressed below. Selected studies from the synthesis are cited in
support of the themes. Additional research affording further clarification is cited as well.
KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS
____________________________________________________________________________5
Expectancies, Stereotypes, and Attitudes Related to the LD Label
A review of the literature relevant to sociological labeling theory offers relatively strong
support for the view that negative or deviant labels generate unfortunate expectancies. A number
of studies addressed this particular theme – some more directly than others. Minner (1982)
investigated the academic and behavioral expectations for students labeled with learning
disabilities and students labeled with educable mental retardation. A “no-label” group served as
the control. Vocational teachers read a vignette describing student attributes germane to
academic performance, as well as typical behaviors exhibited. Some subjects received vignettes
in which the student was described as learning disabled, while others were led to believe that
their student had educable mentally retardation. The research reports a significant negative effect
for diagnostic labels. Additionally, post hoc tests revealed that nonlabeled students with negative
descriptions and students labeled LD with positive descriptions were not differentiated. Even
more surprisingly, teachers made no distinction between LD students with promising attributes
and those described negatively.
Minner and Prater (1984) asked college teachers to judge the academic promise of a
student portrayed as having learning disabilities, as well as their own ability to work successfully
with such college students. According to the researchers, “the LD label significantly and
negatively influenced faculty members’ initial expectations” (p. 228). They were less definitive
with regard to the impact of the label on teachers’ perceived ability to work effectively with the
LD student. In a subsequent study, Minner (1990) reports that teachers were significantly less
likely to refer a child labeled LD to a gifted program, even though descriptive vignettes were
otherwise identical to those describing other children who were referred. For teachers familiar
with the discrepancy formula so often applied in diagnosing LD, Minner’s vignette may have
been confusing since it described a student with congruent achievement and intelligence.
Though results are not entirely consistent, these and other quantitative studies suggest
that the LD label has potentially negative implications for those who bear it. Relevant themes
from qualitative studies were examined to determine whether context-specific information would
inform the quantitative data. Albinger (1995) quotes one young man as saying, “Mrs. Albinger, if
you make me keep coming to resource, I’ll just be a bum on the street [pointing out the window].
All the bums out there went to resource!” (p. 621) The painful impact of such an expectation on
the boy’s self-esteem cannot be overstated. Several students reported repeated name-calling
incidents involving pejorative terms such as “stupid” and “retard.” Labeled students reported
insensitive teacher behavior as well. One instructor criticized a labeled child’s academic efforts
publicly, chastising her for acting like a kindergartner. Barga (1996) quotes a math teacher who
fondly called a favorite labeled student “D.D.” – an abbreviation for “Darling Dummy.”
In summary, analysis suggests that the learning disabilities label generates reduced or
negative expectations, as well as negative stereotypes and attitudes. Lower expectations often
translate into reduced effort and lower achievement. Studies characterized by design flaws and
inadequate reporting may reduce confidence in such conclusions, however. Further and better
research is essential in clarifying this issue. IDEIA 2004 indirectly supports this premise,
stating: “Almost 30 years of research and experience has demonstrated that the education of
children with disabilities can be made more effective by … having high expectations for such
children.”
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
6___________________________________________________________________________________________
Stigmatization, Rejection, and Social Distance Related to the LD Label
Several studies included in this synthesis suggest that children designated as learning
disabled experience emotional and physical isolation as a result. Cartledge, Frew, and Zaharias
(1985) report that fourth- and fifth-graders rated a child labeled LD significantly more negatively
on 12 of 15 questions presented than they did a similar boy labeled “non-handicapped.” Though
the labeled boy was rated as less likely to treat others children unkindly, students expressed
lower levels of interest in establishing a friendship with him. Researchers did not assess level of
student understanding of the term learning disabled. With very young subjects, such a
determination might be especially important.
The concepts of expectancy and stereotypy have overlapping and cyclical relationships
with stigmatization, rejection, and social distance. Studies purporting to address one of these
phenomena often subtly inform our understanding of the others. For example, the previously
cited study on academic expectations for college students with learning disabilities (Minner &
Prater, 1984) is relevant to the second theme as well. College faculty members made a
distinction between the labeled and nonlabeled students with regard to predictions of academic
success, as well as the teachers’ perceived ability to work with the individual portrayed. Upon
superficial consideration of these outcomes, one might deduce that faculty members feel
underprepared for the challenge rather than averse to the student. Unsolicited comments written
on the questionnaires suggest otherwise. One professor wrote, “We cannot allow everyone into
college – the integrity of the B.A. degree cannot be challenged” (p. 228). “I am trained to teach
bright students, not handicapped ones,” opined another. These remarks imply stigmatization,
rejection, and desire for increased social distance from students with learning disabilities.
Research on stigmatization and rejection relative to the LD label is not entirely
consistent. Cohen’s (1977) study cited previously used the Mann-Whitney U to compare
numbers of positive adjectives attributed to various student groups. Teacher responses did
suggest a significant bias against students labeled LD compared to those identified as “normal.”
Though this might seem purely a study of attitudes or stereotypes, an additional manipulation of
the variables informs acceptance versus rejection due to label. When the written vignette
described a learning disabled individual but no actual label was provided, lower ratings were
given than when the same description was labeled LD. Perhaps individuals displaying atypical
behaviors are more likely to be stigmatized and rejected when there is no ready explanation for
their “difference.”
Studies that fail to distinguish clearly the impact of the LD label from effects of the actual
disorder (and thus are ineligible for actual inclusion in the synthesis) enhance understanding of
stigmatization. When physical segregation from nonlabeled co-learners is part of labeled
students’ experience, feelings of stigmatization are legitimized regardless of whether the label is
appropriately applied. Albinger (1995) reports that labeled students’ efforts to avoid
stigmatization usually included fabricated stories about their activities apart from time with
nonlabeled students. Some children carried the lie for several years, as when one student told
friends that she took piano lessons at a certain time each day. Another child told friends on
several occasions that he arrived late due to problems with the family car.
In his qualitative study on African American and Hispanic American teenagers who bear
the LD label in New York schools, Bryant (1989) offers even sadder commentary on the
KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS
____________________________________________________________________________7
embarrassment experienced by labeled students. One individual reported that students in the
resource room would try to conceal their identities when regular education students walked by.
“Regular ed. kids could come by and everyone would cover their faces. We’d cover our faces
and we would all hide because we’d be embarrassed” (p. 91).
Many students reported that “pullout” programs were especially problematic, as
nonlabeled students bore witness when labeled students were called to a special place for special
students. The social distancing phenomenon inherent in such segregation is a concern as well.
Even labeled students who consider the LD diagnosis a relief or welcome explanation for
previously misunderstood “differences” suggest that physical separation from non-labeled peer is
disheartening and potentially stigmatizing (Barga, 1996). IDEIA 2004 lends credence to the
importance of minimizing social and physical difference, stating that “the education of children
with disabilities can be made more effective by … ensuring their access to the general education
curriculum in the regular classroom, to the maximum extent possible.”
Action versus Attitude
The potential pain derived from the first two themes to emerge from this synthesis should
certainly not be diminished. Some evidence suggests, however, that attitudes toward those
labeled learning disabled are typically more negative than actions taken. In other words,
disheartening responses to attitude and acceptability scales notwithstanding, nonlabeled others
may behave more responsibly when action is required of them. Certainly, this is not new idea.
Social psychologists have long wondered at the apparent disconnect sometimes observed
between attitudes and behavior (Brehm & Kassin, 1996; Ajzen, 2000).
Cohen (1977) illustrates this phenomenon in her dissertation involving teachers’
stereotypes of three groups: learning disabled students, individuals whose reading requires
remediation (RR), and their normal peers. Teachers consistently ascribed more negatively
connotative adjectives to LD and RR label-bearers compared to nonlabeled students. Yet they
did not translate these negative perceptions or attitudes into discriminatory action. Regardless of
the label applied and the information provided, teachers in Cohen’s study were able to score all
students’ essays fairly and accurately. Thus, their expectations did not dictate their behavior.
Several studies comparing diagnostic labels but lacking an unlabeled control group lend
support to the suggestion that attitude and action are relatively independent phenomena. Boucher
and Deno (1979) found that teachers who read a three-page case study with either the LD or ED
label affixed were capable of objective programming decisions. The ED label was more
consistently recalled than the LD label, which might suggest a stronger reaction to the former.
Yet any differential reaction to the two labels was not translated into behavioral distinctions.
Fairbanks and Stinnett (1997) tapped a more diverse subject population. Teachers, school
psychologists, and school workers were presented with a vignette describing a third-grader with
a pattern of disruptive behavior. Different diagnostic labels were assigned to the hypothetical
child for different subjects; LD, behaviorally disordered (BD), and ADD ascriptions were
alternately made. When asked to judge the acceptability of one of two possible intervention
strategies, no labeling effects were noted. However, no nonlabeled control group is in evidence.
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
8___________________________________________________________________________________________
Additionally, no attitude or expectancy measure was taken for comparison with the more action-
oriented plan for intervention. This was not the stated purpose of the study, so the researchers
cannot be faulted here. The report states, “an attempt was made to distribute each of the labels
and interventions equally within each of the groups” (p. 330). This was perhaps a practical
alternative to random distribution within groups or across groups, given the relatively small
sample sizes and the large number of treatment combinations.
In summary, studies seem to suggest that prejudicial attitudes toward individuals labeled
LD do not necessarily result in discriminatory behavior. The research on stigmatization and
social distance might be interpreted as suggesting quite the opposite, however, at least among
student populations.
Differential Influence of the LD Label Given Other Salient Information
A fourth theme emerging from the data involves whether the LD label loses potency
when additional information is available to moderate it. Huebner (1990) asked school
psychologists to make diagnostic and placement decisions for fictitious students, providing them
with a student profile, identification as having learning disabilities or as “normal,” and current
diagnostic testing data. Results indicate that only current psychological test results influenced
psychologists’ decisions. In other words, even when a student was currently in a special
placement for her or his disabilities, most psychologists were quite willing to declare that neither
the special placement nor the label was necessary. Huebner considers this outcome evidence that
confirmation bias may not be as strong as previously believed. Similarly, Dukes (1987) found
that expectancies were mediated by exposure to additional salient information. Elementary
teachers’ pretest scores reflected the predicted expectancy effects for learning disabled students.
However, following exposure to two videos in which teacher and students interacted in a
classroom setting, posttest scores were not impacted by label.
Sichel (1984) designed a creative study to investigate interaction effects between the LD
label and teacher personality, as well as several elements irrelevant to the current synthesis.
Results on a dogmatism scale enabled him to separate subjects into two groups representing
opposite ends of the dogmatic/open-minded continuum. Several dependent measures offered
impressive triangulation. Teachers evaluated an essay reportedly written by a hypothetical
student who was described to half the subjects as learning disabled. This study is unique among
the quantitative selections because subjects actually interacted with the student rather than
reading vignettes or viewing videotapes. Though the student was a confederate of the researcher,
this design allowed a closer approximation to reality than most others found in the literature.
Interpersonal impressions constituted one dependent variable, while objective observers’ ratings
of teachers’ support and liking for the student were examined as well. The presence or absence
of the LD label had no impact on outcomes. Teachers were similarly objective, supportive, and
pleasant regardless of the student’s ascribed status.
Graham and Dwyer (1987) studied the degree of objectivity that college students could
muster when faced with grading an essay reportedly written by a student labeled learning
disabled. Control group participants believed that nonlabeled students wrote the essays they read.
KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS
____________________________________________________________________________9
Though results indicated that the LD label did affect scores assigned, the quality of the essay
wielded even greater influence. These and other studies offer relatively strong support for the
view that salient information ameliorates label bias.
Concluding Remarks
In conclusion, despite design and conceptual flaws evident in several studies utilized in
this synthesis, tentative conclusions may be drawn. Overall, assignment of the learning disabled
label is associated with teachers’ lowered expectations, as well as more negative stereotypes and
attitudes toward the labeled individual. Most synthesized studies employed written vignettes or
brief videotaped segments, both of which are rather artificial stimuli. Others failed to employ
appropriate control groups. However, when children rather consistently rate hypothetical peers
labeled LD as less acceptable, less socially attractive, and even less valuable, the potential social
and emotional implications for real children so labeled are disheartening. Labeled individuals
report stigmatization and both social and physical distancing from nonlabeled peers, factors that
render the school experience even less appealing for those targeted.
These unfortunate conditions notwithstanding, evidence indicates that negative
perceptions of the labeled student do not necessarily generate biased grading practices or
inappropriate programming. Further, compelling evidence suggests that the negative impact of
the label is diminished when other salient information is available. Sensitivity and diversity
training, as well as educational efforts aimed at increasing all stakeholders’ understanding of
learning disabilities, might further reduce the negative impact of the label.
This synthesis included studies published from 1970 through 2000 that met selection
criteria delineated earlier. However, 27 of the 34 studies were published before 1990, perhaps
skewing results relative to all themes identified. With hope that progress has been made
regarding attitudes toward diversity in general and learning differences in particular, future
researchers might compare studies from different decades including those published most
recently.
The debate regarding the potentially negative outcomes of being labeled learning
disabled as opposed to possible benefits derived is ongoing. As Sack-Min (2007) notes, however,
applying a diagnostic label to an individual “is a profound decision that affects the rest of his or
her educational career and life” (p. 23). Continued exploration of the clarity and utility of the
label itself is warranted.
References
Ajzen, I. (2000). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 27-58.
Albinger, P. (1995) Stories from the resource room: Piano lessons, imaginary illness, and
broken-down cars. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 615-621.
NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS
10___________________________________________________________________________________________
Barga, N. K. (1996). Students with learning disabilities in education: Managing a disability.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 413-421.
Boeije, H. (2002). A purposeful approach to the constant comparative method in the analysis of
qualitative interviews. Quality and Quantity, 36(4), 391-409.
Boucher, C. R., & Deno, S. L. (1979). Learning disabled and emotionally disturbed: Will the
labels affect teacher planning? Psychology in the Schools, 16, 395-402.
Brehm, S. S., & Kassin, S. M. (1996). Social psychology (3rd
ed.). Boston,MA: Houghton
Mifflin.
Bryant, G. (1989). The experience of junior high school minority students in special education
who are labeled “learning disabled”: A qualitative study. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, New York University, New York.
Cartledge, G., Frew, T., & Zaharias, J. (1985). Social skill needs of mainstreamed students: Peer
and teacher perceptions. Learning Disability Quarterly, 8, 132-140.
Cohen, L. G. (1977). Stereotyping by teachers of remedial reading, learning disabled, and
normal students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Boston.
Collins, K. M. T., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Sutton, I. L. (2006). A model incorporating the
rationale and purpose for conducting mixed methods research in special education and
beyond. Learning Disabilities. A Contemporary Journal, 4(1), 67-100.
Dukes, M. (1987). The impact of the learning disabilities label upon teachers’ evaluations
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville.
Fairbanks, L. D., & Stinnett, T. A. (1997). Effects of professional group membership,
intervention type, and diagnostic label on treatment acceptability. Psychology in the
Schools, 34(4), 329-335.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for
qualitative research. Chicago,IL: Aldine.
Graham, S., & Dwyer, A. (1987). Effects of the learning disability label, quality of writing
performance, and examiner’s level of expertise on the evaluation of written products.
Reston, VA: National Center for Research on Handicapped and Gifted Children. (ERIC
Document Reproduction Service No. ED 202 520).
Hallahan, D. P., & Kauffman, J. M. (1994). Toward a culture of disability in the aftermath of
Deno and Dunn. Journal of Special Education, 27, 496-508.
Hart, H. (1999). Terminology to benefit children. Development Medicine and Child Neurology
41, 651.
Hebding, D. E., & Glick, L. (1987). Introduction to sociology: A text with readings (3rd
ed.).
New York: Random House.
Huebner, E. S. (1990). The generalizability of the confirmation bias among school
psychologists. School Psychology International, 11, 281-286.
Individuals with Disabilities Improvement Act of 2004, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq.
Keogh, B. K. (1987). Learning disabilities: In defense of a construct. Learning Disabilities
Research, 3(1), 4-9.
Kuther, T. L. (1994). Diagnostic classification of children within the educational system: Should
it be eliminated? Reston, VA: National Center for Research on Handicapped and Gifted
Children. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 393 259).
McGrew, K. S., & Evans, J. (2003). Expectations for students with cognitive disabilities: Is the
cup half empty or half full? Can the cup flow over? (Synthesis Report 55). Minneapolis,
KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS
____________________________________________________________________________11
MN: University of Minnesota, National Center on Educational Outcomes.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. E. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Minner, S. (1990). Teacher evaluations of case descriptions of LD gifted children. Gifted Child
Quarterly, 34, 37-39.
Minner, S. (1982). Expectancies of vocational teachers for handicapped students. Exceptional
Children, 48, 451-453.
Minner, S., & Prater, G. (1984). College teachers’ expectations of LD students. Academic
Therapy, 20, 225-229.
Ong-Dean, C. (2005). Reconsidering the social location of the medical model: An examination
of disability in parenting literature. Journal of Medical Humanities, 26(2/3), 141-158.
Poole, E. D., Regoli, R. M., & Pogrebin, M. R. (1986). A study of the effects of self-labeling and
public labeling. The Social Science Journal, 23, 345-360.
Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communications in classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubicles.
American Psychologist, 57(11), 839-849.
Sack-Min, J. (2007). The issues of IDEA. American School Board Journal, 194(3), 20-25.
Sichel, S. M. (1984). The effects of a handicapped label, its source and teacher personality on
teacher responses to a student. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University,
New York.
Formatted by Dr. Mary Alice Kritsonis, National Research and Manuscript Preparation Editor,
National FORUM Journals, Houston, Texas www.nationalforum.com

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Mais procurados

Social cognitive theory journal article
Social cognitive theory journal articleSocial cognitive theory journal article
Social cognitive theory journal articleJorge Barzanas
 
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...Alexander Decker
 
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...William Kritsonis
 
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, H
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, HScott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, H
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, HHouston Barber
 
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...Sheila Raja
 
Demaris final presentation
Demaris final presentationDemaris final presentation
Demaris final presentationguest2b32b2e
 
Petrill wilkerson
Petrill wilkersonPetrill wilkerson
Petrill wilkersonSpyank
 
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008William Kritsonis
 
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...Kirstin Anderson
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-ChiefWilliam Kritsonis
 
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)Yafi Azhari
 
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practice
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of PracticeMD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practice
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practiceeckchela
 
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...Monica Feliu-Mojer, Ph.D.
 

Mais procurados (15)

Social cognitive theory journal article
Social cognitive theory journal articleSocial cognitive theory journal article
Social cognitive theory journal article
 
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...
A comparative study between introverts and extraverts in their conforming beh...
 
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...
Foss, nate the experiences of early service teachers using facebook nftej v23...
 
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, H
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, HScott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, H
Scott, T. M., Hirn, R., and Barber, H
 
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...
Characteristics of Secondary School Classrooms Associated with Understanding ...
 
Webb new
Webb newWebb new
Webb new
 
Poster
PosterPoster
Poster
 
Demaris final presentation
Demaris final presentationDemaris final presentation
Demaris final presentation
 
Petrill wilkerson
Petrill wilkersonPetrill wilkerson
Petrill wilkerson
 
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008
Halcrow,cheryl,focus,vol2,num1,2008
 
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...
Public, Private, and Persistence: Operationalizing Tinto’s “Pre-Schooling” At...
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
 
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)
Knowing the right stuff attrition, gender, and scientific literacy (reading)
 
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practice
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of PracticeMD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practice
MD1 Compare and Contrast Principles of Practice
 
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...
Identity, Academia & Community: Research & Implications for Broadening Partic...
 

Destaque

LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...
LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...
LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...William Kritsonis
 
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...William Kritsonis
 
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...William Kritsonis
 
Creative Commons for City of Salisbury
Creative Commons for City of SalisburyCreative Commons for City of Salisbury
Creative Commons for City of SalisburyRhys Moult
 
Dawn Proctor - Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...
Dawn Proctor -  Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...Dawn Proctor -  Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...
Dawn Proctor - Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...William Kritsonis
 
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational Philosophy
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational PhilosophyDr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational Philosophy
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational PhilosophyWilliam Kritsonis
 
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhDFunctions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhDWilliam Kritsonis
 
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...William Kritsonis
 
CNCS Web 2.0
CNCS Web 2.0CNCS Web 2.0
CNCS Web 2.0jmproulx
 
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...William Kritsonis
 
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...William Kritsonis
 
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011William Kritsonis
 
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administration
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administrationJ2447 d7 educational psychology & administration
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administrationWilliam Kritsonis
 
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for Change
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for ChangeIntentions, Processes and Frameworks for Change
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for ChangeSami Nerenberg
 
Real Americans Respect The Flag
Real Americans Respect The FlagReal Americans Respect The Flag
Real Americans Respect The Flagguest3f6492
 

Destaque (20)

LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...
LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...
LIVING LEGACIES: A PHENOMENOLOGICAL STUDY OF SEVEN AFRICAN AMERICAN MALE EDUC...
 
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...
Book Review: Ethical Decision Making in School Administration: Leadership as ...
 
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...
NATIONAL FORUM OF EDUCATIONAL ADMINISTRATION AND SUPERVISION - National FORUM...
 
Creative Commons for City of Salisbury
Creative Commons for City of SalisburyCreative Commons for City of Salisbury
Creative Commons for City of Salisbury
 
Dawn Proctor - Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...
Dawn Proctor -  Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...Dawn Proctor -  Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...
Dawn Proctor - Iceberg melting summary, Submitted to William Allan Kritsonis...
 
Jordans hokkaido
Jordans hokkaidoJordans hokkaido
Jordans hokkaido
 
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational Philosophy
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational PhilosophyDr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational Philosophy
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Educational Philosophy
 
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhDFunctions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
Functions of the Doctoral Dissertation Advisor by William Allan Kritsonis, PhD
 
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...
Authors - NFEAS JOURNAL, 31(1) 2013-2014 - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Edito...
 
CNCS Web 2.0
CNCS Web 2.0CNCS Web 2.0
CNCS Web 2.0
 
Fielding Slideshow
Fielding SlideshowFielding Slideshow
Fielding Slideshow
 
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...
Dr. Fred C. Lunenburg, Merchant Professor, Sam Houston State University - Pub...
 
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...
Presentation: Passing the Principal TExES Exam: Ch. 5 Learner-Centered Values...
 
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011
Lunenburg, fred c. components of a curriculum plan schooling v2 n1 2011
 
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administration
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administrationJ2447 d7 educational psychology & administration
J2447 d7 educational psychology & administration
 
Los servicios de publicación desarrollados por las bibliotecas universitarias
 Los servicios de publicación desarrollados por las bibliotecas universitarias Los servicios de publicación desarrollados por las bibliotecas universitarias
Los servicios de publicación desarrollados por las bibliotecas universitarias
 
Foodscapes
FoodscapesFoodscapes
Foodscapes
 
讓我承諾愛你一輩子
讓我承諾愛你一輩子讓我承諾愛你一輩子
讓我承諾愛你一輩子
 
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for Change
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for ChangeIntentions, Processes and Frameworks for Change
Intentions, Processes and Frameworks for Change
 
Real Americans Respect The Flag
Real Americans Respect The FlagReal Americans Respect The Flag
Real Americans Respect The Flag
 

Semelhante a Dr. Karen Osterholm, www.nationalforum.com

AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxAUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxrock73
 
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...NazlFidanDalkl
 
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docx
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docxTheory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docx
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docxchristalgrieg
 
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...Angie Miller
 
Statistics in Art Education
Statistics in Art EducationStatistics in Art Education
Statistics in Art EducationKyle Guzik
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comWilliam Kritsonis
 
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxTHE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxTHE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxjmindy
 
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationAction Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationCynthia King
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comWilliam Kritsonis
 
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docx
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docxConducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docx
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docxdonnajames55
 
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area  grief and students in middle school introductTopic area  grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area grief and students in middle school introductJASS44
 
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docxeugeniadean34240
 

Semelhante a Dr. Karen Osterholm, www.nationalforum.com (20)

Dr. osterholm save!
Dr. osterholm   save!Dr. osterholm   save!
Dr. osterholm save!
 
Dr. Osterholm
Dr. Osterholm   Dr. Osterholm
Dr. Osterholm
 
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxAUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
 
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...
ECR, Diversity Related Experiences of Students, Academic and Administrative S...
 
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docx
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docxTheory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docx
Theory Into Practice, 53158–164, 2014Copyright © The Coll.docx
 
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
 
Final Dissertation
Final DissertationFinal Dissertation
Final Dissertation
 
Statistics in Art Education
Statistics in Art EducationStatistics in Art Education
Statistics in Art Education
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
 
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...
Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence. Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug R...
 
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxTHE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
 
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docxTHE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
THE JOURNAL OF SPECIAL EDUCATION VOL. 38NO. 22004PP. 95–103.docx
 
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationAction Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
 
Annamarie chapter 2
Annamarie chapter 2Annamarie chapter 2
Annamarie chapter 2
 
Oral defense power point
Oral defense power pointOral defense power point
Oral defense power point
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
 
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docx
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docxConducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docx
Conducting Culturally Sensitive Qualitative Research DEVIKADIBYA.docx
 
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area  grief and students in middle school introductTopic area  grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
 
Appleby college research
Appleby college researchAppleby college research
Appleby college research
 
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx
1Quantitative Research Plan (Draft)ByID # A00355270.docx
 

Último

Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)cama23
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Celine George
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYKayeClaireEstoconing
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptxmary850239
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptxiammrhaywood
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSJoshuaGantuangco2
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...JojoEDelaCruz
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translation
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translationActivity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translation
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translationRosabel UA
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxMusic 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxleah joy valeriano
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfTechSoup
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)lakshayb543
 

Último (20)

Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
Global Lehigh Strategic Initiatives (without descriptions)
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...
ENG 5 Q4 WEEk 1 DAY 1 Restate sentences heard in one’s own words. Use appropr...
 
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translation
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translationActivity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translation
Activity 2-unit 2-update 2024. English translation
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
 
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxMusic 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
 

Dr. Karen Osterholm, www.nationalforum.com

  • 1. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS VOLUME 1 NUMBER 1, 2007 Effects of Labeling Students “Learning Disabled”: Emergent Themes in the Research Literature 1970 through 2000 Karen Osterholm, PhD Assistant Professor Department of Educational Leadership and Counseling The Whitelowe R. Green College of Education Prairie View A&M University Member of the Texas A&M University System William R. Nash, EdD Professor Department of Educational Psychology Texas A&M University Member of the Texas A&M University System William Allan Kritsonis, PhD Professor PhD Program in Educational Leadership The Whitelowe R. Green College of Education Prairie View A&M University Member of the Texas A&M University System Visiting Lecturer (2005) Oxford Round Table University of Oxford, Oxford, England Distinguished Alumnus (2004) College of Education and Professional Studies Central Washington University ABSTRACT Using an iterative process similar to the constant comparative method, 34 studies addressing the impact of the learning disabled label were synthesized. Four overlapping primary themes emerged: expectancies, stereotypes, and attitudes; stigmatization, rejection, and social distance; action versus attitude toward labeled individuals; and differential influence of the LD label when other salient information is provided. See: www.nationalforum.com
  • 2. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS 2___________________________________________________________________________________________ Introduction The confusion and conflict surrounding definition, diagnosis, and treatment of learning disabilities is well documented. Debate over definition is common and surprisingly heated. Learning disabilities manifest in diverse ways, making diagnosis difficult. Lacking associated physical signs or symptoms, learning disabilities comprise an invisible disability. As a result, students so labeled are frequently considered simply lacking in motivation or commitment. Ong- Dean (2005) points out that society may view invisible disabilities as “illegitimate excuses for failure to conform” (p. 148) to achievement standards and other expectations. Alternately, the LD diagnosis is sometimes misconstrued by laypersons as a form of mental retardation, with myriad misconceptions deriving from this error. Labeled students, their co-learners, and teachers often experience considerable frustration in negotiating the legal, ethical, educational, and social mazes that learning disabilities present. When ambiguity and the attendant professional disagreement regarding a disorder are ongoing, the question of whether the designation should be used at all inevitably arises. Does such a diagnosis hinder more than help those so labeled? Labeling theory, a sociological model, proposes that labeling of individuals as “different” in the negative connotation of the word creates a potentially distorted reality for those who bear the label, as well as for their teachers, parents, and peers (Hebding & Glick, 1987). Labeling theory further predicts that once an individual is labeled, “the social group seems to assign to that person a new identity and a new role, a new set of expectations. The social group then responds to the individual according to those expectations, thus reinforcing the label and affecting all future interactions” (p. 136). The labeled individual’s self-perception is inextricably bound to others’ perceptions and reactions. As Poole, Regoli, and Pogrebin (1986) suggest, individuals “are not passive recipients of negative labels; rather, they are actively managing or coping with these labels” (p. 347). A self-fulfilling prophecy might result when a person accepts the label and incorporates it into her or his self-conceptualization, with reduced performance expectations and damaged self-esteem as unfortunate byproducts (McGrew & Evans, 2003; Rosenthal, 2002). Reduced effort and lower achievement might logically follow. Others engaged in the debate believe that labels serve useful purposes with a net positive result. Keogh (1987) opines that the LD label serves “as a focus for advocacy and for ensuring attention to the problem, as a category or mechanism for providing services and as a condition or set of conditions that require scientific study” (pp. 4-5). Kuther (1994) adds that diagnostic labels facilitate research for practitioners, which hopefully results in tailored interventions and program improvement. Opposing those who support inclusion and elimination of the LD label, Hallahan and Kauffman (1994) go so far as to endorse development of a “culture of disability.” They suggest that reduced stigma and enhanced learning might be achieved by “developing esprit de corps among congregations of people with disabilities” (p. 505). The Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act 2004 (IDEIA, 2004) speaks to the labeling issue repeatedly. Particularly germane to the concerns of this paper, the new statute prescribes “early intervening services to reduce the need to label children as disabled in order to address the learning and behavioral needs of such children.” The statement is first seen in the 2004 statute, bearing subtle testimony to the recognized dangers of label application.
  • 3. KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS ____________________________________________________________________________3 Purpose of the Article Divergent opinions on the ramifications of the “learning disabled” label abound. The purpose of this study is a synthesis of research related to the effects of the label. Herein, “label” will connote a designation assignment by qualified school personnel, psychologists, and other diagnostic professionals. The reader should be cognizant that informal labeling – as well as self- identification – might certainly impact the labeled individual as well. Research Questions Study selection, examination, and synthesis were guided by the following research questions: 1. How does being formally labeled as learning disabled affect labeled students? 2. Are teachers’ perceptions, attitudes, expectations, and behaviors different for labeled and nonlabeled students? 3. How do nonlabeled co-learners perceive and react to peers identified as learning disabled? Methodology Using all professional databases available, conscientious efforts were made to identify, locate, and collect the entire population of relevant studies from 1970 through 2000. Several promising studies failed to delineate students’ diagnoses precisely, while others did not adequately distinguish the influence of the label from the cumulative impact of experiences germinated by the symptoms of the disorder. The term “learning disabilities” denotes very different phenomena in different countries (Hart, 1999). In response to these and other idiosyncrasies of this particular literature, the following selection criteria evolved: 1. Studies must report data specific to the LD label as a distinct phenomenon, rather than data based on aggregated diagnostic labels. 2. Studies must directly address the formal LD label as distinguished from the actual disorder. 3. Studies involving two or more diagnoses within a single individual described in case studies or presented in vignettes were excluded. 4. Due to differential use of relevant labels across national boundaries, only studies conducted in the United States were utilized in this synthesis.
  • 4. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS 4___________________________________________________________________________________________ Application of these selection criteria yielded 33 studies. Twenty-eight employed quantitative methodologies, and five were qualitative. One quantitative research effort involved two independent reports, yielding a total of 34 studies. The decision to include both quantitative and qualitative studies was a carefully considered one. Despite the obvious philosophical and other distinctions between the two research types, synthesis of diverse approaches has become increasingly utilized and appreciated in recent years (Collins, Onwuegbuzie, & Sutton, 2006). Miles and Huberman (1994) offer strong support for this approach when they state, “The careful measurement, generalizable samples, experiment control, and statistical tools of good quantitative studies are precious assets. When they are combined with the up-close, real-world contexts that characterize good qualitative studies, we have a very powerful mix (p. 42). Specific methods used to analyze and combine results from the 34 identified reports were necessarily driven by the characteristics of the data collected. An iterative approach similar to the constant comparative method was employed (Boeije, 2002; Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Descriptive synopses were developed for each study. The synthesist was alert to potential commonalities and patterns, analogous to the concept of emerging themes in the qualitative literature. She kept an informal record of such intuitions and insights, generating tentative labels for individual clusters. Methodological, theoretical, temporal, and subject-related patterns were of interest, though receptivity to other potential commonalities was a hallmark of the iterative process. Each new synopsis presented potential for addition to and reorganization of groupings previously considered. During this phase, data collection and data analysis proceeded simultaneously and interactively. After completion of the 34 descriptive synopses, all were reviewed by one of the authors and another faculty member in psychology, and sorted with regard to the final iteration of clusters. Many studies fell into multiple clusters. Examination of clusters yielded four overlapping themes or patterns. Again, studies often contributed to more than one theme. Results and Discussion The four overlapping themes or patterns that eventually emerged follow. 1. Expectancies, stereotypes, and attitudes related to the LD label 2. Stigmatization, rejection, and social distance related to the LD label 3. Action versus attitude 4. Differential influence of the LD label given other salient information Each of the four themes is addressed below. Selected studies from the synthesis are cited in support of the themes. Additional research affording further clarification is cited as well.
  • 5. KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS ____________________________________________________________________________5 Expectancies, Stereotypes, and Attitudes Related to the LD Label A review of the literature relevant to sociological labeling theory offers relatively strong support for the view that negative or deviant labels generate unfortunate expectancies. A number of studies addressed this particular theme – some more directly than others. Minner (1982) investigated the academic and behavioral expectations for students labeled with learning disabilities and students labeled with educable mental retardation. A “no-label” group served as the control. Vocational teachers read a vignette describing student attributes germane to academic performance, as well as typical behaviors exhibited. Some subjects received vignettes in which the student was described as learning disabled, while others were led to believe that their student had educable mentally retardation. The research reports a significant negative effect for diagnostic labels. Additionally, post hoc tests revealed that nonlabeled students with negative descriptions and students labeled LD with positive descriptions were not differentiated. Even more surprisingly, teachers made no distinction between LD students with promising attributes and those described negatively. Minner and Prater (1984) asked college teachers to judge the academic promise of a student portrayed as having learning disabilities, as well as their own ability to work successfully with such college students. According to the researchers, “the LD label significantly and negatively influenced faculty members’ initial expectations” (p. 228). They were less definitive with regard to the impact of the label on teachers’ perceived ability to work effectively with the LD student. In a subsequent study, Minner (1990) reports that teachers were significantly less likely to refer a child labeled LD to a gifted program, even though descriptive vignettes were otherwise identical to those describing other children who were referred. For teachers familiar with the discrepancy formula so often applied in diagnosing LD, Minner’s vignette may have been confusing since it described a student with congruent achievement and intelligence. Though results are not entirely consistent, these and other quantitative studies suggest that the LD label has potentially negative implications for those who bear it. Relevant themes from qualitative studies were examined to determine whether context-specific information would inform the quantitative data. Albinger (1995) quotes one young man as saying, “Mrs. Albinger, if you make me keep coming to resource, I’ll just be a bum on the street [pointing out the window]. All the bums out there went to resource!” (p. 621) The painful impact of such an expectation on the boy’s self-esteem cannot be overstated. Several students reported repeated name-calling incidents involving pejorative terms such as “stupid” and “retard.” Labeled students reported insensitive teacher behavior as well. One instructor criticized a labeled child’s academic efforts publicly, chastising her for acting like a kindergartner. Barga (1996) quotes a math teacher who fondly called a favorite labeled student “D.D.” – an abbreviation for “Darling Dummy.” In summary, analysis suggests that the learning disabilities label generates reduced or negative expectations, as well as negative stereotypes and attitudes. Lower expectations often translate into reduced effort and lower achievement. Studies characterized by design flaws and inadequate reporting may reduce confidence in such conclusions, however. Further and better research is essential in clarifying this issue. IDEIA 2004 indirectly supports this premise, stating: “Almost 30 years of research and experience has demonstrated that the education of children with disabilities can be made more effective by … having high expectations for such children.”
  • 6. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS 6___________________________________________________________________________________________ Stigmatization, Rejection, and Social Distance Related to the LD Label Several studies included in this synthesis suggest that children designated as learning disabled experience emotional and physical isolation as a result. Cartledge, Frew, and Zaharias (1985) report that fourth- and fifth-graders rated a child labeled LD significantly more negatively on 12 of 15 questions presented than they did a similar boy labeled “non-handicapped.” Though the labeled boy was rated as less likely to treat others children unkindly, students expressed lower levels of interest in establishing a friendship with him. Researchers did not assess level of student understanding of the term learning disabled. With very young subjects, such a determination might be especially important. The concepts of expectancy and stereotypy have overlapping and cyclical relationships with stigmatization, rejection, and social distance. Studies purporting to address one of these phenomena often subtly inform our understanding of the others. For example, the previously cited study on academic expectations for college students with learning disabilities (Minner & Prater, 1984) is relevant to the second theme as well. College faculty members made a distinction between the labeled and nonlabeled students with regard to predictions of academic success, as well as the teachers’ perceived ability to work with the individual portrayed. Upon superficial consideration of these outcomes, one might deduce that faculty members feel underprepared for the challenge rather than averse to the student. Unsolicited comments written on the questionnaires suggest otherwise. One professor wrote, “We cannot allow everyone into college – the integrity of the B.A. degree cannot be challenged” (p. 228). “I am trained to teach bright students, not handicapped ones,” opined another. These remarks imply stigmatization, rejection, and desire for increased social distance from students with learning disabilities. Research on stigmatization and rejection relative to the LD label is not entirely consistent. Cohen’s (1977) study cited previously used the Mann-Whitney U to compare numbers of positive adjectives attributed to various student groups. Teacher responses did suggest a significant bias against students labeled LD compared to those identified as “normal.” Though this might seem purely a study of attitudes or stereotypes, an additional manipulation of the variables informs acceptance versus rejection due to label. When the written vignette described a learning disabled individual but no actual label was provided, lower ratings were given than when the same description was labeled LD. Perhaps individuals displaying atypical behaviors are more likely to be stigmatized and rejected when there is no ready explanation for their “difference.” Studies that fail to distinguish clearly the impact of the LD label from effects of the actual disorder (and thus are ineligible for actual inclusion in the synthesis) enhance understanding of stigmatization. When physical segregation from nonlabeled co-learners is part of labeled students’ experience, feelings of stigmatization are legitimized regardless of whether the label is appropriately applied. Albinger (1995) reports that labeled students’ efforts to avoid stigmatization usually included fabricated stories about their activities apart from time with nonlabeled students. Some children carried the lie for several years, as when one student told friends that she took piano lessons at a certain time each day. Another child told friends on several occasions that he arrived late due to problems with the family car. In his qualitative study on African American and Hispanic American teenagers who bear the LD label in New York schools, Bryant (1989) offers even sadder commentary on the
  • 7. KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS ____________________________________________________________________________7 embarrassment experienced by labeled students. One individual reported that students in the resource room would try to conceal their identities when regular education students walked by. “Regular ed. kids could come by and everyone would cover their faces. We’d cover our faces and we would all hide because we’d be embarrassed” (p. 91). Many students reported that “pullout” programs were especially problematic, as nonlabeled students bore witness when labeled students were called to a special place for special students. The social distancing phenomenon inherent in such segregation is a concern as well. Even labeled students who consider the LD diagnosis a relief or welcome explanation for previously misunderstood “differences” suggest that physical separation from non-labeled peer is disheartening and potentially stigmatizing (Barga, 1996). IDEIA 2004 lends credence to the importance of minimizing social and physical difference, stating that “the education of children with disabilities can be made more effective by … ensuring their access to the general education curriculum in the regular classroom, to the maximum extent possible.” Action versus Attitude The potential pain derived from the first two themes to emerge from this synthesis should certainly not be diminished. Some evidence suggests, however, that attitudes toward those labeled learning disabled are typically more negative than actions taken. In other words, disheartening responses to attitude and acceptability scales notwithstanding, nonlabeled others may behave more responsibly when action is required of them. Certainly, this is not new idea. Social psychologists have long wondered at the apparent disconnect sometimes observed between attitudes and behavior (Brehm & Kassin, 1996; Ajzen, 2000). Cohen (1977) illustrates this phenomenon in her dissertation involving teachers’ stereotypes of three groups: learning disabled students, individuals whose reading requires remediation (RR), and their normal peers. Teachers consistently ascribed more negatively connotative adjectives to LD and RR label-bearers compared to nonlabeled students. Yet they did not translate these negative perceptions or attitudes into discriminatory action. Regardless of the label applied and the information provided, teachers in Cohen’s study were able to score all students’ essays fairly and accurately. Thus, their expectations did not dictate their behavior. Several studies comparing diagnostic labels but lacking an unlabeled control group lend support to the suggestion that attitude and action are relatively independent phenomena. Boucher and Deno (1979) found that teachers who read a three-page case study with either the LD or ED label affixed were capable of objective programming decisions. The ED label was more consistently recalled than the LD label, which might suggest a stronger reaction to the former. Yet any differential reaction to the two labels was not translated into behavioral distinctions. Fairbanks and Stinnett (1997) tapped a more diverse subject population. Teachers, school psychologists, and school workers were presented with a vignette describing a third-grader with a pattern of disruptive behavior. Different diagnostic labels were assigned to the hypothetical child for different subjects; LD, behaviorally disordered (BD), and ADD ascriptions were alternately made. When asked to judge the acceptability of one of two possible intervention strategies, no labeling effects were noted. However, no nonlabeled control group is in evidence.
  • 8. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS 8___________________________________________________________________________________________ Additionally, no attitude or expectancy measure was taken for comparison with the more action- oriented plan for intervention. This was not the stated purpose of the study, so the researchers cannot be faulted here. The report states, “an attempt was made to distribute each of the labels and interventions equally within each of the groups” (p. 330). This was perhaps a practical alternative to random distribution within groups or across groups, given the relatively small sample sizes and the large number of treatment combinations. In summary, studies seem to suggest that prejudicial attitudes toward individuals labeled LD do not necessarily result in discriminatory behavior. The research on stigmatization and social distance might be interpreted as suggesting quite the opposite, however, at least among student populations. Differential Influence of the LD Label Given Other Salient Information A fourth theme emerging from the data involves whether the LD label loses potency when additional information is available to moderate it. Huebner (1990) asked school psychologists to make diagnostic and placement decisions for fictitious students, providing them with a student profile, identification as having learning disabilities or as “normal,” and current diagnostic testing data. Results indicate that only current psychological test results influenced psychologists’ decisions. In other words, even when a student was currently in a special placement for her or his disabilities, most psychologists were quite willing to declare that neither the special placement nor the label was necessary. Huebner considers this outcome evidence that confirmation bias may not be as strong as previously believed. Similarly, Dukes (1987) found that expectancies were mediated by exposure to additional salient information. Elementary teachers’ pretest scores reflected the predicted expectancy effects for learning disabled students. However, following exposure to two videos in which teacher and students interacted in a classroom setting, posttest scores were not impacted by label. Sichel (1984) designed a creative study to investigate interaction effects between the LD label and teacher personality, as well as several elements irrelevant to the current synthesis. Results on a dogmatism scale enabled him to separate subjects into two groups representing opposite ends of the dogmatic/open-minded continuum. Several dependent measures offered impressive triangulation. Teachers evaluated an essay reportedly written by a hypothetical student who was described to half the subjects as learning disabled. This study is unique among the quantitative selections because subjects actually interacted with the student rather than reading vignettes or viewing videotapes. Though the student was a confederate of the researcher, this design allowed a closer approximation to reality than most others found in the literature. Interpersonal impressions constituted one dependent variable, while objective observers’ ratings of teachers’ support and liking for the student were examined as well. The presence or absence of the LD label had no impact on outcomes. Teachers were similarly objective, supportive, and pleasant regardless of the student’s ascribed status. Graham and Dwyer (1987) studied the degree of objectivity that college students could muster when faced with grading an essay reportedly written by a student labeled learning disabled. Control group participants believed that nonlabeled students wrote the essays they read.
  • 9. KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS ____________________________________________________________________________9 Though results indicated that the LD label did affect scores assigned, the quality of the essay wielded even greater influence. These and other studies offer relatively strong support for the view that salient information ameliorates label bias. Concluding Remarks In conclusion, despite design and conceptual flaws evident in several studies utilized in this synthesis, tentative conclusions may be drawn. Overall, assignment of the learning disabled label is associated with teachers’ lowered expectations, as well as more negative stereotypes and attitudes toward the labeled individual. Most synthesized studies employed written vignettes or brief videotaped segments, both of which are rather artificial stimuli. Others failed to employ appropriate control groups. However, when children rather consistently rate hypothetical peers labeled LD as less acceptable, less socially attractive, and even less valuable, the potential social and emotional implications for real children so labeled are disheartening. Labeled individuals report stigmatization and both social and physical distancing from nonlabeled peers, factors that render the school experience even less appealing for those targeted. These unfortunate conditions notwithstanding, evidence indicates that negative perceptions of the labeled student do not necessarily generate biased grading practices or inappropriate programming. Further, compelling evidence suggests that the negative impact of the label is diminished when other salient information is available. Sensitivity and diversity training, as well as educational efforts aimed at increasing all stakeholders’ understanding of learning disabilities, might further reduce the negative impact of the label. This synthesis included studies published from 1970 through 2000 that met selection criteria delineated earlier. However, 27 of the 34 studies were published before 1990, perhaps skewing results relative to all themes identified. With hope that progress has been made regarding attitudes toward diversity in general and learning differences in particular, future researchers might compare studies from different decades including those published most recently. The debate regarding the potentially negative outcomes of being labeled learning disabled as opposed to possible benefits derived is ongoing. As Sack-Min (2007) notes, however, applying a diagnostic label to an individual “is a profound decision that affects the rest of his or her educational career and life” (p. 23). Continued exploration of the clarity and utility of the label itself is warranted. References Ajzen, I. (2000). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 27-58. Albinger, P. (1995) Stories from the resource room: Piano lessons, imaginary illness, and broken-down cars. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 615-621.
  • 10. NATIONAL JOURNAL: FOCUS ON COLLEGES, UNIVERSITIES, AND SCHOOLS 10___________________________________________________________________________________________ Barga, N. K. (1996). Students with learning disabilities in education: Managing a disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 413-421. Boeije, H. (2002). A purposeful approach to the constant comparative method in the analysis of qualitative interviews. Quality and Quantity, 36(4), 391-409. Boucher, C. R., & Deno, S. L. (1979). Learning disabled and emotionally disturbed: Will the labels affect teacher planning? Psychology in the Schools, 16, 395-402. Brehm, S. S., & Kassin, S. M. (1996). Social psychology (3rd ed.). Boston,MA: Houghton Mifflin. Bryant, G. (1989). The experience of junior high school minority students in special education who are labeled “learning disabled”: A qualitative study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University, New York. Cartledge, G., Frew, T., & Zaharias, J. (1985). Social skill needs of mainstreamed students: Peer and teacher perceptions. Learning Disability Quarterly, 8, 132-140. Cohen, L. G. (1977). Stereotyping by teachers of remedial reading, learning disabled, and normal students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Boston. Collins, K. M. T., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Sutton, I. L. (2006). A model incorporating the rationale and purpose for conducting mixed methods research in special education and beyond. Learning Disabilities. A Contemporary Journal, 4(1), 67-100. Dukes, M. (1987). The impact of the learning disabilities label upon teachers’ evaluations Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville. Fairbanks, L. D., & Stinnett, T. A. (1997). Effects of professional group membership, intervention type, and diagnostic label on treatment acceptability. Psychology in the Schools, 34(4), 329-335. Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago,IL: Aldine. Graham, S., & Dwyer, A. (1987). Effects of the learning disability label, quality of writing performance, and examiner’s level of expertise on the evaluation of written products. Reston, VA: National Center for Research on Handicapped and Gifted Children. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 202 520). Hallahan, D. P., & Kauffman, J. M. (1994). Toward a culture of disability in the aftermath of Deno and Dunn. Journal of Special Education, 27, 496-508. Hart, H. (1999). Terminology to benefit children. Development Medicine and Child Neurology 41, 651. Hebding, D. E., & Glick, L. (1987). Introduction to sociology: A text with readings (3rd ed.). New York: Random House. Huebner, E. S. (1990). The generalizability of the confirmation bias among school psychologists. School Psychology International, 11, 281-286. Individuals with Disabilities Improvement Act of 2004, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq. Keogh, B. K. (1987). Learning disabilities: In defense of a construct. Learning Disabilities Research, 3(1), 4-9. Kuther, T. L. (1994). Diagnostic classification of children within the educational system: Should it be eliminated? Reston, VA: National Center for Research on Handicapped and Gifted Children. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 393 259). McGrew, K. S., & Evans, J. (2003). Expectations for students with cognitive disabilities: Is the cup half empty or half full? Can the cup flow over? (Synthesis Report 55). Minneapolis,
  • 11. KAREN OSTERHOLM, WILLIAM R. NASH, AND WILLIAM ALLAN KRITSONIS ____________________________________________________________________________11 MN: University of Minnesota, National Center on Educational Outcomes. Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. E. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Minner, S. (1990). Teacher evaluations of case descriptions of LD gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 34, 37-39. Minner, S. (1982). Expectancies of vocational teachers for handicapped students. Exceptional Children, 48, 451-453. Minner, S., & Prater, G. (1984). College teachers’ expectations of LD students. Academic Therapy, 20, 225-229. Ong-Dean, C. (2005). Reconsidering the social location of the medical model: An examination of disability in parenting literature. Journal of Medical Humanities, 26(2/3), 141-158. Poole, E. D., Regoli, R. M., & Pogrebin, M. R. (1986). A study of the effects of self-labeling and public labeling. The Social Science Journal, 23, 345-360. Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communications in classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubicles. American Psychologist, 57(11), 839-849. Sack-Min, J. (2007). The issues of IDEA. American School Board Journal, 194(3), 20-25. Sichel, S. M. (1984). The effects of a handicapped label, its source and teacher personality on teacher responses to a student. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University, New York. Formatted by Dr. Mary Alice Kritsonis, National Research and Manuscript Preparation Editor, National FORUM Journals, Houston, Texas www.nationalforum.com