SlideShare uma empresa Scribd logo
1 de 51
Abstract
This study compared aversive learning through solely social observational learning
and derived avoidance learning using a recorded learning model video where they receiving a
mild electric shock when a selected circle (CS+) is shown on-screen. The stimulus will
predict the delivery of shock (CS+) whilst another contextually similar stimulus will present
no negative consequences (CS-). Participants are instructed of an avoidance behaviour
response (AV+).The inferred condition participants will be presented with RCP trials of non-
arbitrary and arbitrary training and testing of Same and Opposite relations, as shown in
Dymond et al., (2008) study, to test for transformation of functions and derived relational
responding in later trials i.e. avoidance acquisition and extinction.
The between- subject factor Group has two levels; observed and inferred. Two within-
subject factors (1) Conditioned Stimulus, which has two levels: CS+ and CS-. (2) Learned
Behaviour which has two levels: Avoidance (AV) and Non-Avoidance (NAV). Thus
combination of the three factors created six experimental conditions. The DerivedCS+ will be
compared against the inferred response rates of CS+ and CS-. There were three dependent
variables: Avoidance behaviour responses of the shock in each phase, expectancy of shock
taken from Likert scales at the end of each phase, and AAQ-II results.
Comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS- and Inferred EXT CS+ and CS-; no
significant differences were found between the learning groups and conditioned stimuli,
showing the two learning groups were just as effective as each other in producing a fear-
evoked avoidance response. In a 2*2*2 multi-way mixed ANOVA, results showed
significant main effects for conditioned stimuli, learned behaviour and learning group.
Comparing group against conditioned stimuli and learned behaviour (Figure 8 and 9) it is
clear that Observed rated higher in expectancy of shock to both, this could be attributed to the
1
observed group understanding the CS-US contingency and the value of CS- more than the
Inferred group.
In conclusion, the study found that observed group may have been taught through
direct means, by watching the learning model in the avoidance acquisition phase. Future
studies would amend this. The inferred condition was successful as derived relational
responding was produced.
2
A common debate in the topic of fear learning is how fear is firstly acquired, be it
through direct or indirect means. Fear acquisitions to environmental stimuli are fundamental
to the fight or flight mechanism, thus essential for survival of human and non-humans alike.
Understanding fear conditioning will develop and strengthen the understanding of phobias
and anxiety disorders to help build clinical models (Barot, Chung, Kim & Bernstein, 2009;
Field, 2006).
The first model of classical fear conditioning was coined Pavlovian conditioning and
suggested fear acquisition of an object can only surface through first-hand experience with an
aversive stimulus (Olsson, Nearing & Phelps, 2007; Siddle & Bond, 1988). The individual,
be it human or non-human, acquires a conditioned fear response (CR) to a formerly neutral
stimulus (CS) through direct experience pairing with an aversive occurrence, the
unconditioned stimulus (US). This acquisition was thought to be maintained by a two-factor
theory i.e. the escape from the CS and instant decrease in fear eliciting factors. This model is
famously illustrated in the ‘Little Albert’ study (Watson & Rayner, 1920) where they
conditioned a fear response to a previously neutral CS i.e. white rabbit/rat/dog, by pairing
CS-US (loud noise). Results showed Little Albert generalised and elicited a fear response not
only to white rabbit etc., but also Santa Claus and white hair.
Research has shown that the amygdala is essential in the acquisition, storage and
expression of the conditioned fear (Davis & Whalen, 2001; Kim & Jung, 2006; Olsson et al.,
2007). Barot, Kyono, Clark & Bernstein’s (2008) study showed clear visualisations of
neurons within the amygdala receiving CS-US convergence information, during acquisition
phase of a learned association. Barot et al., (2009) study showed the interaction of the
amygdala and the hippocampus, where the hippocampus processes and transmits contextual
CS information to the amygdala i.e. hippocampus is responsible for contextual learning
whilst amygdala responsible for cue learning (Olsson & Phelps, 2007; Phillips & LeDoux,
3
1992). Damage to these areas is coupled with reports of CS-US contingency learning but
inhibition of autonomic response (LaBar, LeDoux, Spencer & Phelps, 1995). Delgado, Jou,
Ledoux & Phelps (2009) also illustrated the role of the striatum due to its reward-related
processing ability to rule emotions and avoid negative outcomes. Participants were instructed
upon seeing the aversive stimulus they could avoid shock by choosing the correct action
(AV+). This depicts a mechanism of active coping, where the striatum exerts control over the
emotional responses to influence the decision making (Delgado, Li, Schiller & Phelps, 2008;
LeDoux & Gorman, 2001; Schultz, 2006). These studies clearly illustrate the networks of
learning paradigms, storage and reinforcer types.
However, over the years research grew and the understanding that fear can only be
acquired through direct means was soon diminished. The Pavlovian Model was suggested to
be extended to consider indirect forms. Field (2006) suggested two pathways of avoidance;
(1) vicarious (observational) learning and (2) verbal information i.e. instructions. Another
issue with the Pavlovian Model was the two-factor theory. Seligman (1968) developed a
variation of the model and described the safety-signal theory, which considered an acquired
conditioned emotional response (CER) only to a signal that predicted the onset of shock, for
any other signal a conditioned response of pressing for food was shown. (Lovibond, Chen,
Mitchell & Weidemann, 2011). The safety-signal theory can be applied to this present study
as it is the intention that participants will produce an avoidance response to the CS-US
contingency and not for any other stimuli, unless derived.
Humans’ current socio-cultural environment can present indirect means of obtaining
fearful stimuli through verbal communications and social modulation i.e. expressions,
imitation etc. Mineka & Cook (1993) hypothesized that indirect fear learning is procedurally
the same as direct fear learning, and can be just as effective. This happens by the observation
of another’s reaction to the CS acting as a US. Others reaction elicits anxiety and therefore a
4
fear response; similarly seen in non-human studies such as monkeys (Cook & Mineka, 1990).
Non-human studies have shed light onto indirect fear learning by showing their abilities to
perceive and react accordingly to external signs of fear and pain. The neural workings aid in
assigning and raising awareness of a threat value to context or cue associated with the threat
(Hauser, 1996; Kavaliers, Choleris, Colwell, 2001). Social learning of aversive stimuli
through a learning model can be explained through several means but for this study the
concentration will be on imitation and empathy. In relation to empathy; Knapska, Mikosz,
Werka & Maren (2009) showed socially transmitted fear in rats through social interaction. If
a rat had previously interacted with a recently fear-conditioned partner, they exhibited
defensive behaviour during the novel task, hypothesizing the social interaction acted as a US
leading the rats to empathize.
Kavaliers et al., (2001) studied mice and their socially mediated acquisition of fear to
the aversive stimulus of biting flies. ‘Observer’ mice watched ‘demonstrator’ mice being bit
by flies and for avoidance exhibited analgesia and self-burying. When exposed to non-biting
flies, the observer mice displayed the conditioned analgesia and self-burying as observed
from before. This imitation has also been shown in humans. Self-reports from children about
their unexplained fear of the dark/animals explain they are developed from observing their
parents fearful emotions in contextual situations (Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006). This was shown
in Gerull & Rapee (2002) when mother displays negative facial expressions towards novel
stimuli, the odds of toddler imitating fearful expressions and avoidance behaviour increases.
Helsen, Goubert, Peters & Vlaeven (2011) demonstrated the effects of observational learning
on pain related fear and if it was associated with increased pain ratings. Self-reports of fear of
pain and expected pain ratings were taken after observing a learning model, whilst actual pain
and avoidance measures were taken post-exposure, and found observing another person
performing the task ended in a higher reporting of intense and unpleasant pain. Guzmán,
5
Tronson, Guedea, Huh, Gao, & Radulovic (2009) showed that even though models on
humans and non-humans have shown that observational learning instils fearful conditioning
after observing others it can also ease some forms of fear. In fear conditioning mice, they
found a social buffering process served to prevent a long-term induction of fear to stressful
events. This was also shown by Egliston & Rapee (2007) when investigating fear and
avoidance learning for toddlers in three conditions (1) modelling group – positive feedback
from mother about aversive stimuli (2) exposed to aversive stimuli alone (3) control group.
All toddlers were then exposed to observational conditioning trial where the experimenter
reacted fearfully of the aversive stimuli. Results showed toddlers in (1) were more likely to
show positive affective reactions in comparison to the other groups, showing the power of
positive parental reactions, a social buffer, to fear evoking stimuli which can over-ride
negative reactions.
All past literature shows fears learned through observational learning engage in the
same neural mechanisms as direct learning, signifying indirect means may be just as effective
and powerful as direct fear conditioning.
Another form of indirect learning is inferred/derived relational responding which can
be best explained through the relational frame theory (RFT) which proposes that humans
transfer fear and avoidance responses across stimuli (Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes,
Smeets, Cullinan & Leader, 2004; Healy, Barnes-Holmes & Smeets, 2000). An important
process which can be undermined is the verbal and language processes which contribute to
the avoidance response of indirect conditioning i.e. if a person has a spider phobia, but has
never directly had fear-evoking experiences with a spider, the word ‘spider’ can act as a CS.
Upon hearing the word ‘spider’, the individual elicits an alarmed response and seeks
avoidance. This transfer of stimulus functions can also be seen through topographically
different ‘spider-like’ stimuli i.e. real-life spiders, pictures, etc. The effect of verbal
6
information has been shown to result from associate learning (Dymond & Rehfeldt, 2000;
Field, 2006). By the functional definition of verbal events, there is the possibility of a
contribution of indirectly acquired human avoidance learning in two concepts: derived
relational responding and the transformation of functions.
Augustson & Dougmer (1997) illustrated transformation of avoidance functions
through training an ‘aversive class’ A1-B1-C1-D1 and a ‘non-aversive class’ A2-B2-C2-D2
then Pavlovian conditioning coupled B1 with shock and B2 without. A differential signalled
avoidance task was issued where participants could avoid shock by pressing a key in relation
to B1 appearing. Results showed that all participants during stimulus presentations of the
aversive class elicited avoidance responses in comparison of non-aversive class, which they
did not. This study was the first of its kind to depict how avoidance can be present in the
absence of direct conditioning. This transformation of functions, i.e. establishing a particular
behavioural response for one stimuli of an equivalence relation and then transferring the
function for its multiple relations, was also seen in Forsyth, Eifert & Barrios’ (2006) study
where by training A-B and B-C, participants derived B-A, C-B, A-C and C-A. If ‘B’ was
paired with a fear-evoking response, then participants would derive to produce fearful
behaviour for ‘A’ as well as the trained relation of ‘C’.
RFT also proposes other forms of transfer of avoidance, other than stimulus
equivalence based accounts, such as the relational frames of Same and Opposite can be learnt
by humans (Dymond, Roche, Forsyth, Whelan & Rhoden, 2007; Dymond, S., Roche, B.,
Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. 2008; Whelan & Barnes-Holmes, 2004). Dymond et
al., (2008) study exposed participants to a relational completion procedure (RCP) of non-
arbitrary and arbitrary testing and training to establish Same and Opposite and derived
relations, which has been shown to be an effective procedure (Dymond & Whelan, 2010).
Participants were taught all possible Same and Opposite relations of A1-B1-C1 and A2-B2-
7
C2 which was followed by an avoidance conditioning procedure where the participants learnt
to escape the discriminative stimulus (SD
) of aversive images and sounds by pressing the
space bar and not omitting the same avoidance response to B2. They were also tested for
transformation of functions for C1 and C2 and found participants that learnt the
transformation of functions also displayed correct derived relational responding, this depicts
the ability of humans to complete this task.
The aim of this study is to examine the effectiveness of observed and derived
relational learning on pathways of avoidance by exposing each participant to a fear and
avoidance conditioning phase. The observed group will watch a video of a learning model
completing a Pavlovian conditioning and avoidance acquisition phase. The video depicts a
learning model receiving a mild electric shock when a selected circle (CS+) is shown on the
screen; where the learning models facial expressions and arm movements whilst receiving the
shock will serve as the US. The stimulus will predict the delivery of shock (CS+) whilst
another contextually similar stimulus will present no negative consequences (CS-).
Participants are instructed that they can avoid the CS+ if they choose the correct button
(AV+). The inferred condition participants will be presented with RCP trials of non-arbitrary
and arbitrary training and testing of Same and Opposite relations, as shown in Dymond et al.,
(2008) study, to test for transformation of functions and derived relational responding in later
trials i.e. avoidance acquisition and extinction. The participants will have the AV+ option to
avoid the upcoming CS+ shock.
The participant’s expectancy of the shock on stimuli will be recorded through Likert
Scales and its two factors of Avoidance and Non-Avoidance, at the end of certain phases (see
Method). Bass, van Ooijen, Goudriann & Kenemans (2008) showed that a failure to learn the
CS-US contingency would result in a higher contextual fear, and then participants would self-
report higher levels of anxiety, which was confirmed by skin conductance (SCR) measures,
8
any indiscrepancies will be followed up in the data. Behavioural avoidance will be recorded
through percentage scores for each participant from eliciting an avoidance response for CS+
and CS- for each appropriate phase. An Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-II;
Bond et al., 2011) will be undertaken by each participants. AAQ-II is a reliable psychometric
7-item scale to measure participant’s willingness to experience unwanted private events in the
intention to reach individual goals and values i.e. ‘My painful memories prevent me from
having a fulfilling life’ which participants would rate from 1=never true to 7=always true.
López et al., (2010) found that people with high AAQ-II scores displayed a higher number of
thought intrusions and higher susceptibility to a lower concentration level to the task at hand;
this will be considered for any rogue data. The results of AAQ-II will be tested against
behavioural avoidance responses to test for any correlations.
An interaction between group (observed vs. inferred) and the three dependent
variables (DV) of avoidance behaviour responses of the shock, expectancy of shock and
AAQ-II will show any superior effectiveness of each group. The IV is conditioned stimuli
(CS+, CS-). This study based upon a two-way hypothesis as it is searching for the
effectiveness of each learning group.
9
Method
Participants
A total of sixty participants (21 males and 39 females), were randomly placed in
either the observed or inferred condition, with 30 in each condition. All recruited participants
had English as their first language and were aged between 19-40 years old (M= 21.9, SD =
3.91). They were recruited via opportunistic, personal contacts and Swansea University’s
Subject Pool. All participants were given an information sheet and a consent form (Appendix
1) before participating in the study, and they were given a debrief form at the end (Appendix
2). They participated in return for either course credit or £5.
Design
The between- subject factor is Group which has two levels: observed (30 participants)
and inferred (30 participants). There are two within-subject factors (1) Conditioned Stimulus
which has two levels: CS+ and CS-. (2) Learned Behaviour which has two levels: Avoidance
(AV) and Non-Avoidance (NAV). Thus combination of the three factors created six
experimental conditions. The DerivedCS+ will be compared against the inferred response
rates of CS+ and CS-. There are three dependent variables: Avoidance behaviour responses of
the shock in each phase, expectancy of shock (taken from Likert scales at the end of each
phase), and AAQ-II results. (Appendix 3).
Apparatus
The computer system was Microsoft Windows XP Professional Version 2002 on a
17’’ LCD colour monitor, 1024 by 768 resolution. The computer program was written in
Visual Basic® 6.0 which controlled all stimulus presentations and recorded all responses for
both conditions. The electrode was 5cm in diameter, the program used for the production of
electric shocks was PowerLab 2/25 Chart 5 for Windows.
10
Stimuli
Observed Condition
A movie was produced for the observation stage of the experiment. The movie (3 min,
8s) depicted a male participant (the learning model) participating in a differential fear
conditioning experiment (Figure 1). Two coloured circles (red and blue) served as CS and
were presented in pseudo-random order for 3s on a computer screen in front of the learning
model. The appearance of the circles was incorporated with an inter-stimulus interval (ITI)
which varied between 4 and 7 s. Each coloured circle was presented six times through Phase
2 and 3. Six presentations of the colour circle serving as the CS+ were coupled with an
uncomfortable shock to the left wrist of the learning model, where the model then displayed
signs of distress i.e. tensing of the left arm, lowering of the eyebrows (US); whereas CS- was
never paired with a shock. The level of shock was selected by the learning model to be
uncomfortable but not painful prior to the recording of the video. In Phase 3, the participants
viewed the learning model being able to cancel the upcoming shocks by pressing the button
‘M’ on the keyboard.
Fig. 1. A snapshot from the movie presented to
participants during the Pavlovian avoidance stage,
showing the model facing a computer screen that
presented the CS+ and CS-.
Inferred Condition
For the RCP trials, two stimuli in ‘wingdings’ font were used as contextual cues for
Same (i.e.) and Opposite (i.e.). Six nonsense syllables were employed as sample and
comparisons during relational training and testing, i.e. ZID, PAF, JOM, BEH, DAX, QAF.
These are clarified using alphanumerics i.e. A1, B1, B2, C1, C2, N2. An N2 stimulus, if
11
selected, was never reinforced.
In Phases 4 – 7, B1, B2, C1 and C2 were used to test derived relations. Six
presentations of the nonsense word serving as the CS+ were coupled with an uncomfortable
shock to the non-dominant arm of the participant, which they set before the trials to be
uncomfortable but not painful, whereas CS- and DerivedCS+ was never paired with a shock.
Procedure
Observed Condition
General Procedure:
On arrival to the lab, participants were given an information sheet depicting what was
expected of them during the study and the use of shocks, any ailment issues which would
mean termination of their participation, and if they wanted to opt out at any time they could
do so with no penalties. If they agreed, then a consent form was signed, the AAQ-II
questionnaire (M=17.13, SD=8.5) was counterbalanced so was either presented at this point
or at the end of the experiment. Participants were then sat in front of a computer and an
electrode was attached to the non-dominant arm and held in place by an elasticized bandage.
Shock was set by the participants to a level that was uncomfortable but not painful (M= 0.51,
SD= 0.2) starting a 0.250mA and working up in increments of 0.5. The participants were
exposed to four conditions (1) Habituation (2) Pavlovian Avoidance (3) Avoidance
Acquisition; and (4) Extinction. Phases (2) and (3) were observed by the participant watching
the pre-recorded video of the learning model.
Phase 1: Habituation
Three presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented where the participant simply
watched the screen. See Appendix 4 for computer instructions throughout both observed and
inferred conditions.
12
Phase 2: Pavlovian Avoidance (PA) and Phase 3: Avoidance Acquisition (AA)
Participants observed the learning model completing the Pavlovian Avoidance stage
where each circle was presented six times and the CS+ circle administered shocks to the
learning model. Participants then observed the learning model completing the Avoidance
Acquisition stage where the model learnt to cancel the upcoming shocks when the CS+ circle
appeared on the screen by pressing the ‘M’ key on the keyboard.
Phase 4: Extinction (EXT)
This was the testing phase, where 5 presentations of the coloured circles were
presented but in a different order to the one shown in the video. Essentially, no shocks were
administered to the participant during this phase to ensure learning was acquired through
indirect, social means only. At the end of testing, participants were presented with a Likert
Scale with four questions; (CS+ is in italics and will keep throughout the method) (1) To
what extent do you expect a shock when the red circle is presented and you do press a key?
(2) To what extent do you expect a shock when the red circle is presented and you do NOT
press a key? (3) To what extent do you expect a shock when the blue circle is presented and
you do press a key? (4) To what extent do you expect a shock when the blue circle is
presented and you do NOT press a key? Participants could rate their answers between -3
(very unlikely) to 3 (very likely). Ratings are the same for all Likert Scales in both
conditions. At the end of the experiment, subjects were given a debrief form.
Inferred Condition
On arrival to the lab, participants were given an information sheet depicting what was
expected of them during the study and the use of shocks, any ailment issues which would
mean termination of their participation, and if they wanted to opt out at any time they could
do so with no penalties. If they agreed, then a consent form was signed, the AAQ-II
questionnaire (M=15.63, SD=6.02) was counterbalanced so was either presented at this point
13
or at the end of the experiment. The participants were exposed to eight conditions (1) Non-
Arbitrary Training (2) Non-Arbitrary Testing (3) Arbitrary Training (4) Arbitrary Testing (5)
Habituation (6) Pavlovian Avoidance (7) Avoidance Acquisition; and (8) Extinction. Phase 1
– 4 used RCP to train and test same and opposite relations and derived relationships.
RCP General Procedure;
Clicking on a tick box at the bottom of the screen began the experiment for the
participants. A sample picture/nonsense words appeared on the top left corner of the screen,
after 1s the contextual cue (i.e. or) appeared in the upper centre, and after another 1s a
blank yellow square would appear on the top right corner of the screen. Three comparison
stimuli appeared on the bottom section of the screen. A selection could be made by clicking
the right mouse button on the intended picture and dragging to the black yellow square, then
releasing the mouse button. When this happened, two red buttons appeared on the bottom of
the screen that displayed the options, ‘Finish Trial’ or ‘Start Again’. Pressing the ‘Start
Again’ button refreshed the page to where all the stimuli were at the start of the trial for the
opportunity for another selection. Pressing the ‘Finish Trial’ button cleared the screen and
either produced a feedback screen during training phases, where if the correct selection was
made, the word ‘Correct’ was displayed, otherwise the word ‘Wrong’ was displayed. The
intertribal interval (ITI) of 3 seconds then happened, until the next trial began.
Phase 1: Non-Arbitrary Relational Training
Phase 1 and 2 are non-arbitrary training and testing intended to establish same and
opposite labelling for the contextual cues. The samples and comparisons stimuli were non-
arbitrary (formally related) and choosing the stimulus that was either the same or opposite of
the sample was reinforced. For instance, if a participant was shown a ‘smiley face’ as the
sample and the opposite contextual cue was shown, the selecting the ‘frowning face’ as the
comparison was reinforced, likewise if the same contextual cue was shown, and the same
14
‘smiley face’ comparison was selected. There were six stimulus sets (shown in Table 1),
presented in a random order for each participant. When participants produced eight
consecutively correct responses, they then progressed onto Phase 2.
Table 1
Stimulus sets used during Phase 1 and 2, and the physical dimensions of each stimulus set.
Physical Dimension
Description End 1 End 2
Phase 1
Squares Black White
Lines Short Long
Smiley Face Frowning Smiling
Cubes Small Big
Trees Small Big
Red Disk Sections Thin Crescent Full disk
Phase 2
Columns Narrow Wide
Wavy lines Small amp. Big amp.
Snowstorm No snow Whiteout
Buildings Small Big
Pointed Star 3 points 20 points
Bowed Trees Straight Very bowed.
Phase 2: Non-Arbitrary Relational Testing
Phase 2 followed the same design as Phase 1, except the crucial removal of feedback
of responses, and was just followed by the ITI. Six new stimulus sets were used which were
presented in a random order. Participants were required to respond correct consistently across
all eight trails to be exposed to Phase 3, failure resulted in re-exposure to Phase 1.
Phase 3: Arbitrary Relational Training
Phase 3 and 4 are arbitrary training and testing intended to establish same and
opposite relations of sample and comparison stimuli of an arbitrary nature (trigrams). For this
example and throughout, it will be explained in this way; the contextual cue used will be
stated in capitals, followed by the sample stimulus, and then followed by the three
comparison stimuli options in brackets. The correct answer which would be reinforced will
be shown in italics i.e. SAME/A1 (C2-C1-N2). The example demonstrates that in the
15
presence of the contextual cue SAME and the sample stimulus A1, choosing C1 would have
been reinforced, whereas choosing C2 and N2 would not have been. All participants were
presented with the following four training trials: SAME/A1 (C2-C1-N2), SAME/A1 (N2-B1-
B2), OPPOSITE/A1 (B2-N2-B1), OPPOSITE/A1 (C2-N2-C1). Training was presented in
eight trials, with each trial presented twice. If participants made the correct answer they were
given feedback by the word ‘Correct’ appearing on the screen. When participants produced
eight consecutively correct answers they were immediately exposed to Phase 4.
Phase 4: Arbitrary Relational Testing
Phase 4 tested for the emergences of derived relations from the training of same and
opposite. Figure 2 depicts the predicated relational network. Feedback was withheld during
this phase, although the alphanumeric in italics is what the participants would choose if
responding in accordance with the predicted relational network; the test trails were as
follows; SAME/B1 (C2-C1-N2), SAME/B2 (C2-N2-C1), SAME/C1 (N2-B1-C2), SAME/C2
(C1-N2-B2), OPPOSITE/B1 (C2-C1-N2), OPPOSITE/B2 (C1-N2-C2), OPPOSITE/C1 (N2-
C2-B2), and OPPOSITE/C2 (B1-B2-N2). The author would like to note that all possible
probe trials i.e. B-C and C-B were tested during the arbitrary relational testing as in Dymond
et al., (2008) paper. Testing was carried out for sixteen trials, with each task repeated twice
per block. It was compulsory that participants produced a minimum score of 14/16 (87.5%)
correct responses to pass. Failing to meet this measure they were immediately exposed to
Phase 1 again and set to go through each trial until meeting the goal.
16
Fig. 2. The predicted relational network of trained and tested stimuli. The alphanumerics
represent the nonsense syllables used in the Phase 3 and the letters S and O indicate Same and
Opposite. Solid lines signify trained relations whilst dashed lines signify derived relations.
General Procedure #2
After meeting the criterion for the RCP trials, the electrode was attached to the non-
dominant arm of the participant and held in place by an elasticized bandage. Shock was set
by the participants to a level that was uncomfortable but not painful (M= 0.51, SD= 0.19)
starting a 0.250mA and working up in increments of 0.05
Phase 5: Habituation
Three presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented on to the screen, exposing
participants to the stimuli.
Phase 6: PA
The participants sat comfortably whilst observing the screen, when the CS+ appeared
a mild shock, which level was selected by the participant before, was administered to their
arm through the electrode. They were exposed to each CS+ and CS- six times. In order to
counterbalance which word served as the CSD+ and CS-, the program was edited to have
either B1 or B2 as the CS+. After the phase was finished they were asked to complete a
17
Likert Scale, the questions were as follows; (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when
PAF is presented? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented?
Phase 7: AA
The aim of this trial was to observe if participants would elicit a simple avoidance
response during the presence of CS+ and not CS-. Six trials of the CS+ and CS- (B1 and/or
B2) were presented to the participant with the option to avoid the shock by pressing the ‘M’
key on the keyboard. Participants had to cancel 5 consecutive shocks to pass onto the next
phase. If they met the criterion they were passed onto Likert Scale questions; (1) To what
extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do press a key? (2) To what
extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do NOT press a key? (3) To
what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do press a key? (4) To
what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do NOT press a key?
Then participants are finally exposed to the final phase.
Phase 8: EXT
This was the testing phase; 6 presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented alongside
introducing the DerivedCS+ i.e. if CS+ was B1, using the predicted relational network the
DerivedCS+ would be C1, same scenario if CS+ was B2 so the DerivedCS+ would be C2., so
the aim of this phase is to test if participants have learnt the derived relations to elicit an
avoidance response. Essentially, no shocks were administered to the participants during this
phase to ensure learning was acquired through learned responses beforehand and also testing
for derived relations. After this phase, they were presented with the last Likert Scale, which is
similar to Phase 7 Likert Scale but with an important last two questions about the
DerivedCS+; (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do
press a key? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do
18
NOT press a key? (3) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you
do press a key? (4) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do
NOT press a key? (5) To what extent do you expect a shock when BEH is presented and you
do press a key? (6) To what extent do you expect a shock when BEH is presented and you do
NOT press a key? At the end of the experiment, subjects were given a debrief form.
19
Results
Table 2 demonstrates the performance of participants throughout Inferred Phases 1 –
4 (RCP). 13/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their
first exposure. P13 passed non-arbitrary testing after two exposures and passed arbitrary
testing on their first exposure. 8/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary
relational testing on their second exposure. 5/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and
arbitrary relational testing on their third exposure. The final 8/30 participants passed both
non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their maximum fourth exposure. Therefore,
all 30 participants were eligible to progress on the avoidance-conditioning phase.
Table 2
Trials to Criterion and Percentage of Correct Responses in Phase 1 – 4
Participant Phase 1: Non-
arbitrary Relational
Training (trials to
criterion)
Phase 2: Non-
arbitrary Relational
Testing (%)
Phase 3: Arbitrary
Relational Training
(trials to criterion)
Phase 4: Arbitrary
Relational Testing
(%)
1 10 100 16 43.75
8 100 12 87.5
2 9 100 13 93.75
3 9 100 44 62.5
8 100 8 68.75
8 100 8 87.5
4 12 100 8 93.75
5 18 100 10 87.5
6 14 100 9 93.75
7 10 100 23 93.75
8 8 100 15 81.25
8 100 8 93.75
9 8 100 9 37.5
8 100 8 81.25
8 100 8 87.5
10 16 100 20 93.75
11 9 100 12 93.75
12 23 100 12 56.25
8 100 9 56.25
8 100 8 93.75
13 15 62.5
8 100 25 87.5
14 8 100 8 68.75
8 100 9 81.25
8 87.5
8 100 8 93.75
15 11 85.71
20
8 87.5
8 100 41 81.25
8 100 9 93.75
16 14 100 24 87.5
17 9 100 16 62.5
8 100 8 81.25
8 100 8 100
18 9 87.5
13 100 36 93.75
19 44 100 52 75
8 100 8 87.5
20 8 100 14 100
21 8 100 9 87.5
22 12 100 31 56.25
8 100 9 56.25
8 87.5
13 100 8 93.75
23 10 100 32 81.25
9 100 8 93.75
24 11 100 28 75
11 100 43 87.5
25 8 100 9 68.75
8 100 8 97.5
26 11 100 52 68.75
16 100 21 25
10 100 14 87.5
27 11 100 34 97.5
28 9 100 48 37.5
13 100 18 87.5
29 13 100 15 100
30 8 100 8 100
Descriptive Graphs
Figure 3 displays the mean percentage of avoidance behaviour across both learning
groups, (Observed: Phase 4; Inferred: Phases 7 & 8) and the three stimuli (CS+, CS- and
DerivedCS+). The graph suggests a significant difference between CS+ and CS- for both
learning groups and also depicts the emergence of derived learning from its similarity from
the CS+ stimuli. The small standard error bars show low variability in avoidance response
results.
21
MeanAvoidanceBehaviour(%)
Observed Inferred
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1.0
1.2
CS+
CS-
DerivedCS+
Fig. 3. Graph showing the mean avoidance behaviour (%) of participants in both learning
groups.
Figure 4 displays the mean expectancy of shock if they were to elicit an avoidance
response, that was self-reported from participants through Likert Ratings across both learning
groups (Observed: Phase 4; Inferred; Phases 6-8) and three stimuli (CS+, CS- and
DerivedCS+). The graph suggests that Observed participants rated the expectancy of shock
after an avoidance response higher than the Inferred group, the standard error shows great
variability across both learning groups responses.
MeanExpectancy+Avoidance
Observed Inferred
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
CS+
CS-
DerivedCS+
22
Fig. 4. Graph showing the mean expectancy of a shock when performing an avoidance
response.
Figure 5 displays the mean expectancy of shock if not eliciting an avoidance response.
Results were self-reported by participants through Likert Ratings across both learning groups
(Observed: Phase 4; Inferred; Phases 6-8) and three stimuli (CS+, CS- and DerivedCS+). The
graph suggests a significant difference for both learning groups between CS+ and CS- as the
former had a greater mean expectancy rate over the latter. It also depicts the emergence of
derived learning from the similarity between the CS+ and DerivedCS+ stimuli. The standard
error bars show low variability in both groups.
MeanExpectancy+NonAvoidance
Observed Inferred
0
2
4
6
8
CS+
CS-
DerivedCS+
Fig. 5. Graph showing the mean expectancy of shock if participants across both learning
groups did not perform an avoidance response.
Aggregate Results
Two independent samples t-tests were performed to examine the statistical evidence
shown from the graphs. In each test, the percentage scores of avoidance responses of CS+
and CS- in the Observation EXT group were compared against Inferred EXT group. For these
two tests, P 16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the criterion.
23
Firstly, Observed EXT CS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS+. Considering
the group statistics, there seemed to be very little difference in the means, which is confirmed
using the top row of the t-test, as Levene’s test is insignificant (F=3.91, p=.053), showing an
insignificant difference (Observed EXT group M=98.33, SD=5.09; Inferred EXT group
M=99.40, SD=3.15; t(56)=.956, p=.343). Secondly; Observed EXT CS- was compared
against Inferred EXT CS-, considering the group statistics, there was possibly a small
difference in the means, and Levene’s test showed a significant difference (F=13.49, p=.001)
between the variances of the two groups. Using the bottom row of the t-test, it can be
concluded there is no significant difference between the means at the 5% significance level
(Observed EXT group M=1.11, SD=4.23; Inferred EXT group M=4.17, SD=8.64;
t(38.63)=1.69, p=.099). However, there is a weak significant difference at the 10% level,
showing that the Observed group preformed marginally better than the Inferred, as they had a
lower mean score. Overall, the independent t-tests showed Observed and Inferred learning
groups were both as effective for eliciting avoidance behaviour.
Six paired samples t-tests were conducted to study the percentage scores of avoidance
responses of CS+ and CS-, and to test the emergence of a derived relational response in
Inferred EXT phase. They also tested the expectancy of shock Likert Scale ratings for the
Inferred condition.
Observed EXT CS+ was compared against Observed EXT CS-. At the 10% significance
level there was a weak negative correlation (correlation=.356, p=.053) between CS+ and CS-
showing participants that elicited a avoidance response on CS+, did not on CS-, as was
expected. This is supported by the descriptive statistical means and a significant difference in
the t-test (Observed EXT CS+ M=98.33, SD=5.09; Observed EXT CS- M=1.11, SD=4.23;
t(29)=69.29, p=.000).
24
Inferred AA CS+ was compared against Inferred AA CS-. P16 data was excluded for
failure to meet the criterion. Samples statistics shows CS+ had a higher mean avoidance
response over CS-, showing a higher proportion of participants showed learnt behaviour and
cancelled the upcoming shocks, over the CS-. Also supported by the t-test showing a
significant difference (Inferred AA CS+ M=93.75, SD=12.52; Inferred AA CS- M=2.08,
SD=5.6; t(31)=40.83, p=.000).
Inferred EXT CS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS-. P16 and 22 data were
excluded for failure to meet the criterion. The samples statistics indicate a significant
difference between the CS+ and CS- from the mean avoidance response, and this is
confirmed by the t-test showing a significant difference. This shows that participants
produced the avoidance response by learning the correct relation (Inferred EXT CS+
M=99.40, SD=3.15; Inferred EXT CS- M=4.17, SD=8.64; t(27)=56.57, p=.000).
Inferred EXT DerivedCS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS+ to test for
emergences of derived relations. P16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the
criterion. The samples statistics indicate no significant difference between CS+ and
DerivedCS+ and is confirmed by the t-test showing no significant difference. Results
illustrate derived transformation of functions emerged and were as effective as CS+ stimuli
(Inferred EXT Derived CS+ M=95.24, SD=19.7; Inferred EXT CS+ M=99.4, SD=3.15;
t(27)=1.1, p=.282).
Inferred EXT DerivedCS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS-. P16 and 22 data
were excluded for failure to meet the criterion. Samples statistics suggested a significant
difference, and was confirmed by the t-test showing a significant difference. This shows that
participants produced the avoidance response by learning the correct derived relation to CS+
therefore learning to cancel the upcoming shock, as would be expected under CS+ (Inferred
25
EXT DerivedCS+ M=95.24, SD=19.7; Inferred EXT CS- M=4.17, SD=8.64; t(31)=40.83,
p=.000).
The final paired samples t-test was conducted on the Inferred PA Likert Scales showing
expectancy of shock when presented with the contextual stimuli. The sample statistics
illustrate a significant difference, and the t-test confirms. This shows that the participants
rated the correct CS+ higher than the CS- on expectancy of shock (PA CS+ M=5.93,
SD=.254; PA CS- M=.133, SD=.434; t(29)=57.67, p=.000).
A Pearson’s r test was conducted to investigate any correlations between AAQ-II data
and learnt behaviour. However, there were no significant correlations identified between:
Observed AAQ-II scores and Observed EXT CS+ (r= -.114, N=30. P=.548); Observed AAQ-
II and Observed EXT CS- (r= .219, N=30, p=.244); Inferred AAQ-II scores and Inferred
EXT CS+ (r= -.008, N=30, p=.966); or between Inferred AAQ-II scores and Inferred EXT
CS- (r=.200, N=30, p=.290). This shows there is no relationship between AAQ-II scores and
avoidance responses of CS+ or CS-.
The total number of responses in the Inferred AA Likert Scales for each of our within
subject experimental conditions was entered into a 2 (CS+, CS-) x 2 (AV, NAV) multi-way
within subjects ANOVA. Results showed a significant main effect of Conditioned Stimuli
(F(1,29)=108.38, p=.000), showing that conditioned stimuli had a significant effect on the
expectancy of shock. Another significant main effect of Learned Behaviour (F(1,29)=73.9,
p=.000) also showed learned behaviour effects expectancy of shock. A significant interaction
was shown between Conditioned Stimuli*Learned Behaviour (F(1,29)=126.68, p=.000).
Figure 6 shows participants understood the significance of the avoidance behaviour. The
difference in the estimated marginal means for AV and NAV for CS+ shows that participants
learnt the importance of the avoidance response, whereas for CS- where the results are
26
similar for AV and NAV, participants learnt that they wouldn’t get shocked regardless of
avoidance response.
Fig. 6. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores displaying the interaction between
Conditioned Stimuli and Learned Behaviour.
Post-hoc tests (paired samples t-test) were conducted to further investigate these
interactions. Results showed the mean difference between CS+NAV was significantly larger
for CS+AV (t(29)=11.43, p=.000), this showed that participants learnt the effectiveness of the
avoidance behaviour to reduce the expectancy of shock. There was no significant mean
difference between CS-AV and CS-NAV (t(29)=.432, p=.669) and no significant mean
difference between CS+AV and CS-AV (t(29)=1.18, p=.247). These both show that
participants learnt when to expect a shock and the appropriate avoidance response. There was
27
a significant mean difference between CS+NAV and CS-NAV (t(29)=21.13, p=.000) which
illustrates that participants learnt the correct shock stimulus.
The total number of responses in the Observed EXT and Inferred EXT Likert Scales for
each of our six experimental conditions was entered into a 2 (observed, inferred) x 2 (CS+,
CS-) x 2 (AV, NAV) multi-way mixed ANOVA. Results showed a significant main effect of
the conditioned stimuli F(1,58)=299.34, p=.000), a significant main effect of learned
behaviour (F (1,58)=114.35, p=.000) and a significant effect on learning group
(F(1,58)=10.97, p=.002) which showed that which learning group the participant was in had a
significant effect on the expectancy of shock.
Within-subjects test showed a non-significant effect of group*conditioned stimuli
interaction (F(1,58)=.148, p=.702); Non-significant effect of group*learnt behaviour
interaction (F(1,58)=2.22, p=.141) and a significant interaction of condition stimuli*learnt
behaviour (F(1,58)=201.38, p=.000). This interaction is shown in Figure 7 participants
understood the significance of the avoidance behaviour. The difference in the estimated
marginal means for AV and NAV for CS+ shows that participants learnt the importance of
the avoidance response, whereas for CS- where the results are similar for AV and NAV,
participants learnt that they wouldn’t get shocked regardless of avoidance response.
The three-way interaction between conditioned stimuli*learnt behaviour*group was not
significant (F (1,58)=.02, p=.889).
28
Fig. 7. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred
EXT displaying the interaction between Conditioned Stimuli and Learned Behaviour.
Figure 8 plots the group against conditioned stimuli whilst Figure 9 plots group against
learned behaviour. Observed is rated higher in expectancy of shock for both conditioned
stimuli and learned behaviour, showing that observed rated the expectancy of shock higher
than Inferred.
29
Fig. 8. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred
EXT displaying the interaction between Group and Conditioned Stimuli.
30
Fig. 9. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred
EXT displaying the interaction between Group and Learned Behaviour.
Post-hoc tests (independent t-test) were conducted to examine the main effects and the
conditioned stimulus*learnt behaviour interaction; results showed no significant difference at
the 5% but a weak significant difference at the 10% level between Observed CS+AV and
Inferred CS+AV. Levene’s test showed significance in variance (F=5.68, p=.020) so the
bottom row was used (t(52.87)=1.74, p=.088). A non-significant difference was shown
between Observed CS+NAV and Inferred CS+NAV (t(58)=.297, p=.768). A significant
difference was found between Observed CS-AV and Inferred CS-AV, Levene’s test
(F=11.35, p=.001) showed bottom row to be used (t(47.8)=2.19, p=.033). And finally a non-
significant difference was shown between Observed CS-NAV and Inferred CS-NAV,
31
Levene’s test (F=10.79, p=.002) so bottom row was used (t(43.84)=1.58, p=.122).
Discussion
32
The aim of this present study was to examine the effectiveness of observed and
inferred (derived) pathways of avoidance. Comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS- and
Inferred EXT CS+ and CS-; no significant differences were found between the learning
groups and conditioned stimuli, showing the two learning groups were just as effective as
each other in producing a fear-evoked avoidance response through indirect means. The
findings suggest that observed and inferred pathways of avoidance are equally as effective as
each other, and from past supporting literature can hypothesize as effective as direct
Pavlovian fear conditioning. (Cook & Mineka, 1990; Dymond et al., 2007; Egliston & Rapee,
2007; Whelan & Barner-Holmes, 2004).
The Observed condition was shown to be effective at inducing an avoidance response
at the correct CS+ after viewing the video of the learning model in the similar task, which
mirrors Mineka & Cook (1993) study findings. This was shown by comparing Observed EXT
CS+ and CS-. There was a weak negative correlation showing participants that elicited an
avoidance response on CS+ did not on CS-, which was to be expected. It could be
hypothesized that the participants produced avoidance behaviour due to feeling empathy
(Knapska et al., 2009; Olsson et al., 2007) or just due to imitation of the learning models
avoidance response (Kavaliers et al., 2001) as 30/30 participants produced an avoidance
response, but this can’t be fully proved without an fMRI scan during the task.
There was no correlation found between AAQ-II scores and avoidance responses of
CS+ or CS-. López et al., (2010) study was inconsistent with our findings as the participants
data that had to be excluded from the Inferred condition actually had low AAQ-II scores
(P16, AAQ-II = 12 and P22, AAQ-II = 9).
33
For the inferred condition, RCP was shown to be effective in training same, opposite
and derived relations as seen by 30/30 participants passing (Appendix 5) albeit first of final
exposure. This supports Dymond & Whelan’s (2010) study of RCP being an effective
procedure.
Comparing Inferred EXT Derived CS+ against Inferred EXT CS+ no significance was
found between the two stimuli, illustrating derived transformation of functions emerged and
avoidance responses matched those of CS+. There was a significant difference shown
between DerivedCS+ and CS- illustrating that participants again, learnt the derived relations
and ignored the CS- stimuli in relation to an upcoming shock. These results support RFT, that
participants transferred fear and avoidance responses across stimuli due to learning derived
relational responding from the RCP (Barnes-Holmes et al., 2004, Healy et al., 2000).
In the 2*2 within subjects ANOVA for the Inferred AA Likert scales, there was a
significant main effect on conditioned stimuli and learnt behaviour showing both variables
had an effect on the perception of the expectancy of shock. The significant interaction
between conditioned stimuli*learned behaviour illustrates that participants learnt the correct
contingency for CS+ and showed they understood they wouldn’t get shocked for CS-
regardless of avoidance response. Post-hoc tests showed that participants understood the
effectiveness of the avoidance behaviour to reduce the expectancy of shock.
In the final 2*2*2 multi-way mixed ANOVA, results showed significant main effects
for conditioned stimuli, learned behaviour and learning group. Comparing conditioned
stimuli and learning group the only significant interaction was between Observed CS-AV and
Inferred CS-AV which could show that participants in both conditions found the understood
the significance of CS- and that they did not have to produce an avoidance response.
34
Comparing group against conditioned stimuli and learned behaviour (Figure 8 and 9)
it is clear that Observed rated higher in expectancy of shock to both, this could be attributed
to the Observed group understanding the CS-US contingency and the value of CS- more than
the Inferred group.
The Observed condition was shown to be effective at inducing an avoidance response
at the correct CS+ after viewing the video of the learning model in the similar task, which
mirrors Mineka & Cook (1993) study findings. This was shown by comparing Observed EXT
CS+ and CS-. There was a weak negative correlation showing participants that elicited an
avoidance response on CS+ did not on CS-, which was to be expected. It could be
hypothesized that the participants produced avoidance behaviour due to feeling empathy
(Knapska et al., 2009; Olsson et al., 2007) or just due to imitation of the learning models
avoidance response (Kavaliers et al., 2001) as 30/30 participants produced an avoidance
response, but this can’t be fully proved without an fMRI scan during the task.
However, a criticism of this condition is that the participants were exposed to fear-
evoking stimuli and then immediately exposed to a directly taught avoidance response; this
can be a serious limitation that could suggest the results may have been taught through direct
means. The direct observation of the learning model completing the avoidance response in
the presence of one stimuli and not the other will have affected the avoidance contingency
and consequently influenced the avoidance behaviour. Research has demonstrated that
explicit instruction is not needed to generate indirect observational fear learning (Guzmán et
al., 2009; Helsen et al., 2011; Olsson et al., 2007). To improve on this condition, the
participants would only be exposed to the learning model experiencing the Pavlovian
Avoidance phase, then be exposed to Likert ratings and finally, the participant will
experience avoidance acquisition phase with the same set of instructions given to the Inferred
group avoidance acquisition phase (Appendix 4). This would ensure that the participants are
35
being indirectly taught the CS-US contingency and then eliciting an avoidance behaviour
response from that.
The inferred condition results illustrate that participants showed transformation of
functions for the DerivedCS+ from the CS+. This condition supports the ideas for
improvement on the observed condition that explicit instructions were not needed for derived
transformation or avoidance behaviours to occur (Dymond et al., 2008; Field, 2006). The
derived extinction phase mirrors clinical procedures in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
(ACT; Hayes, Follette & Linehan, 2004; Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda & Lillis, 2006) called
cognitive diffusion. This is a where patients illiterate negative self-statements for a few
minutes i.e. ‘I have no friends’ until generalized of the extinction is met, i.e. vocally created
stimuli in relational to real-life environmental events, not within the therapy setting.
The observed condition would have to be retested against the inferred condition to test
for any real significant difference, as the limitations of this study hinders the potential
findings reflected in all past literature. The emergence of transformation of functions and
derived relational responding shows the effectiveness of Same and Opposite relational frames
in a RCP, and illustrates a starting point for more procedures to aid in indirect anxiety and
phobias to be built. To improve the effect of indirect learning over direct, a further study
would be to test the observed group and inferred group against an instructed (Pavlovian fear
condition) group. This will affirm the effectiveness of indirect learning of fearful stimuli.
An issue of external validity must also be raised, as it does not necessarily stand that
as participants showed promise in avoidance functions across stimuli in a lab setting with
novel stimuli, that in a real life setting the results would be the same. To conclude, the results
demonstrated participants transferred a fear response to a novel stimuli through observation
36
and that derived relational training can effectively train participants to transfer learned fear
avoidance response across novel stimuli in a lab environment.
37
References
Augustson, E. M., & Dougmer, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions
through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 3, 181-91.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Smeets, P. M., Cullinan, V., & Leader, G. (2004).
Relational frame theory and stimulus equivilance: Conceptual and procedural issues.
International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 181-214.
Barot, S. K., Chung, A., Kim, J. J., & Bernstein, I. L. (2009). Functional Imaging of Stimulus
Convergence in Amygdalar Neurons during Pavlovian Fear Conditioning. PLoS
ONE, 4(7):e6156.
Barot, S. K., Kyono, Y., Clark, E. W., & Bernstein, I. L. (2008). Visualizing Stimulus
Convergence in Amygdala Neurons during Associative Learning. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(52), 20959-63.
Bass, J. M., van Ooijen, L., Goudriaan, A., & Kenemans, J. L. (2008). Failure to condition to
a cue is associated with sustained contextual fear. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 581-92.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., Waltz,
T., & Zettle, R. D. (2011). Properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire –
II: A| revised measure of psychological flexibility and experiential avoidance.
Behavior Therapy, 42, 676-88.
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of
fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour
Processes, 16, 372-89.
Davies, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The Amygdala: Vigilance and Emotion. Molecular
Psychiatry, 6, 13-34.
Delgado, M. R., Jou, R. L., Ledoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2009). Avoiding negative
outcomes: Tracking the mechanisms of avoidance learning in humans during fear
conditioning. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 3(33).
Delgado, M. R., Li, J., Schiller, D., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). The role of the striatum in
aversive learning and aversive prediction errors. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Studies, 363(1511), 3787-800.
Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. (2000). Understanding complex behaviour: The transformation
of stimulus functions. The Behaviour Analyst, 23, 239-54.
Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behaviour analysis of anxiety and
avoidance. The Behaviour Analyst, 32, 7-27.
38
Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of
avoidance response function in accordance with Same and Opposite relational frames.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88(2), 249-62.
Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2008). Derived avoidance
learning: Transformation of avoidance responses function in accordance with Same
and Opposite relational frames. The Psychological Record, 58, 269-86.
Dymond, S., & Whelan, R. (2010). Derived relational responding: A comparison of Match-
to-Sample and the Relational Completion Procedure. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behaviour, 94(1), 37-55.
Egliston, K. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following
positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(8), 1871-
82.
Forsyth, J. P., Eifert, G. H., & Barrios, V. (2006). Fear conditioning research as a clinical
analog: What makes fear learning disorderd? In M. G. Craske, D. Hermans & D.
Vansteenwegen. (Eds.). Fear and learning: Basic science to clinical application (pp.
133-53). Washington DC, USA: American Psychological Association.
Field, A. P. (2006). Is conditioning a useful framework for understanding the development
and treatment of phobias? Clinical Psychology Review, 26(7), 857-75.
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modelling on
the acquisition of fear and avoidance in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
40, 279-87.
Guzmán, Y. F., Tronson, N. C., Guedea, A., Huh, K. H., Gao, C., & Radulovic, J. (2009).
Social modelling of conditioned fear in mice by non-fearful conspecifics.
Behavioural Brain Research, 201(1), 173-78.
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.
Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. M. (2004). Mindfulness and Acceptance:
Expanding the Cognitive-Behavioural Tradition. New York, USA: The Guilford
Press.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and
Therapy (22), 1-25.
Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as
generalized operant behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour,
74, 207-227.
39
Helsen, K., Goubert, L., Peters, M. L., & Vlaeven, J. W. (2011). Observational learning and
pain related fear: An experimental study with coloured cold pressor tasks. The
Journal of Pain, 12(12), 1230-39.
Kavaliers, M., Choleris, E., Colwell, D. D. (2001). Learning from others to cope with biting
flies: social learning of fear-induced conditioned analgesia and active avoidance.
Behavioural Neuroscience, 115, 661-74.
Kim, J. J., & Jung, M. W. (2006). Neural circuits and mechanisms involved in Pavlovian fear
conditioning: A critical review. Neuroscience and Bio-behavioural Reviews,
30(2), 188-202.
Knapska, E., Mikosz, M., Werka, T., & Maren, S. (2009). Social modulation of learning in
rats. Learning and Memory, 17(1), 35-42.
LaBar, K. S., LeDoux, J. E., Spencer, D. D., & Phelps, E. A. (1995). Impaired fear
conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Journal of
Neuroscience, 15, 6846-55.
LeDoux, J. E., & Gorman, J. M. (2001). A call to action: Overcoming anxiety through active
coping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1953-55.
López, J. C., Ruiz, F. J., Feder, J., Rubio, A. B., Suárez-Aguirre, J. J., Rodríguez, J. A., &
Luciano, C. (2010). The role of experiential avoidance in the performance on a high
cognitive demand task. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological
Therapy, 10(3), 475-488.
Lovibond, P. F., Chen, S. X., Mitchell, C. J., Weidemann, G. (2011). Competition between an
avoidance response and a safety signal: Evidence for a single learning system.
Biological Psychology, In press.
Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of
fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122, 23-38.
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the
etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American
Psychologist, 61, 10-26.
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: the
neural systems of social fear transmission. SCAN, 2, 3-11.
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9),
1095-102.
Phillips, R. G., & LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and
hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioural Neuroscience,
106(2), 274-85.
40
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioural theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review
of Psychology, 57, 87-115.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1968). Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. Journal
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(2), 402-11.
Siddle, D. A., & Bond, N. W. (1988). Avoidance learning, Pavlovian conditioning, and the
development of phobias. Biological Psychology, 27(2), 167-83.
Watson, J. B. & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 3, 1-14.
Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in
accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 86, 317-35.
41
Appendix 1 – Information Sheet (Observed)
Information Sheet
In this study, you will be attached to a bar electrode which will deliver electric shocks to your
forearm, you will set the intensity of the shocks at the beginning of the experiment to be
‘uncomfortable but not painful’.
A set of instructions will appear on the screen before each phase begins and time will be
allowed for any questions to be asked to the researcher. There are 4 phases in total one of which
requires you to watch a video. At the end of the study you will be given a debrief form explaining the
nature of the study and the opportunity to find out more about this research and to ask any
unanswered questions you may have.
This study is conducted in accordance with the British Psychological Society and
Departmental ethic guidelines, your participation in this study is completely voluntary and you may
withdraw at any point, without penalty. You can also choose to have any data that you provided in
this study to be disregarded at any time, during or after the study. Otherwise, your results will be kept
anonymous and confidential to only the researchers or supervisors working on this study. All data will
be password protected and not accessed for any other reasons than this study. You must not
participate in this study if you have a pacemaker fitted; suffer with heart palpitations or currently
talking psychotropic medication.
If you agree to take part in this study we would appreciate it if you agree to not discuss your
participation or any details of this study with others.
Please now complete the consent forms and return them to the researcher.
42
Appendix 1 – Information Sheet (Inferred)
Information Sheet
In this study, you will be attached to a bar electrode which will deliver electric shocks to your
forearm, you will set the intensity of the shocks at the beginning of the experiment to be
‘uncomfortable but not painful’.
A set of instructions will be presented on the screen before each phase begins and time will be
allowed for any questions to be asked to the researcher. There are 8 phases in total, phase 6 onwards
may entail up to 12 electric shocks. At the end of the study you will be given a debrief form
explaining the nature of the study and the opportunity to find out more about this research and to ask
any unanswered questions you may have.
This study is conducted in accordance with the British Psychological Society and
Departmental ethic guidelines, your participation in this study is completely voluntary and you may
withdraw at any point, without penalty. You can also choose to have any data that you provided in
this study to be disregarded at any time, during or after the study. Otherwise, your results will be kept
anonymous and confidential to only the researchers or supervisors working on this study. All data will
be password protected and not accessed for any other reasons than this study. You must not
participate in this study if you have a pacemaker fitted; suffer with heart palpitations or currently
talking psychotropic medication.
If you agree to take part in this study, we would appreciate it if you agree to not discuss your
participation or any details of this study with any others.
Please now complete the consent forms and return them to the researcher.
Appendix 1 – Consent Form (Observed & Inferred)
43
Project title: The effectiveness of observed and inferred pathways of avoidance on learning.
Name of Supervisor: Dr Simon Dymond, (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk)
Name of investigators: Katie Davies & Ellyn Moore
Name of participant:
Age:
Sex:
• I consent to take part in this study and am satisfied with the instructions I have
been given so far. I have also been reassured that any further information I request
regarding this study will be supplied to me at the end of this experiment.
• I have been informed that the data I provide will remain confidential and be
password protected. I am free to ask any questions at any time before, during and after
the study and have been provided with a copy of this form and a participant
information sheet. I also understand I will be provided with a debrief form at the end
of the study.
• I understand that during this study I will be required to place a bar electrode on my
forearm which will be held in place by a small bandage. I will then be subject to a
series of electric shocks, of which I am able to set the intensity at the beginning of the
experiment. I understand the shock level will be “uncomfortable but not painful” and
am free to stop the study at any time.
• I am at least 18 years of age and have not been coerced in any way to participate in
this study and can withdraw from the study at any point.
• I understand I cannot participate in this study if I have any medical history of
cardiac problems, or have been fitted with a pacemaker.
Data protection: I agree to the university processing personal data that I have supplied. I
agree to the processing of such data for any purposes connected with the research project
as outlined to me.
Participant signature:
Date:
Name of researcher:
Signature:
Date:
44
Appendix 2 – Debrief Form (Observed)
Thank you for taking part in this study. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of
observed learning and derived learning pathways of avoidance.
Throughout this study we were testing whether you had learnt to avoid the correct stimulu just from
observing someone completing a similar task, so at no point during the experiment, other than the
calibration of the shock strength, were you in danger of receiving an electric shock based on the
procedures of Olsson et al (2007).
We would like to remind participants that if you feel uncomfortable with this study, then you are free
to withdraw your data from this study at any time, although we would also like to remind you that all
results are confidential and at no time will any data be identifiable to you or your name.
If this study has resulted in any discomfort to yourself, we would like to inform you of the Student
Counselling Service provided on campus at Horton House or alternatively, you can contact them on
+44 (0) 1792 295592 or email wellbeing@swansea.ac.uk
We would like if you would refrain from talking about this experiment to others whilst it is still
running as any details you reveal could affect future results.
If you would like to be informed of the results of this study once it is completed, feel free to contact
the experimenters on;
554515@swansea.ac.uk or; 550819@swansea.ac.uk
Supervisior; Dr Simon Dymond (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk) Telephone: 01792 295602
If you would like more information about this area of research, please follow these references;
Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance
response functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88, 249-262.
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: the neural
systems of social fear transmission. SCAN, 2, 3-11.
45
Appendix 2 – Debrief Form (Inferred)
Thank you for taking part in this study. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of
observed learning and derived learning pathways of avoidance.
Throughout this study we were testing whether you had learnt the correct same and opposite pairings
for this experiment phases, so some phases had the threat of electric shocks whilst others did not and
was just to test you.
We would like to remind participants that if you feel uncomfortable with this study, then you are free
to withdraw your data from this study at any time, although we would also like to remind you that all
results are confidential and at no time will any data be identifiable to you or your name.
If this study has resulted in any discomfort to yourself, we would like to inform you of the Student
Counselling Service provided on campus at Horton House or alternatively, you can contact them on
+44 (0) 1792 295592 or email wellbeing@swansea.ac.uk
We would like if you would refrain from talking about this experiment to others whilst it is still
running as any details you reveal could affect future results.
If you would like to be informed of the results of this study once it is completed, feel free to contact
the experimenters on;
554515@swansea.ac.uk or; 550819@swansea.ac.uk
Supervisior; Dr Simon Dymond (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk) Telephone: 01792 295602
If you would like more information about this area of research, please follow these references;
Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance
response functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88, 249-262.
Dymond, S., & Whelan, R. (2010). Derived relational responding: A comparison of matching to
sample and the relational completion procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
94, 37-55.
46
Appendix 3 – AAQ-II (Example)
47
Appendix 4 – Computer instructions for Observed and Inferred
Observed
Phase 1: Habituation.
“Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one
of two coloured circles. Each circle will appear for 3 seconds. Your job is to simply watch the screen. If you
have any questions please ask the experimenter now.”
Phase 2: Pavlovian Avoidance and Phase 3: Avoidance Acquisition
“You will now watch a short film of a person doing an experiment similar to the one you yourself are going to
be doing afterwards. The person in the film is going to receive shocks paired with one of two coloured circles
presented to him. Pay close attention to which coloured circle is followed by shocks and which is not.
The person in the film is then going to learn to press one of two marked keys on the keyboard to cancel the
upcoming shock. Again, it is important you pay close attention to the film, because, in the experiment you are
going to do afterwards, you are going to have to learn which one of the marked keys cancels the upcoming
shock. When you are ready to view the film, press ‘Continue’.”
Phase 4: Extinction
“Now you are going to take part in an experiment similar to the one you have just watched. You will be
presented with the same number of coloured circles as the person in the film, but in a different order.
Importantly, you will receive shocks paired with the same colour as the person in the film. However, you will be
able to cancel upcoming shocks by pressing one of the marked keys on the keyboard. When you are ready to
proceed, press ‘Continue’.”
Derived
RCP General Procedure;
“Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. You will be presented with a series of images or nonsense
words on the top half of the screen from left to right. Then you will be presented with 3 images or nonsense
words on the bottom of the screen.
Your task is to observe the images or words that appear from left to right and drag one of these images or
words from the bottom to the blank, yellow square. Click and hold the mouse over the image or word to drag it
to the blank square. To confirm your choice, then click ‘Finish Trial’. If you wish to make another choice, then
click ‘Start Again’.
Sometimes you will receive feedback on your choices, but at other times you will not. Your aim is to get as many
tasks correct as possible. It is always possible to get a task correct, even if you are not given feedback.”
Phase 5: Habituation
“Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one
of two words. Each word will appear for 3 seconds. Your job is to simply watch the screen. If you have any
questions please ask the experimenter now.”
Phase 6: Pavlovian Avoidance
48
“In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one of two words. Each word will appear for 3 seconds
followed by either a 250ms shock or no shock. The shock will be set at the level you have just selected. At the
end of this phase, you will be asked to make some ratings as to how much you expect shock to follow each of the
words. Please follow the onscreen instructions, and if you have any questions please ask the experimenter
now.”
Phase 7: Avoidance Acquisition
“In this phase, you will again be presented, on every trial, with one of two words. When a word appears on the
screen, the marked keys on the keyboard will be available to be pressed. Pressing the correct key when one of
the words appears will cancel a pending shock. The same key will cancel the shock for the remainder of the
study. You can learn which key is correct by paying close attention to the screen. At the end of this phase you
will be asked to make some ratings as to how much you expect a shock to follow each of the words. Please
follow the onscreen instructions, and if you have any questions, please contact the experimenter now”
Phase 8: Extinction
“This phase is important. Remember that all of the phases you have completed are interrelated and that the
words seen here are the same seen earlier.”
49
Appendix 5 – Excel spreadsheets
50
Appendix 6 – SPSS outputs
51

Mais conteúdo relacionado

Semelhante a Examining the Effectiveness of Observed & Derived Pathways of Avoidance

Dhanani_Asif_Thesis
Dhanani_Asif_ThesisDhanani_Asif_Thesis
Dhanani_Asif_ThesisAsif Dhanani
 
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology An Example Based On ADHD
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology  An Example Based On ADHDAdaptationism And Molecular Biology  An Example Based On ADHD
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology An Example Based On ADHDFaith Brown
 
Avoiding The Approach Trap A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...
Avoiding The Approach Trap  A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...Avoiding The Approach Trap  A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...
Avoiding The Approach Trap A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...Brittany Brown
 
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docxlorainedeserre
 
Riverpoint writer perspectives
Riverpoint writer perspectivesRiverpoint writer perspectives
Riverpoint writer perspectivesJody Marvin
 
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docx
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docxMalec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docx
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docxcroysierkathey
 
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docx
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docxAttentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docx
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docxrock73
 
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxThe Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxcherry686017
 
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment help
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment helpApa sample paper experiment paper assignment help
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment helpSample Assignment
 
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.org
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.orgAPA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.org
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.orgJonathan Underwood
 
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdf
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdfI am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdf
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdfAMITPANCHAL154
 
Neuroeconomic Class
Neuroeconomic ClassNeuroeconomic Class
Neuroeconomic Classtkvaran
 
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Dimitris Vlastos
 

Semelhante a Examining the Effectiveness of Observed & Derived Pathways of Avoidance (20)

Dhanani_Asif_Thesis
Dhanani_Asif_ThesisDhanani_Asif_Thesis
Dhanani_Asif_Thesis
 
Lab Report 3
Lab Report 3Lab Report 3
Lab Report 3
 
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology An Example Based On ADHD
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology  An Example Based On ADHDAdaptationism And Molecular Biology  An Example Based On ADHD
Adaptationism And Molecular Biology An Example Based On ADHD
 
Avoiding The Approach Trap A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...
Avoiding The Approach Trap  A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...Avoiding The Approach Trap  A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...
Avoiding The Approach Trap A Response Bias Theory Of The Emotional Stroop Ef...
 
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx
2MISS ALLISON ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY1Running h.docx
 
Goosebumps Poster Final Draft 1
Goosebumps Poster Final Draft 1Goosebumps Poster Final Draft 1
Goosebumps Poster Final Draft 1
 
Riverpoint writer perspectives
Riverpoint writer perspectivesRiverpoint writer perspectives
Riverpoint writer perspectives
 
Clinical Paper
Clinical PaperClinical Paper
Clinical Paper
 
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docx
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docxMalec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docx
Malec, T. & Newman, M. (2013). Research methods Building a kn.docx
 
Tech N 10
Tech N 10Tech N 10
Tech N 10
 
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docx
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docxAttentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docx
Attentional Changes During Implicit Learning Signal Validity .docx
 
Thesis_40093281
Thesis_40093281Thesis_40093281
Thesis_40093281
 
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxThe Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
 
Cognitive Biases Essay
Cognitive Biases EssayCognitive Biases Essay
Cognitive Biases Essay
 
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment help
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment helpApa sample paper experiment paper assignment help
Apa sample paper experiment paper assignment help
 
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.org
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.orgAPA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.org
APA Corrected Sample Papers -by APA.org
 
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdf
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdfI am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdf
I am fine with the actual analysis of the data on SPSS, however I am.pdf
 
Neuroeconomic Class
Neuroeconomic ClassNeuroeconomic Class
Neuroeconomic Class
 
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
Vlastos, D., Kyritsis, M., Papaioannou-Spiroulia, A., & Varela V.-A. (2017). ...
 
Oxytocin & Vasopressin In Treating Autism
Oxytocin & Vasopressin In Treating AutismOxytocin & Vasopressin In Treating Autism
Oxytocin & Vasopressin In Treating Autism
 

Último

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxPoojaSen20
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptxiammrhaywood
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomnelietumpap1
 
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfAMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfphamnguyenenglishnb
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfTechSoup
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Seán Kennedy
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Celine George
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSJoshuaGantuangco2
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptxmary850239
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17Celine George
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 

Último (20)

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
 
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptxAUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY -  GERBNER.pptx
AUDIENCE THEORY -CULTIVATION THEORY - GERBNER.pptx
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
 
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfAMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
 
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTSGRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
GRADE 4 - SUMMATIVE TEST QUARTER 4 ALL SUBJECTS
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
4.16.24 21st Century Movements for Black Lives.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 

Examining the Effectiveness of Observed & Derived Pathways of Avoidance

  • 1. Abstract This study compared aversive learning through solely social observational learning and derived avoidance learning using a recorded learning model video where they receiving a mild electric shock when a selected circle (CS+) is shown on-screen. The stimulus will predict the delivery of shock (CS+) whilst another contextually similar stimulus will present no negative consequences (CS-). Participants are instructed of an avoidance behaviour response (AV+).The inferred condition participants will be presented with RCP trials of non- arbitrary and arbitrary training and testing of Same and Opposite relations, as shown in Dymond et al., (2008) study, to test for transformation of functions and derived relational responding in later trials i.e. avoidance acquisition and extinction. The between- subject factor Group has two levels; observed and inferred. Two within- subject factors (1) Conditioned Stimulus, which has two levels: CS+ and CS-. (2) Learned Behaviour which has two levels: Avoidance (AV) and Non-Avoidance (NAV). Thus combination of the three factors created six experimental conditions. The DerivedCS+ will be compared against the inferred response rates of CS+ and CS-. There were three dependent variables: Avoidance behaviour responses of the shock in each phase, expectancy of shock taken from Likert scales at the end of each phase, and AAQ-II results. Comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS- and Inferred EXT CS+ and CS-; no significant differences were found between the learning groups and conditioned stimuli, showing the two learning groups were just as effective as each other in producing a fear- evoked avoidance response. In a 2*2*2 multi-way mixed ANOVA, results showed significant main effects for conditioned stimuli, learned behaviour and learning group. Comparing group against conditioned stimuli and learned behaviour (Figure 8 and 9) it is clear that Observed rated higher in expectancy of shock to both, this could be attributed to the 1
  • 2. observed group understanding the CS-US contingency and the value of CS- more than the Inferred group. In conclusion, the study found that observed group may have been taught through direct means, by watching the learning model in the avoidance acquisition phase. Future studies would amend this. The inferred condition was successful as derived relational responding was produced. 2
  • 3. A common debate in the topic of fear learning is how fear is firstly acquired, be it through direct or indirect means. Fear acquisitions to environmental stimuli are fundamental to the fight or flight mechanism, thus essential for survival of human and non-humans alike. Understanding fear conditioning will develop and strengthen the understanding of phobias and anxiety disorders to help build clinical models (Barot, Chung, Kim & Bernstein, 2009; Field, 2006). The first model of classical fear conditioning was coined Pavlovian conditioning and suggested fear acquisition of an object can only surface through first-hand experience with an aversive stimulus (Olsson, Nearing & Phelps, 2007; Siddle & Bond, 1988). The individual, be it human or non-human, acquires a conditioned fear response (CR) to a formerly neutral stimulus (CS) through direct experience pairing with an aversive occurrence, the unconditioned stimulus (US). This acquisition was thought to be maintained by a two-factor theory i.e. the escape from the CS and instant decrease in fear eliciting factors. This model is famously illustrated in the ‘Little Albert’ study (Watson & Rayner, 1920) where they conditioned a fear response to a previously neutral CS i.e. white rabbit/rat/dog, by pairing CS-US (loud noise). Results showed Little Albert generalised and elicited a fear response not only to white rabbit etc., but also Santa Claus and white hair. Research has shown that the amygdala is essential in the acquisition, storage and expression of the conditioned fear (Davis & Whalen, 2001; Kim & Jung, 2006; Olsson et al., 2007). Barot, Kyono, Clark & Bernstein’s (2008) study showed clear visualisations of neurons within the amygdala receiving CS-US convergence information, during acquisition phase of a learned association. Barot et al., (2009) study showed the interaction of the amygdala and the hippocampus, where the hippocampus processes and transmits contextual CS information to the amygdala i.e. hippocampus is responsible for contextual learning whilst amygdala responsible for cue learning (Olsson & Phelps, 2007; Phillips & LeDoux, 3
  • 4. 1992). Damage to these areas is coupled with reports of CS-US contingency learning but inhibition of autonomic response (LaBar, LeDoux, Spencer & Phelps, 1995). Delgado, Jou, Ledoux & Phelps (2009) also illustrated the role of the striatum due to its reward-related processing ability to rule emotions and avoid negative outcomes. Participants were instructed upon seeing the aversive stimulus they could avoid shock by choosing the correct action (AV+). This depicts a mechanism of active coping, where the striatum exerts control over the emotional responses to influence the decision making (Delgado, Li, Schiller & Phelps, 2008; LeDoux & Gorman, 2001; Schultz, 2006). These studies clearly illustrate the networks of learning paradigms, storage and reinforcer types. However, over the years research grew and the understanding that fear can only be acquired through direct means was soon diminished. The Pavlovian Model was suggested to be extended to consider indirect forms. Field (2006) suggested two pathways of avoidance; (1) vicarious (observational) learning and (2) verbal information i.e. instructions. Another issue with the Pavlovian Model was the two-factor theory. Seligman (1968) developed a variation of the model and described the safety-signal theory, which considered an acquired conditioned emotional response (CER) only to a signal that predicted the onset of shock, for any other signal a conditioned response of pressing for food was shown. (Lovibond, Chen, Mitchell & Weidemann, 2011). The safety-signal theory can be applied to this present study as it is the intention that participants will produce an avoidance response to the CS-US contingency and not for any other stimuli, unless derived. Humans’ current socio-cultural environment can present indirect means of obtaining fearful stimuli through verbal communications and social modulation i.e. expressions, imitation etc. Mineka & Cook (1993) hypothesized that indirect fear learning is procedurally the same as direct fear learning, and can be just as effective. This happens by the observation of another’s reaction to the CS acting as a US. Others reaction elicits anxiety and therefore a 4
  • 5. fear response; similarly seen in non-human studies such as monkeys (Cook & Mineka, 1990). Non-human studies have shed light onto indirect fear learning by showing their abilities to perceive and react accordingly to external signs of fear and pain. The neural workings aid in assigning and raising awareness of a threat value to context or cue associated with the threat (Hauser, 1996; Kavaliers, Choleris, Colwell, 2001). Social learning of aversive stimuli through a learning model can be explained through several means but for this study the concentration will be on imitation and empathy. In relation to empathy; Knapska, Mikosz, Werka & Maren (2009) showed socially transmitted fear in rats through social interaction. If a rat had previously interacted with a recently fear-conditioned partner, they exhibited defensive behaviour during the novel task, hypothesizing the social interaction acted as a US leading the rats to empathize. Kavaliers et al., (2001) studied mice and their socially mediated acquisition of fear to the aversive stimulus of biting flies. ‘Observer’ mice watched ‘demonstrator’ mice being bit by flies and for avoidance exhibited analgesia and self-burying. When exposed to non-biting flies, the observer mice displayed the conditioned analgesia and self-burying as observed from before. This imitation has also been shown in humans. Self-reports from children about their unexplained fear of the dark/animals explain they are developed from observing their parents fearful emotions in contextual situations (Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006). This was shown in Gerull & Rapee (2002) when mother displays negative facial expressions towards novel stimuli, the odds of toddler imitating fearful expressions and avoidance behaviour increases. Helsen, Goubert, Peters & Vlaeven (2011) demonstrated the effects of observational learning on pain related fear and if it was associated with increased pain ratings. Self-reports of fear of pain and expected pain ratings were taken after observing a learning model, whilst actual pain and avoidance measures were taken post-exposure, and found observing another person performing the task ended in a higher reporting of intense and unpleasant pain. Guzmán, 5
  • 6. Tronson, Guedea, Huh, Gao, & Radulovic (2009) showed that even though models on humans and non-humans have shown that observational learning instils fearful conditioning after observing others it can also ease some forms of fear. In fear conditioning mice, they found a social buffering process served to prevent a long-term induction of fear to stressful events. This was also shown by Egliston & Rapee (2007) when investigating fear and avoidance learning for toddlers in three conditions (1) modelling group – positive feedback from mother about aversive stimuli (2) exposed to aversive stimuli alone (3) control group. All toddlers were then exposed to observational conditioning trial where the experimenter reacted fearfully of the aversive stimuli. Results showed toddlers in (1) were more likely to show positive affective reactions in comparison to the other groups, showing the power of positive parental reactions, a social buffer, to fear evoking stimuli which can over-ride negative reactions. All past literature shows fears learned through observational learning engage in the same neural mechanisms as direct learning, signifying indirect means may be just as effective and powerful as direct fear conditioning. Another form of indirect learning is inferred/derived relational responding which can be best explained through the relational frame theory (RFT) which proposes that humans transfer fear and avoidance responses across stimuli (Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Smeets, Cullinan & Leader, 2004; Healy, Barnes-Holmes & Smeets, 2000). An important process which can be undermined is the verbal and language processes which contribute to the avoidance response of indirect conditioning i.e. if a person has a spider phobia, but has never directly had fear-evoking experiences with a spider, the word ‘spider’ can act as a CS. Upon hearing the word ‘spider’, the individual elicits an alarmed response and seeks avoidance. This transfer of stimulus functions can also be seen through topographically different ‘spider-like’ stimuli i.e. real-life spiders, pictures, etc. The effect of verbal 6
  • 7. information has been shown to result from associate learning (Dymond & Rehfeldt, 2000; Field, 2006). By the functional definition of verbal events, there is the possibility of a contribution of indirectly acquired human avoidance learning in two concepts: derived relational responding and the transformation of functions. Augustson & Dougmer (1997) illustrated transformation of avoidance functions through training an ‘aversive class’ A1-B1-C1-D1 and a ‘non-aversive class’ A2-B2-C2-D2 then Pavlovian conditioning coupled B1 with shock and B2 without. A differential signalled avoidance task was issued where participants could avoid shock by pressing a key in relation to B1 appearing. Results showed that all participants during stimulus presentations of the aversive class elicited avoidance responses in comparison of non-aversive class, which they did not. This study was the first of its kind to depict how avoidance can be present in the absence of direct conditioning. This transformation of functions, i.e. establishing a particular behavioural response for one stimuli of an equivalence relation and then transferring the function for its multiple relations, was also seen in Forsyth, Eifert & Barrios’ (2006) study where by training A-B and B-C, participants derived B-A, C-B, A-C and C-A. If ‘B’ was paired with a fear-evoking response, then participants would derive to produce fearful behaviour for ‘A’ as well as the trained relation of ‘C’. RFT also proposes other forms of transfer of avoidance, other than stimulus equivalence based accounts, such as the relational frames of Same and Opposite can be learnt by humans (Dymond, Roche, Forsyth, Whelan & Rhoden, 2007; Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. 2008; Whelan & Barnes-Holmes, 2004). Dymond et al., (2008) study exposed participants to a relational completion procedure (RCP) of non- arbitrary and arbitrary testing and training to establish Same and Opposite and derived relations, which has been shown to be an effective procedure (Dymond & Whelan, 2010). Participants were taught all possible Same and Opposite relations of A1-B1-C1 and A2-B2- 7
  • 8. C2 which was followed by an avoidance conditioning procedure where the participants learnt to escape the discriminative stimulus (SD ) of aversive images and sounds by pressing the space bar and not omitting the same avoidance response to B2. They were also tested for transformation of functions for C1 and C2 and found participants that learnt the transformation of functions also displayed correct derived relational responding, this depicts the ability of humans to complete this task. The aim of this study is to examine the effectiveness of observed and derived relational learning on pathways of avoidance by exposing each participant to a fear and avoidance conditioning phase. The observed group will watch a video of a learning model completing a Pavlovian conditioning and avoidance acquisition phase. The video depicts a learning model receiving a mild electric shock when a selected circle (CS+) is shown on the screen; where the learning models facial expressions and arm movements whilst receiving the shock will serve as the US. The stimulus will predict the delivery of shock (CS+) whilst another contextually similar stimulus will present no negative consequences (CS-). Participants are instructed that they can avoid the CS+ if they choose the correct button (AV+). The inferred condition participants will be presented with RCP trials of non-arbitrary and arbitrary training and testing of Same and Opposite relations, as shown in Dymond et al., (2008) study, to test for transformation of functions and derived relational responding in later trials i.e. avoidance acquisition and extinction. The participants will have the AV+ option to avoid the upcoming CS+ shock. The participant’s expectancy of the shock on stimuli will be recorded through Likert Scales and its two factors of Avoidance and Non-Avoidance, at the end of certain phases (see Method). Bass, van Ooijen, Goudriann & Kenemans (2008) showed that a failure to learn the CS-US contingency would result in a higher contextual fear, and then participants would self- report higher levels of anxiety, which was confirmed by skin conductance (SCR) measures, 8
  • 9. any indiscrepancies will be followed up in the data. Behavioural avoidance will be recorded through percentage scores for each participant from eliciting an avoidance response for CS+ and CS- for each appropriate phase. An Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) will be undertaken by each participants. AAQ-II is a reliable psychometric 7-item scale to measure participant’s willingness to experience unwanted private events in the intention to reach individual goals and values i.e. ‘My painful memories prevent me from having a fulfilling life’ which participants would rate from 1=never true to 7=always true. López et al., (2010) found that people with high AAQ-II scores displayed a higher number of thought intrusions and higher susceptibility to a lower concentration level to the task at hand; this will be considered for any rogue data. The results of AAQ-II will be tested against behavioural avoidance responses to test for any correlations. An interaction between group (observed vs. inferred) and the three dependent variables (DV) of avoidance behaviour responses of the shock, expectancy of shock and AAQ-II will show any superior effectiveness of each group. The IV is conditioned stimuli (CS+, CS-). This study based upon a two-way hypothesis as it is searching for the effectiveness of each learning group. 9
  • 10. Method Participants A total of sixty participants (21 males and 39 females), were randomly placed in either the observed or inferred condition, with 30 in each condition. All recruited participants had English as their first language and were aged between 19-40 years old (M= 21.9, SD = 3.91). They were recruited via opportunistic, personal contacts and Swansea University’s Subject Pool. All participants were given an information sheet and a consent form (Appendix 1) before participating in the study, and they were given a debrief form at the end (Appendix 2). They participated in return for either course credit or £5. Design The between- subject factor is Group which has two levels: observed (30 participants) and inferred (30 participants). There are two within-subject factors (1) Conditioned Stimulus which has two levels: CS+ and CS-. (2) Learned Behaviour which has two levels: Avoidance (AV) and Non-Avoidance (NAV). Thus combination of the three factors created six experimental conditions. The DerivedCS+ will be compared against the inferred response rates of CS+ and CS-. There are three dependent variables: Avoidance behaviour responses of the shock in each phase, expectancy of shock (taken from Likert scales at the end of each phase), and AAQ-II results. (Appendix 3). Apparatus The computer system was Microsoft Windows XP Professional Version 2002 on a 17’’ LCD colour monitor, 1024 by 768 resolution. The computer program was written in Visual Basic® 6.0 which controlled all stimulus presentations and recorded all responses for both conditions. The electrode was 5cm in diameter, the program used for the production of electric shocks was PowerLab 2/25 Chart 5 for Windows. 10
  • 11. Stimuli Observed Condition A movie was produced for the observation stage of the experiment. The movie (3 min, 8s) depicted a male participant (the learning model) participating in a differential fear conditioning experiment (Figure 1). Two coloured circles (red and blue) served as CS and were presented in pseudo-random order for 3s on a computer screen in front of the learning model. The appearance of the circles was incorporated with an inter-stimulus interval (ITI) which varied between 4 and 7 s. Each coloured circle was presented six times through Phase 2 and 3. Six presentations of the colour circle serving as the CS+ were coupled with an uncomfortable shock to the left wrist of the learning model, where the model then displayed signs of distress i.e. tensing of the left arm, lowering of the eyebrows (US); whereas CS- was never paired with a shock. The level of shock was selected by the learning model to be uncomfortable but not painful prior to the recording of the video. In Phase 3, the participants viewed the learning model being able to cancel the upcoming shocks by pressing the button ‘M’ on the keyboard. Fig. 1. A snapshot from the movie presented to participants during the Pavlovian avoidance stage, showing the model facing a computer screen that presented the CS+ and CS-. Inferred Condition For the RCP trials, two stimuli in ‘wingdings’ font were used as contextual cues for Same (i.e.) and Opposite (i.e.). Six nonsense syllables were employed as sample and comparisons during relational training and testing, i.e. ZID, PAF, JOM, BEH, DAX, QAF. These are clarified using alphanumerics i.e. A1, B1, B2, C1, C2, N2. An N2 stimulus, if 11
  • 12. selected, was never reinforced. In Phases 4 – 7, B1, B2, C1 and C2 were used to test derived relations. Six presentations of the nonsense word serving as the CS+ were coupled with an uncomfortable shock to the non-dominant arm of the participant, which they set before the trials to be uncomfortable but not painful, whereas CS- and DerivedCS+ was never paired with a shock. Procedure Observed Condition General Procedure: On arrival to the lab, participants were given an information sheet depicting what was expected of them during the study and the use of shocks, any ailment issues which would mean termination of their participation, and if they wanted to opt out at any time they could do so with no penalties. If they agreed, then a consent form was signed, the AAQ-II questionnaire (M=17.13, SD=8.5) was counterbalanced so was either presented at this point or at the end of the experiment. Participants were then sat in front of a computer and an electrode was attached to the non-dominant arm and held in place by an elasticized bandage. Shock was set by the participants to a level that was uncomfortable but not painful (M= 0.51, SD= 0.2) starting a 0.250mA and working up in increments of 0.5. The participants were exposed to four conditions (1) Habituation (2) Pavlovian Avoidance (3) Avoidance Acquisition; and (4) Extinction. Phases (2) and (3) were observed by the participant watching the pre-recorded video of the learning model. Phase 1: Habituation Three presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented where the participant simply watched the screen. See Appendix 4 for computer instructions throughout both observed and inferred conditions. 12
  • 13. Phase 2: Pavlovian Avoidance (PA) and Phase 3: Avoidance Acquisition (AA) Participants observed the learning model completing the Pavlovian Avoidance stage where each circle was presented six times and the CS+ circle administered shocks to the learning model. Participants then observed the learning model completing the Avoidance Acquisition stage where the model learnt to cancel the upcoming shocks when the CS+ circle appeared on the screen by pressing the ‘M’ key on the keyboard. Phase 4: Extinction (EXT) This was the testing phase, where 5 presentations of the coloured circles were presented but in a different order to the one shown in the video. Essentially, no shocks were administered to the participant during this phase to ensure learning was acquired through indirect, social means only. At the end of testing, participants were presented with a Likert Scale with four questions; (CS+ is in italics and will keep throughout the method) (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when the red circle is presented and you do press a key? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when the red circle is presented and you do NOT press a key? (3) To what extent do you expect a shock when the blue circle is presented and you do press a key? (4) To what extent do you expect a shock when the blue circle is presented and you do NOT press a key? Participants could rate their answers between -3 (very unlikely) to 3 (very likely). Ratings are the same for all Likert Scales in both conditions. At the end of the experiment, subjects were given a debrief form. Inferred Condition On arrival to the lab, participants were given an information sheet depicting what was expected of them during the study and the use of shocks, any ailment issues which would mean termination of their participation, and if they wanted to opt out at any time they could do so with no penalties. If they agreed, then a consent form was signed, the AAQ-II questionnaire (M=15.63, SD=6.02) was counterbalanced so was either presented at this point 13
  • 14. or at the end of the experiment. The participants were exposed to eight conditions (1) Non- Arbitrary Training (2) Non-Arbitrary Testing (3) Arbitrary Training (4) Arbitrary Testing (5) Habituation (6) Pavlovian Avoidance (7) Avoidance Acquisition; and (8) Extinction. Phase 1 – 4 used RCP to train and test same and opposite relations and derived relationships. RCP General Procedure; Clicking on a tick box at the bottom of the screen began the experiment for the participants. A sample picture/nonsense words appeared on the top left corner of the screen, after 1s the contextual cue (i.e. or) appeared in the upper centre, and after another 1s a blank yellow square would appear on the top right corner of the screen. Three comparison stimuli appeared on the bottom section of the screen. A selection could be made by clicking the right mouse button on the intended picture and dragging to the black yellow square, then releasing the mouse button. When this happened, two red buttons appeared on the bottom of the screen that displayed the options, ‘Finish Trial’ or ‘Start Again’. Pressing the ‘Start Again’ button refreshed the page to where all the stimuli were at the start of the trial for the opportunity for another selection. Pressing the ‘Finish Trial’ button cleared the screen and either produced a feedback screen during training phases, where if the correct selection was made, the word ‘Correct’ was displayed, otherwise the word ‘Wrong’ was displayed. The intertribal interval (ITI) of 3 seconds then happened, until the next trial began. Phase 1: Non-Arbitrary Relational Training Phase 1 and 2 are non-arbitrary training and testing intended to establish same and opposite labelling for the contextual cues. The samples and comparisons stimuli were non- arbitrary (formally related) and choosing the stimulus that was either the same or opposite of the sample was reinforced. For instance, if a participant was shown a ‘smiley face’ as the sample and the opposite contextual cue was shown, the selecting the ‘frowning face’ as the comparison was reinforced, likewise if the same contextual cue was shown, and the same 14
  • 15. ‘smiley face’ comparison was selected. There were six stimulus sets (shown in Table 1), presented in a random order for each participant. When participants produced eight consecutively correct responses, they then progressed onto Phase 2. Table 1 Stimulus sets used during Phase 1 and 2, and the physical dimensions of each stimulus set. Physical Dimension Description End 1 End 2 Phase 1 Squares Black White Lines Short Long Smiley Face Frowning Smiling Cubes Small Big Trees Small Big Red Disk Sections Thin Crescent Full disk Phase 2 Columns Narrow Wide Wavy lines Small amp. Big amp. Snowstorm No snow Whiteout Buildings Small Big Pointed Star 3 points 20 points Bowed Trees Straight Very bowed. Phase 2: Non-Arbitrary Relational Testing Phase 2 followed the same design as Phase 1, except the crucial removal of feedback of responses, and was just followed by the ITI. Six new stimulus sets were used which were presented in a random order. Participants were required to respond correct consistently across all eight trails to be exposed to Phase 3, failure resulted in re-exposure to Phase 1. Phase 3: Arbitrary Relational Training Phase 3 and 4 are arbitrary training and testing intended to establish same and opposite relations of sample and comparison stimuli of an arbitrary nature (trigrams). For this example and throughout, it will be explained in this way; the contextual cue used will be stated in capitals, followed by the sample stimulus, and then followed by the three comparison stimuli options in brackets. The correct answer which would be reinforced will be shown in italics i.e. SAME/A1 (C2-C1-N2). The example demonstrates that in the 15
  • 16. presence of the contextual cue SAME and the sample stimulus A1, choosing C1 would have been reinforced, whereas choosing C2 and N2 would not have been. All participants were presented with the following four training trials: SAME/A1 (C2-C1-N2), SAME/A1 (N2-B1- B2), OPPOSITE/A1 (B2-N2-B1), OPPOSITE/A1 (C2-N2-C1). Training was presented in eight trials, with each trial presented twice. If participants made the correct answer they were given feedback by the word ‘Correct’ appearing on the screen. When participants produced eight consecutively correct answers they were immediately exposed to Phase 4. Phase 4: Arbitrary Relational Testing Phase 4 tested for the emergences of derived relations from the training of same and opposite. Figure 2 depicts the predicated relational network. Feedback was withheld during this phase, although the alphanumeric in italics is what the participants would choose if responding in accordance with the predicted relational network; the test trails were as follows; SAME/B1 (C2-C1-N2), SAME/B2 (C2-N2-C1), SAME/C1 (N2-B1-C2), SAME/C2 (C1-N2-B2), OPPOSITE/B1 (C2-C1-N2), OPPOSITE/B2 (C1-N2-C2), OPPOSITE/C1 (N2- C2-B2), and OPPOSITE/C2 (B1-B2-N2). The author would like to note that all possible probe trials i.e. B-C and C-B were tested during the arbitrary relational testing as in Dymond et al., (2008) paper. Testing was carried out for sixteen trials, with each task repeated twice per block. It was compulsory that participants produced a minimum score of 14/16 (87.5%) correct responses to pass. Failing to meet this measure they were immediately exposed to Phase 1 again and set to go through each trial until meeting the goal. 16
  • 17. Fig. 2. The predicted relational network of trained and tested stimuli. The alphanumerics represent the nonsense syllables used in the Phase 3 and the letters S and O indicate Same and Opposite. Solid lines signify trained relations whilst dashed lines signify derived relations. General Procedure #2 After meeting the criterion for the RCP trials, the electrode was attached to the non- dominant arm of the participant and held in place by an elasticized bandage. Shock was set by the participants to a level that was uncomfortable but not painful (M= 0.51, SD= 0.19) starting a 0.250mA and working up in increments of 0.05 Phase 5: Habituation Three presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented on to the screen, exposing participants to the stimuli. Phase 6: PA The participants sat comfortably whilst observing the screen, when the CS+ appeared a mild shock, which level was selected by the participant before, was administered to their arm through the electrode. They were exposed to each CS+ and CS- six times. In order to counterbalance which word served as the CSD+ and CS-, the program was edited to have either B1 or B2 as the CS+. After the phase was finished they were asked to complete a 17
  • 18. Likert Scale, the questions were as follows; (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented? Phase 7: AA The aim of this trial was to observe if participants would elicit a simple avoidance response during the presence of CS+ and not CS-. Six trials of the CS+ and CS- (B1 and/or B2) were presented to the participant with the option to avoid the shock by pressing the ‘M’ key on the keyboard. Participants had to cancel 5 consecutive shocks to pass onto the next phase. If they met the criterion they were passed onto Likert Scale questions; (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do press a key? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do NOT press a key? (3) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do press a key? (4) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do NOT press a key? Then participants are finally exposed to the final phase. Phase 8: EXT This was the testing phase; 6 presentations of CS+ and CS- were presented alongside introducing the DerivedCS+ i.e. if CS+ was B1, using the predicted relational network the DerivedCS+ would be C1, same scenario if CS+ was B2 so the DerivedCS+ would be C2., so the aim of this phase is to test if participants have learnt the derived relations to elicit an avoidance response. Essentially, no shocks were administered to the participants during this phase to ensure learning was acquired through learned responses beforehand and also testing for derived relations. After this phase, they were presented with the last Likert Scale, which is similar to Phase 7 Likert Scale but with an important last two questions about the DerivedCS+; (1) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do press a key? (2) To what extent do you expect a shock when PAF is presented and you do 18
  • 19. NOT press a key? (3) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do press a key? (4) To what extent do you expect a shock when JOM is presented and you do NOT press a key? (5) To what extent do you expect a shock when BEH is presented and you do press a key? (6) To what extent do you expect a shock when BEH is presented and you do NOT press a key? At the end of the experiment, subjects were given a debrief form. 19
  • 20. Results Table 2 demonstrates the performance of participants throughout Inferred Phases 1 – 4 (RCP). 13/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their first exposure. P13 passed non-arbitrary testing after two exposures and passed arbitrary testing on their first exposure. 8/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their second exposure. 5/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their third exposure. The final 8/30 participants passed both non-arbitrary and arbitrary relational testing on their maximum fourth exposure. Therefore, all 30 participants were eligible to progress on the avoidance-conditioning phase. Table 2 Trials to Criterion and Percentage of Correct Responses in Phase 1 – 4 Participant Phase 1: Non- arbitrary Relational Training (trials to criterion) Phase 2: Non- arbitrary Relational Testing (%) Phase 3: Arbitrary Relational Training (trials to criterion) Phase 4: Arbitrary Relational Testing (%) 1 10 100 16 43.75 8 100 12 87.5 2 9 100 13 93.75 3 9 100 44 62.5 8 100 8 68.75 8 100 8 87.5 4 12 100 8 93.75 5 18 100 10 87.5 6 14 100 9 93.75 7 10 100 23 93.75 8 8 100 15 81.25 8 100 8 93.75 9 8 100 9 37.5 8 100 8 81.25 8 100 8 87.5 10 16 100 20 93.75 11 9 100 12 93.75 12 23 100 12 56.25 8 100 9 56.25 8 100 8 93.75 13 15 62.5 8 100 25 87.5 14 8 100 8 68.75 8 100 9 81.25 8 87.5 8 100 8 93.75 15 11 85.71 20
  • 21. 8 87.5 8 100 41 81.25 8 100 9 93.75 16 14 100 24 87.5 17 9 100 16 62.5 8 100 8 81.25 8 100 8 100 18 9 87.5 13 100 36 93.75 19 44 100 52 75 8 100 8 87.5 20 8 100 14 100 21 8 100 9 87.5 22 12 100 31 56.25 8 100 9 56.25 8 87.5 13 100 8 93.75 23 10 100 32 81.25 9 100 8 93.75 24 11 100 28 75 11 100 43 87.5 25 8 100 9 68.75 8 100 8 97.5 26 11 100 52 68.75 16 100 21 25 10 100 14 87.5 27 11 100 34 97.5 28 9 100 48 37.5 13 100 18 87.5 29 13 100 15 100 30 8 100 8 100 Descriptive Graphs Figure 3 displays the mean percentage of avoidance behaviour across both learning groups, (Observed: Phase 4; Inferred: Phases 7 & 8) and the three stimuli (CS+, CS- and DerivedCS+). The graph suggests a significant difference between CS+ and CS- for both learning groups and also depicts the emergence of derived learning from its similarity from the CS+ stimuli. The small standard error bars show low variability in avoidance response results. 21
  • 22. MeanAvoidanceBehaviour(%) Observed Inferred 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 CS+ CS- DerivedCS+ Fig. 3. Graph showing the mean avoidance behaviour (%) of participants in both learning groups. Figure 4 displays the mean expectancy of shock if they were to elicit an avoidance response, that was self-reported from participants through Likert Ratings across both learning groups (Observed: Phase 4; Inferred; Phases 6-8) and three stimuli (CS+, CS- and DerivedCS+). The graph suggests that Observed participants rated the expectancy of shock after an avoidance response higher than the Inferred group, the standard error shows great variability across both learning groups responses. MeanExpectancy+Avoidance Observed Inferred 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 CS+ CS- DerivedCS+ 22
  • 23. Fig. 4. Graph showing the mean expectancy of a shock when performing an avoidance response. Figure 5 displays the mean expectancy of shock if not eliciting an avoidance response. Results were self-reported by participants through Likert Ratings across both learning groups (Observed: Phase 4; Inferred; Phases 6-8) and three stimuli (CS+, CS- and DerivedCS+). The graph suggests a significant difference for both learning groups between CS+ and CS- as the former had a greater mean expectancy rate over the latter. It also depicts the emergence of derived learning from the similarity between the CS+ and DerivedCS+ stimuli. The standard error bars show low variability in both groups. MeanExpectancy+NonAvoidance Observed Inferred 0 2 4 6 8 CS+ CS- DerivedCS+ Fig. 5. Graph showing the mean expectancy of shock if participants across both learning groups did not perform an avoidance response. Aggregate Results Two independent samples t-tests were performed to examine the statistical evidence shown from the graphs. In each test, the percentage scores of avoidance responses of CS+ and CS- in the Observation EXT group were compared against Inferred EXT group. For these two tests, P 16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the criterion. 23
  • 24. Firstly, Observed EXT CS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS+. Considering the group statistics, there seemed to be very little difference in the means, which is confirmed using the top row of the t-test, as Levene’s test is insignificant (F=3.91, p=.053), showing an insignificant difference (Observed EXT group M=98.33, SD=5.09; Inferred EXT group M=99.40, SD=3.15; t(56)=.956, p=.343). Secondly; Observed EXT CS- was compared against Inferred EXT CS-, considering the group statistics, there was possibly a small difference in the means, and Levene’s test showed a significant difference (F=13.49, p=.001) between the variances of the two groups. Using the bottom row of the t-test, it can be concluded there is no significant difference between the means at the 5% significance level (Observed EXT group M=1.11, SD=4.23; Inferred EXT group M=4.17, SD=8.64; t(38.63)=1.69, p=.099). However, there is a weak significant difference at the 10% level, showing that the Observed group preformed marginally better than the Inferred, as they had a lower mean score. Overall, the independent t-tests showed Observed and Inferred learning groups were both as effective for eliciting avoidance behaviour. Six paired samples t-tests were conducted to study the percentage scores of avoidance responses of CS+ and CS-, and to test the emergence of a derived relational response in Inferred EXT phase. They also tested the expectancy of shock Likert Scale ratings for the Inferred condition. Observed EXT CS+ was compared against Observed EXT CS-. At the 10% significance level there was a weak negative correlation (correlation=.356, p=.053) between CS+ and CS- showing participants that elicited a avoidance response on CS+, did not on CS-, as was expected. This is supported by the descriptive statistical means and a significant difference in the t-test (Observed EXT CS+ M=98.33, SD=5.09; Observed EXT CS- M=1.11, SD=4.23; t(29)=69.29, p=.000). 24
  • 25. Inferred AA CS+ was compared against Inferred AA CS-. P16 data was excluded for failure to meet the criterion. Samples statistics shows CS+ had a higher mean avoidance response over CS-, showing a higher proportion of participants showed learnt behaviour and cancelled the upcoming shocks, over the CS-. Also supported by the t-test showing a significant difference (Inferred AA CS+ M=93.75, SD=12.52; Inferred AA CS- M=2.08, SD=5.6; t(31)=40.83, p=.000). Inferred EXT CS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS-. P16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the criterion. The samples statistics indicate a significant difference between the CS+ and CS- from the mean avoidance response, and this is confirmed by the t-test showing a significant difference. This shows that participants produced the avoidance response by learning the correct relation (Inferred EXT CS+ M=99.40, SD=3.15; Inferred EXT CS- M=4.17, SD=8.64; t(27)=56.57, p=.000). Inferred EXT DerivedCS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS+ to test for emergences of derived relations. P16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the criterion. The samples statistics indicate no significant difference between CS+ and DerivedCS+ and is confirmed by the t-test showing no significant difference. Results illustrate derived transformation of functions emerged and were as effective as CS+ stimuli (Inferred EXT Derived CS+ M=95.24, SD=19.7; Inferred EXT CS+ M=99.4, SD=3.15; t(27)=1.1, p=.282). Inferred EXT DerivedCS+ was compared against Inferred EXT CS-. P16 and 22 data were excluded for failure to meet the criterion. Samples statistics suggested a significant difference, and was confirmed by the t-test showing a significant difference. This shows that participants produced the avoidance response by learning the correct derived relation to CS+ therefore learning to cancel the upcoming shock, as would be expected under CS+ (Inferred 25
  • 26. EXT DerivedCS+ M=95.24, SD=19.7; Inferred EXT CS- M=4.17, SD=8.64; t(31)=40.83, p=.000). The final paired samples t-test was conducted on the Inferred PA Likert Scales showing expectancy of shock when presented with the contextual stimuli. The sample statistics illustrate a significant difference, and the t-test confirms. This shows that the participants rated the correct CS+ higher than the CS- on expectancy of shock (PA CS+ M=5.93, SD=.254; PA CS- M=.133, SD=.434; t(29)=57.67, p=.000). A Pearson’s r test was conducted to investigate any correlations between AAQ-II data and learnt behaviour. However, there were no significant correlations identified between: Observed AAQ-II scores and Observed EXT CS+ (r= -.114, N=30. P=.548); Observed AAQ- II and Observed EXT CS- (r= .219, N=30, p=.244); Inferred AAQ-II scores and Inferred EXT CS+ (r= -.008, N=30, p=.966); or between Inferred AAQ-II scores and Inferred EXT CS- (r=.200, N=30, p=.290). This shows there is no relationship between AAQ-II scores and avoidance responses of CS+ or CS-. The total number of responses in the Inferred AA Likert Scales for each of our within subject experimental conditions was entered into a 2 (CS+, CS-) x 2 (AV, NAV) multi-way within subjects ANOVA. Results showed a significant main effect of Conditioned Stimuli (F(1,29)=108.38, p=.000), showing that conditioned stimuli had a significant effect on the expectancy of shock. Another significant main effect of Learned Behaviour (F(1,29)=73.9, p=.000) also showed learned behaviour effects expectancy of shock. A significant interaction was shown between Conditioned Stimuli*Learned Behaviour (F(1,29)=126.68, p=.000). Figure 6 shows participants understood the significance of the avoidance behaviour. The difference in the estimated marginal means for AV and NAV for CS+ shows that participants learnt the importance of the avoidance response, whereas for CS- where the results are 26
  • 27. similar for AV and NAV, participants learnt that they wouldn’t get shocked regardless of avoidance response. Fig. 6. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores displaying the interaction between Conditioned Stimuli and Learned Behaviour. Post-hoc tests (paired samples t-test) were conducted to further investigate these interactions. Results showed the mean difference between CS+NAV was significantly larger for CS+AV (t(29)=11.43, p=.000), this showed that participants learnt the effectiveness of the avoidance behaviour to reduce the expectancy of shock. There was no significant mean difference between CS-AV and CS-NAV (t(29)=.432, p=.669) and no significant mean difference between CS+AV and CS-AV (t(29)=1.18, p=.247). These both show that participants learnt when to expect a shock and the appropriate avoidance response. There was 27
  • 28. a significant mean difference between CS+NAV and CS-NAV (t(29)=21.13, p=.000) which illustrates that participants learnt the correct shock stimulus. The total number of responses in the Observed EXT and Inferred EXT Likert Scales for each of our six experimental conditions was entered into a 2 (observed, inferred) x 2 (CS+, CS-) x 2 (AV, NAV) multi-way mixed ANOVA. Results showed a significant main effect of the conditioned stimuli F(1,58)=299.34, p=.000), a significant main effect of learned behaviour (F (1,58)=114.35, p=.000) and a significant effect on learning group (F(1,58)=10.97, p=.002) which showed that which learning group the participant was in had a significant effect on the expectancy of shock. Within-subjects test showed a non-significant effect of group*conditioned stimuli interaction (F(1,58)=.148, p=.702); Non-significant effect of group*learnt behaviour interaction (F(1,58)=2.22, p=.141) and a significant interaction of condition stimuli*learnt behaviour (F(1,58)=201.38, p=.000). This interaction is shown in Figure 7 participants understood the significance of the avoidance behaviour. The difference in the estimated marginal means for AV and NAV for CS+ shows that participants learnt the importance of the avoidance response, whereas for CS- where the results are similar for AV and NAV, participants learnt that they wouldn’t get shocked regardless of avoidance response. The three-way interaction between conditioned stimuli*learnt behaviour*group was not significant (F (1,58)=.02, p=.889). 28
  • 29. Fig. 7. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred EXT displaying the interaction between Conditioned Stimuli and Learned Behaviour. Figure 8 plots the group against conditioned stimuli whilst Figure 9 plots group against learned behaviour. Observed is rated higher in expectancy of shock for both conditioned stimuli and learned behaviour, showing that observed rated the expectancy of shock higher than Inferred. 29
  • 30. Fig. 8. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred EXT displaying the interaction between Group and Conditioned Stimuli. 30
  • 31. Fig. 9. Estimated marginal means of expectancy scores from Observed EXT and Inferred EXT displaying the interaction between Group and Learned Behaviour. Post-hoc tests (independent t-test) were conducted to examine the main effects and the conditioned stimulus*learnt behaviour interaction; results showed no significant difference at the 5% but a weak significant difference at the 10% level between Observed CS+AV and Inferred CS+AV. Levene’s test showed significance in variance (F=5.68, p=.020) so the bottom row was used (t(52.87)=1.74, p=.088). A non-significant difference was shown between Observed CS+NAV and Inferred CS+NAV (t(58)=.297, p=.768). A significant difference was found between Observed CS-AV and Inferred CS-AV, Levene’s test (F=11.35, p=.001) showed bottom row to be used (t(47.8)=2.19, p=.033). And finally a non- significant difference was shown between Observed CS-NAV and Inferred CS-NAV, 31
  • 32. Levene’s test (F=10.79, p=.002) so bottom row was used (t(43.84)=1.58, p=.122). Discussion 32
  • 33. The aim of this present study was to examine the effectiveness of observed and inferred (derived) pathways of avoidance. Comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS- and Inferred EXT CS+ and CS-; no significant differences were found between the learning groups and conditioned stimuli, showing the two learning groups were just as effective as each other in producing a fear-evoked avoidance response through indirect means. The findings suggest that observed and inferred pathways of avoidance are equally as effective as each other, and from past supporting literature can hypothesize as effective as direct Pavlovian fear conditioning. (Cook & Mineka, 1990; Dymond et al., 2007; Egliston & Rapee, 2007; Whelan & Barner-Holmes, 2004). The Observed condition was shown to be effective at inducing an avoidance response at the correct CS+ after viewing the video of the learning model in the similar task, which mirrors Mineka & Cook (1993) study findings. This was shown by comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS-. There was a weak negative correlation showing participants that elicited an avoidance response on CS+ did not on CS-, which was to be expected. It could be hypothesized that the participants produced avoidance behaviour due to feeling empathy (Knapska et al., 2009; Olsson et al., 2007) or just due to imitation of the learning models avoidance response (Kavaliers et al., 2001) as 30/30 participants produced an avoidance response, but this can’t be fully proved without an fMRI scan during the task. There was no correlation found between AAQ-II scores and avoidance responses of CS+ or CS-. López et al., (2010) study was inconsistent with our findings as the participants data that had to be excluded from the Inferred condition actually had low AAQ-II scores (P16, AAQ-II = 12 and P22, AAQ-II = 9). 33
  • 34. For the inferred condition, RCP was shown to be effective in training same, opposite and derived relations as seen by 30/30 participants passing (Appendix 5) albeit first of final exposure. This supports Dymond & Whelan’s (2010) study of RCP being an effective procedure. Comparing Inferred EXT Derived CS+ against Inferred EXT CS+ no significance was found between the two stimuli, illustrating derived transformation of functions emerged and avoidance responses matched those of CS+. There was a significant difference shown between DerivedCS+ and CS- illustrating that participants again, learnt the derived relations and ignored the CS- stimuli in relation to an upcoming shock. These results support RFT, that participants transferred fear and avoidance responses across stimuli due to learning derived relational responding from the RCP (Barnes-Holmes et al., 2004, Healy et al., 2000). In the 2*2 within subjects ANOVA for the Inferred AA Likert scales, there was a significant main effect on conditioned stimuli and learnt behaviour showing both variables had an effect on the perception of the expectancy of shock. The significant interaction between conditioned stimuli*learned behaviour illustrates that participants learnt the correct contingency for CS+ and showed they understood they wouldn’t get shocked for CS- regardless of avoidance response. Post-hoc tests showed that participants understood the effectiveness of the avoidance behaviour to reduce the expectancy of shock. In the final 2*2*2 multi-way mixed ANOVA, results showed significant main effects for conditioned stimuli, learned behaviour and learning group. Comparing conditioned stimuli and learning group the only significant interaction was between Observed CS-AV and Inferred CS-AV which could show that participants in both conditions found the understood the significance of CS- and that they did not have to produce an avoidance response. 34
  • 35. Comparing group against conditioned stimuli and learned behaviour (Figure 8 and 9) it is clear that Observed rated higher in expectancy of shock to both, this could be attributed to the Observed group understanding the CS-US contingency and the value of CS- more than the Inferred group. The Observed condition was shown to be effective at inducing an avoidance response at the correct CS+ after viewing the video of the learning model in the similar task, which mirrors Mineka & Cook (1993) study findings. This was shown by comparing Observed EXT CS+ and CS-. There was a weak negative correlation showing participants that elicited an avoidance response on CS+ did not on CS-, which was to be expected. It could be hypothesized that the participants produced avoidance behaviour due to feeling empathy (Knapska et al., 2009; Olsson et al., 2007) or just due to imitation of the learning models avoidance response (Kavaliers et al., 2001) as 30/30 participants produced an avoidance response, but this can’t be fully proved without an fMRI scan during the task. However, a criticism of this condition is that the participants were exposed to fear- evoking stimuli and then immediately exposed to a directly taught avoidance response; this can be a serious limitation that could suggest the results may have been taught through direct means. The direct observation of the learning model completing the avoidance response in the presence of one stimuli and not the other will have affected the avoidance contingency and consequently influenced the avoidance behaviour. Research has demonstrated that explicit instruction is not needed to generate indirect observational fear learning (Guzmán et al., 2009; Helsen et al., 2011; Olsson et al., 2007). To improve on this condition, the participants would only be exposed to the learning model experiencing the Pavlovian Avoidance phase, then be exposed to Likert ratings and finally, the participant will experience avoidance acquisition phase with the same set of instructions given to the Inferred group avoidance acquisition phase (Appendix 4). This would ensure that the participants are 35
  • 36. being indirectly taught the CS-US contingency and then eliciting an avoidance behaviour response from that. The inferred condition results illustrate that participants showed transformation of functions for the DerivedCS+ from the CS+. This condition supports the ideas for improvement on the observed condition that explicit instructions were not needed for derived transformation or avoidance behaviours to occur (Dymond et al., 2008; Field, 2006). The derived extinction phase mirrors clinical procedures in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Hayes, Follette & Linehan, 2004; Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda & Lillis, 2006) called cognitive diffusion. This is a where patients illiterate negative self-statements for a few minutes i.e. ‘I have no friends’ until generalized of the extinction is met, i.e. vocally created stimuli in relational to real-life environmental events, not within the therapy setting. The observed condition would have to be retested against the inferred condition to test for any real significant difference, as the limitations of this study hinders the potential findings reflected in all past literature. The emergence of transformation of functions and derived relational responding shows the effectiveness of Same and Opposite relational frames in a RCP, and illustrates a starting point for more procedures to aid in indirect anxiety and phobias to be built. To improve the effect of indirect learning over direct, a further study would be to test the observed group and inferred group against an instructed (Pavlovian fear condition) group. This will affirm the effectiveness of indirect learning of fearful stimuli. An issue of external validity must also be raised, as it does not necessarily stand that as participants showed promise in avoidance functions across stimuli in a lab setting with novel stimuli, that in a real life setting the results would be the same. To conclude, the results demonstrated participants transferred a fear response to a novel stimuli through observation 36
  • 37. and that derived relational training can effectively train participants to transfer learned fear avoidance response across novel stimuli in a lab environment. 37
  • 38. References Augustson, E. M., & Dougmer, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 3, 181-91. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Smeets, P. M., Cullinan, V., & Leader, G. (2004). Relational frame theory and stimulus equivilance: Conceptual and procedural issues. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 181-214. Barot, S. K., Chung, A., Kim, J. J., & Bernstein, I. L. (2009). Functional Imaging of Stimulus Convergence in Amygdalar Neurons during Pavlovian Fear Conditioning. PLoS ONE, 4(7):e6156. Barot, S. K., Kyono, Y., Clark, E. W., & Bernstein, I. L. (2008). Visualizing Stimulus Convergence in Amygdala Neurons during Associative Learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(52), 20959-63. Bass, J. M., van Ooijen, L., Goudriaan, A., & Kenemans, J. L. (2008). Failure to condition to a cue is associated with sustained contextual fear. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 581-92. Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., Waltz, T., & Zettle, R. D. (2011). Properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire – II: A| revised measure of psychological flexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42, 676-88. Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 16, 372-89. Davies, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The Amygdala: Vigilance and Emotion. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 13-34. Delgado, M. R., Jou, R. L., Ledoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2009). Avoiding negative outcomes: Tracking the mechanisms of avoidance learning in humans during fear conditioning. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 3(33). Delgado, M. R., Li, J., Schiller, D., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). The role of the striatum in aversive learning and aversive prediction errors. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Studies, 363(1511), 3787-800. Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. (2000). Understanding complex behaviour: The transformation of stimulus functions. The Behaviour Analyst, 23, 239-54. Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behaviour analysis of anxiety and avoidance. The Behaviour Analyst, 32, 7-27. 38
  • 39. Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance response function in accordance with Same and Opposite relational frames. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88(2), 249-62. Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2008). Derived avoidance learning: Transformation of avoidance responses function in accordance with Same and Opposite relational frames. The Psychological Record, 58, 269-86. Dymond, S., & Whelan, R. (2010). Derived relational responding: A comparison of Match- to-Sample and the Relational Completion Procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 94(1), 37-55. Egliston, K. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(8), 1871- 82. Forsyth, J. P., Eifert, G. H., & Barrios, V. (2006). Fear conditioning research as a clinical analog: What makes fear learning disorderd? In M. G. Craske, D. Hermans & D. Vansteenwegen. (Eds.). Fear and learning: Basic science to clinical application (pp. 133-53). Washington DC, USA: American Psychological Association. Field, A. P. (2006). Is conditioning a useful framework for understanding the development and treatment of phobias? Clinical Psychology Review, 26(7), 857-75. Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279-87. Guzmán, Y. F., Tronson, N. C., Guedea, A., Huh, K. H., Gao, C., & Radulovic, J. (2009). Social modelling of conditioned fear in mice by non-fearful conspecifics. Behavioural Brain Research, 201(1), 173-78. Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press. Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. M. (2004). Mindfulness and Acceptance: Expanding the Cognitive-Behavioural Tradition. New York, USA: The Guilford Press. Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy (22), 1-25. Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 74, 207-227. 39
  • 40. Helsen, K., Goubert, L., Peters, M. L., & Vlaeven, J. W. (2011). Observational learning and pain related fear: An experimental study with coloured cold pressor tasks. The Journal of Pain, 12(12), 1230-39. Kavaliers, M., Choleris, E., Colwell, D. D. (2001). Learning from others to cope with biting flies: social learning of fear-induced conditioned analgesia and active avoidance. Behavioural Neuroscience, 115, 661-74. Kim, J. J., & Jung, M. W. (2006). Neural circuits and mechanisms involved in Pavlovian fear conditioning: A critical review. Neuroscience and Bio-behavioural Reviews, 30(2), 188-202. Knapska, E., Mikosz, M., Werka, T., & Maren, S. (2009). Social modulation of learning in rats. Learning and Memory, 17(1), 35-42. LaBar, K. S., LeDoux, J. E., Spencer, D. D., & Phelps, E. A. (1995). Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 6846-55. LeDoux, J. E., & Gorman, J. M. (2001). A call to action: Overcoming anxiety through active coping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1953-55. López, J. C., Ruiz, F. J., Feder, J., Rubio, A. B., Suárez-Aguirre, J. J., Rodríguez, J. A., & Luciano, C. (2010). The role of experiential avoidance in the performance on a high cognitive demand task. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10(3), 475-488. Lovibond, P. F., Chen, S. X., Mitchell, C. J., Weidemann, G. (2011). Competition between an avoidance response and a safety signal: Evidence for a single learning system. Biological Psychology, In press. Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122, 23-38. Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10-26. Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission. SCAN, 2, 3-11. Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1095-102. Phillips, R. G., & LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioural Neuroscience, 106(2), 274-85. 40
  • 41. Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioural theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87-115. Seligman, M. E. P. (1968). Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(2), 402-11. Siddle, D. A., & Bond, N. W. (1988). Avoidance learning, Pavlovian conditioning, and the development of phobias. Biological Psychology, 27(2), 167-83. Watson, J. B. & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1-14. Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 86, 317-35. 41
  • 42. Appendix 1 – Information Sheet (Observed) Information Sheet In this study, you will be attached to a bar electrode which will deliver electric shocks to your forearm, you will set the intensity of the shocks at the beginning of the experiment to be ‘uncomfortable but not painful’. A set of instructions will appear on the screen before each phase begins and time will be allowed for any questions to be asked to the researcher. There are 4 phases in total one of which requires you to watch a video. At the end of the study you will be given a debrief form explaining the nature of the study and the opportunity to find out more about this research and to ask any unanswered questions you may have. This study is conducted in accordance with the British Psychological Society and Departmental ethic guidelines, your participation in this study is completely voluntary and you may withdraw at any point, without penalty. You can also choose to have any data that you provided in this study to be disregarded at any time, during or after the study. Otherwise, your results will be kept anonymous and confidential to only the researchers or supervisors working on this study. All data will be password protected and not accessed for any other reasons than this study. You must not participate in this study if you have a pacemaker fitted; suffer with heart palpitations or currently talking psychotropic medication. If you agree to take part in this study we would appreciate it if you agree to not discuss your participation or any details of this study with others. Please now complete the consent forms and return them to the researcher. 42
  • 43. Appendix 1 – Information Sheet (Inferred) Information Sheet In this study, you will be attached to a bar electrode which will deliver electric shocks to your forearm, you will set the intensity of the shocks at the beginning of the experiment to be ‘uncomfortable but not painful’. A set of instructions will be presented on the screen before each phase begins and time will be allowed for any questions to be asked to the researcher. There are 8 phases in total, phase 6 onwards may entail up to 12 electric shocks. At the end of the study you will be given a debrief form explaining the nature of the study and the opportunity to find out more about this research and to ask any unanswered questions you may have. This study is conducted in accordance with the British Psychological Society and Departmental ethic guidelines, your participation in this study is completely voluntary and you may withdraw at any point, without penalty. You can also choose to have any data that you provided in this study to be disregarded at any time, during or after the study. Otherwise, your results will be kept anonymous and confidential to only the researchers or supervisors working on this study. All data will be password protected and not accessed for any other reasons than this study. You must not participate in this study if you have a pacemaker fitted; suffer with heart palpitations or currently talking psychotropic medication. If you agree to take part in this study, we would appreciate it if you agree to not discuss your participation or any details of this study with any others. Please now complete the consent forms and return them to the researcher. Appendix 1 – Consent Form (Observed & Inferred) 43
  • 44. Project title: The effectiveness of observed and inferred pathways of avoidance on learning. Name of Supervisor: Dr Simon Dymond, (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk) Name of investigators: Katie Davies & Ellyn Moore Name of participant: Age: Sex: • I consent to take part in this study and am satisfied with the instructions I have been given so far. I have also been reassured that any further information I request regarding this study will be supplied to me at the end of this experiment. • I have been informed that the data I provide will remain confidential and be password protected. I am free to ask any questions at any time before, during and after the study and have been provided with a copy of this form and a participant information sheet. I also understand I will be provided with a debrief form at the end of the study. • I understand that during this study I will be required to place a bar electrode on my forearm which will be held in place by a small bandage. I will then be subject to a series of electric shocks, of which I am able to set the intensity at the beginning of the experiment. I understand the shock level will be “uncomfortable but not painful” and am free to stop the study at any time. • I am at least 18 years of age and have not been coerced in any way to participate in this study and can withdraw from the study at any point. • I understand I cannot participate in this study if I have any medical history of cardiac problems, or have been fitted with a pacemaker. Data protection: I agree to the university processing personal data that I have supplied. I agree to the processing of such data for any purposes connected with the research project as outlined to me. Participant signature: Date: Name of researcher: Signature: Date: 44
  • 45. Appendix 2 – Debrief Form (Observed) Thank you for taking part in this study. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of observed learning and derived learning pathways of avoidance. Throughout this study we were testing whether you had learnt to avoid the correct stimulu just from observing someone completing a similar task, so at no point during the experiment, other than the calibration of the shock strength, were you in danger of receiving an electric shock based on the procedures of Olsson et al (2007). We would like to remind participants that if you feel uncomfortable with this study, then you are free to withdraw your data from this study at any time, although we would also like to remind you that all results are confidential and at no time will any data be identifiable to you or your name. If this study has resulted in any discomfort to yourself, we would like to inform you of the Student Counselling Service provided on campus at Horton House or alternatively, you can contact them on +44 (0) 1792 295592 or email wellbeing@swansea.ac.uk We would like if you would refrain from talking about this experiment to others whilst it is still running as any details you reveal could affect future results. If you would like to be informed of the results of this study once it is completed, feel free to contact the experimenters on; 554515@swansea.ac.uk or; 550819@swansea.ac.uk Supervisior; Dr Simon Dymond (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk) Telephone: 01792 295602 If you would like more information about this area of research, please follow these references; Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88, 249-262. Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission. SCAN, 2, 3-11. 45
  • 46. Appendix 2 – Debrief Form (Inferred) Thank you for taking part in this study. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of observed learning and derived learning pathways of avoidance. Throughout this study we were testing whether you had learnt the correct same and opposite pairings for this experiment phases, so some phases had the threat of electric shocks whilst others did not and was just to test you. We would like to remind participants that if you feel uncomfortable with this study, then you are free to withdraw your data from this study at any time, although we would also like to remind you that all results are confidential and at no time will any data be identifiable to you or your name. If this study has resulted in any discomfort to yourself, we would like to inform you of the Student Counselling Service provided on campus at Horton House or alternatively, you can contact them on +44 (0) 1792 295592 or email wellbeing@swansea.ac.uk We would like if you would refrain from talking about this experiment to others whilst it is still running as any details you reveal could affect future results. If you would like to be informed of the results of this study once it is completed, feel free to contact the experimenters on; 554515@swansea.ac.uk or; 550819@swansea.ac.uk Supervisior; Dr Simon Dymond (s.o.dymond@swansea.ac.uk) Telephone: 01792 295602 If you would like more information about this area of research, please follow these references; Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 88, 249-262. Dymond, S., & Whelan, R. (2010). Derived relational responding: A comparison of matching to sample and the relational completion procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 94, 37-55. 46
  • 47. Appendix 3 – AAQ-II (Example) 47
  • 48. Appendix 4 – Computer instructions for Observed and Inferred Observed Phase 1: Habituation. “Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one of two coloured circles. Each circle will appear for 3 seconds. Your job is to simply watch the screen. If you have any questions please ask the experimenter now.” Phase 2: Pavlovian Avoidance and Phase 3: Avoidance Acquisition “You will now watch a short film of a person doing an experiment similar to the one you yourself are going to be doing afterwards. The person in the film is going to receive shocks paired with one of two coloured circles presented to him. Pay close attention to which coloured circle is followed by shocks and which is not. The person in the film is then going to learn to press one of two marked keys on the keyboard to cancel the upcoming shock. Again, it is important you pay close attention to the film, because, in the experiment you are going to do afterwards, you are going to have to learn which one of the marked keys cancels the upcoming shock. When you are ready to view the film, press ‘Continue’.” Phase 4: Extinction “Now you are going to take part in an experiment similar to the one you have just watched. You will be presented with the same number of coloured circles as the person in the film, but in a different order. Importantly, you will receive shocks paired with the same colour as the person in the film. However, you will be able to cancel upcoming shocks by pressing one of the marked keys on the keyboard. When you are ready to proceed, press ‘Continue’.” Derived RCP General Procedure; “Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. You will be presented with a series of images or nonsense words on the top half of the screen from left to right. Then you will be presented with 3 images or nonsense words on the bottom of the screen. Your task is to observe the images or words that appear from left to right and drag one of these images or words from the bottom to the blank, yellow square. Click and hold the mouse over the image or word to drag it to the blank square. To confirm your choice, then click ‘Finish Trial’. If you wish to make another choice, then click ‘Start Again’. Sometimes you will receive feedback on your choices, but at other times you will not. Your aim is to get as many tasks correct as possible. It is always possible to get a task correct, even if you are not given feedback.” Phase 5: Habituation “Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one of two words. Each word will appear for 3 seconds. Your job is to simply watch the screen. If you have any questions please ask the experimenter now.” Phase 6: Pavlovian Avoidance 48
  • 49. “In this phase, on every trial you will be presented with one of two words. Each word will appear for 3 seconds followed by either a 250ms shock or no shock. The shock will be set at the level you have just selected. At the end of this phase, you will be asked to make some ratings as to how much you expect shock to follow each of the words. Please follow the onscreen instructions, and if you have any questions please ask the experimenter now.” Phase 7: Avoidance Acquisition “In this phase, you will again be presented, on every trial, with one of two words. When a word appears on the screen, the marked keys on the keyboard will be available to be pressed. Pressing the correct key when one of the words appears will cancel a pending shock. The same key will cancel the shock for the remainder of the study. You can learn which key is correct by paying close attention to the screen. At the end of this phase you will be asked to make some ratings as to how much you expect a shock to follow each of the words. Please follow the onscreen instructions, and if you have any questions, please contact the experimenter now” Phase 8: Extinction “This phase is important. Remember that all of the phases you have completed are interrelated and that the words seen here are the same seen earlier.” 49
  • 50. Appendix 5 – Excel spreadsheets 50
  • 51. Appendix 6 – SPSS outputs 51